A primary goal of treatment using the cognitive perspective on psychological disorders is to

A primary goal of treatment using the cognitive perspective on psychological disorders is to

Show

  • A primary goal of treatment using the cognitive perspective on psychological disorders is to
    Access through your institution

A primary goal of treatment using the cognitive perspective on psychological disorders is to

A primary goal of treatment using the cognitive perspective on psychological disorders is to

ReviewDepression: A cognitive perspective☆

Abstract

Cognitive science has been instrumental in advancing our understanding of the onset, maintenance, and treatment of depression. Research conducted over the last 50 years supports the proposition that depression and risk for depression are characterized by the operation of negative biases, and often by a lack of positive biases, in self-referential processing, interpretation, attention, and memory, as well as the use of maladaptive cognitive emotion regulation strategies. There is also evidence to suggest that deficits in cognitive control over mood-congruent material underlie these cognitive processes. Specifically, research indicates that difficulty inhibiting and disengaging from negative material in working memory: (1) increases the use of maladaptive emotion regulation strategies (e.g., rumination), decreases the use of adaptive emotion regulation strategies (e.g., reappraisal), and potentially impedes flexible selection and implementation of emotion regulation strategies; (2) is associated with negative biases in attention; and (3) contributes to negative biases in long-term memory. Moreover, studies suggest that these cognitive processes exacerbate and sustain the negative mood that typifies depressive episodes. In this review, we present evidence in support of this conceptualization of depression and discuss implications of research findings for theory and practice. Finally, we advance directions for future research.

Introduction

Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) is one of the most prevalent and debilitating forms of psychopathology (Kessler et al., 2005). Epidemiological surveys indicate that the lifetime prevalence of MDD is 16.6%, with estimates as high as 21.3% in women (Kessler, Berglund, et al., 2005; Kessler & Bromet, 2013); indeed, more than 30 million U.S. adults have met criteria for MDD in their lifetime (Haro et al., 2006). Importantly, MDD is a highly recurrent disorder; moreover, each depressive episode increases the likelihood that individuals will develop a subsequent episode of MDD (Solomon et al., 2000). Depression is also associated with enormous costs at both the individual and societal level; in fact, depression continues to be the leading cause of disability worldwide (World Health Organization, 2017), accounting for almost half of disability-adjusted life years (World Health Organization, 2012). Finally, in addition to documented adverse effects of depression on interpersonal relationships, educational attainment, and financial security (Kessler & Wang, 2009), MDD has been associated both concurrently and prospectively with poor physical health, cardiac problems, and cancer (Knol et al., 2006; Luppino et al., 2010).

Given the high prevalence and substantial burden of depression, it is not surprising that investigators have conducted a great deal of research with the goal of increasing our understanding of the onset, maintenance, and treatment of depressive episodes. In this review, we will examine how cognitive science, in particular, has promoted our understanding of depression. The current review advances our understanding of cognition and depression by including recent research that capitalizes on methodological developments (e.g., eye tracking technology) and techniques (cognitive bias modification paradigms), by offering a theoretical model (Fig. 1) that depicts the relations among cognitive factors and depression, and by discussing in detail the clinical implications of this work. We begin by providing an historical overview of cognitive theories of depression. We then review major advances in our understanding of cognition and depression, focusing specifically on cognitive deficits in executive functioning, working memory, and processing speed; cognitive biases in self-referential processing, attention, interpretation, and memory; deficits in cognitive control over stimuli or information that is congruent with one's emotional state (i.e., mood-congruent material); and the cognitive emotion regulation strategies of rumination, distraction, and reappraisal. Fig. 1 depicts the relations among cognitive factors that are most strongly supported by empirical evidence. Next, we discuss the implications of empirical findings for theory and practice. Finally, we offer our perspective on what we think lies ahead for researchers in this area. Certainly, researchers have studied other aspects of cognition, including goal selection, response selection, performance monitoring, and language; however, because these are relatively less studied in relation to depression, they are outside the scope of this review.

Section snippets

Historical overview of cognitive theories of depression

Researchers and clinicians have long acknowledged that cognition plays a critical role in the onset and maintenance of depressive disorders. Fifty years ago, Beck (1967) posited that biased acquisition and processing of information influence the etiology and course of depressive episodes. Beck argued that internal mental representations, or schemas, affect how depressed individuals perceive themselves and the world around them. He contended that individuals with depression have mood-congruent

Empirical findings

Over the past several decades a significant methodological shift has taken place in the measurement of cognition, and in particular, cognitive biases. The field moved away from an exclusive reliance on self-report measures of cognition toward emphasizing objective indices and experimental manipulations. This methodological shift reflects investigators' increased awareness of the drawbacks of self-report measures, which, for example, are limited by individuals' introspective ability and are

Implications for theory

Findings over the last several decades have largely supported the cognitive models of depression formulated by Beck, Bower, and Teasdale by providing evidence of the role of cognitive biases in the onset, maintenance, and recurrence of depressive episodes. Importantly, however, empirical findings have also served to expand and refine those early cognitive theories of depression in several important ways that are depicted in Fig. 1. For example, studies have highlighted the important role that

