Positive mental health involves a sense of psychological well-being, which depends upon having a

Summary

Read the full fact sheet

  • Every aspect of your life influences your state of wellbeing.
  • Some people believe that wealth is a fast track to happiness. Yet various international studies have shown that it’s the quality of our personal relationships, not the size of our bank balances, which has the greatest effect on our state of wellbeing.
  • Keeping track of a population's wellbeing helps governments to decide on particular policies.

This page has been produced in consultation with and approved by:

Positive mental health involves a sense of psychological well-being, which depends upon having a

Positive mental health involves a sense of psychological well-being, which depends upon having a

This page has been produced in consultation with and approved by:

Positive mental health involves a sense of psychological well-being, which depends upon having a

Positive mental health involves a sense of psychological well-being, which depends upon having a

  • The following services provide specialised healthcare for Aboriginal and Torres Strait Islander people in Victoria.

  • The Alexander technique stresses that movement should be economical and needs only the minimum amount of energy and effort.

  • Well-managed anger can be a useful emotion that motivates you to make positive changes.

  • There are many people you can talk to who can help you overcome feelings of wanting to lash out.

  • Anxiety disorders are common mental health problems that affect many people.

From other websites

Content disclaimer

Content on this website is provided for information purposes only. Information about a therapy, service, product or treatment does not in any way endorse or support such therapy, service, product or treatment and is not intended to replace advice from your doctor or other registered health professional. The information and materials contained on this website are not intended to constitute a comprehensive guide concerning all aspects of the therapy, product or treatment described on the website. All users are urged to always seek advice from a registered health care professional for diagnosis and answers to their medical questions and to ascertain whether the particular therapy, service, product or treatment described on the website is suitable in their circumstances. The State of Victoria and the Department of Health shall not bear any liability for reliance by any user on the materials contained on this website.

  • Journal List
  • HHS Author Manuscripts
  • PMC7031015

Res Aging. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2020 Mar 1.

Published in final edited form as:

PMCID: PMC7031015

NIHMSID: NIHMS1061298

Abstract

Parents who have a child with a developmental problem or mental disorder often provide support and assistance to their child throughout their lives and the burden of caregiving can have an adverse impact on parents’ mental and physical health. Using Erikson’s theory as a framework, the present study investigated generativity as a moderator of the effects of parenting a child with a disability on parents’ well-being during mid to late life. Using data from the Study of Midlife in the United States, we identified 220 parents who had a child with a disability and 3784 parents whose children did not have a disability. Regression analyses showed that the effect of parenting a child with a disability on negative affect, positive affect, and physical health was conditional on both parental gender and generativity, with mothers experiencing greater adverse effects of parenting but showing a benefit from high levels of generativity.

Keywords: generativity, family caregiving, ageing, well-being

Parents of adult children with developmental disabilities or mental health problems are often the primary sources of support and care for their child. Active parenting does not end for these parents when the child reaches adulthood. Instead, many of these individuals continue to provide care and assistance to their adult offspring and the long-term burden of providing this assistance has been shown to take a toll on aging parents’ well-being (Seltzer, Floyd, Song, Greenberg, & Hong, 2011). However, the internal resources that parents bring to their role may shape the way that caregiving influences their physical and mental health. The purpose of the present study was to explore generativity as a psychological resource that may buffer the effects of parenting a child with disabilities on parental health and well-being in mid to late life.

Erikson’s (1950, 1968) developmental theory proposed that the life span could be divided into eight stages with a unique developmental challenge, or crisis, characterizing each stage. He saw development as a process that unfolded as the individual worked to actively resolve the relevant challenge, gaining competence and maturity through the process. During midlife (Erikson’s seventh stage), the focus is generativity, which is defined as a concern for nurturing, establishing, and guiding the well-being of future generations through productive and creative endeavors. This concern is based on both a psychological need to be needed and the societal expectation that each generation will contribute to those people and cultural institutions that will succeed them (Erikson, Erikson, & Kivnick, 1986). For many adults, parenting is a primary means of establishing generativity, but Erikson himself noted that parenthood is neither necessary nor sufficient for achieving generativity. Rather, generative expression involves wide involvement in areas that “guarantee the maintenance of the world” (Erikson et al., 1986, p. 50), such as participation in the lives of children (one’s own and others), teaching, mentoring, volunteering, and helping to strengthen valued institutions and practices (McAdams & de St. Aubin, 1992; Villar, 2012).

By defining generativity as wide involvement in areas that ensure the welfare of the next generation, Erikson implied that generative individuals would be involved in a broad range of social roles. He saw social involvement as imperative for healthy adult development and conducive to generative expression because a more extensive role set would provide more opportunities for such expressions (Erikson et al., 1986). The importance of social involvement can be seen in Erikson’s account of failure to achieve generativity, which he described as a sense of stagnation involving excessive self-concern and self-indulgence, along with “interpersonal impoverishment” (Erikson, 1968, p. 138). Indeed, generativity has been linked with a diverse array of social activities including paid employment, participation in political or charitable causes, and religious or spiritual engagement (Peterson & Duncan, 1999; Son & Wilson, 2011; Versey & Newton, 2013).

