Which statement is inaccurate regarding peer interaction in the first two years?

  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts
  • PMC4840934

Curr Dir Psychol Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 Feb 1.

Published in final edited form as:

PMCID: PMC4840934

NIHMSID: NIHMS728824

Abstract

The study of children's social learning is a topic of central importance to our understanding of human development. Learning from others allows children to acquire information efficiently; however, not all information conveyed by others is accurate or worth learning. A large body of research conducted over the past decade has shown that preschoolers learn selectively from some individuals over others. In the present article we summarize our work and that of others on the developmental origins of selective social learning during infancy. The results of these studies indicate that infants are sensitive to a number of cues, including competence, age, and confidence, when deciding from whom to learn. We highlight the important implications of this research in improving our understanding of the cognitive and social skills necessary for selective learning, and point out promising avenues for future research.

Keywords: Trust, Infancy, Cognitive Development, Social Learning

Social learning is defined as learning that is influenced by observation of or interaction with another individual (Box, 1984). Social learning is widespread in animals, but humans are exceptional in their reliance on information communicated by others. In fact, many of the exceptional cognitive abilities of humans are accounted for by the predominance of social learning in our species, as proposed by the cultural intelligence hypothesis (Tomasello, 2014). However, social learning comes with potential pitfalls: the acquired information could be outdated, misleading, or inappropriate. Understanding when, how, and why individuals learn from others is critical to multiple disciplines (anthropology, economics, cultural evolution, and cognitive psychology) and has been the focus of much research over the past decade (Rendell, Fogarty, Hoppitt, Morgan, Webster & Laland, 2010). The present article explores this issue from a developmental science perspective by showing that from an early age children do not treat all communicated information as equally reliable, but are instead selective in their allocation of trust.

Over the past decade, developmental researchers have devoted great attention to children's selective social learning, or propensity to learn from some sources over others (Koenig & Sabbagh, 2013; Nurmsoo, Robinson & Butterfill, 2010). Much of this work was inspired by a landmark study by Koenig, Clément and Harris (2004), in which 3- and 4-year-olds witnessed two adults naming various familiar objects. One individual named the objects correctly, whereas the other made blatant errors (e.g., calling a ball “a shoe”). Children later preferentially sought and accepted novel words from the previously accurate individual. Multiple subsequent studies demonstrated that children learn selectively in many situations, and use a variety of cues in addition to individuals' accuracy to decide from whom to learn (see Mills, 2013, for a detailed review). The bulk of this work focused on preschool-age and older children; yet, learning is obviously central to children's development long before their third birthday. To better understand the importance of selective learning in children's information acquisition, it is important to study its emergence in the first few years of life. The body of work on infants' selective learning is nowhere near as extensive as that on preschoolers; yet, there is growing evidence that even in their earliest years infants are frequently discriminating when choosing sources of information.

Infants are selective learners

Human infants possess eminent social learning abilities, as shown by their precocious ability to imitate people's actions and monitor others' attention and emotional references when uncertain about a situation. Here, we focus on infants' sensitivity to individual attributes that can signal information quality in a learning context. Infants, like older children, attend to individuals' accuracy: for instance, they show surprise when someone provides inaccurate information (e.g., Koenig & Echols, 2003). This precocious sensitivity influences infants' learning. In the verbal domain, two-year-olds preferentially learn new labels, or accept second labels for familiar objects, from previously accurate rather than inaccurate speakers (Krogh-Jesperson & Echols, 2012; Koenig & Woodward, 2010). Eighteen-month-olds are less willing to imitate a novel action or learn new labels from inaccurate than accurate speakers (Brooker & Poulin-Dubois, 2013). Younger infants can use both verbal and nonverbal behaviors to infer competence. For example, 14-month-olds preferentially imitate an adult who uses familiar objects competently rather than incompetently (e.g., placing a shoe on his hand instead of his foot; Zmyj, Buttelmann, Carpenter & Daum, 2010), and imitate the novel actions of an individual who speaks their language over a foreign-language speaker (Buttelmann, Zmyj, Daum & Carpenter, 2013). Infants as young as 12 months seek information from “experts”: after witnessing an experimenter interact with toys either competently (expert) or non-competently (non-expert), infants attend to and interact with a new toy more when it is presented by the expert (Stenberg, 2013). Even infants as young as 8 months can track the reliability of potential informants: After being familiarized with faces cuing the locations of animated displays with different degrees of reliability (100% vs 25%), infants search longer at locations cued by the reliable face (Tummelsthammer, Wu, Sobel & Kirkham, 2014).

