Self-______is the belief that one can perform adequately in a given situation or circumstance.

See other formats


Understanding === 
Psychology 


ROBERT S. FELDMAN 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


This page intentionally left blank 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


TENTH EDITION 


Understanding 
Psychology 


Robert S. Feldman 
University of Massachusetts, Amherst 






= \\ Connect 
Learn 
Succeed” 


— 





The McGraw-Hill companies 


= \\ Connect 
Learn 
Succeed” 
~ 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Published by McGraw-Hill, a business unit of The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc., 1221 Avenue of 
the Americas, New York, NY 10020. Copyright © 2011, 2008, 2005, 2002, 1999, 1996, 1993, 1990, 
1987 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may 
be reproduced or distributed in any form or by any means, or stored in a database or retrieval 
system, without the prior written consent of The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc., including, but 
not limited to, any network or other electronic storage or transmission, or broadcast for distance 
learning. Some ancillaries, including electronic and print components, may not be available to 


customers outside the United States. 


1234567890 DOW/DOW 10 


ISBN: 978-0-07-338279-1 
MHID: 0-07-338279-5 


Vice President, Editorial: Michael Ryan 
Director, Editorial: Beth Mejia 
Publisher: Michael Sugarman 
Executive Editor: Krista Bettino 
Development Editors: Cara Labell and 

Sue Ewing 
Editorial Coordinator: Megan Stotts 
Executive Marketing Manager: Julia Flohr 
Marketing Manager: Yasuko Okada 
Digital Product Manager: Jay Gubernick 
Media Project Manager: Thomas Brierly 
Production Editor: Anne Fuzellier 
Text Permission Coordinator: Marty Moga 


Art Manager: Robin Mouat 

Art Director: Preston Thomas 

Interior Designer: Ellen Pettengel 

Cover Designer: Laurie Entringer 

Photo Manager: Brian J.Pecko 

Photo Researcher: Toni Michaels 

Buyer II: Tandra Jorgensen 

Production Service: The Left Coast Group, Inc. 

Composition: 9.5/12 Palatino by Aptara, Inc. 

Printing: 45# New Era Thin by RR Donnelley 
& Sons 

Cover image: ©Stockbyte/PunchStock 


Credits: The credits section for this book begins on page C-1 and is considered an extension of the 


copyright page. 


Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data 


Feldman, Robert S. (Robert Stephen), 1947- 


Understanding psychology / Robert S. Feldman.—10th ed. 


p. cm. 


Includes bibliographical references and index. 


ISBN-13: 978-0-07-338279-1 (alk. paper) 


ISBN-10: 0-07-338279-5 (mhid : alk. paper) 


1. Psychology—Textbooks. I. Title. 
BF121.F34 2011 
150—dc22 


2010031463 


The Internet addresses listed in the text were accurate at the time of publication. The inclusion 
of a website does not indicate an endorsement by the authors or McGraw-Hill, and McGraw-Hill 
does not guarantee the accuracy of the information presented at these sites. 


www.mhhe.com 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


To 
Jon, Leigh, Alex, Josh, Julie, 
Sarah, and Kathy 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


This page intentionally left blank 


ie www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 
‘\ 


\ 
\ 


\ 


About the Author 


ROBERT S. FELDMAN is Professor of Psychology and Dean of the College of Social 
and Behavioral Sciences at the University of Massachusetts, Amherst. A recipient of 
the College Distinguished Teacher Award, he teaches psychology classes ranging in 
size from 15 to nearly 500 students. During the course of more than two decades as 
a college instructor, he has taught undergraduate and graduate courses at Mount 
Holyoke College, Wesleyan University, and Virginia Commonwealth University in 
addition to the University of Massachusetts. 

Professor Feldman, who initiated the Minority Mentoring Program at the 
University of Massachusetts, also has served as a Hewlett Teaching Fellow and Senior 
Online Teaching Fellow. He initiated distance learning courses in psychology at the 
University of Massachusetts. 

A Fellow of both the American Psychological Association and the Association 
for Psychological Science, Professor Feldman received a BA with High Honors from 
Wesleyan University and an MS and PhD from the University of Wisconsin—Madison. 
He is a winner of a Fulbright Senior Research Scholar and Lecturer Award and the 
Distinguished Alumnus Award from Wesleyan. He is on the Boards of the Federation 
of Associations in Behavioral and Brain Sciences (FABBS) and the FABBS Foundation, 
which advocate for the field of psychology. 

He has written and edited more than 150 books, book chapters, and scientific 
articles. He has edited Development of Nonverbal Behavior in Children, Applications of 
Nonverbal Behavioral Theory and Research, Improving the First Year of College: Research 
and Practice, and co-edited Fundamentals of Nonverbal Behavior. He is also author of 
Development Across the Life Span, Child Development, and P.O.W.E.R. Learning: Strategies 
for Success in College and Life. His books have been translated into many languages, 
including Spanish, French, Portuguese, Dutch, Chinese, Korean, and Japanese. His 
research interests include deception and honesty in everyday life, work that he 
described in The Liar in Your Life, a trade book published in 2009. His research has 
been supported by grants from the National Institute of Mental Health and the 
National Institute on Disabilities and Rehabilitation Research. 

Professor Feldman loves music, is an enthusiastic pianist, and enjoys cooking 
and traveling. He has three children and a young grandson. He and his wife, a psy- 
chologist, live in western Massachusetts in a home overlooking the Holyoke moun- 
tain range. 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


This page intentionally left blank 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Brief Contents 


CHAPTER 1 


CHAPTER 2 


CHAPTER 3 


CHAPTER 4 


CHAPTER 5 


Preface xxiii 


Introduction to Psychology 2 


MODULE 1 Psychologists at Work 5 
MODULE 2 A Science Evolves: The Past, 
the Present, and the Future 15 
mopute 3 Psychology’s Key Issues and Controversies 23 


Psychological Research 30 

MODULE 4 The Scientific Method 33 

mopuULE 5 Conducting Psychological Research 37 
MODULE 6 Critical Research Issues 49 
Neuroscience and Behavior 58 


mobuLe 7 Neurons: The Basic Elements of Behavior 61 


mopute s The Nervous System and the Endocrine System: 


Communicating Within the Body 70 
MODULE 9 The Brain 79 


Sensation and Perception 96 


MODULE 10 Sensing the World Around Us 99 
mopute 11 Vision: Shedding Light on the Eye 104 
mopuLE 12 Hearing and the Other Senses 114 
MODULE 13 Perceptual Organization: 

Constructing Our View of the World 126 


States of Consciousness 138 


MODULE 14 Sleep and Dreams 141 

moObDULE 15 Hypnosis and Meditation 154 

mopute 16 Drug Use: The Highs and Lows 
of Consciousness 159 


viii Brief Contents 


Com A ee -A i a 


CHAPTER 11 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Learning 172 


MODULE 17 Classical Conditioning 175 
MODULE 18 Operant Conditioning 183 
MODULE 19 Cognitive Approaches to Learning 198 


Memory 206 


MODULE 20 The Foundations of Memory 209 
mopute 21 Recalling Long-Term Memories 222 
MODULE 22 Forgetting: When Memory Fails 232 


Cognition and Language 240 


MODULE 23 Thinking and Reasoning 243 
MODULE 24 Problem Solving 251 
MODULE 25 Language 265 


Intelligence 276 


mMODULE 26 What Is Intelligence? 279 
mopuLE 27 Variations in Intellectual Ability 295 
mopULE 2s Group Differences in Intelligence: 
Genetic and Environmental Determinants 300 


Motivation and Emotion 306 


MODULE 29 Explaining Motivation 309 
MODULE 30 Human Needs and Motivation: 
Eat, Drink, and Be Daring 317 
mopute 31 Understanding Emotional Experiences 328 


Sexuality and Gender 340 


MODULE 32 Gender and Sex 343 

mopuLte 33 Understanding Human Sexual Response: 
The Facts of Life 356 

mopute 34 The Diversity of Sexual Behavior 362 


CHAPTER 12 


Coa W- W oe i =: J 


CHAPTER 14 


CHAPTER 15 


CHAPTER 16 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Brief Contents ix 


Development 378 


mopute 35 Nature and Nurture: The Enduring 
Developmental Issue 381 

MODULE 36 Prenatal Development: Conception to Birth 386 

mopute 37 Infancy and Childhood 393 

MODULE 38 Adolescence: Becoming an Adult 412 

MODULE 39 Adulthood 422 


Personality 436 


MODULE 40 Psychodynamic Approaches to Personality 439 
mopuLE 41 Trait, Learning, Biological and Evolutionary, 
and Humanistic Approaches to Personality 448 
mopuLE 42 Assessing Personality: Determining What 
Makes Us Distinctive 461 


Health Psychology: Stress, Coping, 
and Well-Being 470 


MODULE 43 Stress and Coping 473 
mopute 44 Psychological Aspects of Illness 
and Well-Being 485 
mopuLE 45 Promoting Health and Wellness 492 


Psychological Disorders 500 


mopute 46 Normal Versus Abnormal: 

Making the Distinction 503 
MODULE 47 The Major Psychological Disorders 513 
mopuLe 4s Psychological Disorders in Perspective 534 


Treatment of Psychological Disorders 542 


MODULE 49 Psychotherapy: Psychodynamic, Behavioral, 
and Cognitive Approaches to Treatment 545 

mopuLE 50 Psychotherapy: Humanistic, Interpersonal, 
and Group Approaches to Treatment 556 

mopute 51 Biomedical Therapy: Biological Approaches 
to Treatment 564 


x Brief Contents 


CHAPTER 17 


APPENDIX 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Social Psychology 576 


MODULE 52 Attitudes and Social Cognition 579 
mopuLE 53 Social Influence and Groups 590 

mopuULE 54 Prejudice and Discrimination 598 
MODULE 55 Positive and Negative Social Behavior 604 


Going by the Numbers: 
Statistics in Psychology A-2 


MODULE 56 Descriptive Statistics A-5 

mopute 57 Measures of Variability A-10 

MODULE 58 Using Statistics to Answer Questions: 
Inferential Statistics and Correlation A-14 


Glossary G-1 
References R-1 
Credits C-1 
Name Index l-1 


Subject Index l-15 


N www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Contents 


Preface xxiii 
Making the Grade xxxix 


CHAPTER 1 


Introduction to Psychology 2 


Psychologists at Work 5 

The Subfields of Psychology: Psychology’s Family Tree 6 
Working at Psychology 9 

PSYCHWORK: Licensed Social Worker 10 


A Science Evolves: The Past, the Present, and the Future 15 
The Roots of Psychology 15 

Today's Perspectives 18 

APPLYING PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY: Psychology Matters 21 


Psychology’s Key Issues and Controversies 23 


EXPLORING DIVERSITY: Understanding How Culture, Ethnicity, 
and Race Influence Behavior 25 


Psychology’s Future 26 


BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER OF PSYCHOLOGY: Thinking Critically About 
Psychology: Distinguishing Legitimate Psychology from Pseudo-Psychology 27 


CHAPTER 2 


Psychological Research 30 


The Scientific Method 33 
Theories: Specifying Broad Explanations 34 
Hypotheses: Crafting Testable Predictions 35 





Conducting Psychological Research 37 
Archival Research 37 

Naturalistic Observation 37 

Survey Research 38 

The Case Study 38 

Correlational Research 39 

Experimental Research 41 


APPLYING PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY: Testing the Value of Self-Affirmations: 


Am | Lovable Because | Tell Myself I’m Lovable? 47 ; 
Xl 


xii Contents 





MODULE 8 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Critical Research Issues 49 
The Ethics of Research 49 


EXPLORING DIVERSITY: Choosing Participants Who Represent 
the Scope of Human Behavior 50 


NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: The Importance of Using 
Representative Participants 51 


Should Animals Be Used in Research? 52 
Threats to Experimental Validity: Avoiding Experimental Bias 53 


BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER OF PSYCHOLOGY: 
Thinking Critically About Research 54 


CHAPTER 3 


Neuroscience and Behavior 58 


Neurons: The Basic Elements of Behavior 61 
The Structure of the Neuron 61 

How Neurons Fire 62 

Where Neurons Meet: Bridging the Gap 65 
Neurotransmitters: Multitalented Chemical Couriers 66 


The Nervous System and the Endocrine System: 
Communicating Within the Body 70 

The Nervous System: Linking Neurons 70 

The Evolutionary Foundations of the Nervous System 73 

The Endocrine System: Of Chemicals and Glands 75 


The Brain 79 
Studying the Brain’s Structure and Functions: Spying on the Brain 79 
The Central Core: Our “Old Brain” 81 


APPLYING PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY: Mind Over Cursor: 
Harnessing Brainpower to Improve Lives 82 


The Limbic System: Beyond the Central Core 83 

The Cerebral Cortex: Our “New Brain” 84 

PSYCHWORK: Rehabilitation Counselor 86 

Neuroplasticity and the Brain 88 

NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: The Plastic Brain 89 

The Specialization of the Hemispheres: Two Brains or One? 89 
EXPLORING DIVERSITY: Human Diversity and the Brain 90 
The Split Brain: Exploring the Two Hemispheres 91 


BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER OF PSYCHOLOGY: Learning to Control 
Your Heart—and Mind—Through Biofeedback 92 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 
Contents xiii 


CHAPTER 4 


Sensation and Perception 96 





Sensing the World Around Us 99 

Absolute Thresholds: Detecting What's Out There 100 

APPLYING PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY: Driven to Distraction 101 
Difference Thresholds: Noticing Distinctions Between Stimuli 101 
Sensory Adaptation: Turning Down Our Responses 102 


| MODULE 10 | 
ETE ë Vision: Shedding Light on the Eye 104 
Illuminating the Structure of the Eye 105 
NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: Seeing Vision in the Brain 110 
| MODULE 12 | 
| MODULE 13 | 


Color Vision and Color Blindness: The 7-Million-Color Spectrum 110 


MODULE 12 Hearing and the Other Senses 114 
Sensing Sound 114 
Smell and Taste 118 
The Skin Senses: Touch, Pressure, Temperature, and Pain 120 
BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER OF PSYCHOLOGY: Managing Pain 123 
How Our Senses Interact 123 


Perceptual Organization: Constructing 
Our View of the World 126 

The Gestalt Laws of Organization 126 

Top-Down and Bottom-Up Processing 127 

Depth Perception: Translating 2-D to 3-D 129 
Perceptual Constancy 130 

Motion Perception: As the World Turns 131 

Perceptual Illusions: The Deceptions of Perceptions 131 
EXPLORING DIVERSITY: Culture and Perception 133 


CHAPTER 5 


States of Consciousness 138 





Sleep and Dreams 141 
The Stages of Sleep 142 
REM Sleep: The Paradox of Sleep 143 


APPLYING PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY: Let Me Sleep on It: 
The Role of Sleep in Memory and Thinking 144 


Why Do We Sleep, and How Much Sleep Is Necessary? 145 

NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: Why Are You Cranky? Your Brain Is Too Awake 146 
The Function and Meaning of Dreaming 146 

Sleep Disturbances: Slumbering Problems 149 


xiv Contents 





F 
| MODULE 17 


MODULE 18 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


PSYCHWORK: Sleep Technologist 150 

Circadian Rhythms: Life Cycles 151 

Daydreams: Dreams Without Sleep 151 

BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER OF PSYCHOLOGY: Sleeping Better 152 


Hypnosis and Meditation 154 
Hypnosis: A Trance-Forming Experience? 154 
Meditation: Regulating Our Own State of Consciousness 156 


EXPLORING DIVERSITY: Cross-Cultural Routes to Altered 
States of Consciousness 157 


Drug Use: The Highs and Lows of Consciousness 159 
Stimulants: Drug Highs 161 

Depressants: Drug Lows 164 

Narcotics: Relieving Pain and Anxiety 167 


BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER OF PSYCHOLOGY: 
Identifying Drug and Alcohol Problems 169 


CHAPTER 6 


Learning 172 


Classical Conditioning 175 

The Basics of Classical Conditioning 176 

Applying Conditioning Principles to Human Behavior 178 

Extinction 179 

Generalization and Discrimination 180 

Beyond Traditional Classical Conditioning: Challenging Basic Assumptions 180 


Operant Conditioning 183 
Thorndike’s Law of Effect 183 
The Basics of Operant Conditioning 184 


APPLYING PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY: Dollars for Scholars: 
Should Parents and Children Be Paid for Doing the Right Thing? 188 


PSYCHWORK: Seeing Eye Guide Dog Trainer 192 


BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER OF PSYCHOLOGY: 
Using Behavior Analysis and Behavior Modification 194 


Cognitive Approaches to Learning 198 

Latent Learning 198 

Observational Learning: Learning Through Imitation 200 
NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: Learning Through Observation 201 
EXPLORING DIVERSITY: Does Culture Influence How We Learn? 202 








www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Contents 


CHAPTER 7 


Memory 206 


The Foundations of Memory 209 

Sensory Memory 210 

Short-Term Memory 211 

Working Memory 213 

Long-Term Memory 214 

NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: Experience, Memory, and the Brain 219 
APPLYING PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY: In Pursuit of a Memory Eraser 220 


Recalling Long-Term Memories 222 

Retrieval Cues 222 

Levels of Processing 223 

Explicit and Implicit Memory 224 

Flashbulb Memories 225 

Constructive Processes in Memory: Rebuilding the Past 226 

EXPLORING DIVERSITY: Are There Cross-Cultural Differences in Memory? 229 


Forgetting: When Memory Fails 232 

Why We Forget 233 

Proactive and Retroactive Interference: The Before and After of Forgetting 234 
Memory Dysfunctions: Afflictions of Forgetting 235 

NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: Alzheimer’s Disease and Brain Deterioration 236 


BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER OF PSYCHOLOGY: Improving Your Memory 237 


CHAPTER 8 


Cognition and Language 240 


Thinking and Reasoning 243 
Mental Images: Examining the Mind’s Eye 244 
Concepts: Categorizing the World 245 


NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: How We Learn to Categorize Our World 
Using Concepts 246 


Reasoning: Making Up Your Mind 247 
Computers and Problem Solving: Searching for Artificial Intelligence 249 


Problem Solving 251 

Preparation: Understanding and Diagnosing Problems 252 
Production: Generating Solutions 255 

Judgment: Evaluating Solutions 257 


APPLYING PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY: The Thoughts Behind 
Avoiding Danger: When Hunches Save Lives 258 


Impediments to Solutions: Why Is Problem Solving Such a Problem? 258 


XV 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


xvi Contents 


Creativity and Problem Solving 261 


BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER OF PSYCHOLOGY: 
Thinking Critically and Creatively 262 


MDDS Language 265 


Grammar: Language’s Language 265 
Language Development: Developing a Way with Words 266 


The Influence of Language on Thinking: 
Do Eskimos Have More Words for Snow than Texans Do? 269 


Do Animals Use Language? 270 


EXPLORING DIVERSITY: Teaching with Linguistic Variety: 
Bilingual Education 271 


NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: Bilingualism and the Brain 273 


CHAPTER 9 


Intelligence 276 





What Is Intelligence? 279 

Theories of Intelligence: Are There Different Kinds of Intelligence? 280 

The Biological Basis of Intelligence 283 

NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: Your Intelligent Brain 284 

Practical and Emotional Intelligence: Toward a More Intelligent View of Intelligence 284 


APPLYING PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY: Artificial Intelligence: 
Building a Smarter Robot 286 


Assessing Intelligence 287 
Adaptive Testing: Using Computers to Assess Performance 292 


BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER OF PSYCHOLOGY: Scoring Better on 
Standardized Tests 293 


Variations in Intellectual Ability 295 
Mental Retardation (Intellectual Disabilities) 295 
PSYCHWORK: Director of Special Education 297 
The Intellectually Gifted 297 


Group Differences in Intelligence: 
Genetic and Environmental Determinants 300 


EXPLORING DIVERSITY: The Relative Influence 
of Genetics and Environment: Nature, Nurture, and IQ 301 


CHAPTER 10 


Motivation and Emotion 306 





Explaining Motivation 309 
Instinct Approaches: Born to Be Motivated 309 
Drive-Reduction Approaches: Satisfying Our Needs 310 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Contents xvii 


Arousal Approaches: Beyond Drive Reduction 311 

Incentive Approaches: Motivation’s Pull 311 

Cognitive Approaches: The Thoughts Behind Motivation 313 
Maslow’s Hierarchy: Ordering Motivational Needs 313 
Applying the Different Approaches to Motivation 315 


ESSE Human Needs and Motivation: Eat, Drink, and Be Daring 317 
The Motivation Behind Hunger and Eating 317 
Social Factors in Eating 320 
PSYCHWORK: Nutritionist 320 
Eating Disorders 322 
NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: Cognitive Processing in Anorexic Patients 323 


BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER OF PSYCHOLOGY: Dieting and Losing 
Weight Successfully 323 


The Need for Achievement: Striving for Success 325 
The Need for Affiliation: Striving for Friendship 325 
The Need for Power: Striving for Impact on Others 326 


Understanding Emotional Experiences 328 

The Functions of Emotions 329 

Determining the Range of Emotions: Labeling Our Feelings 329 

The Roots of Emotions 330 

EXPLORING DIVERSITY: Do People in All Cultures Express Emotion Similarly? 335 


APPLYING PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY: Smiling Athletes: 
Do Their Facial Expressions Come Naturally? 337 


CHAPTER 11 


Sexuality and Gender 340 


Gender and Sex 343 

Gender Roles: Society's Expectations for Women and Men 343 

Sexism on the Job 345 

Gender Differences: More Similar than Dissimilar 348 

NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: When Brain and Socialization Meet 350 
Sources of Gender Differences: Where Biology and Society Meet 351 


APPLYING PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY: Trucks and 
Dolls: Does Fetal Exposure to Testosterone Affect How Children Play? 352 





Understanding Human Sexual Response: The Facts of Life 356 
The Basic Biology of Sexual Behavior 356 

Psychological Aspects of Sexual Excitement: What Turns People On? 358 

The Phases of Sexual Response: The Ups and Downs of Sex 358 


EXPLORING DIVERSITY: Female Circumcision: 
A Celebration of Culture—or Genital Mutilation? 360 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


xviii Contents 


The Diversity of Sexual Behavior 362 

Approaches to Sexual Normality 363 

Surveying Sexual Behavior: What’s Happening Behind Closed Doors? 364 
Heterosexuality 365 

Homosexuality and Bisexuality 367 

Transsexualism 369 

Sexual Difficulties 370 


BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER OF PSYCHOLOGY: 
Lowering the Risks of Date Rape 375 


CHAPTER 12 


Development 378 


Nature and Nurture: The Enduring Developmental Issue 381 
Determining the Relative Influence of Nature and Nurture 383 
Developmental Research Techniques 383 


Prenatal Development: Conception to Birth 386 
The Basics of Genetics 386 
The Earliest Development 388 


Infancy and Childhood 393 
The Extraordinary Newborn 393 


NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: Young Infants Recognize Emotions Early 
in Life 396 


Infancy Through Childhood 397 
PSYCHWORK: Child Protection Caseworker 403 


MODULE 38 Adolescence: Becoming an Adult 412 

Physical Development: The Changing Adolescent 412 

Moral and Cognitive Development: Distinguishing Right from Wrong 414 

Social Development: Finding One's Self in a Social World 416 

EXPLORING DIVERSITY: Rites of Passage: Coming of Age Around the World 420 


Adulthood 422 

Physical Development: The Peak of Health 423 

Social Development: Working at Life 424 

Marriage, Children, and Divorce: Family Ties 425 

Changing Roles of Men and Women 426 

The Later Years of Life: Growing Old 426 

Physical Changes in Late Adulthood: The Aging Body 427 

Cognitive Changes: Thinking About—and During—Late Adulthood 428 


APPLYING PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY: Gaming in Late Adulthood: 
How Video Games May Improve Cognitive Functioning in Older Adults 429 


BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER OF PSYCHOLOGY: Adjusting to Death 432 


| P 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 
Contents xix 


CHAPTER 13 


Personality 436 





Psychodynamic Approaches to Personality 439 
Freud's Psychoanalytic Theory: Mapping the Unconscious Mind 439 
The Neo-Freudian Psychoanalysts: Building on Freud 445 


Trait, Learning, Biological and Evolutionary, 

and Humanistic Approaches to Personality 448 

Trait Approaches: Placing Labels on Personality 448 

APPLYING PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY: Judged at First Sight 451 
Learning Approaches: We Are What We've Learned 451 

Biological and Evolutionary Approaches: Are We Born with Personality? 454 


NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: Wired for Thrills—The Biological 
Underpinnings of Personality 457 


Humanistic Approaches: The Uniqueness of You 457 
Comparing Approaches to Personality 459 


Assessing Personality: Determining 
What Makes Us Distinctive 461 


EXPLORING DIVERSITY: Should Race and Ethnicity Be Used to Establish Norms? 462 
Self-Report Measures of Personality 463 

Projective Methods 465 

Behavioral Assessment 466 

PSYCHWORK: Human Resources Manager 467 


BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER OF PSYCHOLOGY: 
Assessing Personality Assessments 467 


CHAPTER 14 
OD oam Health Psychology: Stress, Coping, 
| MODULE 43 | 


and Well-Being 470 


Stress and Coping 473 

Stress: Reacting to Threat and Challenge 473 

The High Cost of Stress 475 

Coping with Stress 480 

NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: Stress and Social Support 482 

BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER OF PSYCHOLOGY: Effective Coping Strategies 483 


Psychological Aspects of Illness and Well-Being 485 
The As, Bs, and Ds of Coronary Heart Disease 485 
Psychological Aspects of Cancer 486 
Smoking 487 


Contents 


% 


MODULE 46 


MODULE 48 


$ e 


MODULE 49 


_ 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: Smoking—Why It’s So Difficult to Quit 488 


EXPLORING DIVERSITY: Hucksters of Death: Promoting Smoking 
Throughout the World 490 


Promoting Health and Wellness 492 
Following Medical Advice 492 
Well-Being and Happiness 495 


APPLYING PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY: Catching Happiness: 
The Contagion of Emotions and Health 496 


CHAPTER 15 


Psychological Disorders 500 


Normal Versus Abnormal: Making the Distinction 503 
Defining Abnormality 503 

Perspectives on Abnormality: From Superstition to Science 505 
Classifying Abnormal Behavior: The ABCs of DSM 508 


The Major Psychological Disorders 513 

Anxiety Disorders 513 

NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: One Step Closer to Understanding OCD 517 
Somatoform Disorders 518 

Dissociative Disorders 518 

Mood Disorders 520 

Schizophrenia 524 

NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: Brain Changes with Schizophrenia 528 
Personality Disorders 529 

Childhood Disorders 530 

Other Disorders 531 


Psychological Disorders in Perspective 534 

The Social and Cultural Context of Psychological Disorders 535 

APPLYING PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY: Students Stressed and Depressed 537 
Exploring Diversity: DSM and Culture—and the Culture of DSM 538 


BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER OF PSYCHOLOGY: 
Deciding When You Need Help 539 


CHAPTER 16 


Treatment of Psychological Disorders 542 


Psychotherapy: Psychodynamic, Behavioral, and Cognitive 
Approaches to Treatment 545 

Psychodynamic Approaches to Therapy 546 

Behavioral Approaches to Therapy 548 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Contents xxi 


NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: How Behavioral Therapy Changes 
Your Brain 552 


Cognitive Approaches to Therapy 552 


EXE Psychotherapy: Humanistic, Interpersonal, and Group 
Approaches to Treatment 556 
Humanistic Therapy 556 
PSYCHWORK: Case Manager and Substance Abuse Counselor 557 
Interpersonal Therapy 558 
Group Therapy, Family Therapy, and Self-Help Groups 558 
Evaluating Psychotherapy: Does Therapy Work? 559 


EXPLORING DIVERSITY: Racial and Ethnic Factors in Treatment: Should 
Therapists Be Color Blind? 562 


EXE ë Biomedical Therapy: Biological Approaches to Treatment 564 
Drug Therapy 564 
Electroconvulsive Therapy (ECT) 567 
Psychosurgery 568 
Biomedical Therapies in Perspective 568 
Community Psychology: Focus on Prevention 570 


APPLYING PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY: Beating the Odds: 
Preventing Psychological Disorders Before They Start 572 


BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER OF PSYCHOLOGY: Choosing the Right 
Therapist 573 


CHAPTER 17 


Social Psychology 576 


Attitudes and Social Cognition 579 
Persuasion: Changing Attitudes 579 

PSYCHWORK: Advertising Agency Creator 581 

Social Cognition: Understanding Others 583 


EXPLORING DIVERSITY: Attributions in a Cultural Context: 
How Fundamental Is the Fundamental Attribution Error? 587 


Social Influence and Groups 590 
Conformity: Following What Others Do 590 
Compliance: Submitting to Direct Social Pressure 593 
Obedience: Following Direct Orders 595 





Prejudice and Discrimination 598 

The Foundations of Prejudice 599 

NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: The Prejudiced Brain 600 

Measuring Prejudice and Discrimination: The Implicit Personality Test 601 
Reducing the Consequences of Prejudice and Discrimination 601 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


xxii Contents 


SLES ë Positive and Negative Social Behavior 604 


Liking and Loving: Interpersonal Attraction and the Development of 
Relationships 604 


APPLYING PSYCHOLOGY IN THE 21st CENTURY: Friends Online: 
Is Facebook the New Student Union? 606 


Aggression and Prosocial Behavior: Hurting and Helping Others 608 
Helping Others: The Brighter Side of Human Nature 611 
NEUROSCIENCE IN YOUR LIFE: Moral Decisions and the Brain 613 


BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER OF PSYCHOLOGY: Dealing Effectively 
with Anger 613 


Going by the Numbers: 
Statistics in Psychology A-2 





Descriptive Statistics A-5 
The Mean: Finding the Average A-6 
The Median: Finding the Middle A-7 


| MODULE 56 __| 
The Mode: Finding What Is Most Frequent A-7 
| MODULE 57 | 





Comparing the Three M's: Mean Versus Median Versus Mode A-8 


Measures of Variability A-10 
The Range: Highest Minus Lowest A-11 
The Standard Deviation: Differences from the Mean A-11 


Using Statistics to Answer Questions: 
Inferential Statistics and Correlation A-14 
The Correlation Coefficient: Measuring Relationships A-15 


Glossary G-1 
References R-1 
Credits C-1 

Name Index l-1 
Subject Index l-15 








Students first. 

If | were to use only a few words to summarize my goal for this book, as 
well as my teaching philosophy, that’s what | would say. Students first. | believe 
that an effective textbook must be oriented to students—informing them, 
engaging them, and exciting them about the field and helping them connect 
it to their worlds. When students are engaged and challenged, they understand 
psychology at a deep and meaningful level. 

Luckily, psychology is a science that is inherently interesting to students. It 
is a discipline that speaks with many voices, offering a personal message to 
each student. To some, psychology provides a better understanding of others’ 
behavior. Others view psychology as a pathway to self-understanding. Still oth- 
ers see the potential for a future career, and some are drawn to psychology by 
the opportunity for intellectual discovery that its study provides. 

No matter what brings students into the introductory course and regardless 
of their initial motivation, Understanding Psychology, Tenth Edition, is designed to 
draw students into the field and stimulate their thinking. By focusing on stu- 
dents first, and connecting with them, the text promotes student success by 
engaging their attention in order to inform them, which results in students learn- 
ing the course content and becoming excited about the field of psychology. 





Ways of Engaging Students 













PROLOGUE Each chapter starts with an Prologue Trapped in a Male’s Body 
account of a real-l ife situation that demon- The images are iconic TV moments: precious little In fact, when Chastity came out as a lesbian to The Advocate in 
Š è 5 Chastity Bono, with her blonde hair and chubby 1995, she said, “As a kid | thought | was a little 
strates the relevance of basic principles and cheeks, gracing her parents’ 1970s variety show boy.” (Bartolomeo et al., 2009, p. 54) 
~The Sonny and Cher Comedy Hour with a family Chastity Bono never actually stopped 
concepts of psychology to perti nent issues song or the sweet si “Goodnight every- thinking of herself as male. In spring 2009, 
body and God bless!” While girls at home may at the age of 40, Chastity announced to 

j Bs have been envious of Cher’s glittery outfits, the world that she was transitioning from 

and problems. These prologues depict well Chastity, who always wore costumes to complement female to male and would henceforth be 
either that of her Mom's and Dad’s, later admitted, “I especially known as Chaz. 





known people and events. 











liked it when they put me in clothes that matched my father's.” 


LOOKING AHEAD 
The Prologue situation is then tied to key 





Chaz’s story illustrates the emotion and confusion that often In this set of modules, we consider human sexuality from 


i i i Š characterize one of the most universal behaviors: sexuality. several of these vantage points. We begin by examining gender 

themes a nd Issues d iscussed In the subse Exemplifying major personal as well as societal concerns, sex and discussing differences in societal expectations about how 
and the interrelated topic of gender are also key topics for men and women should behave and the impact of those 

q ue nt m od u les. psychologists in a variety of specialties. For instance, psychologists expectations on behavior and attitudes. Next we turn to sexual 
interested in motivation view sexuality in terms of sexual needs, behavior. We describe the biological aspects of sexual excitement 
drives, and gratification. Neuroscientists consider sexuality from and arousal and then examine the variety of sexual activities in 
the perspective of the relationship of the brain and nervous which people engage. We conclude with a discussion of non- 
system to the functioning of the sexual organs. Social psycholo- consenting sex, sexually transmitted infections, and the psycho- 


gists and psychologists who specialize in the study of women logical aspects of sexual difficulties. 
focus on society's rules of sexual conduct and the role sexual 
behavior plays in interpersonal behavior. 