According to Erikson, successful resolution of the challenges at each life stage makes a positive contribution to an individual’s psychological well-being. In support of this idea, generativity has been consistently linked with life satisfaction, happiness, and psychological well-being (An & Cooney, 2006; Keyes & Ryff, 1998; Serrat, Villar, Pratt, & Stukas, 2016). Adults with higher perceived generativity report lower levels of depressive symptomology, greater feelings of self-efficacy, more favorable patterns of physical functioning, and greater satisfaction with work (Colby, Sippola, & Phelps, 2001; Gruenewald, Karlamangla, Greendale, Singer, & Seeman, 2009; Gruenewald, Liao, & Seeman, 2012). Generativity has been linked with better adjustment to later-life transitions including grandparenting (Thiele & Whelan, 2008), and retirement (Serrat et al., 2016).

Because generativity involves a “need to be needed”, it is logical to expect that parenting a child with a disability might lead to enhanced feelings of generativity. However, parenthood itself does not necessarily predict generativity (Einolf, 2014), and the relationship between generativity and psychological well-being is equally robust for parents and childless adults (An & Cooney, 2006; Rothrauff & Cooney, 2008). One study that used the same dataset as the present study found that individuals who gave personal care to a friend or relative in the previous year reported higher perceptions of generativity relative to non-caregivers, suggesting that caregiving was a source of generative perceptions (Grossman & Gruenewald, 2017). However, most care recipients in that study were either aging parents or spouses, and the experience of these caregivers may not generalize to parents of children with developmental problems or mental disorders, who may assume the caregiving role for decades. The preponderance of evidence is that rather than enhancing well-being, parenting a child with a disability has a cumulative wear and tear effect (Namkung, Greenberg, Mailick, & Floyd, 2018; Seltzer et al., 2011).

The long-term task of raising a child with a disability is accompanied by substantial exposure to stressors such as extra financial obligations related to the child’s condition, time demands, managing behavior problems, and worry about the child’s future (Brehaut et al., 2011; Lecavalier, Leone, & Wiltz, 2006; Weiss, 2002). This increased exposure to stressors can increase risk for mental and physical health problems, and evidence shows that parents of a child with a disability often experience more physical health symptoms, depressive symptoms, functional limitations on health, and poorer psychological well-being than parents with a child without a disability (Ha, Hong, Seltzer, & Greenberg, 2008; Namkung et al., 2018; Smith & Grzywacz, 2014). Substantial evidence, including a meta-analysis of over two hundred studies, indicates that mothers are more susceptible to these effects than fathers. Specifically, the meta-analysis concluded that caregiving increased gender differences in depression and physical health, with women experiencing more negative outcomes (Pinquart & Sörensen, 2006).

Generativity may play a role in determining how caring for a child with disabilities will affect parental physical and psychological well-being. As noted, Erikson’s theory (1950; 1968) proposes that successful negotiation of the relevant psychosocial challenge at each life stage produces inner strength, maturity and competence. These assets increase the likelihood that the individual will positively resolve subsequent developmental crises, but it is plausible that they also represent psychological resources for dealing with other life challenges. In support of this idea, generativity prospectively predicted reduced subjective burden among midlife women caring for aging parents (Peterson, 2002), and it moderated the effects of depression on the need to cut back on work among midlife caregivers (Grossman & Gruenewald, 2017). A study of World War II veterans found that veterans who had experienced heavy active combat and achieved generativity (coded dichotomously based on qualitative interviews) were consistently healthier, happier, and less distressed than those who failed to achieve generativity (Ardelt, Landes, & Vaillant, 2010). While these three studies suggest that generativity buffered the psychological effects of exposure to stress, an alternative interpretation is that healthier and happier people have more energy for generativity.

The purpose of the present study was to examine the effects of generativity on the relationship between parenting a child with a disability (either a developmental problem or an early onset mental disorder) and parental physical and psychological health in midlife. The study involved secondary analysis of a national population-based study; thus, unlike most studies of the impacts of parenting a child with a disability, participants were not recruited based on their child’s disability. We included three dependent variables: negative affect, positive affect, and physical health. Conceptually, these outcomes assess both positive and negative aspects of psychological functioning, as well as physical health. Based on evidence that mothers and fathers are differentially affected by raising a child with a disability, we investigated gender differences. We included gender as a main effect in all analyses and tested whether it interacts with generativity and parenting status in predicting the outcome variables.

Based on Erikson’s theory (1950, 1968) and previous research in this area, we tested the following hypotheses:

  1. Parents of children with developmental or mental health problems will have higher levels of negative affect than parents whose children do not have disabilities.

  2. The adverse effects of parenting an individual with a disability will be more pronounced for mothers than for fathers.

  3. Generativity will moderate the adverse effect of parenting an individual with a disability on depression, positive affect, and self-rated physical health. Because the effects of parenting a child with a disability are expected to depend upon gender of the parent, we also expected the effects of generativity to be conditional upon parental gender.