Clinical implications

At present, fewer than 40% of individuals who are treated for MDD achieve symptom remission with initial treatment (Gaynes et al., 2009; Holtzheimer & Mayberg, 2011). Given evidence that cognitive biases and emotion regulation strategies influence the onset, maintenance, and recurrence of depressive symptoms, cognition is an important target for intervention. Both theoretical models of depression and empirical findings suggest that ameliorating maladaptive cognition can reduce depressive

Future directions

Over the past several decades we have made exciting progress in our understanding of depression. What should we work towards and expect over the next several years? First and foremost, despite considerable progress in the measurement of cognition in depression, the psychometric properties of many information-processing tasks are still unknown or poor (Brown et al., 2014; Schmukle, 2005). It is critical that researchers use experimental methods and tasks with strong psychometric properties.

Take-home points

Over the last several decades, researchers have made major advances in elucidating factors involved in the onset and maintenance of depression, and have generated a number of important conclusions that have influenced the field; these findings are depicted in the model presented in Fig. 1. First, investigators have documented that depression is characterized by both general cognitive deficits (e.g., impairments in executive functioning and memory) and negative cognitive biases (i.e., the

Role of funding sources

Preparation of this paper was facilitated by Brain & Behavior Research Foundation Young Investigator Award 22337 to JL, SSHRC Grant 430-2017-00408 to JL, and NIMH Grants MH101495 and MH111978 to IHG.

Contributors

JL and IHG discussed the focus of the current review. JL wrote the first draft of the manuscript, and IHG contributed to and approved the final manuscript.

Conflict of interest

All authors declare that they have no conflicts of interest.

Joelle LeMoult is currently an Assistant Professor in the Department of Psychology at the University of British Columbia. The overarching goal of her research is to further our understanding of the onset, maintenance, and recurrence of Major Depressive Disorder (MDD). Using a multimodal approach, she examines the cognitive, emotional, and biological responses to environmental stressors that contribute to symptoms of depression and comorbid anxiety disorders.

References (260)

  • et al.

    Effects of selective serotonin reuptake and dual serotonergic-noradrenergic reuptake treatments on attention and executive functions in patients with major depressive disorder

    Psychiatry Research

    (2010)

  • E.P. Hayden et al.

    Cognitive vulnerability to depression during middle childhood: Stability and associations with maternal affective styles and parental depression

    Personality and Individual Differences

    (2013)

  • M.M. Grant et al.

    Cognitive disturbance in outpatient depressed younger adults: evidence of modest impairment

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2001)

  • P.R. Goldin et al.

    Trajectories of change in emotion regulation and social anxiety during cognitive-behavioral therapy for social anxiety disorder

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2014)

  • E. Goeleven et al.

    Deficient inhibition of emotional information in depression

    Journal of Affective Disorders

    (2006)

  • M.A. Gaddy et al.

    A meta-analytic review of mood-congruent implicit memory in depressed mood

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (2014)

  • J. Everaert et al.

    The combined cognitive bias hypothesis in depression

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (2012)

  • M. Eizenman et al.

    A naturalistic visual scanning approach to assess selective attention in major depressive disorder

    Psychiatry Research

    (2003)

  • C. Donaldson et al.

    Rumination and attention in major depression

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2007)

  • I. Demeyer et al.

    Rumination mediates the relationship between impaired cognitive control for emotional information and depressive symptoms: a prospective study in remitted depressed adults

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (2012)

  • E. De Lissnyder et al.

    Internal cognitive control in clinical depression: General but no emotion-specific impairments

    Psychiatry Research

    (2012)

  • C. Chen et al.

    Remembrance of happy things past: positive autobiographical memories are intrinsically rewarding and valuable, but not in depression

    Frontiers in Psychology

    (2015)

  • G. Butler et al.

    Cognitive processes in anxiety

    Advances in Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1983)

  • M. Browning et al.

    Using attentional bias modification as a cognitive vaccine against depression

    Biological Psychiatry

    (2012)

  • H.M. Brown et al.

    Journal of Anxiety Disorders

    (2014)

  • T. Brockmeyer et al.

    Mood-incongruent processing during the recall of a sad life event predicts the course and severity of depression

    Journal of Affective Disorders

    (2015)

  • B.P. Bradley et al.

    Implicit memory bias in clinical and non-clinical depression

    Behaviour Research and Therapy

    (1996)

  • S. Baert et al.

    Attentional bias training in depression: Therapeutic effects depend on depression severity

    Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry

    (2010)

  • J.M. Azorin et al.

    Stimulus preprocessing and response selection in depression: A reaction time study

    Acta Psychologica

    (1995)

  • T. Armstrong et al.

    Eye tracking of attention in the affective disorders: A meta-analytic review and synthesis

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (2012)

  • R.J. Anderson et al.

    Mental time travel in dysphoria: Differences in the content and subjective experience of past and future episodes

    Consciousness and Cognition

    (2015)

  • A. Aldao et al.