Classical Conditioning 


LOOKING BACK AND EPILOGUE Found at the 
end of every set of modules, Looking Back contains 
critical thinking questions involving the use of 
Web resources and the interactivities that go with 
those modules. Critical thinking questions in the 
Epilogue that follows Looking Back relate to the 
Prologue at the opening of the set of modules. The 
thought-provoking questions in these sections 
provide a way of tying together a set of modules 
on one topic and illustrating how the concepts 
addressed in each module apply to the real-world 
situation described in the Prologue. 


xxiv 








VIDEO CLIPS AND INTERACTIVE EXERCISES 
Students can see what key concepts look like in the 
real world and apply what they are learning through 
video clips interactive exercises you assign through 
Connect. The exercises and clips are keyed to learn- 
ing objectives and levels of Bloom's taxonomy. 






jo 


00:08/03:56 iil, 4) A 


LOOKİNO eac A 


Psychology on the Web 


. Find at least two recent news articles or discussions on the web dealing with the 
issue of gender equality in school or the workplace, especially unequal treatment of 
individuals because of their gender. Summarize in writing what you found, and 
discuss your own attitudes toward this issue. 

Find information on the web about date rape, including guidelines or suggestions 
published by other colleges for preventing rape among students. Summarize your 
findings, including recommendations for behavioral or policy changes that might be 
effective at your college. If your college's policies toward or publications about this 
topic could benefit from your findings, bring them to the attention of the appropriate 
office at your institution and suggest specific changes. 


p 





i= p | log u e We have seen how psychologists are dealing with the 
— ~ topics of gender and sex, first with regard to gender 
roles, gender stereotyping, gender schemas, and sexism. Society's expectations regarding 
male and female characteristics and behaviors lead to inequities in the treatment of men 
and women in school, the workplace, and the rest of society—inequities that largely 
favor men. 

With regard to sexuality, we discussed the nature of the physical processes that 
surround sexuality and addressed the controversial issue of “normality” in sexual behav- 
ior. We saw the broad diversity of sexual behavior, and we discussed premarital sex and 
marital sex, as well as heterosexuality, homosexuality, and bisexuality. We also examined 
rape and other forms of nonconsenting sex, including childhood sexual abuse. 

Finally, we looked at the sexual problems that people may have, including STIs and 
sexual dysfunctions. We explored the nature of these problems and discussed ways to 
treat and deal with them. 

Before we turn to the subject of human development in the next few modules, 
return to the prologue of this chapter concerning Chaz Bono's sense that she is a male 
trapped in a female's body. Using your knowledge of gender and sex, consider the 
following questions. 


. Why might Bono's family, friends, and fans react negatively to her decision to 
become a man? 

How is Bono's transsexualism different from lesbianism? 

Why do you think Bono believed herself to be a lesbian before deciding that she was 
really a man? 

. What difficulties might Bono encounter in her journey to become male? 


Hp 


a 











Ways of Informing Students 


COMPREHENSIVE CONTENT COVERAGE Understanding Psychology provides 
broad coverage of the field of psychology, including the theories, research, and 
applications that permeate the discipline. Along with the traditional areas of psy- 
chology (neuroscience, sensation and perception, states of consciousness, learn- 
ing, memory, cognition, human development, personality, abnormal behavior and 
treatment, and social psychology), the applied topics of gender and sexuality 
(Chapter 11) and health psychology (Chapter 14) receive extensive attention. 


MODULAR ORGANIZATION The text is organized into modules in which each 
of the 17 chapters is divided into three or four manageable, self-contained sec- 
tions that include learning objectives and assessment opportunities. Each module 
ends with questions that assess learning on both an objective and more concep- 
tual, higher-order level. 

Rather than facing a long and potentially daunting chapter, students can 
study material in smaller chunks, which psychological research long ago found to 
be the optimal way to learn. Moreover, instructors can customize assignments for 
their students by asking them to read only those modules that fit their course 
outline and in the sequence that matches their syllabus. Alternatively, instructors 
who prefer to assign whole chapters can do so. 


CONNECTION TO APA STUDENT COMPETENCIES Conforming to recommen- 
dations of an American Psychological Association (APA) task force report on 
undergraduate student competencies (Board of Educational Affairs, 2002), every 
component of the text and its package is tied to specific psychological concepts 
and their application in everyday life. A chart indicating how the features of the 
textbook directly address the APA student competencies is provided in Figure 1 
on page xxvi. Equally important, every one of the thousands of test items in the 
Test Banks available to instructors is keyed to its corresponding APA competency 
in a document that is available on the text Web site. 


KEY CONCEPTS Each major section of a module begins with questions about the 
key concepts addressed in that section. These questions provide a framework for 
understanding and organizing the material that fol- 








lows, as well as providing assessment benchmarks. Exolorin Winch sitesi etc ChliaSin ict ee er aut ts 
ploring . 
DIVERSITY prepare salmon, at first she allows the daughter only to observe the 
or : Does Culture Influence How We Learn? entire procese ALE later, She permits her child to Gy out some basie 
EXPLORING DIVERSITY In addition to substantial Ea ar e E e E 
, , i when the daughter asks about how to do “the bac e part,” 
coverage of material relevant to diversity through- en ec mt epee repent metro with another non, 
: reason? The mi je t one cannot learn the individual parts of the task apart 
out, every set of modules also includes at least one the context of preparing the whole fish. (Tharp, 1989) 
. . . It should not be surprising that children raised in the Chilcotin tradition, which stresses 
special section devoted to an aspect of racial, eth- instruction that starts by communicating the entire task, may have difficulty with 
à . . . traditional Western schooling. In the approach to teaching most characteristic of 
nic, gender, or cultural diversity. These sections Western culture, tasks are broken down into their component parts. Only after each 
h . h | à h h . h . h h | . fi small step is learned is it thought possible to master the complete task. 
Ighllg t the way In wnich psychology Informs Do the differences in teaching approaches between cultures affect how people 
woe . . . learn? Some psychologists, taking a cognitive perspective on learning, suggest that 
(a nd Is | nformed by) Issues relati ng to the Increas- people develop particular learning styles, characteristic ways of approaching material, 
. . . . based on their cultural background and unique pattern of abilities (Anderson & 
ing multiculturalism of our global society. ‘Adams, 1992; Barmeyer, 2004; Wilkinson & Olliver-Gray, 2006). 

































































APA Learning Goals 
Critical Information Sociocultural 
Knowledge | Research | Thinking | Application and and 
Base of | Methods in | Skills in of Values in | Technological} Communication | International Personal Career Planning 

Book Feature Psychology | Psychology | Psychology | Psychology | Psychology | Literacy Skills Awareness | Development | and Development 
Chapter Content x x X x X x x X X X 
Prologue X X X X 
Looking Ahead X X X X 
Key Concepts X x x X 
o BE x <= [|= 
Exploring Diversity X X x x x 
Neuroscience in Your Life X x x x 
PsychWork X X X X 
Video Clips x x 
Interactive Exercises 
Running Glossary X X X 
-o BE x | x x ee 
Study Alerts x X X 
Recap/Evaluate/Rethink X X X 
Looking Back X X X X x 
Epilogue X X X X 
Adaptive Diagnostic Tool X 






































FIGURE 1 This grid shows the relationship between the broad learning goals devised by the 
American Psychological Association and specific types of content in Understanding Psychology. 
In addition, each of the test items in the Test Bank for the book, consisting of nearly 4,000 
individual, scorable items, is keyed to specific learning outcomes. 





STUDY ALERTS Throughout the modules, marginal notes point out especially 
important and difficult concepts and topics. These Study Alerts offer suggestions 
for learning the material effectively and for studying for tests. In Chapter 5, Module 
14, for example, a Study Alert emphasizes the importance of differentiating the 
five stages of sleep; the feature in Module 15 makes clear the key issue about 
hypnosis—whether it represents a different state of consciousness or is similar to 
normal waking consciousness; and in Module 16 it highlights Figure 2 for its clear 
view of the different ways that drugs produce their effects at a neurological level. 


wudy Alert 
Differentiate the five stages 
of sleep (stage 1, stage 2, 
stage 3, stage 4, and REM 
sleep), which produce dif- 
ferent brain-wave patterns. 











xxvi 








RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 


What is sensation, and how do psychologists study it? 

* Sensation is the activation of the sense organs by any 
source of physical energy. In contrast, perception is the 
process by which we sort out, interpret, analyze, and in- 
tegrate stimuli to which our senses are exposed. (p. 99) 


What is the relationship between a physical stimulus and the 
kinds of sensory responses that result from it? 

* Psychophysics studies the relationship between the 
physical nature of stimuli and the sensory responses 
they evoke. (p. 99) 

The absolute threshold is the smallest amount of physi- 
cal intensity at which a stimulus can be detected. Under 
ideal conditions absolute thresholds are extraordinarily 
sensitive, but the presence of noise (background stimuli 
that interfere with other stimuli) reduces detection capa- 
bilities. (p. 100) 
© The difference threshold, or just noticeable difference, is 
the smallest change in the level of stimulation required 
to sense that a change has occurred. According to 
Weber’s law, a just noticeable difference is a constant 
proportion of the intensity of an initial stimulus. (p. 101) 
* Sensory adaptation occurs when we become accustomed. 
to a constant stimulus and change our evaluation of it. 
Repeated exposure to a stimulus results in an apparent 
decline in sensitivity to it. (p. 102) 


KEY TERMS 


sensation p. 99 
perception p. 99 
stimulus p. 99 
psychophysics p. 99 


absolute threshold p. 100 
difference threshold (just 





noticeable difference) p. 101 





EVALUATE 


1, _______is the stimulation of the sense organs; 

is the sorting out, interpretation, analysis, and 
integration of stimuli by the sense organs and the brain. 

2. The term absolute threshold refers to the inten- 
sity of a stimulus that must be present for the stimulus to 
be detected. 

3. Weber discovered that for a difference between two stim- 
uli to be perceptible, the stimuli must differ by at least a 

proportion. 

4. After completing a very difficult rock climb in the morn- 
ing, Carmella found the afternoon climb unexpectedly 
easy. This example illustrates the phenomenon of 














RETHINK 


1. Do you think it is possible to have sensation without 
perception? Is it possible to have perception without 
sensation? 

2. From the perspective of a manufacturer: How might you need 
to take psychophysics into account when developing new 
products or modifying existing ones? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 
uonesdepe p ‘jueysu0s g 4saqeus z ‘uondaorad ‘uonesuag T 


Weber's law p. 102 
adaptation p. 102 





RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK Each module 
concludes with a Recap/Evaluate/Rethink 
section. The Recap sections review the con- 
cept questions found at the beginning of 
each module. Evaluate sections test recall of 
the material, assessing the degree of initial 
learning. The Rethink sections provide 
thought-provoking questions designed to 
provoke critical thinking about the material. 


RUNNING GLOSSARY Key terms are high- 
lighted in boldface type within the text where 
they are introduced, and definitions are given in 
the margin of the page, along with pronuncia- 
tion guides for difficult words. To facilitate study, 
at the end of each module there is a list of the 
key terms and concepts introduced in that 
module. There is also a glossary of all key terms 
and concepts at the end of the book. 


PRE- AND POST-TESTS Students can practice 
with key concepts with pre- and post-tests you 
assign through Connect. The test items are 
keyed to learning objectives and levels of 
Bloom's taxonomy. 











OSes ton EL 
oo 
4. poem 
“You tea! fine as you st down i jour usual sasi in ans. However when Di braus anscences Rat Tere all ta a pop gur oer 100 of he most SMU cabia, words Covered in De last len dass 
AANE our Neat etary, DOOLA sad jou pels gocers eesighr yoo VARION TS NALIN DADIE RNN PON 
ra 
i wen 
re 
rus 
hee 
5 a 
= poris TESS aeei u e e 
Sere a ing m vie aaaea adong aiae Te GotSBAR BCom 12 Me Rap SI DA Lar to eat WNO he harrier BA ORAM DAL MA Groot food inte Te tare ans Men kane on the hight “Aer sever 
‘Vials. fe Bah show mo more Incinabon to Come te the top of he tare abes the Aight is timed os than Re; Sion te Sret tial hat aculd pou suggest Pat Boris Oo io improve his kaining technique? 
fad F Wie should turn ont the Bare betore he cope the food Wnts Pe taah 
a (> He abad beep the ight on at ai bres 
(> Me BAOLIG ter get Be SANGIS ides Cecause gotstar sent prod INDEMANA fer Clee ticel ConaBoning Bameng 
O He should tum he ight on and then of belare dopping De food into he tare 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Ways of Exciting Students 
and Helping Them Relate 
Psychology to Their World Ž 


These boxes highlight the rele- 
vance of psychology by presenting current and 
potential applications of psychological theory 
and research findings to real-world problems. For 
example, one box discusses the psychological 
principles that explain the dangers of texting 
while driving, while another highlights how 
artificial intelligence researchers are building 
“smarter” robots that have the potential to trans- 
form our daily lives. 











Applying Psychology 


‘Twenty-four year old Aiden Quinn 
couldn't believe his good fortune. At his 
young age, he had beaten the competition 
to land his dream job of conducting 
trolleys through the streets of Boston. But 
in just one moment his world came 
crashing down around him: It was the 
moment when he took his eyes off the 
tracks to send a text message to his 
girlfriend and the trolley he was driving 
crashed into the one ahead of it. 


If you've ever texted while driving, you're 
not alone. A recent survey conducted by 
the AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety 
shows that nearly half of drivers aged 18 to 
24 admit to doing so, even though the ma- 
jority of them were aware that they were 
increasing their risk of having an accident. 
In fact, nearly 90% of survey respondents 
rated texting while driving as a very seri- 
ous threat to safety (AAA Foundation for 
Traffic Safety, 2008, 2009). 

Researchers are now confirming what 
intuition suggests is true: The distraction 
caused by texting while driving is ex- 
tremely dangerous. One study used video 
cameras to monitor truckers on long hauls 
and found that they were 23 times more 





[ Neuroscience in Your Life: Wt 


(Source: Garn et al., 2009.) 


(a) Areas more strongly activated in females to tools 





6 Differences in how men and women process information reflect differences in 
learning and experience with those objects. In an fMRI study, (a) women showed greater 
activation in certain areas of the brain when they processed names for tools and (b) men 
showed greater activation in other areas of the brain when they processed names of plants. 





(b) Areas more strongly activated in males to plants 


n while texting than 
isions caused by 
ically taken his 

t 5 seconds—a 








Increasing research shows that texting while 
driving is extremely dangerous. 


seemingly short period of time until you 
realize that a vehicle at highway speed 
travels about 150 yards in those few sec- 
onds (Virginia Tech Transportation Insti- 
tute, 2009). 

Another study using college students 
and a driving simulator showed that text- 
ing produced the same duration of about 
5 seconds of distraction and that the stu- 
dents were 8 times more likely to have a 
collision while texting. Although those 
may seem like better odds, they're still 


much worse than the risk presented by 
other distractors such as changing the sta- 
tion on a car’s radio. Even though the 
study used students, who are likely skillful 
at texting and multitasking, and light ve- 
hicles that are more maneuverable than 
trucks, the results clearly show that texting 
while driving poses a terrific risk (Cooper 
& Strayer, 2008). 

Multitasking in general is difficult. For 
instance, in one study, researchers divided 
a group of college students into two 
groups, one composed of students who 
multitasked online regularly and a second 
that did not. The frequent multitaskers 
were more easily distracted, and they were 
unable to filter out stimuli that were irrele- 
vant to tasks that they were trying to com- 
plete (Ophir, Nass, & Wagner, 2009) 

If you, like many of the respondents to 
the AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety sur- 
vey, believe that you're the exception to the 
rule, just keep one thing in mind: that’s 
what Aiden Quinn thought, too. He lost his 
job and is facing civil lawsuits and criminal 
prosecution. 


* Most people assume that they are able to text and drive at the same time without 
declines in their driving ability. Why do you think that is the case, and how accu- 


rate is their assumption? 


¢ Why would frequent multitaskers be more distractible than those who multitask 


less frequently? 


EUR I R LIFE This feature, 
which appears in each chapter, emphasizes 
the importance of neuroscientific research 
within the various subfields of the discipline 
and in students’ lives. Compelling brain 
scans, with both caption and textual explana- 
tion, illustrate significant neuroscientific find- 
ings that are increasingly influencing the 
field of psychology. For example, one 
Neuroscience in Your Life feature shows MRI 
techniques that illustrate how our brains pro- 
cess information coming from each eye sepa- 
rately. Another shows how the brains of 
smokers—even those who have quit—are 
activated when they see images related to 


xxviii 


smoking. Yet another discusses the effects of 
sleep on memory. 





BECOMING AN INFORMED CONSUMER 
OF PSYCHOLOGY Every set of modules 
includes material designed to make readers 





As we have seen, surveys of college women make clear that the 
greatest danger of rape comes not from some unknown assailant 
but from a fellow student. There are ways, however, to reduce 
the likelihood of date rape. The following suggestions provide 
some guidance for women and men (Shultz, Scherman, & 
Marshall, 2000; American College Health Association, 2006; 


BECOMING AN 
INFORMED CONSUMER 


of Psychology 


Lowering the Risks of Date Rape 








more informed consumers of psychological 
information by giving them the ability to : 
evaluate critically what the field of psychol- : 
ogy offers. These discussions also provide 

sound, useful guidance concerning common 
problems. ‘ 





Watkins, 2007): 


Women should believe in their rights to set limits and communicate them clearly, 
firmly, and early on. They should say no when they mean no. 

Women should be assertive in refusing to engage in an activity in which they 
don’t want to engage. They should keep in mind that men may interpret passiv- 
ity as permission. 

Women should be aware of situations in which they are at risk. 

Women should keep in mind that some men interpret certain kinds of dress as 
sexually provocative, and not all men subscribe to the same standards of sexual 
behavior as they do. 

Women should keep close tabs on what they are given to drink in social situations; 
victims of date rape have sometimes been given mind-altering “date-rape” drugs. 
Men should be aware of their dates’ views on sexual behavior. 

Men should not hold the view that the goal of dating is to “score.” 

The word No should be understood to mean no and not be interpreted as an invi- 
tation to continue. 

Men should not assume that certain kinds of dress or flirtatious behavior are an 


invitation to sex. 
Both men and women should understand that alcohol and drugs cloud judgment 
and hinder communication between them. 





PsychWork 


For decades, guide dogs have provided a set of eyes to the 
visually impaired, expanding the opportunities open to them 


SEEING EYE GUIDE DOG TRAINER 24 increasing their independence. But it takes a great deal of 


training to make a dog an effective seeing eye guide dog, 





Name: Lea Johnson 


Position: Seeing Eye Guide Dog Trainer 
Education: BS, Geography, Dartmouth College, 


Hanover, NH 





according to Lea Johnson, who works with The Seeing Eye 
agency in Morristown, New Jersey. Johnson teaches apprentice 
instructors to carry out the demanding, but rewarding, process 
of training dogs. 

“We hire college graduates, and while we don’t require a 
specific major, a background in psychology or animal science allows employees to 
more easily connect with different aspects of the job,” she said. 

An apprentice instructor needs to have self-motivation in order to complete all 
aspects of the dog’s training. In addition, they need to be able to work in a team 
setting, according to Johnson. But that’s only part of it. 

“The process of training the dogs is complex,” says Johnson. “For example, the 
dog must be obedient and respond to their visually-impaired owner. But they also 
get praised for sometimes refusing their owner’s commands, if it would put their 
owner in danger.” 

Once a dog learns the skills it needs, the trainer must then teach a visually- 
impaired person how to work with the dog. 

“After training dogs for four months, the trainers must be able to teach blind 
people the skills to care for and travel with their Seeing Eye dog safely,” Johnson 
said. Not only must trainers relate well to dogs, but they also must interact well with 
blind people. She adds, “The training of people is intense and emotionally challeng- 
ing in a very different way from the dog training portion. Without a good heart to 
start with, trainers would never be successful.” 





PSYCHWORK New in this edition, PsychWork 
introduces students to different career paths 
to which an understanding of psychology 
leads. Each PsychWork profile illustrates people 
in a variety of occupations whose knowledge 
of psychology informs and enhances their 
work. Among the individuals profiled are a 
social worker, a nutritionist, a physical rehabili- 
tation counselor, and an advertising executive, 
showing that psychology is all around us and 
important to many occupations. 





mCOOonnecE Se 2000 m.t 2pm 


XXX 





Ways of Connecting 
with Today’s Students 





Today’s students are as different from the learners of the last generation as 
today’s discipline of psychology is different from the field 30 years ago. 
Students now learn in multiple modalities; rather than sitting down and read- 
ing traditional printed chapters in linear fashion from beginning to end, their 
work preferences tend to be more visual and more interactive, and their read- 
ing and study often occur in short bursts. For many students, a traditionally 
formatted printed textbook is no longer enough when they have instant, 24/7 
access to news and information from around the globe. 

Connect Psychology is our response to today’s student. The groundbreak- 
ing adaptive diagnostic tool helps students “know what they know” while help- 
ing them learn what they don’t know through engaging interactivities, exercises, 
videos, and readings. Instructors using Connect are reporting their students’ 
performance is improving by a letter grade or more. 

Through this unique tool, Understanding Psychology gives instructors the abil- 
ity to identify struggling students quickly and easily before the first exam. 
Connect Psychology’s adaptive diagnostic tool develops an individualized 
learning plan for every student. Confidence levels tailor the next question to 
each individual, helping students to know what they don’t know. If your stu- 
dent is doing well, the adaptive diagnostic tool 
will challenge the student with more applied 
and conceptual questions. If your student is 
struggling, the system identifies problem areas 
and directs the student to the exact page they 
need to read. In doing so, it works like a GPS, 
helping students master key concepts efficiently 
and effectively. Regardless of individual study 
habits, preparation, and approaches to the 
course, students will find Understanding Psychology, 
coupled with Connect Psychology, adapts to 
them individually and provides a road map for 
success. 





What’s New and Updated 
in the Tenth Edition 





The Tenth Edition of Understanding Psychology contains a significant amount of new 
and updated features and content reflecting the advances in the field and the sug- 
gestions of reviewers. Chapter 4 (Sensation and Perception), Chapter 5 (States of 
Consciousness), and Chapter 9 (Intelligence) have undergone especially heavy revi- 
sions. More than 1,000 new citations have been added, and most of them refer to 
articles and books published since 2008. 

In addition, this edition incorporates a wide range of new topics. The following 
sample of new and revised topics provides a good indication of the book’s currency. 


Chapter 1—Introduction to Psychology 
e Gender ratios in the field 
e Distinction between psychiatrists 
and psychologists 
e Psychologist work sites 
Chapter 2—Psychological Research 
e Replication 
e Random sampling in surveys 
e Correlation and causality 
e Self-affirmations 
Chapter 3—Neuroscience and Behavior 
e Neurogenesis 
e Hemispheric specialization 
emergence 
e Brain wave-controlled devices 
e Optogenetics 
Chapter 4—Sensation and Perception 
e Culture and susceptibility to visual 
illusions 
Divided attention and texting 
Multimodal perception 
Balance and the vestibular system 
Moon illusion (specialist) 
Ewald Hering 
Thomas Young 
Hermann von Helmholtz 
Rarity of complete color blindness 
Pheromones and humans 
Change order of perceptual con- 
stancy and depth perception 
Psychophysics still an active field 
Pheromones 
Otoliths and hair cells 
Pain theories 
Absolute threshold 


Flowers reflecting ultraviolet light 
Primacy of cornea in focusing light 
Aging and auditory perception 
Ring tones and students / teachers 
Apparent motion 
Vomeronasal organ 

e Speech perception 
Chapter 5—States of Consciousness 

e Biological dependence 

e Oxycodone 

e Psychological dependence 

e Activation information modulation 
(AIM) theory 
Added Red Bull to chart on caffeine 
e Added new prologue and 21st 

century 
e Consciousness versus 
unconsciousness 

e Night terrors 
e Insomnia #1 
e Role of sleep in learning and 
memory 
Process of hypnosis 
Hypnosis and divided consciousness 
Daydreaming and brain activation 
Release of growth hormones and 
sleep 
Chapter 6—Learning 

e Description of habituation 
Spontaneous recovery definition 
Stimulus generalization definition 
Biological constraints on learning 
Reinforcment for parenting and 
academic success 
e Conditioning of emotions 


Chapter 7—Memory 
e Hyperthymestic syndrome 
e Neural basis of memory 
consolidation 
e PKM-zeta and traumatic memories 
Chapter 8—Cognition and Language 
e Algorithm 
e Concepts 
e Language in animals 
e Cognition and hunches regarding 
danger 
e Language acquisition in bilingual 
infants 
e Creativity in different domains 
e Language definition 
e Confirmation bias 
Chapter 9—Intelligence 
e G-factor 
e Mental age 
Down syndrome 
Artificial intelligence 
Adaptive testing 
e Flynn effect 
e Fetal alcohol syndrome statistics 
Chapter 10—Motivation and Emotion 
e Facial expressions across cultures 
e Ghrelin and hunger 
e Insulin 
e Self-determination theory 
e High fiber foods and weight loss 
Chapter 11—Sexuality and Gender 
e Biological explanations for gender 
differences 
e Gender similarities in math 
performance 


xxxi 





e Sexual fantasies 
e Sexual desire 
e Intersex persons 
e Worldwide gender equality 
Chapter 12—Development 
e Midlife crisis myth 
e Video game play and cognitive 
declines in late adulthood 
e Disengagement theory lack of 
support 
e Emerging adulthood 
Chapter 13—Personality 
e Biological basis of personality 
e Situationism 
e Cognitive-affective processing 
system (CAPS) 


e Quick judgments of personality 
Chapter 14—Health Psychology: Stress, 


Coping, and Well-Being 
e General Adaptation Model and 
cortisol secretion 


xxxii 


e Cancer and quality of life 
e Smoking and loss of autonomy 
e Smoking cessation and social 
support 
e Smoking in developing countries 
e Gender differences in happiness 
Chapter 15—Psychological Disorders 
e College student stress and 
depression 
e Autism prevalence increase 
e Formal thought disorder 
e Definition of obsessive-compulsive 
disorder 
e Type land II schizophrenia treat- 
ment outcomes 
Chapter 16—Treatment of 
Psychological Disorders 
e Prescription drugs prescribed by 
clinical psychologists 


Self-actualization and humanistic 
therapy 

Interventions for prevention of 
psychological disorders 
Cingulotomy 

Gamma knife surgery 

Ketamine in the treatment of 
depression 

PsychWork on halfway house 


Chapter 17—Social Psychology 


PsychWork on advertising agency 
owner 

Behavioral economics 

Social networking sites and 
friendship 

Milgram replication 

Roots of morality 


Students First: The Bottom Line 





Based on extensive student feedback, systematic research involving a wide range of 
instructors, and endorsements received from reviewers at a variety of schools, I am 
confident that this edition reflects what instructors want and need: a book that moti- 
vates students to understand and apply psychology to their own lives. Understanding 
Psychology, Tenth Edition, is designed to expose readers to the content—and prom- 
ise—of psychology, and to do so in a way that will nurture students’ excitement 
about psychology and keep their enthusiasm alive for a lifetime. 


State-of-the-Art Support 
Materials for Students 


and Instructors 





Resources available for use with this text support both new and veteran instructors, 
whether they favor traditional text-based instruction or a blend of traditional and 
electronic media. The Tenth Edition text and support materials provide complemen- 
tary experiences for instructors and students. All of these components are built 
around the core concepts articulated in the text to promote a deeper understanding 
of psychology. This type of integration gives instructors the flexibility to use any of 
the text-specific electronic or print materials knowing they are completely compatible 
with one another. Please see your McGraw-Hill sales representative for information 
on policy, price, and availability of the following materials. 





FOR THE INSTRUCTOR 
What if... 


e You could recreate the one-on-one experience of working through difficult 
concepts in office hours with every one of your students without having to 
invest any office-hour time to do so? 

e You could see at a glance how well each of your students (or sections) was 
performing in each segment of your course? 

e You had all of the assignments and resources for your course preorganized by 
learning objective and with point-and-click flexibility? 


Over the course of developing Understanding Psychology, we asked these questions 
and many more. But we did not stop at simply asking questions. We visited with 
faculty across the country and also observed you doing what you do to prepare and 
deliver your courses. We observed students as they worked through assignments and 
studied for exams. The result of these thousands of hours of research and develop- 
ment is a state of the art learning environment tool that bolsters student performance 
at the same time as it makes instructors’ lives easier and more efficient. To experience 
this environment for yourself, please visit www.mcgraw-hillconnect.com. 


Online Learning Center for Instructors. The password-protected instructor side of 
the Online Learning Center (www.mhhe.com/feldmanup10e) contains the Instruc- 
tor’s Manual, Test Bank files, PowerPoint slides, Classroom Performance System 
(CPS) Questions, Image Gallery, and other valuable material to help you design and 
enhance your course. See more information about specific assets below. Ask your 
local McGraw-Hill representative for password information. 


Instructor’s Manual by Susan Krauss Whitbourne, University of Massachusetts, 
Amherst. This comprehensive guide provides all the tools and resources instructors 
need to present and enhance their introductory psychology course. The Instructor’s 
Manual contains detailed lecture launchers, learning objectives, interesting lecture 
and media presentation ideas, student assignments and handouts. The many tips and 
activities in this manual can be used with any class, regardless of size or teaching 
approach. 


Test Banks revised by Michelle Merwin, University of Tennessee at Martin, and 
Matthew Isaak, University of Louisiana at Lafayette. Both test banks incorporate the 
new content in Understanding Psychology, Tenth Edition. Each test bank contains 
more than 2,000 multiple-choice items, classified by cognitive type and level of 
difficulty and keyed to the appropriate key concept and page in the textbook. Fill- 
in-the-blank and short-answer questions are provided for all modules. Moreover, 
each of the thousands of test items is keyed to the APA core psychology competen- 
cies. All questions are compatible with EZ Test, McGraw-Hill’s Computerized Test 
Bank program. 


Classroom Performance System Content by Robert Moore of lowa Valley Commu- 
nity College District. The Classroom Performance System (CPS) from eInstruction 
allows instructors to gauge immediately what students are learning during lectures. 
Instructors can draw on the quiz and poll questions to get instant feedback, even from 


xxxiii 


xxxiv 





students who are reluctant to speak out in class. In addition, CPS facilitates taking 
attendance, giving and grading pop quizzes, and giving formal, printed class tests 
with multiple versions of the test using CPS for immediate grading. 


Image Gallery. More than 100 figures from the text can be downloaded from the 
Image Gallery on the Instructor’s Online Learning Center. 


PowerPoint Presentations by Cathy Hunt of Pennsylvania State University. These 
presentations cover the key points of each chapter and include charts and graphs from 
the text. They can be used as is, or you may modify them to meet your specific needs. 


FOR THE STUDENT 


Adaptive Diagnostic Tool. This diagnostic tool is an unparalleled, intelligent learn- 
ing system based on cognitive mapping that diagnoses your students’ knowledge of 
a particular subject and then creates an individualized learning path geared toward 
student success in your course. It offers individualized assessment by delivering 
appropriate learning material in the form of questions at the right time, helping 
students attain mastery of the content. Whether the system is assigned by you or 
used independently by students as a study tool, the results can be recorded in an 
easy-to-use grade report that allows you to measure student progress at all times and 
coach your students to success. 

As an added benefit, all content covered in this adaptive diagnostic is tied to 
learning objectives for your course so that you can use the results as evidence of 
subject mastery. This tool also provides a personal study plan that allows the student 
to estimate the time it will take and number of questions required to learn the subject 
matter. Your students will learn faster, study more efficiently, and retain more knowl- 
edge when using Understanding Psychology. 


Optional Modules on Diversity and I/O Psychology. For instructors who like to incor- 
porate lectures on diversity or industrial/organizational issues in their introductory 
psychology course, optional full-color modules on these topics can be packaged with 
students’ copies of Understanding Psychology, Tenth Edition. The Diversity module, writ- 
ten by Mark H. Chae of William Paterson University, discusses the roots of diversity 
and addresses related issues, such as conflict and cooperation. The module on 
Industrial-Organizational Psychology, written by Carnot Nelson and Russell Johnson of 
the University of South Florida, broadly introduces this growing area of interest. Instruc- 
tors may request these modules through their McGraw-Hill sales representative. 


Annual Editions: Psychology 10/11 (edited by William Buskist, Auburn University). 
This annually updated reader provides convenient, inexpensive access to current 
articles selected from the best of the public press. Organizational features include an 
annotated listing of selected Worldwide Web sites; an annotated table of contents; a 
topic guide; a general introduction; brief overviews for each section; a topical index; 
and an instructor’s resource guide with testing materials. 


Classic Edition Sources: Psychology, 4e (edited by Terry Pettijohn of Ohio State 
University—Marion). This reader provides more than 40 selections of enduring intel- 
lectual value—classic articles, book excerpts, and research studies—that have shaped 
the study of psychology and our contemporary understanding of it. 