Method

Study sample

Data for this study are from the Study on Midlife in the United States (MIDUS; Brim, Ryff, & Kessler, 2004), a nationally representative probability sample of 7,108 English-speaking, non-institutionalized adults ranging in age from 25 to 74 when they were first studied in 1995–1996. A second wave of data collection (MIDUS II) took place from 2004 to 2006, and a total of 4,032 adults (60.8% of the original MIDUS sample) completed a telephone interview and a self-administered questionnaire. This present study analyzed data from MIDUS II because screening questions were added to the child roster at MIDUS II survey to identify respondents who had a child with a developmental or serious mental health problem. If parents responded affirmatively, he or she was asked the child’s diagnosis and the child’s age when the symptoms first appeared. These screening questions were not administered at MIDUS I. MIDUS is a publicly available data set, and this study was deemed exempt from institutional review.

We used two analytic samples. The first group consisted of 220 respondents who had a child with a developmental problem (including conditions such as attention deficit disorder, learning disabilities, cerebral palsy, Down syndrome, and other types of intellectual disability) or a serious mental health problem (including anxiety disorders, bipolar disorder, depression, and schizophrenia) that had an onset before the child was 18 years old. The typical time of onset for serious mental health problems is often late adolescence or early adulthood; because we were interested in the effects of parenting, we restricted the sample to cases that began while the child was most likely still at home. For consistency, we used the same criteria for the cases involving a developmental disorder. The mean age of the offspring with developmental or mental health problems at the time of MIDUS II was 25.91 (SD = 12.45) and 48.6% were living with the parent who was a MIDUS participant. Of the respondents who reported a child with a disability, 13.2% reported more than one child with a disability. However, these respondents were not coded differently from parents with one child with a disability. The second analytic sample was the comparison group, consisting of 3784 respondents who had at least one living child, but did not report that any of their children had a developmental disability or serious mental health condition. Of this group, 39.7% reported that at least one child was living at home. Although we refer to “fathers” and “mothers,” the data are based on the primary MIDUS respondents and none of the sample members are married to one another.

As shown in Table 1, there were more mothers in the group of parents whose child had a disability and this group was younger on average than those in the comparison group. The groups did not differ significantly regarding education, marital status, employment status, or number of children.

Table 1

Characteristics of the Sample

VariableComparison group (n = 3784)Parents of a child with a disability (n = 220)χ2
Percent mothers 52.6 61.9 7.28**
Percent with some college or more 57.7 59.8 0.39
Percent married 73.2 76.4 1.08
Percent employed 62.4 62.8 0.02
Mean (SD)Mean (SD)t
Age 57.16 (11.93) 53.76 (11.90) 4.10***
Number of children 2.73 (1.36) 2.91 (1.56) −1.83
Generativity 17.04 (3.85) 17.22 (3.74) −0.66
Negative affect 1.49 (0.57) 1.72 (0.74) −5.67***
Positive affect 3.47 (0.70) 3.21 (0.71) 5.28***
Physical health 7.38 (1.73) 7.05 (1.73) 3.00**

Measures

Negative affect was measured with six items culled from well-validated measures and described in detail by Mroczek and Kolarz (1998). Respondents were asked “During the past 30 days, how much of the time did you feel (a) so sad nothing could cheer you up; (b) nervous; (c) restless or fidgety; (d) hopeless; (e) that everything was an effort; and (f) worthless?” Positive affect was similarly assessed with the items, “During the past 30 days, how much of the time did you feel: (a) cheerful; (b) in good spirits; (c) extremely happy; (d) calm and peaceful; (e) satisfied; (f) full of life?” Participants answered using a 5-point scale (1 = none of the time, 2 = a little of the time, 3 = some of the time, 4 = most of the time, 5 = all of the time) and responses to the six items for each scale were added, with higher scores indicating greater negative and positive affect, respectively. The 30-day response frame was intended to capture contextual influences on emotions and feelings. Both the negative and positive affect scales have been widely used and have demonstrated good reliability and validity (Mrockzek & Kolarz, 1998; Keyes, 2000). Cronbach’s alpha for the present study was .87 for negative affect and .91 for positive affect. Physical health was assessed by asking participants, “Using a scale from 0 to 10 where 0 means “the worst possible health” and 10 means “the best possible health,” how would you rate your health these days?” Self-rated health is a subjective global assessment of health status and has been consistently linked to health outcomes, including mortality, independent of objective health measures (Ferraro & Wilkinson, 2015; Benyamini, 2011).

In addition to parental status (parent of a child with a developmental disability or a childhood-onset serious mental health problem or parent of a child who did not have such disabilities) the key predictor variable in this study was generativity, which was assessed with an abbreviated version of the Loyola Generativity Scale (LGS; McAdams & de St. Aubin, 1992). Participants responded to each of six items using a 4-point scale ranging from 1 (not at all) to 4 (a lot). The items included: “Others would say that you have made unique contributions to society,” “You have important skills you can pass along to others,” “Many people come to you for advice,” “You feel that other people need you,” “You have had a good influence on the lives of many people,” and “You like to teach things to people.” A total score was computed by summing all six items. The LGS in general and the abbreviated scale in particular measure the extent to which an individual perceives him or herself as generative (Keyes & Ryff, 1998). Both forms of the LGS have been widely used (e.g., An & Cooney, 2006; Einolf, 2014; Gruenewald et al., 2012; Keyes & Ryff, 1998; Son & Wilson, 2011). The scale has high test-retest reliability and it correlates with other measurements of generativity (An & Cooney, 2006; Gruenewald et al., 2012; McAdams & de St. Aubin, 1992; Son & Wilson, 2011). Cronbach’s alpha for the present study was .85.