Given that infants are sensitive to emotional signals from the first months of life, they have the potential to recognize inaccurate emotional signalers, or people who display emotional expressions that are incongruent with the context (e.g., angry expression after receiving a desirable toy). Recent research based on looking time (Chiarella & Poulin-Dubois, 2014; Skerry & Spelke 2014) or pupil dilation measures (Hepach & Westerman, 2013) suggests that they do so by 8 months. In our laboratory, we have shown that infants as young as 14 months take into account the past reliability of an adult's affective signals when later interacting with that person. In some studies, infants were introduced to a person whose emotional signals were reliable: she showed positive facial and verbal expressions while looking inside a container and infants subsequently found an object inside the container. In another condition, an unreliable looker also expressed positive emotions, but the container later proved to be empty. In subsequent interactions, 14- to 16-month-olds were less likely to follow the looker's gaze (Chow, Poulin-Dubois & Lewis, 2008) and copy her action of turning on a lamp with her forehead (Poulin-Dubois, Brooker & Polonia, 2011) if she was unreliable rather than reliable. More recently, Chiarella and Poulin-Dubois (2014) examined 18-month-olds' imitation of someone who reacts negatively to positive experiences versus someone with the expected positive reaction. Although infants subsequently imitated the simple actions of both models (e.g., shaking a rattle), they were less willing to trust the unreliable emoter when she provided guidance about the content of two containers through emotional signals.

Infants also modulate their learning based on other cues that can serve as indirect competence signals (though it is unclear how infants interpret these cues, a point we return to below). One such cue is consensus: 24-month-olds, and to a lesser extent 18-month-olds, preferentially accept information that received agreement rather than disagreement (i.e., nodding vs. head shaking) by a third party (Fusaro & Harris, 2013). Age may also be considered an indirect indicator of competence, as adults typically know more than children. Infants sometimes imitate selectively based on age (e.g., Ryalls, Gull & Ryalls, 2000; Zmyj, Daum, Prinz, Nielsen & Aschersleben, 2012). However, the direction of this effect varies: infants sometimes imitate adults over children, but in other contexts are more likely to imitate a peer. For example, 14-month-olds preferentially imitate adults when observing novel actions (e.g., turning on a light with one's head) but preferentially imitate same-age infants when the action is already in their repertoire (e.g., pulling objects apart using their hands; Zmyj et al, 2012).

Another potential competence cue is confidence. Two-year-olds preferentially imitate novel actions demonstrated by an individual showing confidence rather than uncertainty (Birch, Akmal & Frampton, 2010). This sensitivity, however, appears to develop later than some other cues, as preferential imitation of confidently-demonstrated actions is present at 24 months but absent at 18 months (Brosseau-Liard & Poulin-Dubois, 2014).

Outstanding areas of investigation

Beyond acknowledging that infants can be selective in their social learning, which the studies reviewed above have demonstrated, much work is needed still to understand the exact nature of this ability. More specifically, the cognitive and motivational mechanisms underlying this precocious learning selectivity remain to be identified. Are these mechanisms stable or do they change across development? Given the current state of knowledge in this field, it is premature to offer a conclusive answer to this question. However, there are some hints at the form that this answer might take, as discussed below.