Taking Sides: Clashing Views on Psychological Issues, 16e (edited by Brent Slife of 
Brigham Young University). This reader presents current controversial issues in a 
debate-style format designed to stimulate student interest and develop critical think- 
ing skills. Each issue is thoughtfully framed with an issue summary, an issue intro- 
duction, and a postscript. An instructor’s manual with testing material is available 
for each volume. 


Acknowledgments 


One of the central features of Understanding Psychology is the involvement of both 
professionals and students in the review process. The tenth edition of Understanding 
Psychology has relied heavily—and benefited substantially—from the advice of 
instructors and students from a wide range of backgrounds. 

I am extraordinarily grateful to the following reviewers, who provided their time 
and expertise to help insure that Understanding Psychology, Tenth Edition, reflects the 
best that psychology has to offer. 





MANUSCRIPT REVIEWERS 


Miriam Abety 
Miami Dade Community College 


Bernard J. Baars 
The Neurosciences Institute 


Michael Barber 
Santa Fe College 


Joyce Bateman-Jones 
Central Texas College 


Richard Bernstein 
Broward Community College 


Joy Berrenberg 
University of Colorado, Denver 


Josette Bonewitz 
Vincennes University 


Maria Bravo 
Central Texas College 


Kathleen Brown 
Claremont McKenna College 


Rita Butterfield 
Sonoma State University 


Keith Conn 
Cleveland Institute of Electronics 


Katrina Cooper 
Bethany College 


Katharyn Couch 
Eastern Oklahoma State 


Jennifer Dale 
Community College of Aurora 


Stephanie Hayes 
Oklahoma City Community College 


Byron Heidenreich 
Illinois State University 


Lauren Holupka 
Molloy College 


Cathleen Hunt 
Pennsylvania State University 


Linda Jackson 
Michigan State University 


Shirley P. Kuhn 
Pittsburgh Community College 


Elizabeth Lanthier 


Northern Virginia Community College 


Kathy McCormick 
Ocean County College 


Robert Mather 
University of Central Oklahoma 


Brian Mehrer 
Western Nevada College 


xxxvi 





Michelle Merwin Victoria V. Roark 


University of Tennessee at Martin Troy University 

Peter Metzner Justin Rueb 

Vance-Granville Community College University of Wisconsin, Stevens Point 
Karen Miranda Elizabeth Satchell 

Mount Ida College Austin Community College 

Robert Moore John E. Sparrow 

Iowa Valley Community College University of New Hampshire at Manchester 
Ronnie Naramore Jason Spiegelman 

Angelina College Community College of Baltimore 
Jennifer Ortiz-Garza Amber Vesotski 

Victoria College Alpena Community College 

Diane Reddy Susan Whitbourne 

University of Wisconsin, Milwaukee University of Massachusetts, Amherst 


In addition, Jane W. Couperus of Hampshire College provided exceptional sup- 
port in helping identify appropriate neuroscientific research to include in the Neuro- 
science in Your Life features. I thank her for her superb work. 

Many teachers along my educational path have shaped my thinking. I was intro- 
duced to psychology at Wesleyan University, where several committed and inspiring 
teachers—and in particular Karl Scheibe—conveyed their sense of excitement about 
the field and made its relevance clear to me. Karl epitomizes the teacher-scholar 
combination to which I aspire, and I continue to marvel at my good fortune in hav- 
ing such a role model. 

By the time I left Wesleyan I could envision no other career but that of psy- 
chologist. Although the nature of the University of Wisconsin, where I did my grad- 
uate work, could not have been more different from the much smaller Wesleyan, the 
excitement and inspiration were similar. Once again, a cadre of excellent teachers— 
led, especially, by the late Vernon Allen—molded my thinking and taught me to 
appreciate the beauty and science of the discipline of psychology. 

My colleagues and students at the University of Massachusetts, Amherst, pro- 
vide ongoing intellectual stimulation, and I thank them for making the university a 
fine place to work. Several people also provided extraordinary research and editorial 
help. In particular, I am grateful to my superb students, past and present, including 
Erik Coats, Sara Levine, Jim Tyler, Chris Poirier, and Matt Zimbler. John Bickford, in 
particular, provided editorial input that has enhanced the book considerably. Finally, 
I am grateful to John Graiff, whose hard work and dedication helped immeasurably 
on just about everything involving this book. 

I also offer great thanks to the McGraw-Hill editorial team that participated in 
this edition of the book. Steve Debow’s hands-on interest, as well as his friendship, 
helped support the book through the last decade. Editorial Vice President Mike Ryan 
and Editorial Director Beth Mejia created a creative, energetic, and supportive envi- 
ronment, and I am in awe of their enthusiasm, commitment, and never-ending good 
ideas. I also thank Cara Labell and Sue Ewing, Developmental Editors on this edition. 
They did a superb job of managing a myriad of details (as well as me). I’m also 





pleased that Sponsoring Editor Krista Bettino worked on this edition of Understand- 
ing Psychology. She brought motivation, intelligence, and good ideas to the project. 
Finally, every reader of this book owes a debt to Rhona Robbin and Judith Kromm, 
developmental editors on earlier editions of Understanding Psychology. Their relentless 
pursuit of excellence helped form the core of this book, and they taught me a great 
deal about the craft and art of writing. 

Central to the design, production, and marketing process were the Production 
Service Manager Chris Schabow, Lead Production Editor Anne Fuzellier, Buyer II 
Tandra Jorgensen, and Designer Ellen Pettengel. Photo editor Toni Michaels did her 
usual superb job in identifying photos and, as always, was a pleasure to work with. 
I would also like to thank marketing managers Julia Flohr and Yasuko Okada for 
their enthusiasm and commitment to this project. I am proud to be a part of this 
world-class team. 

Finally, I remain completely indebted to my family. My parents, Leah Brochstein 
and the late Saul D. Feldman, provided a lifetime foundation of love and support, 
and I continue to see their influence in every corner of my life. I am grateful, too, to 
the late Harry Brochstein, who enriched my life and thinking in many ways. 

My extended family also plays a central role in my life. They include, more or less 
in order of age, my nieces and nephews, my terrific brother, and my brothers- and 
sisters-in-law, and the late Ethel Radler. Finally, my mother-in-law, the late Mary Evans 
Vorwerk, had an important influence on this book, and I remain ever grateful to her. 

Ultimately, my children, Jonathan, Joshua, and Sarah; my daughters-in-law Leigh 
and Julie; my grandson Alex; and my wife, Katherine, remain the focal point of my 
life. I thank them, with immense love. 


Robert S. Feldman 
Amherst, Massachusetts 


xxxvii 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


No matter why you are taking introductory psychology, it’s a safe bet you're 
interested in maximizing your understanding of the material and getting a good 
grade. And you want to accomplish these goals as quickly and efficiently as 
possible. 

Good news: You're taking the right course, and you're learning the right 
material. Several subfields of psychology have identified a variety of guidelines 
and techniques that will help you learn and remember material not only 
related to psychology, but also relevant to every other discipline that you will 
study. 

We'll consider a variety of guidelines relating to doing well in your psychol- 
ogy class—and every other class you'll take in your college career. Here’s my 
guarantee to you: If you learn and follow the guidelines in each of these areas, 
you'll become a better student and get better grades. Always remember that 
good students are made, not born, and these suggestions will help you become 
an all-around better student. 


Adopt a General Study Strategy 


Let’s begin with a brief consideration of a general study strategy, applicable to 
all of your courses, including introductory psychology. Psychologists have 
devised several excellent (and proven) techniques for improving study skills, 
two of which are described here: “P.O.W.E.R,’ or Prepare, Organize, Work, Evaluate, 
and Rethink; and “SQ3R,” or Survey, Question, Read, Recite, and Review. By 
employing one of these two procedures, you can increase your ability to learn 
and retain information and to think critically. 





P.O.W.E.R. The PO.W.E.R. learning strategy systematizes the acquisition of new 
material by providing a learning and critical thinking framework. It stresses the 
importance of learning objectives and appropriate preparation before you 
begin to study as well as the significance of self-evaluation and the incorpora- 
tion of critical thinking into the learning process. Specifically, use of the P.O.W.E.R. 
learning system entails the following steps: 























Prepare. Before starting any journey, we need to know where we are 
headed. Academic journeys are no different; we need to know what our 
goals are. The Prepare stage consists of thinking about what we hope to 
gain from reading a specific section of the text by 
identifying specific goals that we seek to accom- 
plish. In Understanding Psychology, Tenth Edition, 
these goals are presented as broad questions at 
the start of each chapter and again at the begin- 
ning of each module. 

e Organize. Once we know what our goals are, we 
can develop a route to accomplish those goals. The 
Organize stage involves developing a mental road- 
map of where we are headed. Understanding 


=A 








Psychology highlights the organization of each upcoming chapter. Read 
the outline at the beginning of each chapter to get an idea of what top- 
ics are covered and how they are organized. 

Work. The key to the P.O.W.E.R. learning system is actually reading and 
studying the material presented in the book. In some ways Work is the 
easy part because, if you have carried out the steps in the preparation 
and organization stages, you'll know where you're headed and how 
you'll get there. Remember, the main text isn’t the only material that you 
need to read and think about. It’s also important to read the boxes and 
the material in the margins in order to gain a full understanding of the 
material. 

Evaluate. The fourth step, Evaluate, provides the opportunity to deter- 
mine how effectively you have mastered the material. In Understanding 
Psychology, a series of questions at the end of each module permits a 
rapid check of your understanding of the material. Quizzes on the book's 
website, or Online Learning Center, provide additional opportunities to 
test yourself. Evaluating your progress is essential to assessing your 
degree of mastery of the material. 

Rethink. The final step in the RO.W.E.R. learning system requires that you 
think critically about the content. Critical thinking entails re-analyzing, 
reviewing, questioning, and challenging assumptions. It affords you the 
opportunity to consider how the material fits with other information you 
have already learned. Every major section of Understanding Psychology 
ends with a Rethink section. Answering its thought-provoking questions 
will help you understand the material more fully and at a deeper level. 


SQ3R. Use of the SQ3R learning system entails the following specific steps: 


+ Survey. The first step of the SQ3R method is to survey the material by 


reading the outlines that open each module, the headings, figure cap- 
tions, recaps, and Looking Ahead and Looking Back sections, providing 
yourself with an overview of the major points of the chapter. 

Question. The next step—the “Q”—is to question. Formulate questions 
about the material, either aloud or in writing, prior to actually reading a 
section of text. The questions posed at the beginning of each module 
and the Evaluate and Rethink questions that end each part of the chapter 
are examples. 

Read. Read carefully and, even more importantly, read actively and criti- 
cally. While you are reading, answer the questions you have asked your- 
self. Critically evaluate material by considering the implications of what 
you are reading, thinking about possible exceptions and contradictions, 
and examining underlying assumptions. 

Recite. This step involves describing and explaining to yourself (or to a 
friend) the material you have just read and answering the questions you 


XXXİX 


































www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


have posed earlier. Recite aloud; the recitation process helps to identify 
your degree of understanding of the material you have just read. 

e Review. In this final step, review the material, looking it over, reading the 
Looking Back summaries, and answering the in-text review questions. 





Manage Your Time 


Without looking up from the page, answer this question: What time is it? 

Most people are pretty accurate in their answer. And if you don’t know 
for sure, it’s very likely that you can find out. There may be a cell phone in 
your pocket; there may be a clock on the wall, desk, or computer screen; or 
maybe you're riding in a car that shows the time. Even if you don’t have a 
timepiece of some sort nearby, your body keeps its own beat. Humans have 
an internal clock that regulates the beating of our heart, the pace of our 
breathing, the discharge of chemicals within our bloodstream, and myriad 
other bodily functions. 

Managing your time as you study is a central aspect of academic success. 
But remember: The goal of time management is not to schedule every moment 
so we become pawns of a timetable that governs every waking moment of the 
day. Instead, the goal is to permit us to make informed choices about how we 
use our time. Rather than letting the day slip by, largely without our awareness, 
the time management procedures we'll discuss can make us better able to har- 
ness time for our own ends. 

We'll consider a number of steps to help you improve your time manage- 
ment skills. 


Set Your Priorities. To figure out the best use of your time, you need to deter- 
mine your priorities. Priorities are the tasks and activities you need and want to 
do, rank-ordered from most important to least important. There are no right or 
wrong priorities; maybe spending time on your studies is most important to 
you, or maybe your top priority is spending time with your family. Only you 
can decide. Furthermore, what's important to you now may be less of a priority 
to you next month, next year, or in five years. 

The best procedure is to start off by identifying priorities for an entire term. 
What do you need to accomplish? Don’t just choose obvious, general goals, 
such as “passing all my classes.” Instead, think in terms of specific, measurable 
activities, such as “studying 10 hours before each chemistry exam.” 


Identify Your Prime Time. Do you enthusiastically bound out of bed in the 
morning, ready to start the day and take on the world? Or is the alarm clock a 
hated and unwelcome sound that jars you out of pleasant slumber? Are you 
zombie-like by 10 at night, or are you a person who is just beginning to rev up 
at midnight? Each of us has his or her own style based on some inborn body 
clock. Being aware of the time or times of day when you can do your best work 
will help you plan and schedule your time most effectively. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Master the Moment. Here's what you'll need to organize your time: 


«+ A master calendar that shows all the weeks of the term on one page. It 
should include every week of the term and seven days per week. Using 
your class syllabi, write on the master calendar every assignment and test 
you will have, noting the date that it is due. Pencil in tentative assign- 
ments on the appropriate date. Also include on the master calendar 
important activities from your personal life, drawn from your list of priori- 
ties. And don’t forget to schedule some free time for yourself. 

e A weekly timetable, a master grid with the days of the week across the top 
and the hours, from 6:00 a.m. to midnight, along the side. Fill in the times 
of all your fixed, prescheduled activities—the times that your classes meet, 
when you have to be at work, the times you have to pick up your child at 
day care, and any other recurring appointments. Add assignment due 
dates, tests, and any other activities on the appropriate days of the week. 
Then pencil in blocks of time necessary to prepare for those events. 

e A daily to-do list. Your daily to-do list can be written on a small, portable 
calendar that includes a separate page for each day of the week, or you 
can maintain a calendar electronically in a cell phone or a device such as 
a Treo, iPhone, or Blackberry. List all the things that you intend to do dur- 
ing the next day and their priority. Start with the things you know you 
must do and that have fixed times, such as classes, work schedules, and 
appointments. Then add in the other things that you should accomplish, 
such as an hour of study for an upcoming test, work on research for an 
upcoming paper, or finishing up a lab report. Finally, list things that are a 
low priority but enjoyable, like a run or a walk. 























Control Your Time. If you follow the schedules that you've prepared, you've 
taken the most important steps in time management. However, our lives are 
filled with surprises: Things always seem to take longer than we’ve planned. A 
crisis occurs, buses are late, computers break down, kids get sick. 

The difference between effective time management and time management 
that doesn’t work lies in how well you deal with the inevitable surprises. There 
are several ways to take control of your days and permit yourself to 
follow your intended schedule: 


e Just say no. You don't have to agree to every request and 
every favor that others ask of you. 

+ Get away from it all. Go to the library. Lock yourself 
in your bedroom. Find an out-of-the-way unused 
classroom. Adopt a specific spot as your own, such 
as a corner desk in a secluded nook in the library. If 
you use it enough, your body and mind will auto- 
matically get into study mode as soon as you seat 
yourself at it. 


Seeeeer ewww Vo os 


xli 


xlii 








+ Enjoy the sounds of silence. Although many students insist 
they accomplish most while a television, radio, or CD is playing, 
scientific studies suggest otherwise—we are able to concentrate 
most when our environment is silent. Experiment and work in 
silence for a few days. You may find that you get more done in 
less time than you would in a more distracting environment. 

e Take an e-break. We may not control when communications 
arrive, but we can make the messages wait until we are ready to 
receive them. Take an e-break and shut down your communica- 
tion sources for some period of time. Phone calls can be stored 
on voice-mail systems; text messages, IMs, and e-mail can be 
saved on a phone or computer. They'll wait. 

e Expect the unexpected. You'll never be able to escape from 
unexpected interruptions and surprises that require your atten- 
tion. But by trying to anticipate them in advance and thinking 
about how you'll react to them, you'll be positioning yourself to 
react more effectively when they do occur. 


Read Your Textbook Effectively 





Reading a textbook is different from reading for pleasure. With textbooks, you 
have specific goals: understanding, learning, and ultimately recalling the infor- 
mation. There are several steps you can take to achieve these goals: 


Read the front matter. If you'll be using a text extensively throughout 
the term, start by reading the preface and/or introduction and scanning 
the table of contents—what publishers call the front matter. It is there 
that the author has a chance to explain, often more personally than else- 
where in the text, what he or she considers important. Knowing this will 
give you a sense of what to expect as you read. (Note: You're reading part 
of the front matter at this very moment!) 

Identify your personal objectives. Before you begin an assignment, 
think about what your specific objectives are. Will you be reading a text- 
book on which you'll be thoroughly tested? Or, will your reading provide 
background information for future learning that won't itself be tested? Is 
the material going to be useful to you personally? Your objectives for 
reading will help you determine which reading strategy to adopt and 
how much time you can devote to the reading assignment. You aren't 
expected to read everything with the same degree of intensity. You may 
feel comfortable skimming some material. You'll want to put in the maxi- 
mum effort into other material. 

Identify and use the advance organizers. The next step in reading a 
textbook is to become familiar with the advance organizers—outlines, 
overviews, section objectives, or other clues to the meaning and organi- 
zation of new material—provided in the material you are reading. Look at 





the start of every chapter in this book, which includes a chapter outline 

plus a set of questions at the end of the “Looking Ahead” section. You 

can also create your own advance organizers by skimming material to be 
read and sketching out the general outline of the material you'll be read- 
ing. These steps can help you recall material better after you've read it. 

Stay focused as you read. There are a million and one possible distrac- 

tions that can invade your thoughts as you read. Your job is to keep dis- 

tracting thoughts at bay and focus on the material you are supposed to 
be reading. Here are some things you can do to help yourself stay 
focused: 

+ Read in small bites. If you think it is going to take you 4 hours to 
read an entire chapter, break up the 4 hours into more manageable 
time periods. Promise yourself that you'll read for 1 hour in the after- 
noon, another hour in the evening, and the next 2 hours spaced out 
during the following day. 

e Take a break. Actually plan to take several short breaks to reward your- 
self while you're reading. During your break, do something enjoyable— 
eat a snack, watch a bit of a ball game on television, play a video game, 
or the like. Just try not to get drawn into your break activity to the 
point that it takes over your reading time. 

Highlight and take notes as you read. Highlighting and taking notes as 

you read a textbook are essential activities. Good annotations can help 

you learn and review the information prior to tests as well as help you to 
stay focused as you read. There are several things you can do to maxi- 
mize the effectiveness of your notes: 

e Rephrase key points. Make notes to yourself, in your own words, 
about what the author is trying to get across. Don’t just copy 
what’s been said. Think about the material, and rewrite it in words 
that are your own. The very act of writing engages 
an additional type of perception that involves the 
physical sense of moving a pen or pressing a 
keyboard. 

«+ Highlight or underline key points. Often the 
first or last sentence in a paragraph, or the first 
or last paragraph in a section, will present a 
key point. Before you highlight anything, 
though, read the whole paragraph through. 
Then you'll be sure that what you highlight 
is, in fact, the key information. You should 
find yourself highlighting only one or 
two sentences or phrases per page. /n 
highlighting and underlining, less is 
more. One guideline: No more than 10 
percent of the material should be 
highlighted or underlined. 

















xliii 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


e Use arrows, diagrams, outlines, tables, timelines, charts, and other 
visuals to help you understand and later recall what you are read- 
ing. If three examples are given for a specific point, number them. If a 
sequence of steps is presented, number each step. If a paragraph dis- 
cusses a situation in which an earlier point does not hold, link the 
original point to the exception by an arrow. Representing the material 
graphically will get you thinking about it in new and different ways. 
The act of creating visual annotations will not only help you to under- 
stand the material better, but will also ease its later recall. 

e Look up unfamiliar words. Even though you may be able to figure 
out the meaning of an unfamiliar word from its context, look up un- 
familiar words in a dictionary or online. You'll also find out what the 
word sounds like, which will be important if your instructor uses the 
word in class. 











Perhaps you know students who manage to write down nearly every- 
thing their instructors say in class. And perhaps you have thought to 
yourself, “If only | took such painstaking notes, I’d do much better in my 
classes.’ Contrary to what many students think, however, good notetaking 
does not mean writing down every word that an instructor utters. With 
notetaking, less is often more. Let's consider some of the basic principles 
of notetaking: 


- Identify the instructor’s—and your—goals for the course. On the 
first day of class, most instructors talk about their objectives for the 
course. Most review the information on the class syllabus, the writ- 
ten document that explains the assignments for the semester. The 
information you get during that first session and through the sylla- 
bus is critical. In addition to the instructor's goals, you should have 
your own. What is it you want to learn from the course? How will 
the information from the course help you to enhance your knowl- 
edge, improve yourself as a person, achieve your goals? 

e Complete assignments before coming to class. Your instructor 
enthusiastically describes the structure of the neuron, recounting 
excitedly how electrons flow across neurons, changing their electri- 
cal charge. One problem: You have only the vaguest idea what a 
neuron is. And the reason you don't know is that you haven't read 
the assignment. 

Chances are you have found yourself in this situation at least a few 
times, so you know firsthand that sinking feeling as you become more 
and more confused. The moral: Always go to class prepared. Instructors 
assume that their students have done what they've assigned, and their 
lectures are based on that assumption. 





xliv 





+ Choose a notebook that assists in notetaking. Loose-leaf notebooks 
are especially good for taking notes because they permit you to go back 
later and change the order of the pages or add additional material. 
Whatever kind of notebook you use, use only one side of the page for writ- 
ing; keep one side free of notes. There may be times that you'll want to 
spread out your notes in front of you, and it’s much easier if no material 
is written on the back of the pages. 

Walter Pauk devised what is sometimes called the Cornell Method 
of Notetaking. Using this method, draw a line down the left side of your 
notebook page, about 2⁄2 inches from the left-hand margin. Keep the 
notes you write in class to the right of the line. Indent major supporting 
details beneath each main idea, trying to use no more than one line for 
each item, and leave space between topics to add information. When it 
comes time to review your notes later, you'll be able to jot down a key- 
word, catch phrase, or major idea on the left side of the page (Pauk, 
2007). 

+ Listen for the key ideas. Not every sentence in a lecture is equally 
important. One of the most useful skills you can develop is separating 
the key ideas from supporting information. Good lecturers strive to make 
just a few main points. The rest of what they say consists of explanation, 
examples, and other supportive material that expand upon the key ideas. 
To distinguish the key ideas from their support, you need to be alert and 
always searching for the meta-message of your instructor's words—that is, 
the underlying main ideas that a speaker is seeking to convey. 

How can you discern the meta-message? One way is to listen for key 
words. Phrases like “you need to know... “the most important thing that 
must be considered... “there are four problems with this approach...,’ 
and—a big one—“this will be on the test. . ” should cause you to sit up 
and take notice. Also, if an instructor says the same thing in several ways, 
it’s a clear sign that the material being discussed is important. 

+ Use short, abbreviated phrases—not full sentences when taking notes. 
Forget everything you've ever heard about always writing in full sen- 
tences. In fact, it’s often useful to take notes in the form of an outline. An 
outline summarizes ideas in short phrases and indicates the relationship 
among concepts through the use of indentations. 

e Pay attention to what is written on the board or projected from over- 
heads and PowerPoint slides. Remember these tips: 

e Listening is more important than seeing. The information that your 
instructor projects on screen, while important, ultimately is less critical 
than what he or she is saying. Pay primary attention to the spoken 
word and secondary attention to the screen. 

+ Don’t copy everything that is on every slide. Instructors can present 
far more information on their slides than they would if they were writ- 
ing on a blackboard. Oftentimes there is so much information that it’s 


xlv 


xlvi 





impossible to copy it all down. Don't even try. Instead, concentrate on 
taking down the key points. 

+ Remember that key points on slides are . . . key points. The key 
points (often indicated by bullets) often relate to central concepts. Use 
these points to help organize your studying for tests, and don't be sur- 
prised if test questions directly assess the bulleted items on slides. 

+ Check to see if the presentation slides are available online. Some 
instructors make their class presentations available to their students on 
the Web, either before or after class time. If they do this before class, 
print them out and bring them to class. Then you can make notes on 
your copy, clarifying important points. If they are not available until 
after a class is over, you can still make good use of them when it 
comes time to study the material for tests. 

- Remember that presentation slides are not the same as good notes 
for a class. If you miss a class, don’t assume that getting a copy of the 
slides is sufficient. Studying the notes of a classmate who is a good 
notetaker will be far more beneficial than studying only the slides. 


Memorize Efficiently: 
Use Proven Strategies 
to Memorize New Materials 


Here's a key principle of effective memorization: Memorize what you need to 
memorize. Forget about the rest. 

The average textbook chapter has some 20,000 words. But, within those 
20,000 words, there may be only 30 to 40 specific concepts that you need to learn. 
And perhaps there are only 25 key words. Those are the pieces of information on 
which you should focus in your efforts to memorize. By extracting what is impor- 
tant from what is less crucial, you'll be able to limit the amount of the material 
that you need to recall. You'll be able to focus on what you need to remember. 

You have your choice of dozens of techniques of memorization. As we dis- 
cuss the options, keep in mind that no one strategy works by itself. Also, feel free 
to devise your own strategies or add those that have worked for you in the past. 





Rehearsal. Say it aloud: rehearsal. Think of this word in terms of its three syl- 
lables: re—hear—sal. If you're scratching your head about why you should do 
this, it’s to illustrate the point of rehearsal: to transfer material that you encoun- 
ter into long-term memory. 

To test if you’ve succeeded in transferring the word “rehearsal” into your 
memory, put down this book and go off for a few minutes. Do something 
entirely unrelated to reading this book. Have a snack, catch up on the latest 
sports scores on ESPN, or read the front page of a newspaper. If the word 
“rehearsal” popped into your head when you picked up this book again, you've 


|" 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


passed your first memory test—the word “rehearsal” has been transferred into 
your memory. 

Rehearsal is the key strategy in remembering information. If you don't 
rehearse material, it will never make it into your memory. Repeating the infor- 
mation, summarizing it, associating it with other memories, and above all think- 
ing about it when you first come across it will ensure that rehearsal will be 
effective in placing the material into your memory. 


Mnemonics. This odd word (pronounced with the “m” silent—“neh MON ix”) 
describes formal techniques used to make material more readily remembered. 
Mnemonics are the tricks-of-the-trade that professional memory experts use, 
and you too can use them to nail down the information you will need to recall 
for tests. 

Among the most common mnemonics are the following: 


e Acronyms. Acronyms are words or phrases formed by the first letters of a 
series of terms. The word “laser” is an acronym for “light amplification by 
stimulated emissions of radiation,’ and “radar” is an acronym for “radio 
detection and ranging.’ 

Acronyms can be a big help in remembering things. For example, Roy G. 
Biv is a favorite of physics students who must remember the colors of the 
spectrum (red, orange, yellow, green, blue, indigo, and violet.) The benefit of 
acronyms is that they help us to recall a complete list of steps or items. 

e Acrostics. Acrostics are sentences in which the first letters spell out some- 
thing that needs to be recalled. The benefits—as well as the drawbacks— 
of acrostics are similar to those of acronyms. 

e Rhymes and jingles. “Thirty days hath September, April, June, and 
November.’ If you know the rest of the rhyme, you're familiar with one of 
the most commonly used mnemonic jingles in the English language. 






















Use of Multiple Senses. The more senses you can involve when you're trying 
to learn new material, the better you'll be able to remember. Here’s why: Every 
time we encounter new information, all of our senses are potentially at work. 
Each piece of sensory information is stored in a separate location in the brain, 
and yet all the pieces are linked together in extraordinarily intricate ways. 
What this means is that when we seek to remember the details of a specific 
event, recalling a memory of one of the sensory experiences can trigger recall 
of the other types of memories. You can make use of the fact that memories 
are stored in multiple ways by applying the following techniques: 


-+ When you learn something, use your body. Don't sit pas- 
sively at your desk. Instead, move around. Stand up; sit down. 
Touch the page. Trace figures with your fingers. Talk to your- 
self. Think out loud. By involving every part of your body, you've 
increased the number of potential ways to trigger a relevant memory 
later, when you need to recall it. And when one memory is triggered, 
other related memories may come tumbling back. 


xlvii 


xlviii 





Draw and diagram the material. Structuring written material by graphi- 
cally grouping and connecting key ideas and themes is a powerful tech- 
nique. When we draw and diagram material, one of the things we're doing 
is expanding the modalities in which information can be stored in our 
minds. Other types of drawing can be useful in aiding later recall. Creating 
drawings, sketches, and even cartoons can help us remember better. 
Visualize. You already know that memory requires three basic steps: the 
initial recording of information, the storage of that information, and, ulti- 
mately, the retrieval of the stored information. Visualization is a technique 
by which images are formed to ensure that material is recalled. Don't 
stop at visualizing images just in your mind's eye. Actually drawing what 
you visualize will help you to remember the material even better. 
Visualization is effective because it serves several purposes. It helps make 
abstract ideas concrete; it engages multiple senses; it permits us to link 
different bits of information together; and it provides us with a context 
for storing information. 

Overlearning. Lasting learning doesn’t come until you have overlearned 
the material. Overlearning consists of studying and rehearsing material 
past the point of initial mastery. Through overlearning, recall becomes 
automatic. Rather than searching for a fact, going through mental contor- 
tions until perhaps the information surfaces, overlearning permits us to 
recall the information without even thinking about it. 





|" 


Test-Taking Strategies 


Preparing for tests is a long-term proposition. It’s not a matter of “giving your 
al 
aspect of the course. 


the night before the test. Instead, it’s a matter of giving your all to every 


Here are some guidelines that can help you do your best on tests: 


Know What You Are Preparing For. Determine as much as you can about the 
test before you begin to study for it. The more you know about a test before- 
hand, the more efficient your studying will be. 


To find out about an upcoming test, ask this question: 


Is the test called a “test; an “exam,” a “quiz,’ or something else? The names 

imply different things. 

+ Essay: Requires a fairly extended, on-the-spot composition about 
some topic. Examples include questions that call on you to describe a 
person, process, or event, or those that ask you to compare or con- 
trast two separate sets of material. 

+ Multiple-choice: Usually contains a question or statement, followed 
by a number of possible answers (usually 4 or 5 of them). You are sup- 
posed to choose the best response from the choices offered. 

+ True-false: Presents statements about a topic that are either accurate 
or inaccurate. You are to indicate whether each statement is accurate 
(true) or inaccurate (false). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Matching: Presents two lists of related information, arranged in col- 
umn form. Typically, you are asked to pair up the items that go 
together (for example, a scientific term and its definition, or a writer 
and the title of a book he or she wrote). 

Short-answer: Requires brief responses (usually a few sentences at 
most) in a kind of mini-essay. 

Fill-in: Requires you to add one or more missing words to a sentence 
or series of sentences. 


Match Test Preparation to Question Types. Each kind of test question 
requires a somewhat different style of preparation. 


Essay questions. Essay tests focus on the big picture—ways in which the 
various pieces of information being tested fit together. You'll need to 
know not just a series of facts, but also the connections between them, 
and you will have to be able to discuss these ideas in an organized and 
logical way. The best approach to studying for an essay test involves four 
steps: 


1. 


Multiple-choice, true-false, and matching questions. While the 
focus of review for essay questions should be on major issues and 
controversies, studying for multiple-choice, true—-false, and match- 
ing questions requires more attention to the details. Almost any- 
thing is fair game for multiple-choice, true-false, and matching 
questions, so you can't afford to overlook anything when 


Carefully reread your class notes and any notes you've made on 
assigned readings that will be covered on the upcoming exam. Also 
go through the readings themselves, reviewing underlined or high- 
lighted material and marginal notes. 

Think of likely exam questions. For example, use the key words, 
phrases, concepts, and questions that come up in your class notes or 
in your text. Some instructors give out lists of possible essay topics; if 
yours does, focus on this list, but don’t ignore other possibilities. 
Without looking at your notes or your readings, answer each potential 
essay question—aloud. Don't feel embarrassed about doing this. 
Talking aloud is often more useful than answering the question in 
your head. You can also write down the main points that any answer 
should cover. (Don’t write out complete answers to the questions 
unless your instructor tells you in advance exactly what is going to be 
on the test. Your time is probably better spent learning the material 
than rehearsing precisely formulated responses.) 

After you've answered the questions, check yourself by looking at the 
notes and readings once again. If you feel confident that you've 
answered specific questions adequately, check them off. You can go 
back later for a quick review. But if there are questions that you had 
trouble with, review that material immediately. Then repeat the third 
step above, answering the questions again. 


studying. It’s a good idea to write down important facts on 











xlix 





index cards: They're portable and available all the time, and the act of 
creating them helps drive the material into your memory. Furthermore, 
you can shuffle them and test yourself repeatedly until you’ve mastered 
the material. 

+ Short-answer and fill-in questions. Short-answer and fill-in questions 
are similar to essays in that they require you to recall key pieces of infor- 
mation rather than finding it on the page in front of you, as is the case 
with multiple-choice, true—-false, and matching questions. However, short- 
answer and fill-in questions typically don’t demand that you integrate or 
compare different types of information. Consequently, the focus of your 
study should be on the recall of specific, detailed information. 