Based on prior research on factors associated with individual psychological and physical well-being (Bookwala, 2005; Ha et al., 2008; Mroczek & Kolarz, 1998), age, gender, education, marital status and employment status were included as covariates in the analysis. Respondents reported their age, gender (0 = male, 1 = female), marital status (0 = non-married, 1 = married or living with a partner), education (0 = finished high school or less, 1 = some college or more), and employment status (0 = not working, 1 = working or self-employed).

Data Analysis

First, we examined descriptive statistics for both analytic samples and tested for differences using chi-square tests (for dichotomous variables) and t-tests (for continuous variables). We calculated bivariate correlations among all study variables.

To determine whether generativity moderated the effects of parenting a child with a disability, we used ordinary least squares hierarchical regression. Separate models were estimated for each of the three dependent variables. The first step included control variables (age, gender, education, marital status, and employment status), a dummy variable for parent status (parent of a child with a disability = 1), and generativity. In the second step, we entered interaction terms that allowed the effect of each of the primary predictors to vary as a function of gender, generativity, or both. Specifically, we entered three two-way interactions (parent status by gender, generativity by gender, and parent status by generativity) and one three-way interaction (parent status by gender by generativity). In follow-up analyses, we tested the simple effects of generativity for each of the groups and plotted the regressions for the separate groups to contrast their slopes and intercepts. Because this was an exploratory study, it seemed prudent to test these relationships cross-sectionally rather than longitudinally.

Results

To address whether the two groups differed on generativity and the well-being variables, we used t-tests to examine mean differences. Results are presented in Table 1. The group of parents of a child with a disability scored higher on measures of negative affect, and they scored lower on measures of positive affect and physical health. There were no differences between groups regarding generativity. Intercorrelations among all variables are presented in Table 2. Generativity was inversely related to depression and positively related to positive affect and self-rated health. In regard to control variables, generativity was positively correlated with education and employment status.

Table 2

Correlations among Major Study Variables

Variable123456789
1.Generativity --
2. Parent status .01 --
3.Negative affect −.13*** .10*** --
4.Positive affect .22*** −.09*** −.62*** --
5.Health .14*** −.05** −.38*** .40*** --
6.Age −.03 −.07** −.11*** .14*** −.06*** --
7.Gender .01 .04** .10*** −.04* .00 −.03 --
8.Education .16*** −.01 −.14*** .06** .13*** −.06*** −.07*** --
9.Marital status −.01 .01 −.07*** .06** .07*** −.07*** −.16*** .08*** --
10.Employment .07*** .00 −.09*** .00 .19*** −.50*** −.12*** .13*** .03*

Results of the regression analyses are summarized in Table 3. Generativity was significantly related to lower levels of negative affect. As a group, parents of a child with a disability had higher rates of negative affect than the comparison group. However, the significant interaction between parent status and gender indicated that the effect of parenting a child with a disability was stronger for women. Given this significant interaction, we tested the moderating effect of generativity by including its three-way interaction with parent status and gender. This interaction was significant, indicating that generativity moderated the effect of parenting a child with a disability on negative affect, but only for mothers. Specifically, for men, the interaction between generativity and parent status was not significant (B = 0.02, p = .35), but for women, there was a significant interaction between generativity and parent status (B = −0.05, p < .001). To probe this interaction, we tested the slope of the effect of parent status on negative affect at three levels of generativity (the mean, one standard deviation above the mean, and one standard deviation below the mean), separately by gender. These relationships are illustrated in Figure 1. Fathers of children with a disability did not have significantly different levels of negative affect from fathers whose children did not have a disability, regardless of their level of generativity. Mothers of children with a disability with low or medium levels of generativity had significantly higher levels of negative affect than mothers of children who did not have a disability (low generativity: B = 0.51, p < .001; average generativity: B = 0.29, p < .001). However, at high generativity, mothers of a child with a disability did not differ in their levels of negative affect from the comparison mothers (B = 0.08, p = .25). The complete model explained 9.5% of the variance in negative affect, R2 = .095, p < .001, which is regarded as a small to medium effect size (Cohen, 1992).

Positive mental health involves a sense of psychological well-being, which depends upon having a

Effect of parent status on depressed affect at three levels of generativity, by sex.