A rich theoretical account of children's social learning from others' testimony has recently been proposed (Sobel & Kushnir, 2013). In a nutshell, this proposal states that children make rational inferences about the reliability of testimony based on their prior knowledge plus new evidence about the informants they interact with. To what extent do similar inferences underlie the emergence of selective social learning? It is well established that infants are active information gatherers who are apt at using observed data to guide their actions and attention. For instance, infants' sensitivity to statistical regularities and capacity for probabilistic inferences are present very early in life (Aslin & Newport, 2012; Denison & Xu, 2014), and such general learning mechanisms may explain some of the earliest manifestations of selective learning. One outstanding question is whether infants make any inferences about the person's competence or knowledge, even implicitly.

In the preschooler literature, one specific cognitive mechanism has received a lot of attention: theory of mind, or the ability to reason about other people's mental states such as intentions, desires, knowledge and beliefs. Individuals' (in)accuracy could prompt children to attribute knowledge or ignorance and subsequently expect knowledgeable individuals to provide good information (and ignorant individuals bad information). Children may also make other mental-state-based inferences based on the behavior of potential informants, such as inferring that they intend to be informative or deceptive. If children are making these types of mental state inferences, and if these inferences are driving their propensity to be selective about the information they accept, then we would expect those children with superior theory-of-mind skills to be more successful on selective learning tasks.

A few recent studies have demonstrated associations in preschoolers between theory of mind and greater selective learning from accurate individuals (e.g., DiYanni, Nini, Rheel & Livelli, 2012; Lucas, Lewis, Pala, Berridge & Wong, 2013). Additionally, in a recent study (Brosseau-Liard, Penney & Poulin-Dubois, 2015), we showed that success on a selective learning task based on informants' prior accuracy was predicted by children's theory-of-mind skills, whereas theory of mind did not predict performance on a selective learning task where informants were differentiated by an attribute that is unrelated to knowledge (specifically, physical strength). This suggests that theory of mind likely contributes to older children's abilities at selectively learning from reliable individuals.

One key question for future research: Is this also true of infants? Over the past decade, it has been proposed that infants possess precocious theory-of-mind competencies when their abilities are assessed with age-appropriate tasks (Baillargeon, Scott & He, 2010), but infants' level of understanding is still hotly debated, and a substantial research effort is attempting to clarify this question (Ruffman, 2014). We believe that selective learning in infants and very young children likely emerges with the support of some form of understanding of humans' motivational states such as goals and “intentions-in-action” that does not require a representational theory of mind, that is, an understanding of false belief (Sodian, 2011). For example, based on non-verbal cues such as vocalizations and gestures, infants can detect when someone's action is intentional or accidental or the unfulfilled intention behind an incomplete action (Olineck & Poulin-Dubois, 2009). With age, children gradually integrate their initial ability for selective trust with their richer understanding of mental states and their increasing comprehension of pragmatics and communication, including the comprehension of situations that lead others to make mistaken statements, tell playful jokes, or intentionally mislead others. This likely accounts for more sophisticated and nuanced selectivity in the later preschool years. However, this account is purely speculative at this point, as there is no research directly comparing the selective learning performance of toddlers and preschoolers and limited investigation of the cognitive mechanisms underlying selective learning. We plan to conduct future research on these issues. Furthermore, the current literature contains disproportionate speculation regarding how children interpret individuals' accuracy; we plan to also investigate the ways in which infants and young children interpret all the other cues reviewed above – confidence, age, and expertise, to name a few.

Lastly, much of the selective learning literature, and of research on imitation and learning more generally, focuses exclusively on the cognitive benefits of learning in terms of the acquisition of useful information. However, copying behaviors can also be motivated by a desire for affiliation (Over & Carpenter, 2012). With older children, one can witness selective social learning based on attributes that would seem much more relevant to a desire for affiliation than to accurate information seeking, for instance preferring a model who displays similarity with the child (Reyes-Jaquez & Echols, 2013) or imitates the child (Over, Carpenter, Spears & Gattis, 2013). Probing infants' motivations in attending to different model attributes will shed light on the purpose of these biases.