Test Yourself. Once you feel you’ve mastered the material, test yourself on it. 
There are several ways to do this. Often textbooks are accompanied by web- 
sites that offer automatically scored practice tests and quizzes. (Understanding 
Psychology does: go to www.mhhe.com/feldman/up10 to try one!) You can also 
create a test for yourself, in writing, making its form as close as possible to what 
you expect the actual test to be. For instance, if your instructor has told you 
the classroom test will be primarily made up of short-answer questions, your 
test should reflect that. 

You might also construct a test and administer it to a classmate or a mem- 
ber of your study group. In turn, you could take a test that someone else has 
constructed. Constructing and taking practice tests are excellent ways of study- 
ing the material and cementing it into memory. 


Deal with Test Anxiety. What does the anticipation of a test do to you? Do 
you feel shaky? Is there a knot in your stomach? Do you grit your teeth? Test 
anxiety is a temporary condition characterized by fears and concerns about 
test-taking. Almost everyone experiences it to some degree, although for some 
people it’s more of a problem than for others. You'll never eliminate test anxiety 
completely, nor do you want to. A little bit of nervousness can energize us, 
making us more attentive and vigilant. Like any competitive event, testing can 
motivate us to do our best. 

On the other hand, for some students, anxiety can spiral into the kind of 
paralyzing fear that makes their minds go blank. There are several ways to keep 
this from happening to you: 

1. Prepare thoroughly. The more you prepare, the less test anxiety you'll 
feel. Good preparation can give you a sense of control and mastery, 
and it will prevent test anxiety from overwhelming you. 

2. Take a realistic view of the test. Remember that your future success 
does not hinge on your performance on any single exam. Think of the 
big picture: Put the task ahead in context, and remind yourself of all 
the hurdles you've passed so far. 

3. Learn relaxation techniques. These techniques are covered in the text's 
chapter on health psychology, but the basic process is straightforward: 
Breathe evenly, gently inhaling and exhaling. Focus your mind on a 





pleasant, relaxing scene such as a beautiful forest or a peaceful farm 
or on a restful sound such as that of ocean waves breaking on the 
beach. 

4. Visualize success. Think of an image of your instructor handing back 
your test marked with a big “A” Or imagine your instructor congratu- 
lating you on your fine performance the day after the test. Positive 
visualizations that highlight your potential success can help replace 
images of failure that may fuel test anxiety. 

What if these strategies don't work? If your test anxiety is so great that it’s 
getting in the way of your success, make use of your college's resources. Most 
provide a learning resource center or a counseling center that can provide you 
with personalized help. 


Form a Study Group. Study groups are small, informal groups of students who 
work together to learn course material and study for a test. Forming such a 
group can be an excellent way to prepare for any kind of test. Some study 
groups are formed for particular tests, while others meet consistently through- 
out the term. The typical study group meets a week or two before a test and 
plans a strategy for studying. Members share their understanding of what will 
be on the test, based on what an instructor has said in class and on their review 
of notes and text material. Together, they develop a list of review questions to 
guide their individual study. The group then breaks up, and the members study 
on their own. 

A few days before the test, members of the study group meet again. They 
discuss answers to review questions, go over the material, and share any new 
insights they may have about the upcoming test. They may also quiz one 
another about the material to identify any weaknesses or gaps in their knowledge. 

Study groups can be extremely powerful tools because they help accom- 
plish several things: 


+ They help members organize and structure the material to approach their 
studying in a systematic and logical way. 

+ They allow students to share different perspectives on the material. 

e They make it more likely that students will not overlook any potentially 
important information. 

+ They force members to rethink the course material, explaining it in words 
that other group members will understand. As we will discuss in Chapter 
14, this helps both understanding and recall of the information when it is 
needed on the test. 

+ Finally, they help motivate members to do their best. When you're part of 
a study group, you’re no longer working just for yourself; your studying 
also benefits the other study group members. Not wanting to let down 
your classmates in a study group may encourage you to put in your best 
effort. 





Some Final Comments 





We have discussed numerous techniques for increasing your study, classroom, 
and test effectiveness. But you need not feel tied to a specific strategy. You 
might want to combine other elements to create your own study system. 
Additional learning tips and strategies for critical thinking are presented 
throughout Understanding Psychology. 

Whatever learning strategies you use, you will maximize your understand- 
ing of the material in this book and master techniques that will help you learn 
and think critically in all of your academic endeavors. More important, you will 
optimize your understanding of the field of psychology. It is worth the effort: 
The excitement, challenges, and promise that psychology holds for you are 
significant. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Understanding 
Psychology 








A 
T 


LEAR Tr D a 
| A Pp T -H >) 1 
| a ce FY L 


Introduction to Psychology 














www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Key Concepts for Chapter 1 


MODULE 1 








What is the science of psychology? @ Psychologists at Work 


The Subfields of Psychology: 


What are the major specialties in the field of Peyehologrs Family Tee 


psychology? @ Where do psychologists work? Working at Psychology 


PsychWork: Licensed Social Worker 


MODULE 2 





What are the origins of psychology? @ What are A Science Evolves: The Past, 


the Present, and the Future 


the major approaches in contemporary The Rooisor Psychology 


psychology? Today's Perspectives 
Applying Psychology in the 
21st Century: Psychology Matters 





MODULE 3 





What are psychology’s key issues and Psychology’s Key Issues 


ies? @ What is the f f and Controversies 
E OUAEDIES S alls the TUTE Q Exploring Diversity: Understanding How 


psychology likely to hold? ela and Race Influence 


Psychology's Future 

Becoming an Informed Consumer of 
Psychology: Thinking Critically About 
Psychology: Distinguishing Legitimate 
Psychology from Pseudo-Psychology 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Prologue Deepwater Disaster 





One of the greatest environmental 
accidents in history began with a 
fire and explosion on an oil rig in 
the Gulf of Mexico. Eleven men 
were killed immediately, and 
dozens were injured. But the 
aftermath will extend for 
j decades. 
~N When the Deepwater 
Horizon oil rig blew up in the 
spring of 201 0, tens of thousands 
of barrels of oil escaped from pipes 

















that had been drilled deep into the ocean floor. Scientists debated 
for days about how to stop the uncontrolled flow of oil. After a 
number of failed attempts to stop the oil, engineers finally placed 
a containment cap over the cascading oil, and relief wells were 
drilled that eventually stopped the gushing oil. 

But the damage had been done. Large expanses of water 
were covered with oil, and there were plumes of oil underneath 
the seas. Beaches and fragile marshlands along the coast were 
ruined, and animals, coated with oil, died above and below the 
waters. Tens of thousands of people lost their livelihoods. It will 
be decades before life returns to normal. 





Although the oil spill in the Gulf originated as an environmental 
accident, it also gave rise to a host of important psychological 
issues. Consider, for example, these questions: 


+ What biological reactions did the workers experience as they 
responded to the initial explosion on the rig? 

e How did scientists and engineers devise solutions to stop 
the gushing oil? 


+ What would be the long-term effects of the health of individ- 
uals whose livelihoods were wiped out? 


e How did the anger and frustration that people experienced 
as they watched the disaster unfold affect their behavior? 

e How did the families of those killed in the disaster react and 
cope with their grief? 

+ What explains why many people came to the Gulf region to 
help save oil-soaked animals and clean up the beaches? 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 1 


Psychology is the scientific study of behavior and mental processes. The simplicity 
of this definition is in some ways deceiving, concealing ongoing debates about how 
broad the scope of psychology should be. Should psychologists limit themselves to 
the study of outward, observable behavior? Is it possible to study thinking scientifi- 
cally? Should the field encompass the study of such diverse topics as physical and 
mental health, perception, dreaming, and motivation? Is it appropriate to focus solely 
on human behavior, or should the behavior of other species be included? 

Most psychologists would argue that the field should be receptive to a variety of 
viewpoints and approaches. Consequently, the phrase behavior and mental processes in 
the definition of psychology must be understood to mean many things: It encompasses 
not just what people do but also their thoughts, emotions, perceptions, reasoning pro- 
cesses, memories, and even the biological activities that maintain bodily functioning. 

Psychologists try to describe, predict, and explain human behavior and mental 
processes, as well as helping to change and improve the lives of people and the world 
in which they live. They use scientific methods to find answers that are far more 
valid and legitimate than those resulting from intuition and speculation, which are 
often inaccurate (see Figure 1). 


- f= f—t+_#+_f++_-#-/ | 


Psychological Truths? 
To test your knowledge of psychology, try answering the following questions: 


1. Infants love their mothers primarily because their mothers fulfill their basic biological 
needs, such as providing food. True or false? 

2. Geniuses generally have poor social adjustment. True or false? 

3. The best way to ensure that a desired behavior will continue after training is completed is 
to reward that behavior every single time it occurs during training rather than rewarding it 
only periodically. True or false? 

4. People with schizophrenia have at least two distinct personalities. True or false? 

5. Parents should do everything they can to ensure their children have high self-esteem 
and a strong sense that they are highly competent. True or false? 

6. Children’s IQ scores have little to do with how well they do in school. 
True or false? 

7. Frequent masturbation can lead to mental illness. True or false? 

8. Once people reach old age, their leisure activities change radically. 
True or false? 

9. Most people would refuse to give painful electric shocks to other people. 
True or false? 

10. People who talk about suicide are unlikely to actually try to kill themselves. 
True or false? 


Scoring: The truth about each of these items: They are all false. Based on psychological research, each 
of these “facts” has been proven untrue. You will learn the reasons why as we explore what psycholo- 
gists have discovered about human behavior. 


What is the science of 
psychology? 


What are the major specialties 
in the field of psychology? 


Where do psychologists work? 


The scientific study of 
behavior and mental processes. 


The scientific method is the 
basis of all psychological research and is 
used to find valid answers. Test your 
knowledge of psychology by 
answering these questions. (Source: 
Adapted from Lamal, 1979.) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


6 Chapter 1 Introduction to Psychology 


ly Alert 


The different subfields 
of psychology allow 
psychologists to explain 
the same behavior in 
multiple ways. Review 
Figure 2 for a summary of the 
subfields. 


The questions in Figure 1 provide just a hint of the topics that we will encounter 
in the study of psychology. Our discussions will take us through the range of what 
is known about behavior and mental processes. 


The Subfields of Psychology: 
Psychology’s Family Tree 


As the study of psychology has grown, it has given rise to a number of subfields 
(described in Figure 2). The subfields of psychology can be likened to an extended 
family, with assorted nieces and nephews, aunts and uncles, and cousins who, 
although they may not interact on a day-to-day basis, are related to one another, 
because they share a common goal: understanding behavior. One way to identify 
the key subfields is to look at some of the basic questions about behavior that 
they address. 





WHAT ARE THE BIOLOGICAL FOUNDATIONS OF BEHAVIOR? 


In the most fundamental sense, people are biological organisms. Behavioral neuro- 
science is the subfield of psychology that mainly examines how the brain and the 
nervous system—but other biological processes as well—determine behavior. Thus, 
neuroscientists consider how our bodies influence our behavior. For example, they 
may examine the link between specific sites in the brain and the muscular tremors 
of people affected by Parkinson’s disease or attempt to determine how our emotions 
are related to physical sensations. Behavioral neuroscientists might want to know 
what physiological changes occurred as oil from the damaged well began coming 
ashore (Willis, 2008). 


HOW DO PEOPLE SENSE, PERCEIVE, LEARN, 
AND THINK ABOUT THE WORLD? 


If you have ever wondered why you are susceptible to optical illusions, how your 
body registers pain, or how to make the most of your study time, an experimental 
psychologist can answer your questions. Experimental psychology is the branch of psy- 
chology that studies the processes of sensing, perceiving, learning, and thinking 
about the world. (The term experimental psychologist is somewhat misleading: Psy- 
chologists in every specialty area use experimental techniques.) 

Several subspecialties of experimental psychology have become specialties 
in their own right. One is cognitive psychology, which focuses on higher mental 
processes, including thinking, memory, reasoning, problem solving, judging, 
decision making, and language. For example, a cognitive psychologist might be 
interested in what the survivors of the downed flight remembered about their 
experience. 


WHAT ARE THE SOURCES OF CHANGE AND STABILITY 
IN BEHAVIOR ACROSS THE LIFE SPAN? 


A baby producing her first smile . . . taking his first step . . . saying its first word. These 
universal milestones in development are also singularly special and unique for each 
person. Developmental psychology studies how people grow and change from the 
moment of conception through death. Personality psychology focuses on the consis- 
tency in people’s behavior over time and the traits that differentiate one person 
from another. 





Subfield 
Behavioral genetics 
Behavioral neuroscience 


Clinical psychology 


Clinical neuropsychology 


Cognitive psychology 


Counseling psychology 


Cross-cultural psychology 


Developmental psychology 


Educational psychology 


Environmental psychology 
Evolutionary psychology 
Experimental psychology 
Forensic psychology 
Health psychology 
ndustrial/organizational 
psychology 


Personality psychology 


Program evaluation 





Psychology of women 


School psychology 


Social psychology 


Sport psychology 


FIGURE 2 The major subfields of psychology. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 1 Psychologists at Work 


Description 
Behavioral genetics studies the inheritance of traits related to behavior 
Behavioral neuroscience examines the biological basis of behavior 


Clinical psychology deals with the study, diagnosis, and treatment of 
psychological disorders. 


Clinical neuropsychology unites the areas of biopsychology and clinical 
psychology, focusing on the relationship between biological factors 
and psychological disorders. 


Cognitive psychology focuses on the study of higher mental processes. 


Counseling psychology focuses primarily on educational, social, and 
career adjustment problems. 


Cross-cultural psychology investigates the similarities and differences in 
psychological functioning in and across various cultures and ethnic 
groups. 

Developmental psychology examines how people grow and change 
from the moment of conception through death. 


Educational psychology is concerned with teaching and learning 
processes, such as the relationship between motivation and school 
performance. 


Environmental psychology considers the relationship between people 
and their physical environment. 


Evolutionary psychology considers how behavior is influenced by our 
genetic inheritance from our ancestors. 


Experimental psychology studies the processes of sensing, perceiving, 
learning, and thinking about the world. 


Forensic psychology focuses on legal issues, such as determining the 
accuracy of witness memories. 


Health psychology explores the relationship between psychological 
factors and physical ailments or disease. 


Industrial/organizational psychology is concerned with the psychology of 
the workplace. 


Personality psychology focuses on the consistency in people's behavior 
over time and the traits that differentiate one person from another. 


Program evaluation focuses on assessing large-scale programs, such as 
the Head Start preschool program, to determine whether they are 
effective in meeting their goals. 


Psychology of women focuses on issues such as discrimination against 
women and the causes of violence against women. 


School psychology is devoted to counseling children in elementary and 
secondary schools who have academic or emotional problems. 


Social psychology is the study of how people's thoughts, feelings, and 
actions are affected by others. 


Sport psychology applies psychology to athletic activity and exercise. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 1 Introduction to Psychology 


HOW DO PSYCHOLOGICAL FACTORS 
AFFECT PHYSICAL AND MENTAL HEALTH? 


Frequent depression, stress, and fears that prevent people from carrying out their 
normal activities are topics that would interest a health psychologist, a clinical psy- 
chologist, and a counseling psychologist. Health psychology explores the relationship 
between psychological factors and physical ailments or disease. For example, health 
psychologists are interested in assessing how long-term stress (a psychological factor) 
can affect physical health and in identifying ways to promote behavior that brings 
about good health (Belar, 2008; Yardley & Moss-Morris, 2009). 

Clinical psychology deals with the study, diagnosis, and treatment of psychologi- 
cal disorders. Clinical psychologists are trained to diagnose and treat problems that 
range from the crises of everyday life, such as unhappiness over the breakup of a 
relationship, to more extreme conditions, such as profound, lingering depression. 
Some clinical psychologists also research and investigate issues that vary from iden- 
tifying the early signs of psychological disturbance to studying the relationship 
between family communication patterns and psychological disorders. 

Like clinical psychologists, counseling psychologists deal with people’s psycho- 
logical problems, but the problems they deal with are more specific. Counseling psy- 
chology focuses primarily on educational, social, and career adjustment problems. 
Almost every college has a center staffed with counseling psychologists. This is 
where students can get advice on the kinds of jobs they might be best suited for, on 
methods of studying effectively, and on strategies for resolving everyday difficulties, 
such as problems with roommates and concerns about a specific professor’s grading 
practices. Many large business organizations also employ counseling psychologists 
to help employees with work-related problems. 


HOW DO OUR SOCIAL NETWORKS AFFECT BEHAVIOR? 


Our complex networks of social interrelationships are the focus for many subfields 
of psychology. For example, social psychology is the study of how people’s thoughts, 
feelings, and actions are affected by others. Social psychologists concentrate on such 
diverse topics as human aggression, liking and loving, persuasion, and conformity. 

Cross-cultural psychology investigates the similarities and differences in psycho- 
logical functioning in and across various cultures and ethnic groups. For example, 
cross-cultural psychologists examine how cultures differ in their use of punishment 
during child rearing. 


EXPANDING PSYCHOLOGY’S FRONTIERS 


The boundaries of the science of psychology are constantly growing. Three newer 
members of the field’s family tree—evolutionary psychology, behavioral genetics, 
and clinical neuropsychology—have sparked particular excitement, and debate, 
within psychology. 


Evolutionary Psychology Evolutionary psychology considers how behavior is influ- 
enced by our genetic inheritance from our ancestors. The evolutionary approach 
suggests that the chemical coding of information in our cells not only determines 
traits such as hair color and race but also holds the key to understanding a broad 
variety of behaviors that helped our ancestors survive and reproduce. 

Evolutionary psychology stems from Charles Darwin’s arguments in his ground- 
breaking 1859 book, On the Origin of Species. Darwin suggested that a process of 
natural selection leads to the survival of the fittest and the development of traits that 
enable a species to adapt to its environment. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Evolutionary psychologists take Darwin’s arguments a step further. They argue 
that our genetic inheritance determines not only physical traits such as skin and eye 
color but certain personality traits and social behaviors as well. For example, evolu- 
tionary psychologists suggest that behavior such as shyness, jealousy, and cross- 
cultural similarities in qualities desired in potential mates are at least partially 
determined by genetics, presumably because such behavior helped increase the sur- 
vival rate of humans’ ancient relatives (Buss, 2003; Sefcek, Brumbach, & Vasquez, 2007). 

Although they are increasingly popular, evolutionary explanations of behavior 
have stirred controversy. By suggesting that many significant behaviors unfold auto- 
matically, because they are wired into the human species, evolutionary approaches 
minimize the role of environmental and social forces. Still, the evolutionary approach 
has stimulated a significant amount of research on how our biological inheritance 
influences our traits and behaviors (Begley, 2005; Buss, 2004; Neher, 2006). 


Behavioral Genetics Another rapidly growing area in psychology focuses on the 
biological mechanisms, such as genes and chromosomes, that enable inherited behav- 
ior to unfold. Behavioral genetics seeks to understand how we might inherit certain 
behavioral traits and how the environment influences whether we actually display 
such traits (Bjorklund & Ellis, 2005; Moffitt & Caspi, 2007; Rende, 2007). 


Clinical Neuropsychology Clinical neuropsychology unites the areas of neuroscience 
and clinical psychology: It focuses on the origin of psychological disorders in bio- 
logical factors. Building on advances in our understanding of the structure and chem- 
istry of the brain, this specialty has already led to promising new treatments for 
psychological disorders as well as debates over the use of medication to control 
behavior (Boake, 2008). 





Working at Psychology 


Help Wanted: Assistant professor at a small liberal arts college. Teach undergraduate 
courses in introductory psychology and courses in specialty areas of cognitive psychology, 
perception, and learning. Strong commitment to quality teaching, as well as evidence of 
scholarship and research productivity, necessary. 


Help Wanted: Industrial-organizational consulting psychologist. International firm seeks 
psychologists for full-time career positions as consultants to management. Candidates 
must have the ability to establish a rapport with senior business executives and help 
them find innovative and practical solutions to problems concerning people and 
organizations. 


Help Wanted: Clinical psychologist. PhD, internship experience, and license required. 
Comprehensive clinic seeks psychologist to work with children and adults providing 
individual and group therapy, psychological evaluations, crisis intervention, and 
development of behavior treatment plans on multidisciplinary team. 


As these job ads suggest, psychologists are employed in a variety of settings. Many 
doctoral-level psychologists are employed by institutions of higher learning (univer- 
sities and colleges) or are self-employed, usually working as private practitioners 
treating clients (see Figure 3). Other work sites include hospitals, clinics, mental 
health centers, counseling centers, government human-services organizations, busi- 
nesses, schools, and even prisons. Psychologists are employed in the military, work- 
ing with soldiers, veterans, and their families, and they work for the federal 
government Department of Homeland Security, fighting terrorism (American Psy- 
chological Association, 2007; DeAngelis & Monahan, 2008). 


Module 1 Psychologists at Work 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


10 Chapter 1 Introduction to Psychology 





Position: Social Worker 


PsychWork 
LICENSED SOCIAL WORKER 


Name: Christin Poirier, LICSW 


Education: BA in Psychology, Stonehill College; 
MA in Social Work, University of New Hampshire 


Business, government, 


or other settings, 13.2% 
Managed care 


settings, 4.3% 











Colleges, 
universities, and 
other academic 


Other human settings, 30.6% 


services, 7.0% 


Hospitals, 7.7% 


School 
districts, 3.9% 


Private 
practice, 31.4% 


FIGURE 3 The breakdown of where U.S. psychologists (who have a PhD or PsyD degree) 
work. Why do you think so many psychologists work in college and university settings? 
(Source: American Psychological Association, 2007.) 


Most psychologists, though, work in academic settings, allowing them to com- 
bine the three major roles played by psychologists in society: teacher, scientist, and 
clinical practitioner. Many psychology professors are also actively involved in 
research or in serving clients. Whatever the particular job site, however, psychol- 
ogists share a commitment to improving individual lives as well as society in 
general. 

Keep in mind that many professionals from a variety of professions use the find- 
ings of psychologists. As you can see in each PsychWork box here and throughout 
the text, we focus on how a non-psychologist uses psychology. 


For Christin Poirier, psychology is central to her occupation as 
a social worker, a field dedicated to enhancing the well-being 
of individuals, families, groups, and communities. As a social 
worker, Poirier works at a community mental health center 
where she helps children and adolescents who are experienc- 
ing emotional or behavioral difficulties or both. Says Poirier, 
“The strategies I employ in counseling sessions are derived 
from basic psychological concepts and theories. In addition, in 
order to know what strategies are age-appropriate for a par- 
ticular client, I need to consider their stage of psychological 
development. Finally, it is necessary to consider how culture and ethnicity affect 
clients, so I incorporate these aspects into my clients’ treatment plans.” 





PSYCHOLOGISTS: A PORTRAIT 


Although there is no “average” psychologist in terms of personal characteristics, we 
can draw a statistical portrait of the field. There are close to 300,000 psychologists 
working today in the United States, but they are outnumbered by psychologists in 
other countries. Europe has more than 290,000 psychologists, and in Brazil alone 
there are 140,000 licensed psychologists. Although most research is conducted in the 
United States, psychologists in other countries are increasingly influential in adding 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


to the knowledge base and practices of psychology (Nelson, 2007; Peiro & Lunt, 2003; 
Stevens & Gielen, 2007). 

In the United States, women outnumber men in the field, a big change from 
earlier years when women faced bias and were actively discouraged from becoming 
psychologists. Today, around three-fourths of new psychology doctorate degrees are 
earned by women. There is an active debate about whether, and how, to seek balance 
in the percentage of men and women in the field (Cynkar, 2007; Frincke & Pate, 2004). 

The vast majority of psychologists in the United States are white, limiting the 
diversity of the field. Only 6% of all psychologists are members of racial minority 
groups. Although the number of minority individuals entering the field is higher than 
a decade ago—around 20% of new master’s degrees and 16% of new doctorate degrees 
are awarded to people of color—the numbers have not kept up with the dramatic 
growth of the minority population at large (Hoffer et al., 2005; Maton et al., 2006). 

The underrepresentation of racial and ethnic minorities among psychologists is 
significant for several reasons. First, the field of psychology is diminished by a lack 
of the diverse perspectives and talents that minority-group members can provide. 
Furthermore, minority-group psychologists serve as role models for members of 
minority communities, and their underrepresentation in the profession might deter 
other minority-group members from entering the field. Finally, because members of 
minority groups often prefer to receive psychological therapy from treatment provid- 
ers of their own race or ethnic group, the rarity of minority psychologists can dis- 
courage some members of minority groups from seeking treatment (Bernal et al., 
2002; Bryant et al., 2005; Jenkins et al., 2003). 


THE EDUCATION OF A PSYCHOLOGIST 


How do people become psychologists? The most common route is a long one. Most 
psychologists have a doctorate, either a PhD (doctor of philosophy) or, less frequently, 
a PsyD (doctor of psychology). The PhD is a research degree that requires a dissertation 
based on an original investigation. The PsyD is obtained by psychologists who wish to 
focus on the treatment of psychological disorders. (Psychologists are distinct from psy- 
chiatrists, who have a medical degree and specialize in the diagnosis and treatment of 
psychological disorders, often using treatments that involve the prescription of drugs.) 

Both the PhD and the PsyD typically take four or five years of work past the 
bachelor’s level. Some fields of psychology involve education beyond the doctorate. 
For instance, doctoral-level clinical psychologists, who deal with people with psy- 
chological disorders, typically spend an additional year doing an internship. 

About a third of people working in the field of psychology have a master’s 
degree as their highest degree, which they earn after two or three years of graduate 
work. These psychologists teach, provide therapy, conduct research, or work in 
specialized programs dealing with drug abuse or crisis intervention. Some work in 
universities, government, and business, collecting and analyzing data. 


CAREERS FOR PSYCHOLOGY MAJORS 


Although some psychology majors head for graduate school in psychology or an unre- 
lated field, the majority join the workforce immediately after graduation. Most report 
that the jobs they take after graduation are related to their psychology background. 

An undergraduate major in psychology provides excellent preparation for a vari- 
ety of occupations. Because undergraduates who specialize in psychology develop 
good analytical skills, are trained to think critically, and are able to synthesize and 
evaluate information well, employers in business, industry, and the government 
value their preparation (Kuther, 2003). 

The most common areas of employment for psychology majors are in the social 
services, including working as an administrator, serving as a counselor, and providing 


Module 1 Psychologists at Work 11 







y Alert 


Be sure you can differentiate 
between a PhD (doctor of 
philosophy) and a PsyD 
(doctor of psychology), as 
well as between psycholo- 
gists and psychiatrists. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


12 Chapter 1 Introduction to Psychology 


Positions Obtained by Psychology Majors 





Business Field 


Administrative assistant 

Affirmative action officer 

Advertising trainee 

Benefits manager 

Claims specialist 

Community relations officer 

Customer relations 

Data management 

Employee recruitment 

Employee counselor 

Human resources coordinator/ 
manager/specialist 

Labor relations manager/specialist 

Loan officer 

Management trainee 

Marketing 

Personnel manager/officer 

Product and services research 

Programs/events coordination 

Public relations 

Retail sales management 

Sales representative 

Special features writing/reporting 


Staff training and development 


Trainer/training office 


Education/ 
Academic Field 


Administration 
Child-care provider 
Child-care worker/ 
supervisor 
Data management 
Laboratory assistant 
Parent/family education 
Preschool teacher 
Public opinion surveyor 
Research assistant 
Teaching assistant 


Social Field 


Activities coordinator 
Behavioral specialist 
Career counselor 

Case worker 

Child protection worker 
Clinical coordinator 
Community outreach worker 
Corrections officer 
Counselor assistant 

Crisis intervention counselor 
Employment counselor 
Group home attendant 
Occupational therapist 
Probation officer 

Program manager 
Rehabilitation counselor 
Residence counselor 
Mental health assistant 
Social service assistant 
Social worker 

Substance abuse counselor 
Youth counselor 


FIGURE 4 Although many psychology majors pursue employment in social services, a 
background in psychology can prepare one for many professions outside the social services 
field. What is it about the science and art of psychology that makes it such a versatile field? 
(Source: From Tara L. Kuther, The Psychology Major's Handbook, 1st ed., p. 114. © 2003 Wadsworth, a part 
of Cengage Learning, Inc. Reproduced by permission. www.cengage.com/permissions.) 


direct care. Some 20% of recipients of bachelor’s degrees in psychology work in the 
social services or in some other form of public affairs. In addition, psychology majors 
often enter the fields of education or business or work for federal, state, and local 
governments (see Figure 4; American Psychological Association, 2000; Murray, 2002). 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


RECAP 


What is the science of psychology? 
e Psychology is the scientific study of behavior and mental 


processes, encompassing not just what people do but 
also their biological activities, feelings, perceptions, 
memory, reasoning, and thoughts. (p. 5) 


What are the major specialties in the field of psychology? 
e Behavioral neuroscientists focus on the biological basis 


of behavior, and experimental psychologists study the 
processes of sensing, perceiving, learning, and thinking 
about the world. (p. 6) 

Cognitive psychology, an outgrowth of experimental 
psychology, studies higher mental processes, including 
memory, knowing, thinking, reasoning, problem solving, 
judging, decision making, and language. (p. 6) 
Developmental psychologists study how people grow 
and change throughout the life span. (p. 6) 

Personality psychologists consider the consistency and 
change in an individual’s behavior, as well as the indi- 
vidual differences that distinguish one person’s behavior 


Module 1 Psychologists at Work 13 


Health psychologists study psychological factors that af- 
fect physical disease, whereas clinical psychologists con- 
sider the study, diagnosis, and treatment of abnormal 
behavior. Counseling psychologists focus on educa- 
tional, social, and career adjustment problems. (p. 8) 
Social psychology is the study of how people’s thoughts, 
feelings, and actions are affected by others. (p. 8) 
Cross-cultural psychology examines the similarities and 
differences in psychological functioning among various 
cultures. (p. 8) 

Other increasingly important fields are evolutionary 
psychology, behavioral genetics, and clinical 
neuropsychology. (p. 8) 


Where do psychologists work? 


Psychologists are employed in a variety of settings. 
Although the primary sites of employment are private 
practice and colleges, many psychologists are found in 
hospitals, clinics, community mental health centers, and 
counseling centers. (p. 9) 


from another’s. (p. 6) 


EVALUATE 


Match each subfield of psychology with the issues or questions posed below. 


a. 
b. 
C. 
d. 
e. 
f 
g- 
h. 
ls 
j. 
k. 
1. 


. Joan, a college freshman, is worried about her grades. She needs to learn better 


organizational skills and study habits to cope with the demands of college. 

At what age do children generally begin to acquire an emotional attachment to their fathers? 
It is thought that pornographic films that depict violence against women may prompt 
What chemicals are released in the human body as a result of a stressful event? What are 


Luis is unique in his manner of responding to crisis situations, with an even temperament 


The teachers of 8-year-old Jack are concerned that he has recently begun to withdraw 


behavioral neuroscience 1 

experimental psychology 

cognitive psychology Pe 

developmental psychology 3. 

personality psychology aggressive behavior in some men. 
health psychology 4. 

clinical psychology their effects on behavior? 
counseling psychology 5. 

educational psychology and a positive outlook. 

school psychology 6. 

social psychology socially and to show little interest in schoolwork. 
industrial psychology Te 


Janetta’s job is demanding and stressful. She wonders if her lifestyle is making her more 
prone to certain illnesses, such as cancer and heart disease. 


. A psychologist is intrigued by the fact that some people are much more sensitive to painful 


stimuli than others are. 


. Astrong fear of crowds leads a young man to seek treatment for his problem. 
. What mental strategies are involved in solving complex word problems? 
. What teaching methods most effectively motivate elementary school students to success- 


fully accomplish academic tasks? 


. Jessica is asked to develop a management strategy that will encourage safer work prac- 


tices in an assembly plant. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


14 Chapter 1 Introduction to Psychology 


RETHINK unusual difficulty learning to read. Imagine that you 
could consult as many psychologists with different 
specialties as you wanted. What are the different types of 
psychologists that you might approach to address the 


1. Do you think intuition and common sense are sufficient 
for understanding why people act the way they do? In 
what ways is a scientific approach appropriate for study- 


roblem? 
ing human behavior? P 
2. From an educator’s perspective: Suppose you are a teacher Answers to Evaluate Questions 
who has a 7-year-old child in your class who is having TT- ‘E-A ‘9-Í TLE T-U 6-3 ‘2-3 “G-9 ‘T-P ‘OT ‘8-4 ‘P-e "T 


KEY TERM 


psychology p. 5 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 2 


Seven thousand years ago, people assumed that psychological problems were caused by 
evil spirits. To allow those spirits to escape from a person’s body, ancient healers chipped 
a hole in a patient’s skull with crude instruments—a procedure called trephining. 


According to the 17th-century philosopher Descartes, nerves were hollow tubes through 
which “animal spirits” conducted impulses in the same way that water is transmitted 
through a pipe. When a person put a finger too close to a fire, heat was transmitted to 
the brain through the tubes. 


Franz Josef Gall, an 18th-century physician, argued that a trained observer could 
discern intelligence, moral character, and other basic personality characteristics from the 
shape and number of bumps on a person’s skull. His theory gave rise to the field of 
phrenology, employed by hundreds of practitioners in the 19th century. 


Although these explanations might sound far-fetched, in their own times they repre- 
sented the most advanced thinking about what might be called the psychology of the 
era. Our understanding of behavior has progressed tremendously since the 18th century, 
but most of the advances have been recent. As sciences go, psychology is one of the 
new kids on the block. (For highlights in the development of the field, see Figure 1 on 
page 16.) 