    Emotion-regulation strategies across psychopathology: A meta-analytic review

    Clinical Psychology Review

    (2010)

  • E. Ahern et al.

    Cognitive functioning in the first-episode of major depressive disorder: A systematic review and meta-analysis

    Neuropsychology

    (2017)

  • A. Aldao et al.

    One versus many: Capturing the use of multiple emotion regulation strategies in response to an emotion-eliciting stimulus

    Cognition & Emotion

    (2013)

  • L.B. Alloy et al.

    Judgment of contingency in depressed and nondepressed students: sadder but wiser?

    Journal of Experimental Psychology: General

    (1979)

  • A. Althubaiti

    Information bias in health research: definition, pitfalls, and adjustment methods

    Journal of Multidisciplinary Healthcare

    (2016)

  • American Psychiatric Association

    Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders

    (2013)

  • M.C. Anderson et al.

    Suppressing unwanted memories by executive control

    Nature

    (2001)

  • K.A. Arditte et al.

    Emotion regulation in depression: Reflection predicts recovery from a major depressive episode

    Cognitive Therapy and Research

    (2011)

  • K.A. Arditte Hall et al.

    Positive memory enhancement training for individuals with major depressive disorder

    Cognitive Behaviour Therapy

    (2018)

  • R.P. Auerbach et al.

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2015)

  • A. Baddeley

    Working memory

    Science

    (1992)

  • A. Baddeley

    Exploring the central executive

    Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology

    (1996)

  • A.T. Beck

    Depression: Clinical, experimental, and theoretical aspects

    (1967)

  • A.T. Beck

    Beck depression inventory

    Depression

    (1978)

  • A.T. Beck et al.

    A unified model of depression: integrating clinical, cognitive, biological, and evolutionary perspectives

    Clinical Psychological Science: A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science

    (2016)

  • J.S. Beck

    Cognitive behavior therapy: basics and beyond

    (2011)

  • M. Beckwé et al.

    Worrying and rumination are both associated with reduced cognitive control

    Psychological Research

    (2014)

  • C.G. Beevers et al.

    Attentional bias and mood persistence as prospective predictors of dysphoria

    Cognitive Therapy and Research

    (2003)

  • C.G. Beevers et al.

    Attention bias modification for major depressive disorder: Effects on attention bias, resting state connectivity, and symptom change

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2015)

  • M.S. Bisson et al.

    The effect of depressed mood on the interpretation of ambiguity, with and without negative mood induction

    Cognition and Emotion

    (2007)

  • R.A. Bjork

    Theoretical implications of directed forgetting

  • S.E. Blackwell et al.

    Modifying interpretation and imagination in clinical depression: a single case series using cognitive bias modification

    Applied Cognitive Psychology

    (2010)

  • G.A. Bonanno et al.

    Regulatory flexibility: An individual differences perspective on coping and emotion regulation

    Perspectives on Psychological Science

    (2013)

  • G.H. Bower

    Mood and memory

    American Journal of Psychology

    (1981)

  • D.G. Byrne

    Choice reaction times in depressive states

    The British Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology

    (1976)

  • L. Campbell-Sills et al.

    Incorporating emotion regulation into conceptualizations and treatments of anxiety and mood disorders

  • T. Canli et al.

    Brain activation to emotional words in depressed vs healthy subjects

    Neuroreport

    (2004)

  • X. Caseras et al.

    Biases in visual orienting to negative and positive scenes in dysphoria: An eye movement study

    Journal of Abnormal Psychology

    (2007)

  • T. Chakrabarty et al.

    Cognitive dysfunction in major depressive disorder: assessment, impact, and management

    FOCUS: The Journal of Lifelong Learning in Psychiatry

    (2016)

  • Cited by (187)

    Joelle LeMoult is currently an Assistant Professor in the Department of Psychology at the University of British Columbia. The overarching goal of her research is to further our understanding of the onset, maintenance, and recurrence of Major Depressive Disorder (MDD). Using a multimodal approach, she examines the cognitive, emotional, and biological responses to environmental stressors that contribute to symptoms of depression and comorbid anxiety disorders.

    View full text

    © 2018 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved.

    What is the primary goal of treatment using the cognitive perspective?

    The major goal of cognitive therapy is to teach you a method for testing the accuracy of your thoughts so that you are able to "lighten your load of unnecessary emotional baggage" and use the extra energy to pursue more productive courses in life.

    Which of the following is the essence of the cognitive perspective?

    The essence of cognition is judgment, which occurs when a certain object is distinguished from other objects and is characterized by some concept or concepts. The psychologist is concerned with cognitive processes as they affect learning and behaviour.

    Which perspective assumes that psychological causes are at the root of psychological disorders?

    The medical perspective. Those who hold a medical perspective focus on biological and physiological factors as causes of abnormal behavior, which is treated as a disease, or mental illness, and is diagnosed through symptoms and cured through treatment.

    Which of the following is true of the cognitive approach to treatment?

    Which of the following is true of the cognitive approach to treatment? It is flexible and open to incorporating elements of other approaches to treatment.