Table 3

Unstandardized Regression Coefficients for Parent Status (PS), Generativity, and their Interaction

Negative affectPositive affectHealth
VariableStep 1Step 2Step 1Step 2Step 1Step 2
Control variables
Gender
 B 0.06** 0.14 −0.01* 0.01 0.13* 0.39
 SE 0.02 0.10 0.03 0.11 0.06 0.26
Age
 B −0.01*** −0.01*** 0.01*** 0.01*** 0.01* 0.01*
 SE 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.003 0.003
Education
 B −0.13*** −0.13*** 0.03 0.03 0.27*** 0.27***
 SE 0.02 0.02 0.03 0.03 0.06 0.06
Marital status
 B −0.11*** −0.11*** 0.11*** 0.11*** 0.25*** 0.24***
 SE 0.03 0.03 0.03 0.03 0.07 0.07
Employment
 B −0.18*** −0.18*** 0.10*** 0.10*** 0.61*** 0.60***
 SE 0.02 0.02 0.03 0.03 0.07 0.07
Main effects
Parent status
 B 0.20*** −0.19 −0.23*** 0.11 −0.37** 0.91
 SE 0.04 0.28 0.05 0.41 0.11 0.94
Generativity
 B −0.02** −0.01** 0.04*** 0.04*** 0.05*** 0.06***
 SE 0.003 0.004 0.003 0.005 0.01 0.01
Interaction effects
PS × Gender
 B 1.45*** −1.01* −2.89**
 SE 0.41 0.50 1.10
PS × Generativity
 B 0.02 −0.02 −0.07
 SE 0.02 0.02 0.05
Gender × Generativity
 B −0.01 0.00 −0.02
 SE 0.01 0.007 0.01
PS × Gender × Generativity
 B −0.07** 0.06* 0.16*
 SE 0.02 0.03 0.06
R2 for complete model .095 .088 .068

In terms of positive outcomes, generativity had a significant positive effect for the sample overall, with higher levels of generativity associated with higher levels of positive affect. Parenting a child with a developmental disorder or mental health problem was associated with reduced positive affect but this relationship was conditional on gender, indicating that mothers of a child with a disability tended to experience lower positive affect. The three-way interaction between gender, parent status, and generativity was also significant, indicating that the effect of having a child with a disability was conditional on both gender and generativity. For men, generativity did not significantly moderate the effect of parent status (B = −.02, p = .43) but it did significantly moderate the effect of parent status for women (B = 0.04, p = .02). We probed these relationships using the same procedure described previously and results are depicted in Figure 3. Mothers of a child with a disability reported lower levels of positive affect than mothers of children who did not have a disability when their levels of generativity were low or medium (low generativity: B = −0.41, p < .001; average generativity: B = −0.27, p < .001). However, at high levels of generativity, there was no difference in positive affect between mothers of children with a disability and the comparison mothers (B = −0.13, p =.11). R-squared for the complete model was .088, p < .001, which is regarded as a small to medium effect size (Cohen, 1992).

Positive mental health involves a sense of psychological well-being, which depends upon having a

Effect of parent status on self-rated health at three levels of generativity, by sex.

Finally, generativity was related to better self-rated physical health for the overall sample. Parents of a child with a disability tended to have poorer health but the two-way interaction between parent status and gender indicated that the adverse effect of parenting a child with a disability on physical health was greater for women. However, similar to the findings for the other outcomes, the effect of parent status on physical health depended on both gender and generativity. Generativity did not moderate the effect of parent status for fathers (B = −0.06, p = .19), but it did significantly interact with parent status for mothers (B = 0.09, p = .01). This interaction was probed in the same manner as described previously and the relationships are depicted in Figure 4. At low or medium levels of generativity, mothers of a child with a disability reported poorer health than the comparison mothers (low generativity: B = −0.82, p < .001; average generativity: B = −0.49, p < .001). However, at high levels of generativity, there was no difference in self-reported health between mothers of children with a disability and mothers of children who did not have a disability (B = −0.15, p =.42). R-squared for the complete model was .068, p < .001, which is regarded as a small to medium effect (Cohen, 1992).

Discussion

The present study examined the effects of having a child with a disability on parents’ mental and physical health. We predicted that these effects would be more pronounced for women. We also tested the moderating influence of generativity, expecting that generativity would buffer the adverse effects of parenting a child with a disability.

As expected, the group of parents who had a child with a disability reported higher negative affect, lower positive affect, and lower subjective physical health. However, each of these effects was dependent on gender, which is consistent with previous meta-analyses that have reported statistically reliable gender differences in caregiver outcomes (Pinquart & Sörensen, 2006). Some scholars have argued that women fare worse than men not because they are more vulnerable to the stress of raising children and providing care, but because they are exposed to more stressors (Ross & Van Willigen, 1996). In general, mothers perform a disproportionate amount of household duties including those related to children (Craig, 2006) and while this disparity has shrunk in past decades, it still exists (Bianchi, Sayer, Milkie, & Robinson, 2012). Regarding caregiving, it has been shown that women are exposed to higher levels of caregiving stressors including behavior problems, more hours of care provided, higher number of caregiving tasks, and higher perceived burden (Pinquart & Sörensen, 2006). Furthermore, women tend to ascribe greater importance to the parenting role than men do (Simon, 1992) and strains are more distressing when they occur in role domains that are personally salient (Simon, 1992; Thoits, 1991). Thus, the gender differences in our results are likely a reflection of relatively greater stress for the mothers of a child with a disability and the greater salience of the parenting role for these women.