The Roots of Psychology 


We can trace psychology’s roots back to the ancient Greeks, who considered the 
mind to be a suitable topic for scholarly contemplation. Later philosophers argued 
for hundreds of years about some of the questions psychologists grapple with 
today. For example, the 17th-century British philosopher John Locke believed that 
children were born into the world with minds like “blank slates” (tabula rasa in 
Latin) and that their experiences determined what kind of adults they would 
become. His views contrasted with those of Plato and the 17th-century French 
philosopher René Descartes, who argued that some knowledge was inborn in 
humans. 

However, the formal beginning of psychology as a scientific discipline is gener- 
ally considered to be in the late 19th century, when, in Leipzig, Germany, Wilhelm 
Wundt established the first experimental laboratory devoted to psychological phe- 
nomena. At about the same time, William James was setting up his laboratory in 
Cambridge, Massachusetts. 

When Wundt set up his laboratory in 1879, his aim was to study the building 
blocks of the mind. He considered psychology to be the study of conscious experi- 
ence. His perspective, which came to be known as structuralism, focused on uncov- 
ering the fundamental mental components of perception, consciousness, thinking, 
emotions, and other kinds of mental states and activities. 

To determine how basic sensory processes shape our understanding of the world, 
Wundt and other structuralists used a procedure called introspection, in which they 





What are the origins of 
psychology? 


What are the major 
approaches in contemporary 
psychology? 





Wilhelm Wundt 


Wundt’s approach, 
which focuses on uncovering the 
fundamental mental components of 
consciousness, thinking, and other 
kinds of mental states and activities. 


A procedure used to 
study the structure of the mind in 
which subjects are asked to describe in 
detail what they are experiencing 
when they are exposed to a stimulus. 


15 


16 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 1 Introduction to Psychology 


1690 John Locke introduces 
idea of tabula rasa 


<4 5,000 BCE Trephining 
used to allow the escape of 
evil spirits 


4430 BCE Hippocrates 
argues for four 
temperaments of 
personality 


Forerunners 
of Psychology 





1637 Descartes 
describes animal 
spirits 








1915 
Strong emphasis on 
intelligence testing 
1879 Wilhelm Wundt raan 
inaugurates first 
psychology laboratory 
in Leipzig, Germany 





VA 


Mary Calkins works on 
memory 


1807 1895 1920 
Franz Josef Gall Panctonalist Gestalt 
Proposes model formulated psychology 
phrenology 1900 Sigmund Freud becomes 

influential 





develops the 
psychodynamic 
perspective 


1890 Principles 
of Psychology 
published by 
William James 


1904 Ivan Pavlov 
wins Nobel Prize 
for work on 
digestion that led 
to fundamental 
principles of 
learning 





FIGURE 1 This time line illustrates major milestones in the development of psychology. 


functionalism An early approach 

to psychology that concentrated on 
what the mind does—the functions 
of mental activity—and the role of 
behavior in allowing people to adapt 
to their environments. 


presented people with a stimulus—such as a bright green object or a sentence printed 
on a card—and asked them to describe, in their own words and in as much detail as 
they could, what they were experiencing. Wundt argued that by analyzing people’s 
reports, psychologists could come to a better understanding of the structure of the mind. 

Over time, psychologists challenged Wundt’s approach. They became increas- 
ingly dissatisfied with the assumption that introspection could reveal the structure 
of the mind. Introspection was not a truly scientific technique, because there were 
few ways an outside observer could confirm the accuracy of others’ introspections. 
Moreover, people had difficulty describing some kinds of inner experiences, such as 
emotional responses. Those drawbacks led to the development of new approaches, 
which largely replaced structuralism. 

The perspective that replaced structuralism is known as functionalism. Rather 
than focusing on the mind’s structure, functionalism concentrated on what the mind 
does and how behavior functions. Functionalists, whose perspective became promi- 
nent in the early 1900s, asked what role behavior plays in allowing people to adapt 
to their environments. For example, a functionalist might examine the function of 
the emotion of fear in preparing us to deal with emergency situations. 

Led by the American psychologist William James, the functionalists examined how 
behavior allows people to satisfy their needs and how our “stream of consciousness” 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 2 A Science Evolves: The Past, the Present, and the Future 17 


1980 
Jean Piaget, 
an influential 


1924 
John B.Watson, an 
early behaviorist, 


publishes y developmental 
Behaviorism psychologist, dies 
2010 
New subfields 
1957 Leon Festinger develop such as 
publishes A Theory of clinical 





Cognitive Dissonance, neuropsychology 





producing a major impact and evolutionary 
on social psycholo, 
ioe psychology psychology 
Carl Rogers publishes 1990 Greater emphasis 
Client-Centered Therapy, helping to on multiculturalism and 
establish the humanistic diversity 


perspective 





i953 1969 
i Arguments 1985 Increasing 
B. F. Skinner publishes a Í 
, : regarding the emphasis on 
Science and Human Behavior, i a 
; ; genetic basis of cognitive 
advocating the behavioral ; ; f 
a IQ fuel lingering perspective 
Perspective controversies 
1928 
Leta Stetter 1981 David Hubel 


Hollingworth publishes 


and Torsten Wiesel 
work on adolescence 


win Nobel Prize for 
work on vision cells 
in the brain 


1954 

Abraham Maslow 
publishes Motivation and 
Personality, developing 
the concept of 


self-actualization 
2000 


Elizabeth Loftus does 
pioneering work on false 
memory and eyewitness 
testimony 





permits us to adapt to our environment. The American educator John Dewey drew 
on functionalism to develop the field of school psychology, proposing ways to best 
meet students’ educational needs. 

Another important reaction to structuralism was the development of gestalt 
psychology in the early 1900s. Gestalt psychology emphasizes how perception is gestalt (geh-SHTALLT) 
organized. Instead of considering the individual parts that make up thinking, gestalt psychology An approach to 
psychologists took the opposite tack, studying how people consider individual psychology that focuses on the 
elements together as units or wholes. Led by German scientists such as Hermann organization of perception and 
Ebbinghaus and Max Wertheimer, gestalt psychologists proposed that “The whole is thinking in a “whole” sense rather 
different from the sum of its parts,” meaning that our perception, or understanding, than on the individual elements of 
of objects is greater and more meaningful than the individual elements that make up perception. 
our perceptions. Gestalt psychologists have made substantial contributions to our 
understanding of perception. 






y Alert 


Knowing the basic outlines of 
the history of the field will 
As in many scientific fields, social prejudices hindered women’s participation in the help you understand how 
early development of psychology. For example, many universities would not even today’s major perspectives 
admit women to their graduate psychology programs in the early 1900s. have evolved. 


WOMEN IN PSYCHOLOGY: FOUNDING MOTHERS 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


18 Chapter 1 Introduction to Psychology 


y Alert 


Use Figure 2 to differentiate 
the five perspectives, which 
are important, because 
they provide a foundation 
for every topic covered 
throughout the text. 


Despite the hurdles they faced, women made notable contributions to psychology, 
although their impact on the field was largely overlooked until recently. For example, 
Margaret Floy Washburn (1871-1939) was the first woman to receive a doctorate in 
psychology, and she did important work on animal behavior. Leta Stetter Hollingworth 
(1886-1939) was one of the first psychologists to focus on child development and on 
women’s issues. She collected data to refute the view, popular in the early 1900s, that 
women’s abilities periodically declined during parts of the menstrual cycle (Denmark 
& Fernandez, 1993; Furumoto & Scarborough, 2002; Hollingworth, 1943/1990). 

Mary Calkins (1863-1930), who studied memory in the early part of the 20th 
century, became the first female president of the American Psychological Association. 
Karen Horney (pronounced “HORN-eye”) (1885-1952) focused on the social and cul- 
tural factors behind personality, and June Etta Downey (1875-1932) spearheaded the 
study of personality traits and became the first woman to head a psychology depart- 
ment at a state university. Anna Freud (1895-1982), the daughter of Sigmund Freud, 
also made notable contributions to the treatment of abnormal behavior, and Mamie 
Phipps Clark (1917-1983) carried out pioneering work on how children of color grew 
to recognize racial differences (Horney, 1937; Lal, 2002; Stevens & Gardner, 1982). 


Today’s Perspectives 


The men and women who laid the foundations of psychology shared a common goal: 
to explain and understand behavior using scientific methods. Seeking to achieve the 
same goal, the tens of thousands of psychologists who followed those early pioneers 
embraced—and often rejected—a variety of broad perspectives. 

The perspectives of psychology offer distinct outlooks and emphasize different 
factors. Just as we can use more than one map to find our way around a particular 
region—for instance, a map that shows roads and highways and another map that 
shows major landmarks—psychologists developed a variety of approaches to under- 
standing behavior. When considered jointly, the different perspectives provide the 
means to explain behavior in its amazing variety. 

Today, the field of psychology includes five major perspectives (summarized in 
Figure 2). These broad perspectives emphasize different aspects of behavior and mental 
processes, and each takes our understanding of behavior in a somewhat different 
direction. 





























Neuroscience Psychodynamic Behavioral Cognitive Humanistic 

Views behavior from the Believes behavior is motivated Focuses on observable Examines how people Contends that people can 

perspective of biological by inner, unconscious forces behavior understand and think control their behavior and 

functioning over which a person has little about the world that they naturally try to 
control reach their full potential 


FIGURE 2 The major perspectives of psychology. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 2 A Science Evolves: The 


THE NEUROSCIENCE PERSPECTIVE: BLOOD, SWEAT, AND FEARS 


When we get down to the basics, humans are animals made of skin and bones. The 
neuroscience perspective considers how people and nonhumans function biologi- 
cally: how individual nerve cells are joined together, how the inheritance of certain 
characteristics from parents and other ancestors influences behavior, how the func- 
tioning of the body affects hopes and fears, which behaviors are instinctual, and so 
forth. Even more complex kinds of behaviors, such as a baby’s response to strangers, 
are viewed as having critical biological components by psychologists who embrace 
the neuroscience perspective. This perspective includes the study of heredity and 
evolution, which considers how heredity may influence behavior; and behavioral 
neuroscience, which examines how the brain and the nervous system affect behavior. 

Because every behavior ultimately can be broken down into its biological com- 
ponents, the neuroscience perspective has broad appeal. Psychologists who sub- 
scribe to this perspective have made major contributions to the understanding and 
betterment of human life, ranging from cures for certain types of deafness to drug 
treatments for people with severe mental disorders. Furthermore, advances in meth- 
ods for examining the anatomy and functioning of the brain have permitted the 
neuroscientific perspective to extend its influence across a broad range of subfields 
in psychology. (We’ll see examples of these methods throughout this book in 
Neuroscience in Your Life.) 


THE PSYCHODYNAMIC PERSPECTIVE: 
UNDERSTANDING THE INNER PERSON 


To many people who have never taken a psychology course, psychology begins and 
ends with the psychodynamic perspective. Proponents of the psychodynamic 
perspective argue that behavior is motivated by inner forces and conflicts about 
which we have little awareness or control. They view dreams and slips of the tongue 
as indications of what a person is truly feeling within a seething cauldron of uncon- 
scious psychic activity. 

The origins of the psychodynamic view are linked to one person: Sigmund 
Freud. Freud was a Viennese physician in the early 1900s whose ideas about uncon- 
scious determinants of behavior had a revolutionary effect on 20th-century thinking, 
not just in psychology but in related fields as well. Although some of the original 
Freudian principles have been roundly criticized, the contemporary psychodynamic 
perspective has provided a means not only to understand and treat some kinds of 
psychological disorders but also to understand everyday phenomena such as preju- 
dice and aggression. 


THE BEHAVIORAL PERSPECTIVE: 
OBSERVING THE OUTER PERSON 


Whereas the neuroscience and psychodynamic approaches look inside the organism 
to determine the causes of its behavior, the behavioral perspective takes a very dif- 
ferent approach. The behavioral perspective grew out of a rejection of psychology’s 
early emphasis on the inner workings of the mind. Instead, behaviorists suggested 
that the field should focus on observable behavior that can be measured objectively. 

John B. Watson was the first major American psychologist to advocate a behav- 
ioral approach. Working in the 1920s, Watson was adamant in his view that one could 
gain a complete understanding of behavior by studying and modifying the environ- 
ment in which people operate. 

In fact, Watson believed rather optimistically that it was possible to elicit any 
desired type of behavior by controlling a person’s environment. This philosophy 
is clear in his own words: “Give me a dozen healthy infants, well-formed, and my 
own specified world to bring them up in and I'll guarantee to take any one at 
random and train him to become any type of specialist I might select—doctor, 


Past, the Present, and the Future 19 


neuroscience perspective The approach 
that views behavior from the perspective 
of the brain, the nervous system, and 
other biological functions. 





Sigmund Freud 


psychodynamic perspective The 
approach based on the view that 
behavior is motivated by unconscious 
inner forces over which the individual 
has little control. 


behavioral perspective The approach 
that suggests that observable, measur- 
able behavior should be the focus of 
study. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


20 Chapter 1 Introduction to Psychology 


cognitive perspective The approach 
that focuses on how people think, 
understand, and know about the 
world. 


humanistic perspective The approach 
that suggests that all individuals 
naturally strive to grow, develop, and 
be in control of their lives and behavior. 


lawyer, artist, merchant-chief, and yes, even beggar-man and thief, regardless of 
his talents, penchants, tendencies, abilities, vocations and race of his ancestors” 
(Watson, 1924). 

The behavioral perspective was championed by B. F. Skinner, a pioneer in the 
field. Much of our understanding of how people learn new behaviors is based on 
the behavioral perspective. As we will see, the behavioral perspective crops up along 
every byway of psychology. Along with its influence in the area of learning processes, 
this perspective has made contributions in such diverse areas as treating mental 
disorders, curbing aggression, resolving sexual problems, and ending drug addiction 
(Silverman, Roll, & Higgins, 2008). 


THE COGNITIVE PERSPECTIVE: 
IDENTIFYING THE ROOTS OF UNDERSTANDING 


Efforts to understand behavior lead some psychologists straight into the mind. 
Evolving in part from structuralism and in part as a reaction to behaviorism, which 
focused so heavily on observable behavior and the environment, the cognitive 
perspective focuses on how people think, understand, and know about the world. 
The emphasis is on learning how people comprehend and represent the outside 
world within themselves and how our ways of thinking about the world influence 
our behavior. 

Many psychologists who adhere to the cognitive perspective compare human 
thinking to the workings of a computer, which takes in information and transforms, 
stores, and retrieves it. In their view, thinking is information processing. 

Psychologists who rely on the cognitive perspective ask questions on subjects 
ranging from how people make decisions to whether a person can watch televi- 
sion and study at the same time. The common elements that link cognitive 
approaches are an emphasis on how people understand and think about the 
world and an interest in describing the patterns and irregularities in the operation 
of our minds. 


THE HUMANISTIC PERSPECTIVE: 
THE UNIQUE QUALITIES OF THE HUMAN SPECIES 


Rejecting the view that behavior is determined largely by automatically unfolding 
biological forces, unconscious processes, or the environment, the humanistic 
perspective instead suggests that all individuals naturally strive to grow, develop, 
and be in control of their lives and behavior. Humanistic psychologists maintain that 
each of us has the capacity to seek and reach fulfillment. 

According to Carl Rogers and Abraham Maslow, who were central figures in the 
development of the humanistic perspective, people strive to reach their full potential 
if they are given the opportunity. The emphasis of the humanistic perspective is on 
free will, the ability to freely make decisions about one’s own behavior and life. The 
notion of free will stands in contrast to determinism, which sees behavior as caused, 
or determined, by things beyond a person’s control. 

The humanistic perspective assumes that people have the ability to make their 
own choices about their behavior rather than relying on societal standards. More than 
any other approach, it stresses the role of psychology in enriching people’s lives and 
helping them achieve self-fulfillment. By reminding psychologists of their commit- 
ment to the individual person in society, the humanistic perspective has been an 
important influence (Dillon, 2008; Robbins, 2008). 

Don’t let the abstract qualities of the broad approaches we have discussed lull 
you into thinking that they are purely theoretical: These perspectives underlie ongo- 
ing work of a practical nature, as we discuss throughout this book. To start seeing 
how psychology can improve everyday life, read Applying Psychology in the 21st 
Century. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Psychology Matters 


“Investigators search for clues at site of 
suicide bombing.” 

“Deepest recession in decades produces 
huge rates of unemployment.” 
“Eyewitness to killing proves unable to 
provide reliable clues.” 

“Texting while driving blamed for rise 
in traffic fatalities” 

“Childhood obesity rates surge.” 


A quick review of any day’s news head- 
lines reminds us that the world is beset by 
a variety of stubborn problems that resist 
easy solutions. At the same time, a con- 
siderable number of psychologists are de- 
voting their energies and expertise to 
addressing these problems and improving 
the human condition. Let’s consider some 
of the ways in which psychology has ad- 
dressed and helped work toward solutions 
of major societal problems: 


e What are the causes of terrorism? What 
motivates suicide bombers? Are they 
psychologically disordered, or can their 
behavior be seen as a rational response 
to a particular system of beliefs? As 
we'll see in Module 48 when we discuss 
psychological disorders, psychologists 
are gaining an understanding of the fac- 
tors that lead people to embrace suicide 
and to engage in terrorism to further a 
cause in which they deeply believe (Lo- 
cicero & Sinclair, 2008; Mintz & Brule 
2009; Post et al., 2009; Stronink, 2007). 

e How can people best cope with an eco- 
nomic crisis? When the economy sours, 
financial loss and the hovering threat of 
unemployment create a great deal of 
stress. Psychologists study how these 
stressors affect family relations, produc- 
ing psychological harm—particularly 
to children of stressed and depressed 
parents—that can long outlast the eco- 
nomic crisis. They can then help people 
use coping strategies that are effective at 
preventing long-term harm (Conger and 
Donnellan, 2007; Unal-Karagiiven, 2009). 

e Why do eyewitnesses to crimes often 
remember the events inaccurately, and 
how can we increase the precision of 
eyewitness accounts? Psychologists’ 
research has come to an important 
conclusion: Eyewitness testimony in 





Terrorism and its causes are among the world’s most pressing issues. What can 
psychologists add to our understanding of the problem? 


criminal cases is often inaccurate and 
biased. Memories of crimes are often 
clouded by emotion, and the questions 
asked by police investigators often elicit 
inaccurate responses. Work by psychol- 
ogists has been used to provide national 
guidelines for obtaining more accurate 
memories during criminal investiga- 
tions (Busey & Loftus, 2007; Kassin, 
2005; Loftus & Bernstein, 2005). 

e Does texting while driving impair 
people’s driving ability? Several states 
have enacted controversial laws banning 
cell phone use and texting while driving. 
Although many people believe that they 
are perfectly able to talk and drive at the 
same time, psychological research on at- 
tention tells a different story: Merely talk- 
ing ona cell phone, whether hands-free or 
not, impairs people’s driving about as 
muchas if they were legally drunk (Charl- 
ton, 2009; Drews, Pasupathi, & Strayer, 
2008; Strayer et al., 2005; Taggi et al., 2007). 

e What are the roots of obesity, and how 
can healthier eating and better physical 


ETHINK 


fitness be encouraged? Why are some 
people more predisposed to obesity than 
others are? What social factors might be 
at play in the rising rate of obesity in 
childhood? As we discuss in Module 30, 
obesity is a complex problem with bio- 
logical, psychological, and social under- 
pinnings. Approaches to treating obesity 
therefore must take many factors into ac- 
count in order to be successful. There is 
no magic bullet providing a quick fix, 
but psychologists recommend a number 
of strategies that help make weight-loss 
goals more achievable (MacLean et al., 
2009; Neumark-Sztainer, 2009; Puhl & 
Latner, 2007). 


These topics represent just a few of the 
issues that psychologists address on a daily 
basis. To further explore the many ways 
that psychology has an impact on every- 
day life, check out the American Psycho- 
logical Association (APA) website, which 
features psychological applications in 
everyday life, at www.apa.org. 








e What do you think are the major problems affecting society today? 
e What are the psychological issues involved in these problems, and how might 
psychologists help find solutions to them? 








21 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


22 Chapter 1 Introduction to Psychology 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 


What are the origins of psychology? 


e Wilhelm Wundt laid the foundation of psychology in 
1879, when he opened his laboratory in Germany. (p. 15) 

e Early perspectives that guided the work of psychologists 
were structuralism, functionalism, and gestalt 
theory. (p. 15) 


What are the major approaches in contemporary psychology? 


E 
1. 


2 


e The neuroscience approach focuses on the biological 
components of the behavior of people and animals. 
(p. 19) 

e The psychodynamic perspective suggests that powerful, 
unconscious inner forces and conflicts about which peo- 
ple have little or no awareness are the primary determi- 
nants of behavior. (p. 19) 

e The behavioral perspective deemphasizes internal pro- 
cesses and concentrates instead on observable, measur- 
able behavior, suggesting that understanding and control 
of a person’s environment are sufficient to fully explain 
and modify behavior. (p. 19) 

e Cognitive approaches to behavior consider how people 
know, understand, and think about the world. (p. 20) 

e The humanistic perspective emphasizes that people are 
uniquely inclined toward psychological growth and 
higher levels of functioning and that they will strive to 
reach their full potential. (p. 20) 


VALUATE 


Wundt described psychology as the study of conscious 
experience, a perspective he called 

Early psychologists studied the mind by Sine people to 
describe what they were experiencing when exposed to 
various stimuli. This procedure was known as 


. The statement “In order to study human behavior, we 


must consider the whole of perception rather than its 


KEY TERMS 


structuralism p. 15 neuroscience 
introspection p. 15 perspective p. 19 
functionalism p. 16 psychodynamic 


gestalt (geh-SHTALLT) 


perspective p. 19 
psychology p.17 


behavioral 


component parts” might be made by a person subscribing 
to which perspective of psychology? 


. Jeanne’s therapist asks her to recount a violent dream she 


recently experienced in order to gain insight into the un- 
conscious forces affecting her behavior. Jeanne’s therapist 
is working from a perspective. 


. “It is behavior that can be observed that should be stud- 


ied, not the suspected inner workings of the mind.” This 
statement was most likely made by someone with which 
perspective? 

a. cognitive perspective 

b. neuroscience perspective 

c. humanistic perspective 

d. behavioral perspective 


. “My therapist is wonderful! He always points out my pos- 


itive traits. He dwells on my uniqueness and strength as 
an individual. I feel much more confident about myself— 
as if I’m really growing and reaching my potential.” The 
therapist being described most likely follows a 
perspective. 


RETHINK 


1. Focusing on one of the five major perspectives in use 


today (that is, neuroscience, psychodynamic, behavioral, 

cognitive, and humanistic), can you describe the kinds of 
research questions and studies that researchers using that 
perspective might pursue? 


. From a journalist's perspective: Choose a current major polit- 


ical controversy. What psychological approaches or per- 
spectives can be applied to that issue? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


oystueumny *9 
{p'o ‘ormmeuApoypdsd 'p 44yeyse8 tg ‘uoMoedsoyut'z ‘wstpemyoniys T 


humanistic 


perspective p. 19 perspective p. 20 


cognitive 


perspective p. 20 


~ www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


As you consider the many topics and perspectives that make up psychology, rang- 
ing from a narrow focus on minute biochemical influences on behavior to a broad 
focus on social behaviors, you might find yourself thinking that the discipline lacks 
cohesion. However, the field is more unified than a first glimpse might suggest. 
For one thing, no matter what topical area a psychologist specializes in, he or she 
will rely primarily on one of the five major perspectives. For example, a develop- 
mental psychologist who specializes in the study of children could make use of the 
cognitive perspective or the psychodynamic perspective or any of the other major 
perspectives. 

Psychologists also agree on what the key issues of the field are (see Figure 1). 
Although there are major arguments regarding how best to address and resolve the 
key issues, psychology is a unified science, because psychologists of all perspectives 
agree that the issues must be addressed if the field is going to advance. As you con- 
template these key issues, try not to think of them in “either/or” terms. Instead, 
consider the opposing viewpoints on each issue as the opposite ends of a continuum, 
with the positions of individual psychologists typically falling somewhere between 
the two ends. 

Nature (heredity) versus nurture (environment) is one of the major issues that psy- 
chologists address. How much of people’s behavior is due to their genetically determined 


























What are psychology’s key 
issues and controversies? 


What is the future of 
psychology likely to hold? 


> Study Alert 
Use Figure 1 to learn the key 
issues that underlie every 
subfield of psychology. 





a] 

No 
Issue Neuroscience Psychodynamic Behavioral 
Nature (heredity) vs. nurture Nature Nature Nurture 
(environment) (heredity) (heredity) (environment) 


Conscious vs. unconscious 


causes of behavior 


Observable behavior vs. internal 


mental processes 


Free will vs. determinism 


Individual differences vs. universal 


principles 


Unconscious 


Internal 
emphasis 


Determinism 


Universal 
emphasis 


Unconscious 


Internal 
emphasis 


Determinism 


Universal 
emphasis 


Conscious 


Observable 
emphasis 


Determinism 


Both 


( 
Humanistic 


Cognitive 

Both Nurture 
(environment) 

Both Conscious 

Internal Internal 

emphasis emphasis 

Free will Free will 

Individual Individual 

emphasis emphasis 


FIGURE 1 Key issues in psychology and the positions taken by psychologists subscribing to 
the five major perspectives of psychology 


23 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


24 Chapter 1 Introduction to Psychology 


free will The idea that behavior is 
caused primarily by choices that are 
made freely by the individual. 


determinism The idea that people’s 
behavior is produced primarily by 


factors outside of their willful control. 


nature (heredity), and how much is due to nurture, the influences of the physical 
and social environment in which a child is raised? Furthermore, what is the interplay 
between heredity and environment? These questions have deep philosophical and 
historical roots, and they are involved in many topics in psychology. 

A psychologist’s take on this issue depends partly on which major perspective 
he or she subscribes to. For example, developmental psychologists whose focus is on 
how people grow and change throughout the course of their lives, may be most 
interested in learning more about hereditary influences if they follow a neuroscience 
perspective. In contrast, developmental psychologists who are proponents of the 
behavioral perspective would be more likely to focus on environment (Rutter, 2002, 
2006). 

However, every psychologist would agree that neither nature nor nurture alone 
is the sole determinant of behavior; rather, it is a combination of the two. In a sense, 
then, the real controversy involves how much of our behavior is caused by heredity 
and how much is caused by environmental influences. 

A second major question addressed by psychologists concerns conscious versus 
unconscious causes of behavior. How much of our behavior is produced by forces of 
which we are fully aware, and how much is due to unconscious activity—mental 
processes that are not accessible to the conscious mind? This question represents one 
of the great controversies in the field of psychology. For example, clinical psycholo- 
gists adopting a psychodynamic perspective argue that psychological disorders are 
brought about by unconscious factors, whereas psychologists employing the cogni- 
tive perspective suggest that psychological disorders largely are the result of faulty 
thinking processes. 

The next issue is observable behavior versus internal mental processes. Should psy- 
chology concentrate solely on behavior that can be seen by outside observers, or 
should it focus on unseen thinking processes? Some psychologists, particularly those 
relying on the behavioral perspective, contend that the only legitimate source of 
information for psychologists is behavior that can be observed directly. Other psy- 
chologists, building on the cognitive perspective, argue that what goes on inside a 
person’s mind is critical to understanding behavior, and so we must concern our- 
selves with mental processes. 

Free will versus determinism is another key issue. How much of our behavior is 
a matter of free will (choices made freely by an individual), and how much is 
subject to determinism, the notion that behavior is largely produced by factors 
beyond people’s willful control? An issue long debated by philosophers, the free- 
will/determinism argument is also central to the field of psychology (Cary, 2007; 
Dennett, 2003). 

For example, some psychologists who specialize in psychological disorders argue 
that people make intentional choices and that those who display so-called abnormal 
behavior should be considered responsible for their actions. Other psychologists dis- 
agree and contend that such individuals are the victims of forces beyond their con- 
trol. The position psychologists take on this issue has important implications for the 
way they treat psychological disorders, especially in deciding whether treatment 
should be forced on people who don’t want it. 

The last of the key issues concerns individual differences versus universal principles. 
How much of our behavior is a consequence of our unique and special qualities, 
and how much reflects the culture and society in which we live? How much of our 
behavior is universally human? Psychologists who rely on the neuroscience perspec- 
tive tend to look for universal principles of behavior, such as how the nervous 
system operates or the way certain hormones automatically prime us for sexual 
activity. Such psychologists concentrate on the similarities in our behavioral desti- 
nies despite vast differences in our upbringing. In contrast, psychologists who 
employ the humanistic perspective focus more on the uniqueness of every indi- 
vidual. They consider every person’s behavior a reflection of distinct and special 
individual qualities. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 3 Psychology’s Key Issues and Controversies 25 


The question of the degree to which psychologists can identify universal prin- 
ciples that apply to all people has taken on new significance in light of the tremen- 
dous demographic changes now occurring in the United States and around the 
world. As we discuss next, these changes raise new and critical issues for the disci- 
pline of psychology in the 21st century. 


A mother in Burr Ridge, Illinois, helps her son with his math Ex p | O rl nN g 
assignment. After he complains that he is “terrible at math,” DIVERSITY 


he tries to cheer hi by saying, “Don’t feel bad; . Y 
E E e EE E eee ee eee Understanding How Culture, Ethnicity, 


people are born to do well in math, and others have a lot of and Race Influence Behavior 





trouble with it. It’s just the way things are.” At the same time, 

on the other side of the world in Taipei, Taiwan, a mother is 

helping her daughter with her math homework. When the 

daughter complains that she’s no good at math, the mother tells her to keep at it, 
because everyone has pretty much the same ability in math, and it is hard work that 
guarantees success. 


These two apparently simple parent-child exchanges reveal a deep difference in 
perspectives on the world. People in Europe and North America are far more likely to 
attribute success to unchanging causes, such as intelligence, than are people in Asia, 
who are more likely to attribute school performance to temporary, situational factors, 
such as the amount of expended effort. 

These different perspectives may help explain the fact that Asian students often 
outperform U.S. students in international comparisons of student achievement. Asian 
students are taught that hard work and increased effort lead to academic success, and 
so they may be more willing to put in more effort to achieve success. In contrast, North 
American students tend to believe that their ability is fixed at birth and largely 
determines their success, and so they may be less willing to work hard (Chen & 
Stevenson, 1995; Pearce & Lin, 2007). 

Our knowledge that people in different cultures can have very different views of the 
world underlines the importance of moving beyond North America and studying 
other cultural groups in order to identify universal principles of behavior. Furthermore, 
broad cultural differences are not the only ones taken into account by psychologists in 
their attempts to identify general principles of behavior. Subcultural, ethnic, racial, and 





Members of different cultures attribute academic success to different factors. How might 
differing cultural perspectives affect the performance of Asian students and American students? 


26 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 1 Introduction to Psychology 


socioeconomic differences are increasingly important targets of study by psychologists 
(Cardemil, 2005; Li, 2005; Tucker & Herman, 2002). 

Although the discipline is growing more aware of the importance of taking cultural 
and subcultural factors into account, progress has not been rapid in actual practice. For 
example, the amount of research conducted in the United States on groups other than 
white middle-class college students is woefully small. 

There isn’t even universal agreement on the use of terms such as race and ethnic 
group. Race, for instance, is a biological concept that, technically, should be used only 
to refer to classifications based on the physical characteristics of an organism or species. 
But in practice, the term has been used to denote anything from skin color to culture. 
Incontrast, ethnic group and ethnicity are broader terms that refer to cultural background, 
nationality, religion, and language (Bonham, Warshauer-Baker, & Collins, 2005; 
Freeman, 2006; Paabo, 2001). 

To compound the difficulty, there are no universally acceptable names for races and 
ethnic groups. Psychologists—like other members of U.S. society—are divided on 
whether they should use the label African American (which focuses on geographical 
origins) or black (which focuses on skin color), just as they disagree about whether to 
use Caucasian or white, Hispanic or Latino, and Native American or American Indian 
(Council of National Psychological Associations for the Advancement of Ethnic 
Minority Interests, 2000; Phinney, 2003; Wang & Sue, 2005). 

Psychologists also know that the consequences of race cannot be understood without 
taking into account environmental and cultural factors. People’s behavior is a joint 
product of their race and of the treatment they receive from others because of it. In sum, 
only by examining behavior across ethnic, cultural, and racial lines can psychologists 
differentiate principles that are universal from those that are culture-bound. 





Psychology’s Future 


We have examined psychology’s foundations, but what does the future hold for the 
discipline? Although the course of scientific development is notoriously difficult to 
predict, several trends seem likely: 


e As its knowledge base grows, psychology will become increasingly specialized 
and new perspectives will evolve. For example, our growing understanding 
of the brain and the nervous system, combined with scientific advances in 
genetics and gene therapy, will allow psychologists to focus on prevention of 
psychological disorders rather than only on their treatment (Cuijpers et al., 
2008). 

e The evolving sophistication of neuroscientific approaches is likely to have an 
increasing influence over other branches of psychology. For instance, social 
psychologists already are increasing their understanding of social behaviors 
such as persuasion by using brain scans as part of an evolving field known 
as social neuroscience (Bunge & Wallis, 2008; Cacioppo & Decety, 2009). 

e Psychology’s influence on issues of public interest also will grow. The major 
problems of our time—such as violence, terrorism, racial and ethnic prejudice, 
poverty, and environmental and technological disasters—have important 
psychological aspects (Hobfoll, Hall, & Canetti-Nisim, 2007; Marshall, Bryant, 
& Amsel, 2007; Zimbardo, 2004). 

e Finally, as the population becomes more diverse, issues of diversity—embodied 
in the study of racial, ethnic, linguistic, and cultural factors—will become 
more important to psychologists providing services and doing research. The 
result will be a field that can provide an understanding of human behavior in 
its broadest sense (Chang & Sue, 2005; Leung & Blustein, 2000; Quintana et 
al., 2006). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


“Expand Your Mind beyond Virtual Reality . . . & Learn at the 
Speed of Light!” declares the sales pitch that arrives in the day’s 
spam. “I have an astounding technology I’m going to share 
with you. Something so profound it will . . . transform your 
mind and soul forever.” 