Conclusions

We have reviewed substantial recent work showing that infants are selective social learners: They are remarkably sensitive to a range of model-based characteristics when deciding from whom to learn. While the laboratory-based studies that we reviewed provide clear evidence that infants possess model-based social learning strategies, one might wonder to what extent such strategies are necessary in the real world? After all, rarely do adults intentionally call a ball a dog or put a sock on their hand, except playfully. Nonetheless, infants can be exposed to speakers who are not fluent in their native language; to practical jokers, dishonest, or absent-minded people; and to older siblings who are ignorant in many learning contexts. Sensitivity to attributes that indicate whether someone is a good or bad source of information is thus likely to have a real impact on learning, even in infancy. Furthermore, many of the cues used in infancy are at least superficially similar to those used in social learning across the animal kingdom. For example, small fish are sensitive to the age, size, and familiarity of their tutors (Rendell et al., 2010). From an evolutionary perspective, it would be beneficial for young children to be predisposed to attend to behaviors that signal knowledge, success, or status, as these cues could indicate that an individual's behaviors are adaptive and worth copying (Chudek, Brosseau-Liard, Birch & Henrich, 2013). The early emergence of many selective learning skills is consistent with this perspective.

We believe that the continued investigation of selective social learning in infancy will have important implications for developmental science. In and of itself, this research will further our understanding of the learning and affiliation strategies and abilities present in the first few years of life. Additionally, this work can make crucial contributions to our knowledge of the cognitive and social underpinnings of social learning strategies throughout the lifespan.

Acknowledgments

This paper was supported by a Social Sciences and Humanities Research Council of Canada (SSHRC) grant (435-2012-1403) to Diane Poulin-Dubois and by a postdoctoral fellowship from SSHRC (756-2012-0284) and a Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (NSERC) grant (2015-05775) to Patricia Brosseau-Liard. Both authors express their gratitude to Susan Birch and to Jacqueline Legacy for their helpful comments on previous drafts of this manuscript.

Contributor Information

Diane Poulin-Dubois, Concordia University.

Patricia Brosseau-Liard, University of Ottawa.

References

1. Aslin RN, Newport EL. Statistical learning: From acquiring specific items to forming general rules. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2012;21:170–176. doi: 10.1177/0963721412436806. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

2. Baillargeon R, Scott RM, He Z. False-belief understanding in infants. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2010;14:110–118. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2009.12.006. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

3. Birch SAJ, Akmal N, Frampton KL. Two-year-olds are vigilant of others' non-verbal cues to credibility. Developmental Science. 2010;13:363–369. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2009.00906.x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

4. Box HO. Primate Behavior and Social Ecology. Chapman & Hall; London: 1984. [Google Scholar]

5. Brooker I, Poulin-Dubois D. Is a bird an apple? The effect of speaker labeling accuracy on infants' word learning, imitation, and helping behaviors. Infancy. 2013;18:E46–E68. doi: 10.1111/infa.12027. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

6. Brosseau-Liard P, Penney D, Poulin-Dubois D. Theory of mind selectively predicts preschoolers' knowledge-based selective word learning. British Journal of Developmental Psychology. 2015 doi: 10.1111/bjdp.12107. Advance online publication. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

7. Brosseau-Liard PE, Poulin-Dubois D. Sensitivity to confidence cues increases during the second year of life. Infancy. 2014;19:461–475. doi: 10.1111/infa.12056. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

8. Buttelmann D, Zmyj N, Daum M, Carpenter M. Selective imitation of in-group over out-group members in 14-month-old infants. Child Development. 2013;84:422–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2012.01860.x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