Generativity was related to lower negative affect, and higher positive affect and subjective health. For women, it also moderated the effect of parenting a child with a disability, suggesting that generativity can function as an important psychosocial resource. According to Lazarus and Folkman’s transactional model of stress (1984), part of the appraisal process for determining a situation’s potential threat is evaluating one’s own personal and social resources for dealing with the threat. Our results suggest that people with a strong sense of generativity have an inner sense of strength or competence (“You have important skills you can pass along to others,” “Many people come to you for advice,”) that may enhance their appraisal of their ability to cope. According to the transactional model, this positive appraisal leads to a reduction in perceived threat and more effective coping responses. Another important psychosocial resource is social support (Haley, Levine, Brown, & Bartolucci, 1987). Because highly generative people tend to occupy many social roles (Erikson et al., 1986), they tend to have access to wider social networks and stronger social support (Hart, McAdams, Hirsch, & Bauer, 2001). Consistent with this idea, a longitudinal study of female caregivers found that women who scored high on generativity listed more people whom they felt they could call on to provide help or support and were more satisfied with the support they received from others (Peterson, 2002).

Previous research has found that parenting itself does not necessarily produce generativity (Einolf, 2014), and our study found that among parents, having a child with a disability was not uniquely linked with higher generativity. Although generativity stems in part from a need to be needed, theoretically it is also motivated by desires to produce a legacy beyond the self, to assist in the continuity of the species, and to contribute meaningfully to the lives of others (e.g., Erikson, 1950; McAdams & de St. Aubin, 1992). These admirable desires can be satisfied through many avenues outside of parenthood. In fact, there is evidence that nonfamilial endeavors such as volunteering and civic engagement can fulfill adults’ generative desires and enhance their well-being equally as well as parenting (An & Cooney, 2006). In light of this finding, we speculate that the mothers in our study who scored highest on generativity were those who were involved in a variety of social roles outside of the family. Supporting this notion, our data showed that generativity was positively correlated with both employment and higher education; presumably, women with more schooling and income have the resources to create larger and more beneficial social roles for themselves.

This study builds upon the extant literature showing that positive perceptions of generativity are associated with better mental and physical well-being (An & Cooney, 2006; Gruenewald et al., 2012; Serrat et al., 2016). Although most of the existing research used cross-sectional observational designs, at least two longitudinal studies showed that generativity predicted positive outcomes ten years later (Gruenewald et al., 2012; Peterson, 2002) suggesting that interventions that enhance perceptions of generativity may yield benefits. Indeed, recent evidence indicates that generativity is modifiable (Gruenewald et al., 2016). The Baltimore Experience Corps program is an intergenerational civic engagement program in which older adult volunteers serve in a variety of roles (such as tutoring, friendship, guidance) in local elementary schools (Fried et al., 2004). The program was intentionally designed to provide older adults with opportunities to meaningfully contribute to the younger generation while simultaneously engaging the adults socially, cognitively, and physically. Initial results from a randomized controlled trial of the program showed that Corps participants showed increases in perceptions of generativity at the two-year follow-up (Gruenewald et al., 2016). An exciting direction for future research will be to explore whether these increases in generativity were accompanied by improvements in mental and physical health. Another promising direction will be to develop different programs given that there are numerous ways to promote and satisfy generative desires. Specifically, the findings of this study suggest that mothers of individuals with disabilities may benefit from opportunities to get involved in advocacy activities related to their son or daughter’s disability as such activities are likely to generate feelings of generativity.

The present study has some limitations. Although the Loyola Generativity Scale is widely used and its validity is empirically supported, it likely does not capture all aspects of generativity. Theoretical conceptualizations of generativity often make a distinction between generative behavior, generative desire, and perceptions of generative achievement (e.g., McAdams, Hart, & Maruna, 1998; Gruenewald, 2016); however, the Loyola scale primarily assesses perceptions. Another limitation is that the cross-sectional research design limits our interpretation of the findings. We hypothesized that generativity contributes to better mental and physical functioning, but it is possible that people with better mental and physical health are more likely to be socially engaged in ways that lead to higher generativity. Future research should test whether generativity predicts change in well-being over time. Finally, this study did not test the moderating effect of generativity for different types of disabilities; future work should explore whether this effect is conditional on the type of the offspring’s disability.

The present study also had some unique strengths. First, we used a nationally representative data set which increases generalizability of the findings. Notably, the sample of parents of children with disabilities were not specifically recruited for a study of caregiving effects as is the case with most research on such parents. Our data is thus less vulnerable to self-selection bias. Second, we found a three-way interaction between gender, parent status, and generativity for three distinct aspects of adult well-being. Although effect sizes were small, this consistency across outcomes increases our confidence in our results. Finally, to our knowledge this is the first study to directly test the idea that generativity can moderate the adverse effects of parenting a child with a developmental or mental health problem on mental and physical health. This study thus adds to the extant literature supporting the importance of generativity for well-being in mid to late life in general, and for coping with the challenges of caring for offspring with disabilities in particular.