From advertisements to television and radio talk shows to 
the Internet, we are subjected to a barrage of information about 


Module 3 Psychology’s Key Issues and Controversies 27 


BECOMING AN 
INFORMED CONSUMER 


of Psychology 
Thinking Critically About Psychology: 
Distinguishing Legitimate Psychology 
from Pseudo-Psychology 


psychology. We are told that we can become better adjusted, 
smarter, more insightful, and happier individuals by learning 
the secrets that psychologists have revealed. 

However, such promises are usually empty. So how can we separate accurate 
information, which is backed by science and objective research, from pseudo- 
psychology based on anecdotes, opinions, and even outright fraud? The best approach 
is to employ critical thinking techniques. Developed by psychologists who specialize 
in learning, memory, cognition, intelligence, and education, critical thinking 
procedures provide the tools to scrutinize assumptions, evaluate assertions, and think 
with greater precision (Halpern & Riggio, 2002; Lilienfeld, Lynn, & Lohr, 2003; 
Roediger & McCabe, 2007). 

We'll be considering ways to boost critical thinking skills in Becoming an Informed 
Consumer of Psychology sections throughout the book. To get started, let’s consider what 
you need in order to evaluate information of a psychological nature, whether the 
source is an advertisement, a television show, a magazine article, or even a book as 
seemingly reliable as a college textbook. 


e For starters, know who is offering the information and advice. Are the providers 
of the information trained psychologists? What kinds of degrees do they have? 
Are they licensed? Are they affiliated with a particular institution? Before 
seriously relying on experts’ advice, check out their credentials. 

° Keep in mind that there is no free ride. If it is possible to solve major psychologi- 
cal ills by buying a $29.95 book, why do many people who suffer from such 
problems typically expend a considerable amount of time and money before they 
can be helped? If you could buy a computer program that would really “unlock 
the hidden truths” about others, wouldn’t it be in widespread use? Be wary of 
simple, glib responses to major difficulties. 

e Be aware that few universal cures exist for humankind’s ills. No method or 
technique works for everyone. The range of difficulties attached to the human 
condition is so broad that any procedure that purports to resolve all problems is 
certain to disappoint. 

e Finally, remember that no source of information or advice is definitive. The 
notion of infallibility is best left to the realm of religion; you should approach 
psychological information and advice from a critical and thoughtful perspective. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


28 Chapter 1 Introduction to Psychology 
RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 
RECAP RETHINK 


What are psychology’s key issues and controversies? 1. “The fact that some businesses now promote their ability 


e Psychology’s key issues and controversies center on how 
much of human behavior is a product of nature or nur- 
ture, conscious or unconscious thoughts, observable 
actions or internal mental processes, free will or deter- 
minism, and individual differences or universal 


i 


to help people ‘expand their minds beyond virtual reality 
shows the great progress psychology has made lately.” 
Criticize this statement in light of what you know about 
professional psychology and pseudo-psychology. 


. From a social worker's perspective: Imagine that you have a 


caseload of clients who come from diverse cultures, eth- 
nicities, and races. How might you consider their diverse 
backgrounds when interacting with them and when 
assisting them with identifying and obtaining social 
services? 


principles. (p. 23) 
What is the future of psychology likely to hold? 

e Psychology will become increasingly specialized, will 
pay greater attention to prevention instead of just 
treatment, will become more and more concerned 
with the public interest, and will take the growing 
diversity of the country’s population into account more 
fully. (p. 26) 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


any "¢ UsUTUOITAUS ‘g SUISTUTULIO}ep *T 


EVALUATE 


1. The view that behavior is largely produced by factors be- 
yond people’s willful control is known as 

2. In the nature-nurture issue, nature refers to heredity, ana 
nurture refers to the 

3. Race is a biological concept, not a psychological one. True 
or false? 


KEY TERMS 


free will p. 24 
determinism p. 24 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Psychology on the Web 


1. Practice using several search strategies to find information on the web about one of 
the key issues in psychology (for example, free will versus determinism, nature versus 
nurture, or conscious versus unconscious determinants of behavior), using (a) a 
general-purpose search engine (such as Google at www.google.com) and (b) a more 
specialized search engine (such as Yahoo's Psychology section, under the “Social 
Science” heading, at www.yahoo.com). Summarize and then compare the kinds of 
information you have found through each strategy. 

2. Search the web for discussions of youth violence and try to find (a) an article in the 
general news media, (b) information from a psychological point of view (for instance, 
experimental information or recommendations for parents from a professional 
organization), and (c) political opinion or debate about how to address the issue of 
youth violence. 


È D | log U e The field of psychology, as we have seen, is broad and 


diverse. It encompasses many different subfields and 
specialties practiced in a variety of settings, with new 
subfields continually arising. We have also seen that even within the various subfields of 
the field, it is possible to adopt several different approaches, including the neuroscience, 
psychodynamic, behavioral, cognitive, and humanistic perspectives. 

For all its diversity, though, psychology focuses on certain key issues that serve to 
unify the field along common lines and shared findings. These issues reappear as themes 
throughout this book as we discuss the work and accomplishments of psychologists in 
the many subfields of the discipline. 

In light of what you've already learned about the field of psychology, reconsider the 
questions raised regarding the oil spill in the Gulf of Mexico and answer the following 
questions: 





1. What kinds of factors might psychologists using the neuroscience perspective focus 
on to explain reactions to the oil spill? 

2. How would developmental psychologists consider the effects of watching television 
news reports of the disaster on a child’s later development? 

3. What aspects of the disaster would a clinical psychologist likely focus on? 

4. How might social psychologists explore the helpfulness of people in providing aid to 
the victims of the disaster? 


29 








Psychological Research 




















www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Key Concepts for Chapter 2 


MODULE 4 








What is the scientific method? @ What role The Scientific Method 


Theories: Specifying Broad 
Explanations 


do theories and hypotheses play in 


Hypotheses: Crafting 
Testable Predictions 


psychological research? 









What research methods do psychologists use? Conducting Psychological Research 
R : Archival Research 
@ How do psychologists establish cause- es T 
and-effect relationships in research studies? Survey Research 
The Case Study 
Correlational Research 
Experimental Research 
Applying Psychology in the 
21st Century: Testing the Value 
of Self-Affirmations: Am | Lovable 
Because | Tell Myself I'm Lovable? 


MODULE 6 





What major issues confront psychologists Critical Research Issues 


conducting research? The Ethics of Research 


Exploring Diversity: Choosing 
Participants Who Represent the Scope 
of Human Behavior 


Neuroscience in Your Life: 
The Importance of Using 
Representative Participants 


Should Animals Be Used in Research? 





Threats to Experimental Validity: 
Avoiding Experimental Bias 
Becoming an Informed Consumer 
of Psychology: Thinking 

Critically About Research 


31 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Prologue Why Did No One Help? 


Hartford, Connecticut, police released a chilling video of a 
78-year-old man trying to cross astreet with a carton of milk. 
He steps off the curb just as two carsthat appear to be racing 
swerve on the wrong side of the street: The first car swerves 
around the man. The second car hits him`and throws him into 
the air like a doll, then speeds away. \ 


What follows is even more chilling: People walk by. Nine 
vehicles pass him lying in the street. Some drivers slow down 
to look but drive away. 

Angel Arce Torres lies in the street for more than a minute 
before a police car arrives. He died a year later from injuries 
sustained in the accident (Brown, 2008, p. E5; Owens, 2009) 





Why didn't any passersby help the 78-year-old Torres? Did they 
not notice him? Or did they simply decide not to bother? 
Whatever the reason, they offered no help to him, and he 
sustained serious injuries. 

If Torres’s experience were an isolated incident, we might be 
able to attribute the bystanders’ inaction to something particular 
about the situation or to the specific people involved. However, 
events such as this one are all too common. 

In one infamous case, a woman named Kitty Genovese was 
attacked by a man near an apartment building in Queens, New 
York. At one point during the assault, which lasted 30 minutes, 
she managed to free herself and screamed, “Oh, my God, he 
stabbed me. Please help me!" In the stillness of the night, no 
fewer than 38 neighbors heard her screams. Windows opened, 
and lights went on. One couple pulled chairs up to the window 
and turned off the lights so that they could see better. Someone 
called out, “Let that girl alone.” But shouts were not enough to 
scare off the killer. He chased Genovese, stabbing her eight more 
times, and sexually molested her before leaving her to die. And 
how many of those 38 witnesses came to her aid? As in Torres's 
case, not one person helped (Rogers & Eftimiades, 1995). 

Such incidents remain dismaying—and perplexing. Why don't 
bystanders intervene in such situations, particularly when there 
are many of them who could potentially offer help? At the time 
of the Kitty Genovese murder, editorial writers suggested that 
the incidents could be attributed to the basic shortcomings of 


32 


“human nature,” but such an assumption is woefully inadequate. 
Many people have risked their own lives to help others in 
dangerous situations, and so “human nature” encompasses a 
wide range of both negative and positive responses. 

Psychologists puzzled over the problem for many years. After 
much research they reached an unexpected conclusion: Kitty 
Genovese probably would have been better off if only a few 
people, rather than many, had heard her cries for help. In fact, 
if only one bystander had been present, the chances that this 
person would have intervened might have been fairly high. It 
turns out that the fewer the witnesses to an assault, the better 
the victim's chances of getting help. 

How did psychologists come to such a curious conclusion? 
After all, logic and common sense clearly suggest that more 
bystanders would produce a greater likelihood that someone 
would help a person in need. This seeming contradiction—and 
the way psychologists resolved it—illustrates a central challenge 
for the field of psychology: asking useful questions about the 
unknown, and getting valid answers. 

Like professionals in any science, psychologists are vitally 
concerned with refining and expanding knowledge within their 
field. In the following modules we'll see how psychologists pose 
questions of interest and answer them through scientific research. 
We will find that the answers psychologists obtain from their 
research not only advance our understanding of behavior but 
also offer the potential to improve the human condition. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 4 


“Birds of a feather flock together”. . . or “Opposites attract”? “Two heads are better 
than one”. . . or “If you want a thing done well, do it yourself’? “The more the 
merrier”. . . or “Two’s company, three’s a crowd”? 

If we were to rely on common sense to understand behavior, we'd have consid- 
erable difficulty—especially because commonsense views are often contradictory. In 
fact, one of the major undertakings for the field of psychology is to develop sup- 
positions about behavior and to determine which of those suppositions are accurate. 

Psychologists—as well as scientists in other disciplines—meet the challenge of pos- 
ing appropriate questions and properly answering them by relying on the scientific 
method. The scientific method is the approach used by psychologists to systematically 
acquire knowledge and understanding about behavior and other phenomena of interest. 
As illustrated in Figure 1, it consists of four main steps: (1) identifying questions of 
interest, (2) formulating an explanation, (3) carrying out research designed to support 
or refute the explanation, and (4) communicating the findings. 


Identify questions of interest Perimulate'an explanation 
stemming from 


* Behavior and phenomenon peci e Mhaor 


requiring explanation 


= 


+ Prior research findings Develop a hypothesis 


e Curiosity, creativity, insight 


Carry out research 


Devise an operational definition 
of the hypothesis 


’ 


Communicate the findings Select a research method 


Collect the data 


Analyze the data 





The scientific method, which encompasses the process of identifying, asking, and 
answering questions, is used by psychologists, and by researchers from every other scientific 
discipline, to come to an understanding about the world. What do you think are the advantages 
of this method? 


What is the scientific method? 


What role do theories and 
hypotheses play in psycho- 
logical research? 


The approach 
through which psychologists system- 
atically acquire knowledge and 
understanding about behavior and 
other phenomena of interest. 


idy Alert 


Use Figure 1 to remember the 
four steps of the scientific 
method (identifying 
questions, formulating an 
explanation, carrying out 
research, and communicating the 
findings). 


33 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


34 Chapter 2 Psychological Research 


theories Broad explanations and 
predictions concerning phenomena 
of interest. 


TH BORIES oF Piven tale À 





© Roz Chast/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


Theories: Specifying Broad 
Explanations 


In using the scientific method, psychologists start by identifying questions of inter- 
est. We have all been curious at some time about our observations of everyday 
behavior. If you have ever asked yourself why a particular teacher is so easily 
annoyed, why a friend is always late for appointments, or how your dog under- 
stands your commands, you have been formulating questions about behavior. 

Psychologists, too, ask questions about the nature and causes of behavior. They 
may wish to explore explanations for everyday behaviors or for various phenomena. 
They may also pose questions that build on findings from their previous research or 
from research carried out by other psychologists. Or they may produce new ques- 
tions that are based on curiosity, creativity, or insight. 

Once a question has been identified, the next step in the scientific method is to 
develop a theory to explain the observed phenomenon. Theories are broad explanations 
and predictions concerning phenomena of interest. They provide a framework for under- 
standing the relationships among a set of otherwise unorganized facts or principles. 

All of us have developed our own informal theories of human behavior, such as 
“People are basically good” or “People’s behavior is usually motivated by self-interest.” 
However, psychologists’ theories are more formal and focused. They are established 
on the basis of a careful study of the psychological literature to identify earlier rel- 
evant research and previously formulated theories, as well as psychologists’ general 
knowledge of the field. 

Growing out of the diverse approaches employed by psychologists, theories 
vary both in their breadth and in their level of detail. For example, one theory might 
seek to explain and predict a phenomenon as broad as emotional experience. A 
narrower theory might attempt to explain why people display the emotion of fear 
nonverbally after receiving a threat (Guerrero, La Valley, & Farinelli, 2008; Waller, 
Cray, & Burrows, 2008). 

Psychologists Bibb Latané and John Darley, responding to the failure of bystand- 
ers to intervene when Kitty Genovese was murdered in New York, developed what 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


they called a theory of diffusion of responsibility (Latané & Darley, 1970). According to 
their theory, the greater the number of bystanders or witnesses to an event that calls 
for helping behavior, the more the responsibility for helping is perceived to be shared 
by all the bystanders. Thus, the greater the number of bystanders in an emergency 
situation, the smaller the share of the responsibility each person feels—and the less 
likely that any single person will come forward to help. 


Hypotheses: Crafting 
Testable Predictions 


Although the diffusion of responsibility theory seems to make sense, it repre- 
sented only the beginning phase of Latané and Darley’s investigative process. 
Their next step was to devise a way to test their theory. To do this, they needed 
to create a hypothesis. A hypothesis is a prediction stated in a way that allows it 
to be tested. Hypotheses stem from theories; they help test the underlying sound- 
ness of theories. 

In the same way that we develop our own broad theories about the world, we also 
construct hypotheses about events and behavior. Those hypotheses can range from 
trivialities (such as why our English instructor wears those weird shirts) to more mean- 
ingful matters (such as what is the best way to study for a test). Although we rarely 
test these hypotheses systematically, we do try to determine whether they are right. 
Perhaps we try comparing two strategies: cramming the night before an exam versus 
spreading out our study over several nights. By assessing which approach yields better 
test performance, we have created a way to compare the two strategies. 

A hypothesis must be restated in a way that will allow it to be tested, which 
involves creating an operational definition. An operational definition is the translation 
of a hypothesis into specific, testable procedures that can be measured and observed. 

There is no single way to go about devising an operational definition for a 
hypothesis; it depends on logic, the equipment and facilities available, the psycho- 
logical perspective being employed, and ultimately the creativity of the researcher. 
For example, one researcher might develop a hypothesis that uses as an operational 
definition of “fear” an increase in heart rate. In contrast, another psychologist might 
use as an operational definition of “fear” a written response to the question “How 
much fear are you experiencing at this moment?” 

Latané and Darley’s hypothesis was a straightforward prediction from their more 
general theory of diffusion of responsibility: The more people who witness an emer- 
gency situation, the less likely it is that help will be given to a victim. They could, 
of course, have chosen another hypothesis (try to think of one!), but their initial 
formulation seemed to offer the most direct test of the theory. 

Psychologists rely on formal theories and hypotheses for many reasons. For one 
thing, theories and hypotheses allow them to make sense of unorganized, separate 
observations and bits of information by permitting them to place the pieces within 
a coherent framework. In addition, theories and hypotheses offer psychologists the 
opportunity to move beyond known facts and make deductions about unexplained 
phenomena and develop ideas for future investigation (Cohen, 2003; Gurin, 2006; 
Howitt & Cramer, 2000). 

In short, the scientific method, with its emphasis on theories and hypotheses, 
helps psychologists pose appropriate questions. With properly stated questions in 
hand, psychologists then can choose from a variety of research methods to find 
answers. 





Module 4 The Scientific Method 35 


hypothesis A prediction, stemming 
from a theory, stated in a way that 
allows it to be tested. 


operational definition The translation 
of a hypothesis into specific, testable 
procedures that can be measured and 
observed. 


y Alert 


Remember that a theory is a 
broad explanation, while a 
hypothesis is a more 
narrow prediction. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


36 Chapter 2 Psychological Research 
RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 
RECAP 3. An experimenter is interested in studying the relationship 


between hunger and aggression. She decides that she will 
measure aggression by counting the number of times a 
participant will hit a punching bag. In this case, her 

definition of aggression is the number of times 
the participant hits the bag. 


What is the scientific method? 

e The scientific method is the approach psychologists use to 
understand behavior. It consists of four steps: identifying 
questions of interest, formulating an explanation, carrying 
out research that is designed to support or refute the 


explanation, and communicating the findings. (p. 33) 


RETHINK 
What role do theories and hypotheses play in psychological 


research? 1. Starting with the theory that diffusion of responsibility 


e Research in psychology is guided by theories (broad 
explanations and predictions regarding phenomena of 
interest) and hypotheses (theory-based predictions 
stated in a way that allows them to be tested). (p. 34) 


causes responsibility for helping to be shared among by- 
standers, Latané and Darley derived the hypothesis that 
the more people who witness an emergency situation, the 
less likely it is that help will be given to a victim. How 


many other hypotheses can you think of that are based on 
the same theory of diffusion of responsibility? 

2. From a lawyer's perspective: Imagine that you are assigned 
to a case similar to the one of Kitty Genovese. Your super- 
visor, who is unfamiliar with psychological research, asks 
you to provide information about the eyewitnesses to ex- 
plain why they did not help her. What would you include 
in your report? 


e To test a hypothesis, researchers must formulate an 
operational definition, which translates the abstract 
concepts of the hypothesis into the actual procedures 
used in the study. (p. 35) 


EVALUATE 


1. An explanation for a phenomenon of interest is known 
asa : 

2. To test this explanation, a researcher must state it in terms 
of a testable question known as a 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 
Jeuonesedo ‘g ‘stsayjoddy z ‘ATOaUR “TL 


KEY TERMS 


scientific method p. 33 
theories p. 34 

hypothesis p. 35 
operational definition p. 35 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 5 


Research—systematic inquiry aimed at the discovery of new knowledge—is a central 
ingredient of the scientific method in psychology. It provides the key to understand- 
ing the degree to which hypotheses (and the theories behind them) are accurate. 

Just as we can apply different theories and hypotheses to explain the same phe- 
nomena, we can use a number of alternative methods to conduct research. As we 
consider the major tools that psychologists use to conduct research, keep in mind 
that their relevance extends beyond testing and evaluating hypotheses in psychology. 
All of us carry out elementary forms of research on our own. For instance, a super- 
visor might evaluate an employee’s performance; a physician might systematically 
test the effects of different doses of a drug on a patient; a salesperson might compare 
different persuasive strategies. Each of these situations draws on the research prac- 
tices we are about to discuss. 

Let’s begin by considering several types of descriptive research designed to sys- 
tematically investigate a person, group, or patterns of behavior. These methods 
include archival research, naturalistic observation, survey research, and case studies. 


Archival Research 


Suppose that, like the psychologists Latané and Darley (1970), you were interested 
in finding out more about emergency situations in which bystanders did not provide 
help. One of the first places you might turn to would be historical accounts. By 
searching newspaper records, for example, you might find support for the notion 
that a decrease in helping behavior historically has accompanied an increase in the 
number of bystanders. 

Using newspaper articles is an example of archival research. In archival research, 
existing data, such as census documents, college records, and newspaper clippings, 
are examined to test a hypothesis. For example, college records may be used to 
determine if there are gender differences in academic performance (Sullivan, Riccio, 
& Reynolds, 2008). 

Archival research is a relatively inexpensive means of testing a hypothesis because 
someone else has already collected the basic data. Of course, the use of existing data 
has several drawbacks. For one thing, the data may not be in a form that allows the 
researcher to test a hypothesis fully. The information could be incomplete, or it could 
have been collected haphazardly (Riniolo et al., 2003; Simonton, 2000a; Vega, 2006). 

Most attempts at archival research are hampered by the simple fact that records 
with the necessary information often do not exist. In these instances, researchers often 
turn to another research method: naturalistic observation. 





Naturalistic Observation 


In naturalistic observation, the investigator observes some naturally occurring 
behavior and does not make a change in the situation. For example, a researcher 
investigating helping behavior might observe the kind of help given to victims in a 





What research methods do 
psychologists use? 


How do psychologists estab- 
lish cause-and-effect relation- 
ships in research studies? 


Research in which 
existing data, such as census docu- 
ments, college records, and newspaper 
clippings, are examined to test a 
hypothesis. 


Research in 
which an investigator simply observes 
some naturally occurring behavior and 
does not make a change in the situation. 


37 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


38 Chapter 2 Psychological Research 





Dian Fossey, a pioneer in the study of 
endangered mountain gorillas in their 
native habitat, relied on naturalistic 
observation for her research. What are 
the advantages of this approach? 


survey research Research in which 
people chosen to represent a larger 
population are asked a series of 
questions about their behavior, 
thoughts, or attitudes. 


case study An in-depth, intensive 
investigation of an individual or small 
group of people. 


high-crime area of a city. The important point to remember about naturalistic 
observation is that the researcher simply records what occurs, making no modifica- 
tion in the situation that is being observed (Moore, 2002; Rustin, 2006; Schutt, 2001). 

Although the advantage of naturalistic observation is obvious—we get a sample 
of what people do in their “natural habitat’”—there is also an important drawback: 
the inability to control any of the factors of interest. For example, we might find so 
few naturally occurring instances of helping behavior that we would be unable to 
draw any conclusions. Because naturalistic observation prevents researchers from 
making changes in a situation, they must wait until the appropriate conditions occur. 
Furthermore, if people know they are being watched, they may alter their reactions 
and produce behavior that is not truly representative. 





Survey Research 


There is no more straightforward way of finding out what people think, feel, and do 
than asking them directly. For this reason, surveys are an important research method. 
In survey research, a sample of people chosen to represent a larger group of interest 
(a population) is asked a series of questions about their behavior, thoughts, or attitudes. 
Survey methods have become so sophisticated that even with a very small sample 
researchers are able to infer with great accuracy how a larger group would respond. 
For instance, a sample of just a few thousand voters is sufficient to predict within one 
or two percentage points who will win a presidential election—if the representative 
sample is chosen with care (Groves et al., 2004; Igo, 2006; Sommer & Sommer, 2001). 

Researchers investigating helping behavior might conduct a survey by asking 
people to complete a questionnaire in which they indicate their reluctance for giving 
aid to someone. Similarly, researchers interested in learning about sexual practices 
have carried out surveys to learn which practices are common and which are not 
and to chart changing notions of sexual morality over the last several decades (Reece 
et al., 2009; Santelli et al., 2009). 

However, survey research has several potential pitfalls. For one thing, if the sam- 
ple of people who are surveyed is not representative of the broader population of 
interest, the results of the survey will have little meaning. For instance, if a sample of 
voters in a town only includes Republicans, it would hardly be useful for predicting 
the results of an election in which both Republicans and Democrats are voting. Con- 
sequently, researchers using surveys strive to obtain a random sample of the population 
in question, in which every voter in the town has an equal chance of being included 
in the sample receiving the survey (Dale, 2006; Daley et al., 2003). 

In addition, survey respondents may not want to admit to holding socially unde- 
sirable attitudes. (Most racists know they are racists and might not want to admit 
it.) And in some cases, people may not even be consciously aware of what their true 
attitudes are or why they hold them. 


The Case Study 


When they read of a suicide bomber in the Middle East, many people wonder what 
it is about the terrorist’s personality or background that leads to such behavior. To 
answer this question, psychologists might conduct a case study. In contrast to a 
survey, in which many people are studied, a case study is an in-depth, intensive 
investigation of a single individual or a small group. Case studies often include 
psychological testing, a procedure in which a carefully designed set of questions is 
used to gain some insight into the personality of the individual or group (Addus, 
Chen, & Khan, 2007; Gass et al., 2000). 

When case studies are used as a research technique, the goal is often not only 
to learn about the few individuals being examined but also to use the insights 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 5 Conducting Psychological Research 39 


gained from the study to improve our understanding of people in 
general. Sigmund Freud developed his theories through case stud- 
ies of individual patients. Similarly, case studies of terrorists might 
help identify others who are prone to violence. 

The drawback to case studies? If the individuals examined are 
unique in certain ways, it is impossible to make valid generalizations 
to a larger population. Still, they sometimes lead the way to new 
theories and treatments for psychological disorders. 


Correlational Research 


In using the descriptive research methods we have discussed, 
researchers often wish to determine the relationship between two 
variables. Variables are behaviors, events, or other characteristics 
that can change, or vary, in some way. For example, in a study to 
determine whether the amount of studying makes a difference in test 
scores, the variables would be study time and test scores. 

In correlational research, two sets of variables are examined to 
determine whether they are associated, or “correlated.” The strength and direction of 
the relationship between the two variables are represented by a mathematical statistic 
known as a correlation (or, more formally, a correlation coefficient), which can range from 
+1.0 to -1.0. 

A positive correlation indicates that as the value of one variable increases, we 
can predict that the value of the other variable will also increase. For example, if 
we predict that the more time students spend studying for a test, the higher their 
grades on the test will be, and that the less they study, the lower their test scores 
will be, we are expecting to find a positive correlation. (Higher values of the vari- 
able “amount of study time” would be associated with higher values of the vari- 
able “test score,” and lower values of “amount of study time” would be associated 
with lower values of “test score.”) The correlation, then, would be indicated by a 
positive number, and the stronger the association was between studying and test 
scores, the closer the number would be to +1.0. For example, we might find a 
correlation of +.85 between test scores and amount of study time, indicating a 
strong positive association. 

In contrast, a negative correlation tells us that as the value of one variable increases, 
the value of the other decreases. For instance, we might predict that as the number 
of hours spent studying increases, the number of hours spent partying decreases. 
Here we are expecting a negative correlation, ranging between 0 and —1.0. More 
studying is associated with less partying, and less studying is associated with more 
partying. The stronger the association between studying and partying is, the closer 
the correlation will be to —1.0. For instance, a correlation of —.85 would indicate a 
strong negative association between partying and studying. 

Of course, it’s quite possible that little or no relationship exists between two 
variables. For instance, we would probably not expect to find a relationship 
between number of study hours and height. Lack of a relationship would be indi- 
cated by a correlation close to 0. For example, if we found a correlation of —.02 or 
+.03, it would indicate that there is virtually no association between the two vari- 
ables; knowing how much someone studies does not tell us anything about how 
tall he or she is. 

When two variables are strongly correlated with each other, we are tempted to 
assume that one variable causes the other. For example, if we find that more study 
time is associated with higher grades, we might guess that more studying causes 
higher grades. Although this is not a bad guess, it remains just a guess—because 
finding that two variables are correlated does not mean that there is a causal relation- 
ship between them. The strong correlation suggests that knowing how much a person 








Do you feel better or worse about the economy 
than you did twenty minutes ago?” 


“This is the New York ‘Times’ 


Business Poll again, Mr. Landau. 


© J.B. Handelsman/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


variables Behaviors, events, or other 
characteristics that can change, or vary, 
in some way. 


correlational research Research in 
which the relationship between two 
sets of variables is examined to deter- 
mine whether they are associated, or 
“correlated.” 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


40 Chapter 2 Psychological Research 


FIGURE 1 If we find that frequent 
viewing of television programs with 
aggressive content is associated with high 
levels of aggressive behavior, we might 
cite several plausible causes, as suggested 
in this figure. For example, (a) choosing to 
watch shows with aggressive content 
could produce aggression; or (b) being a 
highly aggressive person might cause one 
to choose to watch televised aggression; 
or (c) having a high energy level might 
cause a person to both choose to watch 
aggressive shows and to act aggressively. 
Correlational findings, then, do not permit 
us to determine causality. Can you think 
of a way to study the effects of televised 
aggression on aggressive behavior that is 
not correlational? 


y Alert 


The concept that “correlation 
does not imply causation’ is 
a key principle. 








Many studies show that the observation 
of violence in the media is associated 
with aggression in viewers. Can we 
conclude that the observation of 
violence causes aggression? 








Potential Result 


Possible Cause 






pecccccccccces 





ecccccccccccccccs 


High viewer aggression 


e TTE 


High viewer aggression ’eeeseeeese eeo 





(b) 


High viewer aggression 


eesesecoseee 





(c) 





studies can help us predict how that person will do on a test, but it does not mean 
that the studying causes the test performance. Instead, for instance, people who are 
more interested in the subject matter might study more than do those who are less 
interested, and so the amount of interest, not the number of hours spent studying, 
would predict test performance. The mere fact that two variables occur together does 
not mean that one causes the other. 

Similarly, suppose you learned that the number of houses of worship in a large 
sample of cities was positively correlated with the number of people arrested, 
meaning that the more houses of worship, the more arrests there were in a city. 
Does this mean that the presence of more houses of worship caused the greater 
number of arrests? Almost surely not, of course. In this case, the underlying cause 
is probably the size of the city: in bigger cities, there are both more houses of wor- 
ship and more arrests. 

One more example illustrates the critical point that correlations tell us nothing 
about cause and effect but merely provide a measure of the strength of a relationship 
between two variables. We might find that children who watch a lot of television 
programs featuring high levels of aggression are likely to demonstrate a relatively 
high degree of aggressive behavior and that those who watch few television shows 
that portray aggression are apt to exhibit a relatively low degree of such behavior 
(see Figure 1). But we cannot say that the aggression is caused by the TV viewing, 
because many other explanations are possible. 

For instance, it could be that children who have an unusually high level of 
energy seek out programs with aggressive content and are more aggressive. The 
children’s energy level, then, could be the true cause of the children’s higher inci- 
dence of aggression. Also, people who are already highly aggressive might choose 
to watch shows with a high aggressive content because they are aggressive. Clearly, 
then, any number of causal sequences are possible—none of which can be ruled out 
by correlational research (Feshbach & Tangney, 2008; Grimes & Bergen, 2008). 

The inability of correlational research to demonstrate cause-and-effect relation- 
ships is a crucial drawback to its use. There is, however, an alternative technique that 
does establish causality: the experiment. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 5 Conducting Psychological Research 41 





Experimental Research 


The only way psychologists can establish cause-and-effect relationships through 
research is by carrying out an experiment. In a formal experiment, the researcher 
investigates the relationship between two (or more) variables by deliberately chang- 
ing one variable in a controlled situation and observing the effects of that change on 
other aspects of the situation. In an experiment, then, the conditions are created and 
controlled by the researcher, who deliberately makes a change in those conditions in 
order to observe the effects of that change. 

The change that the researcher deliberately makes in an experiment is called the 
experimental manipulation. Experimental manipulations are used to detect relation- 
ships between different variables. 

Several steps are involved in carrying out an experiment, but the process 
typically begins with the development of one or more hypotheses for the experi- 
ment to test. For example, Latané and Darley, in testing their theory of the diffu- 
sion of responsibility in bystander behavior, developed this hypothesis: The higher 
the number of people who witness an emergency situation is, the less likely it is 
that any of them will help the victim. They then designed an experiment to test 
this hypothesis. 

Their first step was to formulate an operational definition of the hypothesis 
by conceptualizing it in a way that could be tested. Latané and Darley had to take 
into account the fundamental principle of experimental research mentioned earlier: 
Experimenters must manipulate at least one variable in order to observe the effects 
of the manipulation on another variable while keeping other factors in the situa- 
tion constant. However, the manipulation cannot be viewed by itself, in isolation; 
if a cause-and-effect relationship is to be established, the effects of the manipula- 
tion must be compared with the effects of no manipulation or a different kind of 
manipulation. 


EXPERIMENTAL GROUPS AND CONTROL GROUPS 


Experimental research requires, then, that the responses of at least two groups be 
compared. One group will receive some special treatment—the manipulation imple- 
mented by the experimenter—and another group will receive either no treatment or 
a different treatment. Any group that receives a treatment is called an experimental 
group; a group that receives no treatment is called a control group. (In some exper- 
iments there are multiple experimental and control groups, each of which is com- 
pared with another group.) 