9. Chiarella SS, Poulin-Dubois D. “Are you really sad?” Infants show selectivity in their behaviors towards an unreliable emoter. Manuscript submitted for publication 2014 [Google Scholar]

10. Chiarella SS, Poulin-Dubois D. Cry babies and pollyannas: Infants can detect unjustified emotional reactions. Infancy. 2013;18:E81–E96. doi: 10.1111/infa.12028. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

11. Chow V, Poulin-Dubois D, Lewis J. To see or not to see: infants prefer to follow the gaze of a reliable looker. Developmental Science. 2008;11:761–70. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-7687.2008.00726.x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

12. Chudek M, Brosseau-Liard P, Birch S, Henrich J. Culture-gene coevolutionary theory and children's selective social learning. In: Banaji M, Gelman S, editors. Navigating the Social World: What Infants, Children, and Other Species Can Teach Us. Oxford University Press; 2013. pp. 181–185. [Google Scholar]

13. Denison S, Xu F. The origins of probabilistic inference in human infants. Cognition. 2014;130:335–347. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

14. DiYanni C, Nini D, Rheel W, Livelli A. ‘I won't trust you if I think you're trying to deceive me’: Relations between selective trust, theory of mind, and imitation in early childhood. Journal of Cognition and Development. 2012;13:354–371. doi: 10.1080/15248372.2011.590462. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

15. Fusaro M, Harris PL. Dax gets the nod: Toddlers detect and use social cues to evaluate testimony. Developmental Psychology. 2013;49:514–22. doi: 10.1037/a0030580. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

16. Hepach R, Westermann G. Infants' sensitivity to the congruence of others' emotions and actions. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2013;115:16–29. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2012.12.013. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

17. Koenig M, Clément F, Harris P. Trust in testimony: Children's use of true and false statements. Psychological Science. 2004;15:694–699. doi: 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2004.00742.x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

18. Koenig M, Echols C. Infants' understanding of false labeling events: The referential roles of words and the speakers who use them. Cognition. 2003;87:179–208. doi: 10.1016/S0. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

19. Koenig MA, Sabbagh MA. Selective social learning: New perspectives on learning from others. Developmental Psychology. 2013;49:399–403. doi: 10.1037/a0031619. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

20. Krogh-Jespersen S, Echols CH. The influence of speaker reliability on first versus second label learning. Child Development. 2012;83(2):581–90. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2011.01713.x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

21. Lucas AJ, Lewis C, Pala FC, Wong K, Berridge D. Social-cognitive processes in preschoolers' selective trust: Three cultures compared. Developmental Psychology. 2013;49:579–590. doi: 10.1037/a0029864. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

22. Mills CM. Knowing when to doubt: Developing a critical stance when learning from others. Developmental Psychology. 2013;49:404–18. doi: 10.1037/a0029500. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

23. Nurmsoo E, Robinson EJ, Butterfill SA. Children's selective learning from others. Review of Philosophy and Psychology. 2010;1:551–561. doi: 10.1007/s13164-010-0043-y. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

24. Olineck K, Poulin-Dubois D. Infants' understanding of intention from 10 to 14 months: Interrelations among visual attention and imitation tasks. Infant Behavior and Development. 2009;32:404–415. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2009.06.05. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

25. Over H, Carpenter M. The social side of imitation. Child Development Perspectives. 2013;7:6–11. doi: 10.1111/cdep.12006. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

26. Over H, Carpenter M, Spears R, Gattis M. Children selectively trust individuals who have imitated them. Social Development. 2013;22:215–224. doi: 10.1111/sode.12020. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

27. Poulin-Dubois D, Brooker I, Polonia A. Infants prefer to imitate a reliable person. Infant Behavior & Development. 2011;34:303–309. doi: 10.1016/j.infbeh.2011.01.006. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

28. Reyes-Jaquez B, Echols CH. Developmental differences in the relative weighing of informants' social attributes. Developmental Psychology. 2013;49:602–613. doi: 10.1037/a0031674. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