Positive mental health involves a sense of psychological well-being, which depends upon having a

Effect of parent status on positive affect at three levels of generativity, by sex.

Acknowledgments

Support for this research was provided by the Waisman Center NICHD funded core grant (U54 HD090256) and the NIA funded MIDUS grant (U19 AG051426). Many thanks to Jieun Song for sharing her expertise with navigating MIDUS.

References

  • An JS, & Cooney TM (2006). Psychological well-being in mid to late life: The role of generativity development and parent-child relationships across the lifespan. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 410–421. doi: 10.1177/0165025406071489 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ardelt M, Landes SD, & Vaillant GE (2010). The long-term effects of World War II combat exposure on later life well-being moderated by generativity. Research in Human Development, 7, 202–220. doi: 10.1080/15427609.2010.504505 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Benyamini Y (2011). Why does self-rated health predict mortality? An update on current knowledge and a research agenda for psychologists. Psychology & Health, 26, 1407–1413. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2011.621703 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Bianchi SM, Sayer LC, Milkie MA, & Robinson JP (2012). Housework: Who did, does or will do it, and how much does it matter? Social Forces, 91, 55–63. doi: 10.1093/sf/sos120 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Bookwala J (2005). The role of marital quality in physical health during the mature years. Journal of Aging and Health, 17, 85–104. doi: 10.1177/0898264304272794 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Brehaut JC, Garner RE, Miller AR, Lach LM, Klassen AF, Rosenbaum PL, & Kohen DE (2011). Changes over time in the health of caregivers of children with health problems: Growth-curve findings from a 10-year Canadian population-based study. American Journal of Public Health, 101, 2308–2316. doi: 10.2105/AJPH.2011.300298 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Brim OG, Ryff CD, & Kessler RC (2004). How healthy are we? A national study of well-being at midlife. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  • Craig L (2006). Does father care mean fathers share? A comparison of how mothers and fathers in intact families spend time with children. Gender and Society, 20, 259–281. doi: 10.1177/0891243205285212 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Cohen J (1992). A power primer. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 155–159. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Colby A, Sippola L, & Phelps E (2001). Social responsibility and paid work in contemporary American life In Rossi A (Ed.), Caring and doing for others: Social responsibility in the domains of family, work, and community (pp. 463–501). Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
  • Einolf CJ (2014). Stability and change in generative concern: Evidence from a longitudinal survey. Journal of Research in Personality, 51, 54–61. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2014.04.003 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Erikson EH (1950). Childhood and society New York: Norton. [Google Scholar]
  • Erikson EH (1968). Identity: Youth and crisis New York: Norton. [Google Scholar]
  • Erikson EH, Erikson JM, & Kivnick HQ (1986). Vital involvement in old age New York: Norton. [Google Scholar]
  • Ferraro KF, & Wilkinson LR (2015). Alternative measures of self-rated health for predicting mortality among older people: Is past or future orientation more important? The Gerontologist, 55, 836–844. doi: 10.1093/geront/gnt098 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Fried LP, Carlson MC, Freedman M, Frick KD, Glass TA, Hill J, … Zeger S (2004). A social model for health promotion for an aging population: Initial evidence on the Experience Corps model. Journal of Urban Health, 81, 64–78. doi: 10.1093/jurban/jth094 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Grossman MR, & Gruenewald TL (2017). Caregiving and perceived generativity: A positive and protective aspect of providing care? Clinical Gerontologist, 40, 435–447. doi: 10.1080/07317115.2017.1317686 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Gruenewald TL, Karlamangla AS, Greendale GA, Singer BH, & Seeman TE (2009). Increased mortality risk in older adults with persistently low or declining feelings of usefulness to others. Journal of Aging and Health, 21, 398–425. doi: 10.1177/0898264308329023 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Gruenewald TL, Liao DH, & Seeman TE (2012). Contributing to others, contributing to oneself: Perceptions of generativity and health in later life. The Journals of Gerontology, Series B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences, 67, 660–665. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbs034 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Gruenewald TL, Tanner EK, Fried LP, Carlson MC, Xue Q, Parisi JM, … Seeman TE. (2016). The Baltimore Experience Corps Trial: Enhancing generativity via intergenerational activity engagement in later life. Journals of Gerontology: Psychological Sciences, 71, 661–670. doi: 10.1093/geronb/gbv005 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ha JH, Hong J, Seltzer MM, & Greenberg JS (2008). Age and gender differences in the well-being of midlife and aging parents with children with mental health or developmental problems: Report of a national study. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 49, 301–316. doi: 10.1177/00221465080490030 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Haley WE, Levine EG, Brown SL, & Bartolucci AA (1987). Stress, appraisal, coping, and social support as predictors of adaptational outcome among dementia caregivers. Psychology and Aging, 2, 323–330. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.2.4.323 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Hart HM, McAdams DP, Hirsch BJ, & Bauer JJ (2001). Generativity and social involvement among African Americans and White adults. Journal of Research in Personality, 35, 208–230. doi: 10.1006/jrpe.2001.2318. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Keyes CLM (2000). Subjective change and its consequences for emotional well-being. Motivation and Emotion, 24 67–83. doi: 10.1023/a:1005659114155 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Keyes CLM, & Ryff CD (1998). Generativity and adult lives: Social structural contours and quality of life consequences In McAdams DP, & de St. Aubin E (Eds.), Generativity and adult development: How and why we care for the next generation (pp. 227–263). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