By employing both experimental and control groups in an experiment, research- 
ers are able to rule out the possibility that something other than the experimental 
manipulation produced the results observed in the experiment. Without a control 
group, we couldn’t be sure that some other variable, such as the temperature at the 
time we were running the experiment, the color of the experimenter’s hair, or even 
the mere passage of time, wasn’t causing the changes observed. 

For example, consider a medical researcher who thinks he has invented a 
medicine that cures the common cold. To test his claim, he gives the medicine one 
day to a group of 20 people who have colds and finds that 10 days later all of 
them are cured. 

Eureka? Not so fast. An observer viewing this flawed study might reasonably 
argue that the people would have gotten better even without the medicine. What the 
researcher obviously needed was a control group consisting of people with colds who 
don’t get the medicine and whose health is also checked 10 days later. Only if there 
is a significant difference between experimental and control groups can the effective- 
ness of the medicine be assessed. Through the use of control groups, then, researchers 
can isolate specific causes for their findings—and draw cause-and-effect inferences. 


experiment The investigation of the 
relationship between two (or more) 
variables by deliberately producing a 
change in one variable in a situation 
and observing the effects of that 
change on other aspects of the situation. 


experimental manipulation The 
change that an experimenter deliber- 
ately produces in a situation. 


treatment The manipulation imple- 
mented by the experimenter. 


experimental group Any group 
participating in an experiment that 
receives a treatment. 


control group A group participating 
in an experiment that receives no 
treatment. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


42 Chapter 2 Psychological Research 





In this experiment, preschoolers’ reactions to the 
puppet are monitored. Can you think of a hypothesis 
that might be tested in this way? 


independent variable The vari- 
able that is manipulated by an 
experimenter. 


dependent variable The variable that 
is measured and is expected to change 
as a result of changes caused by the 
experimenter’s manipulation of the 
independent variable. 


dy Alert 


To remember the difference 
between dependent and 
independent variables, 
recall that a hypothesis 
predicts how a dependent 
variable depends on the manipula- 
tion of the independent variable. 


Returning to Latané and Darley’s experiment, we see that the 
researchers needed to translate their hypothesis into something testable. 
To do this, they decided to create a false emergency situation that would 
appear to require the aid of a bystander. As their experimental manipula- 
tion, they decided to vary the number of bystanders present. They could 
have had just one experimental group with, say, two people present, and 
a control group for comparison purposes with just one person present. 
Instead, they settled on a more complex procedure involving the creation 
of groups of three sizes—consisting of two, three, and six people—that 
could be compared with one another. 


INDEPENDENT AND DEPENDENT VARIABLES 


Latané and Darley’s experimental design now included an operational 
definition of what is called the independent variable. The independent 
variable is the condition that is manipulated by an experimenter. (You 
can think of the independent variable as being independent of the actions 
of those taking part in an experiment; it is controlled by the experi- 
menter.) In the case of the Latané and Darley experiment, the indepen- 
dent variable was the number of people present, which was manipulated by the 
experimenters. 

The next step was to decide how they were going to determine the effect that 
varying the number of bystanders had on behavior of those in the experiment. 
Crucial to every experiment is the dependent variable, the variable that is 
measured and is expected to change as a result of changes caused by the experi- 
menter’s manipulation of the independent variable. The dependent variable is 
dependent on the actions of the participants or subjects—the people taking part in 
the experiment. 

Latané and Darley had several possible choices for their dependent measure. 
One might have been a simple yes/no measure of the participants’ helping behav- 
ior. But the investigators also wanted a more precise analysis of helping behavior. 
Consequently, they also measured the amount of time it took for a participant to 
provide help. 

Latané and Darley now had all the necessary components of an experiment. The 
independent variable, manipulated by them, was the number of bystanders present 
in an emergency situation. The dependent variable was the measure of whether 
bystanders in each of the groups provided help and the amount of time it took them 
to do so. Consequently, like all experiments, this one had both an independent vari- 
able and a dependent variable. All true experiments in psychology fit this straight- 
forward model. 


RANDOM ASSIGNMENT OF PARTICIPANTS 


To make the experiment a valid test of the hypothesis, Latané and Darley needed to 
add a final step to the design: properly assigning participants to a particular experi- 
mental group. 

The significance of this step becomes clear when we examine various alternative 
procedures. For example, the experimenters might have assigned just males to the 
group with two bystanders, just females to the group with three bystanders, and both 
males and females to the group with six bystanders. If they had done this, however, 
any differences they found in helping behavior could not be attributed with any 
certainty solely to group size, because the differences might just as well have been 
due to the composition of the group. A more reasonable procedure would be to 
ensure that each group had the same composition in terms of gender; then the 
researchers would be able to make comparisons across groups with considerably 
more accuracy. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 5 Conducting Psychological Research 43 


? ——_ 
eh Oe EN Ea AX 





“What if these guys in white coats who bring us food are, like, studying 
us and we're part of some kind of big experiment?” 


© Mike Twohy/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


Participants in each of the experimental groups ought to be comparable, and it 
is easy enough to create groups that are similar in terms of gender. The problem 
becomes a bit more tricky, though, when we consider other participant characteris- 
tics. How can we ensure that participants in each experimental group will be equally 
intelligent, extroverted, cooperative, and so forth, when the list of characteristics— 
any one of which could be important—is potentially endless? 

The solution is a simple but elegant procedure called random assignment to 
condition: Participants are assigned to different experimental groups, or “condi- 
tions,” on the basis of chance and chance alone. The experimenter might, for 
instance, flip a coin for each participant and assign a participant to one group when 
“heads” came up and to the other group when “tails” came up. The advantage of 
this technique is that there is an equal chance that participant characteristics will 
be distributed across the various groups. When a researcher uses random assignment— 
which in practice is usually carried out using computer-generated random numbers— 
chances are that each of the groups will have approximately the same proportion 
of intelligent people, cooperative people, extroverted people, males and females, 
and so on. 

Figure 2 provides another example of an experiment. Like all experiments, it 
includes the following set of key elements, which you should keep in mind as you 
consider whether a research study is truly an experiment: 


e An independent variable, the variable that is manipulated by the experimenter. 

e A dependent variable, the variable that is measured by the experimenter and 
that is expected to change as a result of the manipulation of the independent 
variable. 

e A procedure that randomly assigns participants to different experimental 
groups, or “conditions,” of the independent variable. 

e A hypothesis that predicts the effect the independent variable will have on the 
dependent variable. 


Only if each of these elements is present can a research study be considered a true 
experiment in which cause-and-effect relationships can be determined. (For a summary 
of the different types of research that we’ve discussed, see Figure 3 on page 45.) 


random assignment to condition 

A procedure in which participants are 
assigned to different experimental 
groups or “conditions” on the basis 
of chance and chance alone. 


44 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 2 Psychological Research 


participants 





assign 
participants 












independent i ident variab results of the tv 
variable groups 


to a condition 


/ Group 1: 


Treatment 


group 


Group 2: 


Control 
\ group 


4 


Ee 





No-drug condition 


FIGURE 2 In this depiction of a study investigating the effects of the drug propranolol on 
stress, we can see the basic elements of all true experiments. The participants in the experiment 
were monkeys who were randomly assigned to one of two groups. Monkeys assigned to the 
treatment group were given propranolol, hypothesized to prevent heart disease, whereas those 
in the control group were not given the drug. Administration of the drugs, then, was the 
independent variable. 

All the monkeys were given a high-fat diet that was the human equivalent of two eggs 
with bacon every morning, and they occasionally were reassigned to different cages to 
provide a source of stress. To determine the effects of the drug, the monkeys’ heart rates and 
other measures of heart disease were assessed after 26 months. These measures constituted 
the dependent variable. (The results? As hypothesized, monkeys who received the drug 
showed lower heart rates and fewer symptoms of heart disease than those who did not.) 
(Source: Based on a study by Kaplan & Manuck, 1989.) 


WERE LATANE AND DARLEY RIGHT? 


To test their hypothesis that increasing the number of bystanders in an emergency 
situation would lower the degree of helping behavior, Latané and Darley placed the 
participants in a room and told them that the purpose of the experiment was to talk 
about personal problems associated with college. The discussion was to be held over 
an intercom, supposedly to avoid the potential embarrassment of face-to-face contact. 
Chatting about personal problems was not, of course, the true purpose of the exper- 
iment, but telling the participants that it was provided a way of keeping their expec- 
tations from biasing their behavior. (Consider how they would have been affected if 
they had been told that their helping behavior in emergencies was being tested. The 
experimenters could never have gotten an accurate assessment of what the partici- 
pants would actually do in an emergency. By definition, emergencies are rarely 
announced in advance.) 

The sizes of the discussion groups were two, three, and six people, which con- 
stituted the manipulation of the independent variable of group size. Participants 
were randomly assigned to these groups upon their arrival at the laboratory. Each 
group included a trained confederate, or employee, of the experimenters. In each two- 
person group, then, there was only one real “bystander.” 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Research Method 


Descriptive and 
correlational research 


Archival research 


Naturalistic 


observation 


Survey research 


Module 5 Conducting Psychological Research 


Description 


Researcher observes a 
previously existing 
situation but does not 
make a change in the 
situation 


Examines existing data 
to confirm hypothesis 


Observation of 
naturally occurring 
behavior, without 
making a change in the 
situation 


A sample is chosen to 
represent a larger 
population and asked a 
series of questions 


Advantages Shortcomings 
Offers insight into Cannot 
relationships between determine 
variables causality 


Ease of data collection 
because data already 
exist 


Provides a sample of 
people in their natural 
environment 


A small sample can be 
used to infer attitudes 
and behavior of a 
larger population 


Dependent on 
availability of data 


Cannot control the 
“natural habitat” being 
observed 


Sample may not be 
representative of the 
larger population; 
participants may not 


45 


Case study Intensive investigation 
of an individual or 


small group 


Experimental 
research 


Investigator produces 
a change in one 
variable to observe 
the effects of that 
change on other 
variables 








FIGURE 3 Research strategies. 


As the participants in each group were holding their discussion, they suddenly 
heard through the intercom one of the other participants—the confederate—having 
what sounded like an epileptic seizure and then calling for help. 

The participants’ behavior was now what counted. The dependent variable was 
the time that elapsed from the start of the “seizure” to the time a participant began 
trying to help the “victim.” If six minutes went by without a participant’s offering 
help, the experiment was ended. 

As predicted by the hypothesis, the size of the group had a significant effect 
on whether a participant provided help. The more people who were present, the 
less likely it was that someone would supply help, as you can see in Figure 4 
(Latané & Darley, 1970). 

Because these results are straightforward, it seems clear that the experiment con- 
firmed the original hypothesis. However, Latané and Darley could not be sure that 
the results were truly meaningful until they determined whether the results repre- 
sented a significant outcome. Using statistical analysis, researchers can determine 
whether a numeric difference is a real difference or is due merely to chance. Only 
when differences between groups are large enough that statistical tests show them 
to be significant is it possible for researchers to confirm a hypothesis (Cohen, 2002; 
Cwikel, Behar, & Rabson-Hare, 2000). 


Provides a thorough, 
in-depth understanding 
of participants 


Experiments offer the 
only way to determine 
cause-and-effect 
relationships 


provide accurate 
responses to survey 
questions 


Results may not be 
generalizable 
beyond the sample 


To be valid, experi- 
ments require 
random assignment 
of participants to 
conditions, well- 
conceptualized 
independent and 
dependent vari- 
ables, and other 
careful controls 


significant outcome Meaningful 
results that make it possible for 
researchers to feel confident that they 
have confirmed their hypotheses. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


46 Chapter 2 Psychological Research 


replicated research Research that 
is repeated, sometimes using other 
procedures, settings, and groups of 
participants, to increase confidence 
in prior findings. 








The smaller the number 
| of bystanders, the 
greater the degree of 


60 helping 
50 
40 
30 
20 
10 
i 2 3 6 


Size of group 
































Percentage helping 























FIGURE 4 The Latané and Darley experiment showed that as the size of the group 
witnessing an emergency increased, helping behavior decreased. (Source: Darley & Latané, 1968.) 


MOVING BEYOND THE STUDY 


The Latané and Darley study contains all the elements of an experiment: an inde- 
pendent variable, a dependent variable, random assignment to conditions, and mul- 
tiple experimental groups. Consequently, we can say with some confidence that 
group size caused changes in the degree of helping behavior. 

Of course, one experiment alone does not forever resolve the question of 
bystander intervention in emergencies. Psychologists-like other scientists—require 
that findings be replicated, or repeated, sometimes using other procedures, in other 
settings, with other groups of participants, before full confidence can be placed in 
the results of any single experiment. A procedure called meta-analysis permits psy- 
chologists to combine the results of many separate studies into one overall conclusion 
(Cooper & Patall, 2009; Tenenbaum & Ruck, 2007). 

In addition to replicating experimental results, psychologists need to test the 
limitations of their theories and hypotheses to determine under which specific cir- 
cumstances they do and do not apply. It seems unlikely, for instance, that increasing 
the number of bystanders always results in less helping. Therefore, it is critical to 
continue carrying out experiments to understand the conditions in which exceptions 
to this general rule occur and other circumstances in which the rule holds (Garcia- 
Palacios, Hoffmann, & Carlin, 2002). 

Before leaving the Latané and Darley study, note that it represents a good illus- 
tration of the basic principles of the scientific method we considered earlier (as 
outlined in Figure 1 of Module 4 on page 33). The two psychologists began with a 
question of interest, in this case stemming from a real-world incident in which 
bystanders in an emergency did not offer help. They then formulated an explanation 
by specifying a theory of diffusion of responsibility, and from that formulated the 
specific hypothesis that increasing the number of bystanders in an emergency situ- 
ation would lower the degree of helping behavior. Finally, they carried out research 
to confirm their hypothesis, and they eventually communicated their findings by 
publishing their results. This four-step process embodied in the scientific method 
underlies all scientific inquiry, allowing us to develop a valid understanding of 
others’—and our own—behavior. (Also see Applying Psychology in the 21st Century 
for more information on how research provides objective answers to questions about 
human behavior.) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


yin the 21st Century 





Testing the Value 

of Self-Affirmations: 

Am | Lovable Because 

I Tell Myself I’m Lovable? 


If you've ever felt down about yourself, 
you might have sought comfort from a 
friend or family member who spent some 
time telling you what a great and wonder- 
ful person you really are. Perhaps you've 
done the same for a friend of yours who 
was having a crisis of confidence. Pop psy- 
chology wisdom tells us that these affirma- 
tive statements, or affirmations, help us feel 
better about ourselves. We’re sometimes 
even advised to use self-affirmations to 
help boost our sense of self worth, and it 
makes good common sense that if you keep 
repeating to yourself statements such as 
“Tm a lovable person” or “I will succeed,” 
you'll believe it and like yourself more. But 
is that really the case? (Gordon, 2001) 
Relying on intuition alone to answer this 
question is not enough. Something that 
sounds like it should be true may not be. 
The only way to tell is to put the hypothesis 
to the test—and that’s exactly what a group 
of psychology researchers did with affirma- 
tions. They brought several dozen college 
students into their laboratory, where they 
first asked students to assess their self- 
esteem level and mood using question- 
naires. Then the researchers instructed the 
students to write down their thoughts and 
feelings for several minutes. Half the stu- 
dents were given an additional instruction: 
Whenever a certain tone sounded, they 
were to give themselves a self-affirmation, 
telling themselves “I’m a lovable person.” 
(The group providing themselves the self- 


RECAP 


What research methods do psychologists use? 
e Archival research uses existing records, such as old 
newspapers or other documents, to test a hypothesis. In 
naturalistic observation, the investigator acts mainly as e 
an observer, making no change in a naturally occurring 
situation. In survey research, people are asked a series of 





Common sense tells us that self-affirmations 
make us feel good, but research has shown 
otherwise. 


affirmation was the experimental group. 
The other group, which did not receive this 
additional instruction and did not engage 
in any self-affirmations, was the control 
group (Wood, Perunovic, & Lee, 2009). 
After several minutes (and 16 repeti- 
tions of the self-affirmation for the experi- 
mental group), both groups were then 
given new tests of their current level of 
self esteem and mood to see if changes 
occurred. The results clearly showed that 
a change did occur for students who had 
low self-esteem and who engaged in self- 
affirmations: their mood and self-esteem 
went down. This is, of course, the exact 


RETHINK 


opposite of the result that proponents of 
self-affirmation would intuitively expect. 
Instead, the study showed that not only 
were self-affirmations not helpful but that 
they actually seemed to backfire. 

Why might this happen? Interpreting 
these surprising findings is easier if we con- 
sider the findings of other, related research. 
If you consider these self-affirmations to be 
a form of self-persuasion, we can look at 
research on persuasion and find that people 
tend to be more persuaded by messages 
that are in line with their pre-existing atti- 
tudes. When a message is strongly contra- 
dictory to what a person already believes, 
he or she may counter that message with an 
argument. 

So when people who have low self- 
esteem tell themselves a contradictory 
message, such as “I’m a lovable person,” 
instead of being able to accept the message, 
they might think of times when they were 
not lovable because they acted unpleas- 
antly or were rejected by others. The self- 
affirmation might therefore backfire by 
causing people with low-self-esteem to lin- 
ger on negative thoughts that just confirm 
their poor self-image. 

These findings, which run counter to 
common-sense explanations, underscore 
the need to submit hypotheses to scientific 
testing. Even when the outcome seems 
“obvious,” we need to do research, because 
our intuitions are often simply wrong 
(Eagly & Chaiken, 1993). 





e Are you convinced by the experiment results that self-affirmations are not effective? 


Why or why not? 


e Could you design another experiment to test the same hypothesis? 





RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


questions about their behavior, thoughts, or attitudes. 
The case study is an in-depth interview and examination 
of one person or group. (p. 37) 

These descriptive research methods rely on correlational 
techniques, which describe associations between variables 
but cannot determine cause-and-effect relationships. (p. 39) 


47 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


48 Chapter 2 Psychological Research 


How do psychologists establish cause-and-effect relationships 
in research studies? 

e Ina formal experiment, the relationship between variables 
is investigated by deliberately producing a change— 
called the experimental manipulation—in one variable 
and observing changes in the other variable. (p. 41) 

e Inan experiment, at least two groups must be compared 
to assess cause-and-effect relationships. The group re- 
ceiving the treatment (the special procedure devised by 
the experimenter) is the experimental group; the second 
group (which receives no treatment) is the control group. 
There also may be multiple experimental groups, each 
of which is subjected to a different procedure and then 
compared with the others. (p. 41) 

e The variable that experimenters manipulate is the in- 
dependent variable. The variable that they measure 
and expect to change as a result of manipulation of the 
independent variable is called the dependent variable. 
(p. 42) 

e Ina formal experiment, participants must be assigned 
randomly to treatment conditions, so that participant 
characteristics are distributed evenly across the different 
conditions. (p. 43) 

e Psychologists use statistical tests to determine whether 
research findings are significant. (p. 45) 


EVALUATE 


1. Match the following forms of research to their definitions: 
1. archival research a. directly asking a sample of people 
2. naturalistic questions about their behavior 
observation b. examining existing records to 
3. survey research test a hypothesis 
4. case study c. looking at behavior in its true 
setting without intervening in 
the setting 
d. doing an in-depth investigation 
of a person or small group 


KEY TERMS 


archival research p. 37 
naturalistic 

observation p. 37 
survey research p. 38 
case study p. 38 


variables p. 39 
correlational research p. 39 
experiment p. 41 
experimental 
manipulation p. 41 


2. Match each of the following research methods with its pri- 
mary disadvantage: 
1. archival research 
2. naturalistic 


a. The researcher may not be able 
to generalize to the population 


observation at large. 
3. survey research b. People’s behavior can change if 
4. case study they know they are being watched. 


c. The data may not exist or may 
be unusable. 

d. People may lie in order to pre- 
sent a good image. 

3. A friend tells you, “Anxiety about speaking in public and 
performance are negatively correlated. Therefore, high 
anxiety must cause low performance.” Is this statement 
true or false, and why? 

4. A psychologist wants to study the effect of attractiveness 
on willingness to help a person with a math problem. 
Attractiveness would be the variable, and the 
amount of helping would be the variable. 

5. The group in an experiment that receives no treatment is 
called the group. 


RETHINK 


1. Can you describe how a researcher might use naturalistic 
observation, case studies, and survey research to investi- 
gate gender differences in aggressive behavior at the 
workplace? First state a hypothesis, and then describe 
your research approaches. What positive and negative 
features does each method have? 

. From a healthcare worker's perspective: Tobacco companies 
have asserted that no experiment has ever proved that to- 
bacco use causes cancer. Can you explain this claim in 
terms of the research procedures and designs discussed in 
this module? What sort of research would establish a 
cause-and-effect relationship between tobacco use and 
cancer? 


N 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 
oquo ‘g ‘Juapuadap ‘yuapuadaput ‘p 
‘sjajyo ƏSƏ} JO YJoq asnvo Aeur a[quITVA PIY} V IO ‘snorxue IIOU 
əwo2əq 0} afdoad əsnvə few sdueUTIOj1ad 100g 10440 ay} SƏSNVI JUO FLY} 
ULU JOU S20P pJ are sƏJqeueLa OM} asnedaq ysn{ 'uoyesne Adun 
OU SƏOP UOHPIaAIOD ‘as[ey `E “L-P ‘P-E “Q-Z -1 `T ‘P-Y “L-E “9-7 ‘G1 ‘T 


treatment p. 41 
experimental group p. 41 
control group p. 41 
independent variable p. 42 
dependent variable p. 42 


random assignment to 
condition p. 43 

significant outcome p. 45 

replicated research p. 46 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


You probably realize by now that there are few simple formulas for psychological 
research. Psychologists must make choices about the type of study to conduct, the 
measures to take, and the most effective way to analyze the results. Even after they 
have made these essential decisions, they must still consider several critical issues. 
We turn first to the most fundamental of these issues: ethics. 


The Ethics of Research 


Put yourself in the place of one of the participants in the experiment conducted by 
Latané and Darley to examine the helping behavior of bystanders, in which another 
“bystander” simulating a seizure turned out to be a confederate of the experimenters 
(Latané & Darley, 1970). How would you feel when you learned that the supposed 
victim was in reality a paid accomplice? 

Although you might at first experience relief that there had been no real emer- 
gency, you might also feel some resentment that you had been deceived by the 
experimenter. You might also experience concern that you had been placed in an 
embarrassing or compromising situation—one that might have dealt a blow to your 
self-esteem, depending on how you had behaved. 

Most psychologists argue that deception is sometimes necessary to prevent par- 
ticipants from being influenced by what they think a study’s true purpose is. (If you 
knew that Latané and Darley were actually studying your helping behavior, wouldn’t 
you automatically have been tempted to intervene in the emergency?) To avoid such 
outcomes, a small proportion of research involves deception. 

Nonetheless, because research has the potential to violate the rights of partici- 
pants, psychologists are expected to adhere to a strict set of ethical guidelines aimed 
at protecting participants (American Psychological Association, 2002). Those guide- 
lines involve the following safeguards: 





e Protection of participants from physical and mental harm. 

e The right of participants to privacy regarding their behavior. 

e The assurance that participation in research is completely voluntary. 

e The necessity of informing participants about the nature of procedures before 
their participation in the experiment. 


All experiments must be reviewed by an independent panel before being con- 
ducted, including the minority of studies that involve deception (Fisher, 2003; Fisher 
et al., 2002; Smith, 2003). 

One of psychologists’ key ethical principles is informed consent. Before par- 
ticipating in an experiment, the participants must sign a document affirming that 
they have been told the basic outlines of the study and are aware of what their 
participation will involve, what risks the experiment may hold, and the fact that 
their participation is purely voluntary and they may terminate it at any time. 


What major issues confront 
psychologists conducting 
research? 


> Study Alert 


Because the protection of 


experiment participants is 


essential, remember the 
key ethical guideline of 
informed consent. 


A document 
signed by participants affirming that 


they have been told the basic outlines 


of the study and are aware of what 
their participation will involve. 


49 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


50 Chapter 2 Psychological Research 





Although readily available and widely used as research subjects, college students may not 
represent the population at large. What are some advantages and drawbacks of using college 
students as subjects? 


Furthermore, after participation in a study, they must be given a debriefing in 
which they receive an explanation of the study and the procedures that were 
involved. The only time informed consent and a debriefing can be eliminated is 
in experiments in which the risks are minimal, as in a purely observational study 
in a public place (Barnett, Wise, & Johnson-Greene, 2007; Fallon, 2006; Koocher, 
Norcross, & Hill, 2005). 


Ex p | O ri n g When Latané and Darley, both college professors, decided who 
DIVERSITY would participate in their experiment, they turned to the people 


at hand: college students. Using college students as participants 
has both advantages and drawbacks. The big benefit is that 
because most research occurs in university settings, college 
students are readily available. Typically, they cost the researcher 
very little: They participate for either extra course credit or a 
relatively small payment. 

The problem is that college students may not represent the general population 
adequately. They tend to be younger and better educated than a significant percentage 
of the rest of the population of the United States. Compared with older adults, their 
attitudes are likely to be less well formed, and they are more apt to be influenced by 
authority figures and peers (Sears, 1986). 

College students are also disproportionately white and middle class. However, 
even in research that does not involve college students, participants are often white, 
middle-class participants; the use of African Americans, Latinos, Asians, and other 
minorities as participants is relatively low (Graham, 1992; Guthrie, 1998). Because 


Choosing Participants Who Represent the 
Scope of Human Behavior 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 6 Critical Research Issues 


The Importance 


of Using Representative Participants 





FIGURE 1 Choosing a diverse set of participants is important in research studies, because 
personal characteristics are reflected in participants’ responses as basic as brain activity. In 
these graphs, we can see that the level of education of a child's mother influences a child's 
ability to pay attention. In the study, scientists recorded electrical brain activity. Children 
whose mothers had a higher level of education show greater brain activation when they were 
attending to a story than when they were ignoring the story, compared with children whose 
mothers had a lower level of education. (Source: Stevens et al., 2009.) 


High Maternal Education 














FT74 




















Attended 


EAEE ATETT Unattended 


Low Maternal Education 



































Attended 


AEOS ENDENRRNEN Unattended 


psychology is a science whose goal is to explain human behavior generally, its 
studies must use participants who are fully representative of the general population 
in terms of gender, age, race, ethnicity, socioeconomic status, and educational level 
(see also Figure 1). To encourage a wider range of participants, the National Institute 
of Mental Health and the National Science Foundation—the primary U.S. funding 
sources for psychological research—now require that experiments address issues of 
diverse populations (Carpenter, 2002; Lindley, 2006). 


51 


52 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 2 Psychological Research 





Research involving animals is controversial but, when conducted within ethical guidelines, 
yields significant benefits for humans. 


Should Animals Be Used 
in Research? 


Like those who work with humans, researchers who use nonhuman animals in exper- 
iments have their own set of exacting guidelines to ensure that the animals do not 
suffer. Specifically, researchers must make every effort to minimize discomfort, ill- 
ness, and pain. Procedures that subject animals to distress are permitted only when 
an alternative procedure is unavailable and when the research is justified by its pro- 
spective value. Moreover, researchers strive to avoid causing physical discomfort, but 
they are also required to promote the psychological well-being of some species of 
research animals, such as primates (Auer et al., 2007; Lutz & Novak, 2005; Rusche, 
2003). 

But why should animals be used for research in the first place? Is it really pos- 
sible to learn about human behavior from the results of research employing rats, 
gerbils, and pigeons? 

The answer is that psychological research that does employ nonhumans is 
designed to answer questions different from those posed in research with humans. 
For example, the shorter life span of animals (rats live an average of two years) 
allows researchers to learn about the effects of aging in a relatively short time frame. 
It is also possible to provide greater experimental control over nonhumans and to carry 
out procedures that might not be possible with people. For example, some studies 
require large numbers of participants that share similar backgrounds or have been 
exposed to particular environments—conditions that could not practically be met with 
human beings. 

Research with animals has provided psychologists with information that has 
profoundly benefited humans. For instance, it furnished the keys to detecting eye 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


disorders in children early enough to prevent permanent damage, to communicating 
more effectively with severely retarded children, and to reducing chronic pain in 
people. Still, the use of research using nonhumans is controversial, involving com- 
plex moral and philosophical concerns. Consequently, all research involving non- 
humans must be carefully reviewed beforehand to ensure that it is conducted ethically 
(Hackam, 2007; Saucier & Cain, 2006; Shankar & Simmons, 2009). 


Threats to Experimental Validity: 
Avoiding Experimental Bias 


Even the best-laid experimental plans are susceptible to experimental bias—factors 
that distort the way the independent variable affects the dependent variable in an 
experiment. One of the most common forms of experimental bias is experimenter 
expectations: An experimenter unintentionally transmits cues to participants about the 
way they are expected to behave in a given experimental condition. The danger is 
that those expectations will bring about an “appropriate” behavior—one that other- 
wise might not have occurred (Rosenthal, 2002, 2003). 

A related problem is participant expectations about appropriate behavior. If you 
have ever been a participant in an experiment, you know that you quickly develop 
guesses about what is expected of you. In fact, it is typical for people to develop 
their own hypotheses about what the experimenter hopes to learn from the study. 
If participants form their own hypotheses, it may be the participant’s expectations, 
rather than the experimental manipulation, that produce an effect (Rutherford et al., 
2009). 

To guard against participant expectations biasing the results of an experiment, 
the experimenter may try to disguise the true purpose of the experiment. Participants 
who do not know that helping behavior is being studied, for example, are more apt 
to act in a “natural” way than they would if they knew. 

Sometimes it is impossible to hide the actual purpose of research; when that is 
the case, other techniques are available to prevent bias. Suppose you were interested 
in testing the ability of a new drug to alleviate the symptoms of severe depression. 
If you simply gave the drug to half your participants and not to the other half, the 
participants who were given the drug might report feeling less depressed, merely 
because they knew they were getting a drug. Similarly, the participants who got 
nothing might report feeling no better, because they knew that they were in a no- 
treatment control group. 

To solve this problem, psychologists typically use a procedure in which all the 
participants receive a treatment, but those in the control group receive only a 
placebo—a false treatment, such as a pill, “drug,” or other substance that has no 
significant chemical properties or active ingredient. Because members of both groups 
are kept in the dark about whether they are getting a real or a false treatment, any 
differences in outcome can be attributed to the quality of the drug and not to the 
possible psychological effects of being administered a pill or other substance (Crum 
& Langer, 2007; Rajagopal, 2006). 

However, there is one more safeguard that a careful researcher must apply in an 
experiment such as this one. To overcome the possibility that experimenter expecta- 
tions will affect the participant, the person who administers the drug shouldn’t know 
whether it is actually the true drug or the placebo. By keeping both the participant 
and the experimenter who interacts with the participant “blind” to the nature of the 
drug that is being administered, researchers can more accurately assess the effects of 
the drug. This method is known as the double-blind procedure. 





Module 6 Critical Research Issues 53 


experimental bias Factors that distort 
how the independent variable affects the 
dependent variable in an experiment. 


y Alert 


Learn the main types of po- 
tential bias in experiments: 
experimenter expecta- 
tions, participant expecta- 
tions, and placebo effects. 


placebo A false treatment, such as a 
pill, “drug,” or other substance, 
without any significant chemical 
properties or active ingredient. 


54 


Chapter 2 Psychological Research 


BECOMING AN 


INFORMED CONSUMER 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


© Donald Reilly/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 





If you were about to purchase an automobile, you would not 
likely stop at the nearest car dealership and drive off with the 
first car a salesperson recommended. Instead, you would 


of Psychology probably mull over the purchase, read about automobiles, 


Thinking Critically About Research 


consider the alternatives, talk to others about their experiences, 
and ultimately put in a fair amount of thought before you made 
such a major purchase. 

In contrast, many of us are considerably less conscientious when we expend our 


intellectual, rather than financial, assets. People often jump to conclusions on the 
basis of incomplete and inaccurate information, and only rarely do they take the time 
to critically evaluate the research and data to which they are exposed. 


Because the field of psychology is based on an accumulated body of research, we 


must scrutinize thoroughly the methods, results, and claims of researchers. Several 
basic questions can help us sort through what is valid and what is not. Among the most 
important questions to ask are these: 


e What was the purpose of the research? Research studies should evolve from a clearly 


specified theory. Furthermore, we must take into account the specific hypothesis 
that is being tested. Unless we know what hypothesis is being examined, we 
cannot judge how successful a study has been. 

How well was the study conducted? Consider who the participants were, how many 
were involved, what methods were employed, and what problems the researcher 
encountered in collecting the data. There are important differences, for example, 
between a case study that reports the anecdotes of a handful of respondents and 
a survey that collects data from several thousand people. 