29. Rendell L, Fogarty L, Hoppitt WJ, Morgan TJ, Webster MM, Laland KN. Cognitive culture: Theoretical and empirical insights into social learning strategies. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2011;15:68–76. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2010.12.002. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

30. Ruffman T. To belief or not belief: Children's theory of mind. Developmental Review. 2014;34:265–293. doi: 10.1016/j.dr.2014.04.001. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

31. Ryalls BO, Gul RE, Ryalls KR. Infant imitation of peer and adult models: Evidence for a peer model advantage. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly. 2000;46:188–202. [Google Scholar]

32. Skerry AE, Spelke ES. Preverbal infants identify emotional reactions that are incongruent with goal outcomes. Cognition. 2014;130:204–216. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2013.11.002. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

33. Sobel DM, Kushnir T. Knowledge matters: How children evaluate the reliability of testimony as a process of rational inference. Psychological Review. 2013;120:779–797. doi: 10.1037/a0034191. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

34. Sodian B. Theory of Mind in Infancy. Child Development Perspectives. 2011;5:39–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1750-8606.2010.00152.x. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

35. Stenberg G. Do 12-month-old infants trust a competent adult? Infancy. 2013;18:873–904. doi: 10.1111/infa.12011. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

36. Tomasello M. A Natural History of Human Thinking. Harvard University Press; 2014. [Google Scholar]

37. Tummeltshammer KS, Wu R, Sobel DM, Kirkham NZ. Infants track the reliability of potential informants. Psychological Science. 2014;25:1730–1738. doi: 10.1177/0956797614540178. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

38. Zmyj N, Buttelmann D, Carpenter M, Daum MM. The reliability of a model influences 14-month-olds' imitation. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology. 2010;106:208–20. doi: 10.1016/j.jecp.2010.03.002. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

39. Zmyj N, Daum MM, Prinz W, Nielsen M, Aschersleben G. Fourteen-month-olds' imitation of differently aged models. Infant and Child Development. 2012;21:250–266. doi: 10.1002/icd.750. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

Recommended Readings

1•. Harris PL, Lane JD. Infants understand how testimony works. Topoi. 2014;33:443–458. This paper discusses the foundations of the ability to learn from others in infancy. [Google Scholar]

2•. Koenig M, Clément M, Harris PL. See reference list. 2004 One of the first papers to test selective learning in young children. [Google Scholar]

3•. Mills C. See reference list. 2013 This paper provides a comprehensive review of the past decade of research examining children's selective social learning. [Google Scholar]

4•. Lucas AJ, Lewis C. Should we trust experiments on trust? Human Development. 2010;53:167–172. A more thorough discussion of the possible reasons underlying children's performance on selective learning tasks, including mental state reasoning and shallower processes. [Google Scholar]

5•. Over H, Carpenter M. See reference list. 2013 This paper discusses motivational aspects of some source selection tasks, more specifically those involving imitation. [Google Scholar]

How old are children when they first begin to manifest stress in reaction to the death of a significant other?

Children between five and nine begin to think more like adults about death, yet they still believe it will never happen to them or anyone they know.

Which of the following statements accurately describes moral development in early childhood?

Which of the following statements accurately describes moral development in early childhood? Perspective taking and being able to understand how others think and feel make children more empathic at this age.

Why might later born children be more popular with their peers quizlet?

Why might later-born children be more popular with their peers? They learn social skills by dealing with their older siblings. What is the difference between associative play and cooperative play? Cooperative play involves the purposes of the group.

Which of the following is accurate regarding attachment during infancy?

Which of the following is accurate regarding attachment during infancy? when an infant fails to attach properly to caregivers, the consequences are damaging to his or her emotional, physical, social, and psychological well-being; attachment is constructed through interactions of an infant with primary caregivers.

Toplist

Neuester Beitrag

Stichworte