  • Lazarus RS, & Folkman S (1984). Stress, appraisal, and coping New York: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  • Lecavalier L, Leone S, & Wiltz J (2006). The impact of behaviour problems on caregiver stress in young people with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Intellectual Disability Research, 50, 172–183. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2788.2005.00732.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • McAdams & de St. Aubin (1992). A theory of generativity and its assessment through self-report, behavioral act, and narrative themes in autobiography. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 1003–1015. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.62.6.1003 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • McAdams D, Hart H, & Maruna S (1998). The anatomy of generativity In McAdams D & De St. Aubin E (Eds.), Generativity and adult development: How and why we care for the next generation (pp. 7–43). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. [Google Scholar]
  • Mroczek DK, & Kolarz CM (1998). The effect of age on positive and negative affect: A developmental perspective on happiness. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 75, 5, 1333–1349. doi: 10.1037//0022-3514.75.5.1333 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Namkung EH, Greenberg JS, Mailick MR, & Floyd FJ (2018). Lifelong parenting of adults with developmental disabilities: Growth trends over 20 years in midlife and later life. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 123, 228–240. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-123.3.228 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Peterson BE (2002). Longitudinal analysis of midlife generativity, intergenerational roles, and caregiving. Psychology and Aging, 17, 161–168. doi: 10.1037/0882-7974.17.1.161 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Peterson BE, & Duncan LE (1999). Generative concern, political commitment, and charitable actions. Journal of Adult Development, 6, 105–118. doi: 10.1023/a:1021620824878 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Pinquart M, & Sörensen S (2006). Gender differences in caregiver stressors, social resources, and health: An updated meta-analysis. The Journals of Gerontology: Series B, 61, P33–P45. Doi: 10.1093/geronb/61.1.P33 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ross CE, & Van Willigen M (1996). Gender, parenthood, and anger. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 58, 572–584. doi: 10.2307/353718 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Rothrauff R, & Cooney TM (2008). The role of generativity in psychological well-being: Does it differ for childless adults and parents? Journal of Adult Development, 15, 148–159. doi: 10.1007/s10804-008-9046-7 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Seltzer MM, Floyd F, Song J, Greenberg J, & Hong J (2011). Midlife and aging parents of adults with intellectual and developmental disabilities: Impacts of lifelong parenting. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 116, 479–499. doi: 10.1352/1944-7558-116.6.479 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Serrat R, Villar F, Pratt MW, & Stukas AA (2016). On the quality of adjustment to retirement: The longitudinal role of personality traits and generativity. Journal of Personality, 86, 435–449. doi: 10.1111/jopy.12326 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Simon RW (1992). Parental role strains, salience of parental identity and gender differences in psychological distress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 33, 25–35. doi: 10.2307/2136855 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Smith AM, & Grzywacz JG (2014). Health and well-being in midlife parents of children with special health needs. Families, Systems, & Health, 32, 303–312. doi: 10.1037/fsh0000049 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Son J, & Wilson J (2011). Generativity and volunteering. Sociological Forum, 26, 644–667. doi: 10.1111/j.1573-7861.2011.01266.x [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Thiele DM, & Whelan TA (2008). The relationship between grandparent satisfaction, meaning, and generativity. International Journal of Aging and Human Development, 66, 21–48. doi: 10.2190/AG.66.1.b [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Thoits P (1991). On merging identity theory and stress research. Social Psychology Quarterly, 54, 101–112. doi: 10.2307/2786929 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Versey HS, & Newton NJ (2013). Generativity and productive pursuits: Pathways to successful aging in late midlife African American and White women. Journal of Adult Development, 20, 185–196. doi: 10.1007/s10804-013-9170-x [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Villar F (2012). Successful ageing and development: the contribution of generativity in older age. Ageing & Society, 32, 1087–1105. doi: 10.1017/S0144686X110000973 [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Weiss MJ (2002). Hardiness and social support as predictors of stress in mothers of typical children, children with autism, and children with mental retardation. Autism, 6, 115–130. doi: 10.1177/1362361302006001009 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

In what ways does parenthood satisfy the need to be generative?

In what ways does parenthood satisfy an adult's need to be generative? The chief form of generativity in establishing and guiding the next generation. Many adults pass along their values as they respond to the hundreds of requests and unspoken needs of their children each day, thus becoming generative.

How does generativity contribute to a healthy middle adulthood?

How does generativity contribute to a healthy middle adulthood? It adds meaning to their lives and helps them to maintain and enhance their self-esteem.

Which of the following is an example of a boomerang kid?

Which of the following is an example of a boomerang kid? A young adult who returns to live with his parents after finishing college and getting a full-time job.

Which of the following describes highly creative people?

Which of the following describes highly creative people? They are self-starters.