Are the results presented fairly? Statements must be assessed on the basis of the 
actual data they reflect and their logic. For instance, when the manufacturer of 
car X boasts that “no other car has a better safety record than car X,” this does not 
mean that car X is safer than every other car. It just means that no other car has 
been proved safer, though many other cars could be just as safe as car X. 
Expressed in the latter fashion, the finding doesn’t seem worth bragging about. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 6 Critical Research Issues 55 


These three basic questions can help you assess the validity of research findings you 
come across—both within and outside the field of psychology. The more you know 
how to evaluate research in general, the better you will be able to assess what the field 
of psychology has to offer. 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 3. Deception is one means experimenters can use to try to 
eliminate participants’ expectations. True or false? 


ae Ota eaves a onion py olope an e 4. A false treatment, such as a pill that has no significant 


research? 


e One of the key ethical principles followed by psycholo- 
gists is that of informed consent. Participants must be in- 
formed, before participation, about the basic outline of 
the experiment and the risks and potential benefits of 
their participation. (p. 49) 

e Although the use of college students as participants 
has the advantage of easy availability, there are draw- 
backs, too. For instance, students do not necessarily 
represent the population as a whole. The use of non- 
human animals as participants may also have costs in 
terms of the ability to generalize to humans, although 
the benefits of using animals in research have been 
profound. (p. 51) 

e Experiments are subject to a number of biases, or threats. 
Experimenter expectations can produce bias when an ex- 
perimenter unintentionally transmits cues to participants 
about her or his expectations regarding their behavior in 
a given experimental condition. Participant expectations 
can also bias an experiment. Among the tools experi- 
menters use to help eliminate bias are placebos and 
double-blind procedures. (p. 52) 


EVALUATE 


1. Ethical research begins with the concept of informed con- 
sent. Before signing up to participate in an experiment, 
participants should be informed of: 

a. the procedure of the study, stated generally. 
b. the risks that may be involved. 

c. their right to withdraw at any time. 

d. all of these. 

. List three benefits of using animals in psychological 
research. 


chemical properties or active ingredient, is known as a 


A study has shown that men differ from women in their 
preference for ice cream flavors. This study was based on 
a sample of two men and three women. What might be 
wrong with this study? 


RETHINK 


1. A researcher strongly believes that college professors tend 


to show female students less attention and respect in the 
classroom than they show male students. He sets up an 
experimental study involving observations of classrooms 
in different conditions. In explaining the study to the pro- 
fessors and the students who will participate, what steps 
should the researcher take to eliminate experimental bias 
based on both experimenter expectations and participant 
expectations? 


. From a research analyst's perspective: You are hired to study 


people’s attitudes toward welfare programs by developing 
and circulating a questionnaire via the Internet. Is this 
study likely to accurately reflect the views of the general 
population? Why or why not? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


‘Japuas uo paseq saduaraserd wear aor ynoge 


UMeIP aq UR suorsn puo? prea ou ‘azdures 19312] e Noy ‘syuedtoynsred 


MƏ} 00} IVJ are atau], 'g foqəejd *p ‘ans 'g ‘atdoad YM UVI ƏM uey} 
‘ueds aft] IƏJI0YS IY} Jo asnedaq ‘speumTue UT ATISea IIOU YONU SPƏJJƏ 
Teuogerauə3 ye YOoT uL? ƏM (E) ‘pauTe}qo ATISea aq ued syuednyied 
Tepus JO sIaqunu ZIET (Z) ‘s1OJVF IYƏUƏZ pure TeJUIUUONAUƏ JIAO 
T01}U02 1972313 JAVY ƏM spəlqns TeuTTUR UTM əsnvəəq 'əjdoəd ut ues am 
uey} Ájrseə a1our sjewtue ur euowouoyd autos Apnys ued ƏM (1) ‘z ‘PT 


KEY TERMS 


informed consent p. 49 
experimental bias p. 52 
placebo p. 53 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


This page intentionally left blank 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Psychology on the Web 


1. Identify a product or a service that is advertised on the Internet using broad, unspe- 
cific claims, such as a weight-loss formula or a body-building method. Find at least 
two advertisements on the Internet for that product or service, and evaluate the 
claims they make according to the principles discussed in this group of modules. 
Summarize the evidence that is presented for those claims, and describe a method 
by which you might confirm the claims by using actual research. 

2. Find a website that focuses on an important social issue (for example, urban violence, 
gender differences in hiring or promotion, poverty), and locate descriptions of a 
research study about the issue. Evaluate the study by identifying the hypotheses that 
were tested, the methods used to test them, and the validity of the results that were 
reported. 


E D | log U Q We have been discussing the ways in which psycholo- 


gists seek to understand phenomena and answer 
questions of interest. We examined the scientific method and its reliance on posing 
good questions, creating productive theories, and crafting testable hypotheses. We also 
looked at the basic methods psychologists use to conduct research studies and com- 
pared correlational methods and experimental methods. Finally, we explored some of 
the major challenges that psychologists have to deal with when conducting research, 
including ethical considerations, the use of animals in research, and potential bias. 

Before leaving this topic, reconsider the lack of bystander help in the case of 
78-year-old Angel Torres, who was hit by a car and lay in the road, receiving no help 
from bystanders. Reflect on the following questions in light of what you now know 
about conducting psychological research. 





1. Suppose you were interested in studying why a hit-and-run driver (like the one who 
hit Torres) would leave the scene of an accident. What theory might you construct to 
explain such behavior? 

2. Design a correlational study to test one of your hypotheses. Which correlational 
method(s) (archival research, naturalistic observation, survey research, case study) 
would you use in your study? 

3. Design an experimental study to test the same or another hypothesis. Describe the 
experiment, including the participants, the experimental manipulation, the treatment, 
and the independent and dependent variables. 





57 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Key Concepts for Chapter 3 






Why do psychologists study the brain and 


Neurons: The Basic Elements 


of Behavior 
the nervous system? @ What are the basic 


The Structure of the Neuron 
elements of the nervous system? @ How How Neurons Fire 
Where Neurons Meet: Bridging the Gap 


does the nervous system communicate Neurotransmitters: Multitalented 


electrical and chemical messages from one part nemi a e OUNT 


to another? 





MODULE 8 






How are the structures of the nervous system The Nervous System 


, , and the Endocrine System: 
linked? @ How does the endocrine system affect Communicating Within the Body 


behavior? he Nervous System: Linking Neurons 


The Evolutionary Foundations of the 
Nervous System 





The Endocrine System: Of Chemicals 
and Glands 


MODULE 9 





The Brain 


Studying the Brain's Structure 
and Functions: Spying on the Brain 


How do researchers identify the major parts 


and functions of the brain? @ What are the 


Applying Psychology in the 21st Century: 
Mind over Cursor: Harnessing 
Brainpower to Improve Lives 

The Central Core: Our “Old Brain” 

The Limbic System: 


@ How can an understanding of the nervous system help us find Beyondiihe Central Core 
The Cerebral Cortex: Our “New Brain” 


major parts of the brain, and for what 
behaviors is each part responsible? @ How do 


the halves of the brain operate interdependently? 





ways to alleviate disease and pain? PsychWork: Rehabilitation Counselor 


Neuroplasticity and the Brain 


Neuroscience in Your Life: 
The Plastic Brain 


[he Specialization of the Hemispheres: 
Two Brains or One? 





Exploring Diversity: Human 
Diversity and the Brain 





The Split Brain: Exploring 

the Two Hemispheres 

Becoming an Informed Consumer 

of Psychology: Learning to Control Your 
Heart—and Mind—Through Biofeedback 


59 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Prologue Brain Diet 





Carol Poe, a 60-year-old grandmother from West 
Virginia, was the second person to receive a new 
obesity treatment called deep brain stimulation. 
~ The procedure involves inserting electrodes into 
the brain to deliver tiny bursts of electricity to 
alter the patient’s behavior. With obese patients, 
the idea is to target the. hypothalamus, the area of 
the brain that controls our desire to eat. It works by 
making the patient feel full. à 







During the surgery, Carol was asked if she felt hungry or not 
to help pinpoint the correct position for the electrodes. “I was 
actually able to experience feelings of hunger and of fullness 
while the neurosurgeon experimented with the best place to put 
the electrodes,” Carol explained. “Once the electrodes were in the 
right place, my desire to eat went away. It was amazing going 
from feeling hungry to feeling full. I’m delighted with what's 
happened so far. Now I’m hoping to start losing some serious 
weight.’ (Halle, 2009) 





Carol Poe had an experience that is difficult even to imagine: She 
experienced physiological sensations of hunger and fullness that 
were triggered by direct stimulation of her brain, rather than by 
the parts of the body where food was being digested. 

The ability of surgeons to identify and stimulate such specific 
areas of the brain is little short of miraculous. The greater miracle, 
though, is the brain itself. An organ roughly half the size of a loaf 
of bread, the brain controls our behavior through every waking 
and sleeping moment. Our movements, thoughts, hopes, 
aspirations, dreams—our very awareness that we are human—all 
depend on the brain and the nerves that extend throughout the 
body, constituting the nervous system. 

Because of the importance of the nervous system in control- 
ling behavior, and because humans at their most basic level are 
biological beings, many researchers in psychology and other 
fields as diverse as computer science, zoology, and medicine 
have made the biological underpinnings of behavior their 
specialty. These experts collectively are called neuroscientists 
(Beatty, 2000; Cartwright, 2006; Gazzaniga, Ivry, & Mangun, 2002; 
Posner & DiGiorlamo, 2000). 

Psychologists who specialize in considering the ways in which 
the biological structures and functions of the body affect 
behavior are known as behavioral neuroscientists (or biopsy- 
chologists). They seek to answer several key questions: How does 


60 


the brain control the voluntary and involuntary functioning of 
the body? How does the brain communicate with other parts 

of the body? What is the physical structure of the brain, and how 
does this structure affect behavior? Are psychological disorders 
caused by biological factors, and how can such disorders be 
treated? 

As you consider the biological processes that we discuss in 
this chapter, keep in mind the reason why behavioral neuro- 
science is an essential part of psychology: Our understanding of 
human behavior requires knowledge of the brain and other 
parts of the nervous system. Biological factors are central to our 
sensory experiences, states of consciousness, motivation and 
emotion, development throughout the life span, and physical 
and psychological health. Furthermore, advances in behavioral 
neuroscience have led to the creation of drugs and other treat- 
ments for psychological and physical disorders. In short, we 
cannot understand behavior without understanding our biological 
makeup (Compagni & Manderscheid, 2006; Kosslyn et al., 2002; 
Plomin, 2003). 


behavioral neuroscientists (or biopsychologists) Psychologists who 
specialize in considering the ways in which the biological structures 
and functions of the body affect behavior. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 7 


Watching Serena Williams hit a stinging backhand, Dario Vaccaro dance a complex 
ballet routine, or Derek Jeter swing at a baseball, you may have marveled at the 
complexity—and wondrous abilities—of the human body. But even the most every- 
day tasks, such as pouring a cup of coffee or humming a tune, depend on a sophis- 
ticated sequence of events in the body that is itself truly impressive. 

The nervous system is the pathway for the instructions that permit our bodies 
to carry out such precise activities. Here we look at the structure and function of 
neurons, the cells that make up the nervous system, including the brain. 


The Structure of the Neuron 


Playing the piano, driving a car, or hitting a tennis ball depends, at one level, on 
exact muscle coordination. But if we consider how the muscles can be activated so 
precisely, we see that more fundamental processes are involved. For the muscles to 
produce the complex movements that make up any meaningful physical activity, the 
brain has to provide the right messages to them and coordinate those messages. 

Such messages—as well as those which enable us to think, remember, and 
experience emotion—are passed through specialized cells called neurons. Neurons, 
or nerve cells, are the basic elements of the nervous system. Their quantity is 
staggering—perhaps as many as 1 trillion neurons throughout the body are involved 
in the control of behavior (Boahen, 2005). 

Although there are several types of neurons, they all have a similar structure, as 
illustrated in Figure 1. Like most cells in the body, neurons have a cell body that con- 
tains a nucleus. The nucleus incorporates the hereditary material that determines how 
a cell will function. Neurons are physically held in place by glial cells. Glial cells provide 
nourishment to neurons, insulate them, help repair damage, and generally support 
neural functioning (Bassotti et al., 2007; Fields, 2004; Kettenmann & Ransom, 2005). 

In contrast to most other cells, however, neurons have a distinctive feature: the 
ability to communicate with other cells and transmit information across relatively 
long distances. Many of the body’s neurons receive signals from the environment or 
relay the nervous system’s messages to muscles and other target cells, but the vast 
majority of neurons communicate only with other neurons in the elaborate informa- 
tion system that regulates behavior. 

As shown in Figure 1, a neuron has a cell body with a cluster of fibers called 
dendrites at one end. Those fibers, which look like the twisted branches of a tree, 
receive messages from other neurons. On the opposite side of the cell body is a long, 
slim, tubelike extension called an axon. The axon carries messages received by the 
dendrites to other neurons. The axon is considerably longer than the rest of the 
neuron. Although most axons are several millimeters in length, some are as long as 
3 feet. Axons end in small bulges called terminal buttons, which send messages to 
other neurons. 

The messages that travel through a neuron are electrical in nature. Although 
there are exceptions, those electrical messages, or impulses, generally move across 
neurons in one direction only, as if they were traveling on a one-way street. Impulses 





Why do psychologists study 
the brain and the nervous 
system? 


What are the basic elements 
of the nervous system? 


How does the nervous system 
communicate electrical and 
chemical messages from one 
part to another? 


Nerve cells, the basic 
elements of the nervous system. 


A cluster of fibers at one end 
of a neuron that receives messages 
from other neurons. 


The part of the neuron that 
carries messages destined for other 
neurons. 


Small bulges at the 
end of axons that send messages to 
other neurons. 


61 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


62 Chapter 3 Neuroscience and Behavior 


á 7 Dendrites 


Cell body 






idy Alert 


Remember that dendrites 
detect messages from other 
neurons; axons carry 
signals away from the cell 
body. 


myelin sheath A protective coat of fat 


and protein that wraps around the axon. 


all-or-none law The rule that neurons 
are either on or off. 


resting state The state in which there 
is a negative electrical charge of about 
-70 millivolts within a neuron. 


Myelin sheath 






Axon (inside myelin sheath) 


—S Terminal buttons 


FIGURE 1 The primary components of the specialized cell called the neuron, the basic element 
of the nervous system. A neuron, like most types of cells in the body, has a cell body, but it also 
contains structures that carry messages: the dendrites, which receive messages from other 
neurons, and the axon, which carries messages to other neurons or body cells. In this neuron, 
as in most neurons, the axon is protected by the sausage-like myelin sheath. What advantages 
does the treelike structure of the neuron provide? 


follow a route that begins with the dendrites, continues into the cell body, and leads 
ultimately along the tubelike extension, the axon, to adjacent neurons. 

To prevent messages from short-circuiting one another, axons must be insulated 
in some fashion (just as electrical wires must be insulated). Most axons are insulated 
by a myelin sheath, a protective coating of fat and protein that wraps around the 
axon like the casing on links of sausage. 

The myelin sheath also serves to increase the velocity with which electrical 
impulses travel through axons. Those axons that carry the most important and 
most urgently required information have the greatest concentrations of myelin. If 
your hand touches a painfully hot stove, for example, the information regarding 
the pain is passed through axons in the hand and arm that have a relatively thick 
coating of myelin, speeding the message of pain to the brain so that you can react 
instantly. 


How Neurons Fire 


Like a gun, neurons either fire—that is, transmit an electrical impulse along the 
axon—or don’t fire. There is no in-between stage, just as pulling harder on a gun 
trigger doesn’t make the bullet travel faster. Similarly, neurons follow an all-or-none 
law: They are either on or off, with nothing in between the on state and the off state. 
Once there is enough force to pull the trigger, a neuron fires. 

Before a neuron is triggered—that is, when it is in a resting state—it has a 
negative electrical charge of about -70 millivolts (a millivolt is one 14,00 of a volt). 
This charge is caused by the presence of more negatively charged ions within the 
neuron than outside it. (An ion is an atom that is electrically charged.) You might 
think of the neuron as a miniature battery in which the inside of the neuron repre- 
sents the negative pole and the outside represents the positive pole. 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 7 Neurons: The Basic Elements of Behavior 63 





Time 3 


J \ s— Voltage 


—=—_—___— 


= Positive charge 


Negative charge 


==» Direction of impulse 


When a message arrives at a neuron, gates along the cell membrane open 
briefly to allow positively charged ions to rush in at rates as high as 100 million 
ions per second. The sudden arrival of these positive ions causes the charge 
within the nearby part of the cell to change momentarily from negative to posi- 
tive. When the positive charge reaches a critical level, the “trigger” is pulled, and 
an electrical impulse, known as an action potential, travels along the axon of the 
neuron (see Figure 2). 

The action potential moves from one end of the axon to the other like a flame 
moving along a fuse. As the impulse travels along the axon, the movement of ions 
causes a change in charge from negative to positive in successive sections of the axon 
(see Figure 3) on page 64. After the impulse has passed through a particular section 
of the axon, positive ions are pumped out of that section, and its charge returns to 
negative while the action potential continues to move along the axon. 

Just after an action potential has passed through a section of the axon, the cell 
membrane in that region cannot admit positive ions again for a few milliseconds, 
and so a neuron cannot fire again immediately no matter how much stimulation it 
receives. It is as if the gun has to be reloaded after each shot. There then follows a 
period in which, though it is possible for the neuron to fire, a stronger stimulus is 
needed than would be if the neuron had reached its normal resting state. Eventually, 
though, the neuron is ready to fire once again. 

These complex events can occur at dizzying speeds, although there is great 
variation among different neurons. The particular speed at which an action poten- 
tial travels along an axon is determined by the axon’s size and the thickness of 
its myelin sheath. Axons with small diameters carry impulses at about 2 miles 
per hour; longer and thicker ones can average speeds of more than 225 miles per 
hour. 

Neurons differ not only in terms of how quickly an impulse moves along the 
axon but also in their potential rate of firing. Some neurons are capable of firing 
as many as 1,000 times per second; others fire at much slower rates. The intensity 
of a stimulus determines how much of a neuron’s potential firing rate is reached. 
A strong stimulus, such as a bright light or a loud sound, leads to a higher rate of 
firing than a less intense stimulus does. Thus, even though all impulses move at 
the same strength or speed through a particular axon—because of the all-or-none 


FIGURE 2 Movement of an action 
potential along an axon. Just before 
Time 1, positively charged ions enter 
the cell membrane, changing the charge 
in the nearby part of the axon from 
negative to positive and triggering an 
action potential. The action potential 
travels along the axon, as illustrated in 
the changes occurring from Time 1 to 
Time 3 (from top to bottom in this 
drawing). Immediately after the action 
potential has passed through a section 
of the axon, positive ions are pumped 
out, restoring the charge in that section 
to negative. The change in voltage 
illustrated at the top of the axon can be 
seen in greater detail in Figure 3. 
(Source: Stevens, 1979.) 


action potential An electric nerve 
impulse that travels through a 
neuron’s axon when it is set off by 
a “trigger,” changing the neuron’s 
charge from negative to positive. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


64 Chapter 3 Neuroscience and Behavior 


Neuron 


cou 
` 






Di 


Outside 
axon 


mirror neurons Specialized neurons 
that fire not only when a person enacts 
a particular behavior, but also when a 
person simply observes another 
individual carrying out the same 
behavior. 





50 
40 
o A sudden, brief reversal 
= 30 of charge within the 
Outside 2 neuron results in an 
axon S action potential. 
E 0 
v 
5 
$ 
i | 
HES g 
estin, 
Bi D ON 
K O-A 








Time —~ 


FIGURE 3 Changes in the electrical charge in a neuron during the passage of an action 
potential. In its normal resting state, a neuron has a negative charge of about -70 millivolts. 
When an action potential is triggered, however, the charge becomes positive, increasing from 
around —70 millivolts to about +40 millivolts. Following the passage of the action potential, the 
charge becomes even more negative than it is in its typical resting state. It is not until the charge 
returns to its normal resting state that the neuron will be fully ready to be triggered once again. 


law—there is variation in the frequency of impulses, providing a mechanism by 
which we can distinguish the tickle of a feather from the weight of someone stand- 
ing on our toes. 


MIRROR NEURONS 


Although all neurons operate through the firing of action potentials, there is signifi- 
cant specialization among different types of neurons. For example, in the last decade, 
neuroscientists have discovered the existence of mirror neurons, neurons that fire 
not only when a person enacts a particular behavior but also when a person simply 
observes another individual carrying out the same behavior (Falck-Ytter, 2006; Lepage 
& Theoret, 2007; Schulte-Ruther et al., 2007). 

Mirror neurons may help explain how (and why) humans have the capacity to 
understand others’ intentions. Specifically, mirror neurons may fire when we view 
someone doing something, helping us to predict what their goals are and what they 
may do next. 

The discovery of mirror neurons suggests that the capacity of even young 
children to imitate others may be an inborn behavior. Furthermore, mirror neurons 
may be at the root of empathy—those feelings of concern, compassion, and sympa- 
thy for others—and even the development of language in humans (Iacoboni, 2009; 
Ramachandra, 2009; Triesch, Jasso, & Deak, 2007). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 7 Neurons: The Basic Elements of Behavior 65 


Where Neurons Meet: 
Bridging the Gap 


If you have looked inside a computer, you’ve seen that each part is physically con- 
nected to another part. In contrast, evolution has produced a neural transmission 
system that at some points has no need for a structural connection between its com- 
ponents. Instead, a chemical connection bridges the gap, known as a synapse, between 
two neurons (see Figure 4). The synapse is the space between two neurons where 
the axon of a sending neuron communicates with the dendrites of a receiving neuron 





synapse The space between two 
neurons where the axon of a sending 
neuron communicates with the 
dendrites of a receiving neuron by 
using chemical messages. 


by using chemical messages (Dean & Dresbach, 2006; Fanselow & Poulos, 2005). neurotransmitters Chemicals that 
When a nerve impulse comes to the end of the axon and reaches a terminal carry messages across the synapse to 
button, the terminal button releases a chemical courier called a neurotransmitter. the dendrite (and sometimes the cell 


Neurotransmitters are chemicals that carry messages across the synapse to a dendrite body) of a receiver neuron. 


Step l: Neurotransmitters Step 3: Neurotransmitters 
are produced and stored travel across the synapse to 


in the axon. receptor sites on another 
neuron’s dendrite. 







Neurotransmitter 


~~ 


© o “4 = 


Receptor site 









Step 2: If an action potential | Step 4: When a neurotransmitter 

arrives, the axon releases 4 fits into a receptor site, it delivers 

neurotransmitters. an excitatory or inhibitory message. 
If enough excitatory messages are 
delivered, the neuron will fire. 







: Dendri Á 
Neurotransmitter endrite ‘ 


Neurotransmitter 
S 
N 


(a) (b) 
FIGURE 4 A synapse is the junction between an axon and a dendrite. The gap between 

the axon and the dendrite is bridged by chemicals called neurotransmitters (Mader, 2000). In 

(a) read Step 1 through Step 4 to follow this process. (b) Just as the pieces of a jigsaw puzzle 

can fit in only one specific location in a puzzle, each kind of neurotransmitter has a distinctive 
configuration that allows it to fit into a specific type of receptor cell (Johnson, 2000). Why is it 
advantageous for axons and dendrites to be linked by temporary chemical bridges rather 

than by the hard wiring typical of a radio connection or telephone hookup? 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


66 Chapter 3 Neuroscience and Behavior 


y Alert 
Remember this key fact: 
Messages inside neurons 
are transmitted in electrical 
form, whereas messages 
traveling between neurons 
travel via chemical means. 


excitatory message A chemical 
message that makes it more likely 
that a receiving neuron will fire and 
an action potential will travel down 
its axon. 


inhibitory message A chemical 
message that prevents or decreases 
the likelihood that a receiving neuron 
will fire. 


reuptake The reabsorption of neuro- 
transmitters by a terminal button. 


(and sometimes the cell body) of a receiving neuron. Like a boat that ferries pas- 
sengers across a river, these chemical messengers move toward the shorelines of 
other neurons. The chemical mode of message transmission that occurs between neu- 
rons is strikingly different from the means by which communication occurs inside 
neurons: Although messages travel in electrical form within a neuron, they move 
between neurons through a chemical transmission system. 

There are several types of neurotransmitters, and not all neurons are capable of 
receiving the chemical message carried by a particular neurotransmitter. In the same 
way that a jigsaw puzzle piece can fit in only one specific location in a puzzle, each 
kind of neurotransmitter has a distinctive configuration that allows it to fit into a 
specific type of receptor site on the receiving neuron (see Figure 4b). It is only when 
a neurotransmitter fits precisely into a receptor site that successful chemical com- 
munication is possible. 

If a neurotransmitter does fit into a site on the receiving neuron, the chemical 
message it delivers is basically one of two types: excitatory or inhibitory. Excitatory 
messages make it more likely that a receiving neuron will fire and an action poten- 
tial will travel down its axon. Inhibitory messages, in contrast, do just the opposite; 
they provide chemical information that prevents or decreases the likelihood that the 
receiving neuron will fire. 

Because the dendrites of a neuron receive both excitatory and inhibitory mes- 
sages simultaneously, the neuron must integrate the messages by using a kind of 
chemical calculator. Put simply, if the excitatory messages (“Fire!”) outnumber the 
inhibitory ones (“Don’t fire!”), the neuron fires. In contrast, if the inhibitory messages 
outnumber the excitatory ones, nothing happens, and the neuron remains in its rest- 
ing state (Flavell et al., 2006; Mel, 2002; Rapport, 2005). 

If neurotransmitters remained at the site of the synapse, receiving neurons would 
be awash in a continual chemical bath, producing constant stimulation or constant 
inhibition of the receiving neurons—and effective communication across the synapse 
would no longer be possible. To solve this problem, neurotransmitters are either 
deactivated by enzymes or—more commonly—treabsorbed by the terminal button in 
an example of chemical recycling called reuptake. Like a vacuum cleaner sucking up 
dust, neurons reabsorb the neurotransmitters that are now clogging the synapse. All 
this activity occurs at lightning speed, with the process taking just several millisec- 
onds (Helmuth, 2000; Holt & Jahn, 2004). 

Our understanding of the process of reuptake has permitted the development of 
a number of drugs used in the treatment of psychological disorders. As we discuss 
later in the book, some antidepressant drugs, called SSRIs, or selective serotonin reup- 
take inhibitors, permit certain neurotransmitters to remain active for a longer period 
at certain synapses in the brain, thereby reducing the symptoms of depression (Mont- 
gomery, 2006; Ramos, 2006). 


Neurotransmitters: 
Multitalented Chemical Couriers 


Neurotransmitters are a particularly important link between the nervous system and 
behavior. Not only are they important for maintaining vital brain and body functions, 
a deficiency or an excess of a neurotransmitter can produce severe behavior dis- 
orders. More than a hundred chemicals have been found to act as neurotransmitters, 
and neuroscientists believe that more may ultimately be identified (Penney, 2000; 
Schmidt, 2006). 

Neurotransmitters vary significantly in terms of how strong their concentration 
must be to trigger a neuron to fire. Furthermore, the effects of a particular neurotrans- 
mitter vary, depending on the area of the nervous system in which it is produced. 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Module 7 Neurons: The Basic Elements of Behavior 67 
Neurotransmitter 
Name Location Effect Function 
Acetylcholine (ACh) Brain, spinal cord, peripheral Excitatory in brain and Muscle movement, 
nervous system, especially autonomic nervous cognitive functioning 
some organs of the system; inhibitory 
parasympathetic nervous elsewhere 
system 
Glutamate Brain, spinal cord Excitatory Memory 
Dopaminepatnways Gamma-amino Brain, spinal cord Main inhibitory Eating, aggression, sleeping 
butyric acid (GABA) neurotransmitter 
Serotonin pathways Dopamine (DA) Brain Inhibitory or excitatory Movement control, 
pleasure and reward, 
attention 
Serotonin Brain, spinal cord Inhibitory Sleeping, eating, mood, 
pain, depression 
Endorphins Brain, spinal cord Primarily inhibitory, Pain suppression, 


except in hippocampus pleasurable feelings, 
appetites, placebos 





FIGURE 5 Major neurotransmitters. 


The same neurotransmitter, then, can act as an excitatory message to a neuron located 
in one part of the brain and can inhibit firing in neurons located in another part. 
(The major neurotransmitters and their effects are described in Figure 5.) 

One of the most common neurotransmitters is acetylcholine (or ACh, its chemical 
symbol), which is found throughout the nervous system. ACh is involved in our 
every move, because—among other things—it transmits messages relating to our 
skeletal muscles. ACh is also involved in memory capabilities, and diminished pro- 
duction of ACh may be related to Alzheimer’s disease (Bazalakova et al., 2007; Mohapel 
et al., 2005). 

Another common excitatory neurotransmitter, glutamate, plays a role in memory. 
Memories appear to be produced by specific biochemical changes at particular syn- 
apses, and glutamate, along with other neurotransmitters, plays an important role in 
this process (Carvalho, 2006; Riedel, Platt, & Micheau, 2003; Winters & Bussey, 2005). 

Gamma-amino butyric acid (GABA), which is found in both the brain and the spi- 
nal cord, appears to be the nervous system’s primary inhibitory neurotransmitter. It 
moderates a variety of behaviors, ranging from eating to aggression. Several common 
substances, such as the tranquilizer Valium and alcohol, are effective because they 
permit GABA to operate more efficiently (Akirav, Raizel, & Maroun, 2006; Ball, 2004; 
Criswell et al., 2008; Lobo & Harris, 2008). 

Another major neurotransmitter is dopamine (DA), which is involved in move- 
ment, attention, and learning. The discovery that certain drugs can have a significant TH SAS SAIL 1 POREO 
effect on dopamine release has led to the development of effective treatments fora | FOX aries 
wide variety of physical and mental ailments. For instance, Parkinson’s disease, from 
which actor Michael J. Fox suffers, is caused by a deficiency of dopamine in the brain. Michael J. Fox suffers from Parkinson’s 
Techniques for increasing the production of dopamine in Parkinson’s patients are disease, and has become a strong 
proving effective (Antonini & Barone, 2008; Iversen & Iversen, 2007; Willis, 2005). advocate for research into the disorder. 





68 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 3 Neuroscience and Behavior 


In other instances, overproduction of dopamine produces negative consequences. 
For example, researchers have hypothesized that schizophrenia and some other 
severe mental disturbances are affected or perhaps even caused by the presence of 
unusually high levels of dopamine. Drugs that block the reception of dopamine 
reduce the symptoms displayed by some people diagnosed with schizophrenia (Di 
Forti, Lappin, & Murray, 2007; Howes & Kapur, 2009; Murray, Lappin, & Di Forti, 
2008). 

Another neurotransmitter, serotonin, is associated with the regulation of sleep, 
eating, mood, and pain. A growing body of research points toward a broader role 
for serotonin, suggesting its involvement in such diverse behaviors as alcoholism, 
depression, suicide, impulsivity, aggression, and coping with stress (Carillo et al., 
2009; Murphy, Lappin, & Di Forti, 2008; Popa et al., 2008). 

Endorphins, another class of neurotransmitters, are a family of chemicals pro- 
duced by the brain that are similar in structure to painkilling drugs such as mor- 
phine. The production of endorphins reflects the brain’s effort to deal with pain as 
well as to elevate mood. 

Endorphins also may produce the euphoric feelings that runners sometimes 
experience after long runs. The exertion and perhaps the pain involved in a long run 
may stimulate the production of endorphins, ultimately resulting in what has been 
called “runner’s high” (Kolata, 2002; Pert, 2002; Stanojevic, Mitic, & Vujic, 2007). 

Endorphin release might also explain other phenomena that have long puzzled 
psychologists. For example, the act of taking placebos (pills or other substances that 
contain no actual drugs but that patients believe will make them better) may induce 
the release of endorphins, leading to the reduction of pain (Crum & Langer, 2007; 
Rajagopal, 2006; Wager, 2005). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 7 Neurons: The Basic Elements of Behavior 69 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP e Endorphins, another type of neurotransmitter, are 
related to the reduction of pain. Endorphins aid in the 


; ; 5 
Why do psychologists study the brain and nervous system? production of a natural painkiller and are probably 


e A full understanding of human behavior requires knowl- 
edge of the biological influences underlying that behav- 
ior, especially those originating in the nervous system. 
Psychologists who specialize in studying the effects of 


responsible for creating the kind of euphoria that joggers 
sometimes experience after running. (p. 68) 


biological structures and functions on behavior are EVALUATE 
known as behavioral neuroscientists: (p60) 1. The is the fundamental element of the nervous 
What are the basic elements of the nervous system? system. 
e Neurons, the most basic elements of the nervous system, 2. Neurons receive information through their 


carry nerve impulses from one part of the body to an- 


and send messages through their 





other. Information in a neuron generally follows a route 3. Just as electrical wires have an outer coating, axons are in- 
that begins with the dendrites, continues into the cell sulated by a coating called the 
body, and leads ultimately down the tubelike extension, 4. The gap between two neurons is bridged by a chemical 
the axon. (p. 61) connection called a 
: ' 5. Endorphins are one kind of , the chemical 
How does the nervous system communicate electrical and 5 3 
messengers” between neurons. 
chemical messages from one part to another? 
e Most axons are insulated by a coating called the myelin 
sheath. When a neuron receives a message to fire, it RETHINK 


releases an action potential, an electric charge that travels 
through the axon. Neurons operate according to an 
all-or-none law: Either they are at rest, or an action 
potential is moving through them. There is no in-between 
state. (p. 62) 

e Once a neuron fires, nerve impulses are carried to other 
neurons through the production of chemical substances, 
neurotransmitters, that actually bridge the gaps—known 
as synapses—between neurons. Neurotransmitters may 
be either excitatory, telling other neurons to fire, or 
inhibitory, preventing or decreasing the likelihood of 
other neurons firing. (p. 65) 


1. How might psychologists use drugs that mimic the effects 
of neurotransmitters to treat psychological disorders? 

2. From the perspective of a health care provider: How would 
you explain the placebo effect and the role of endorphins 
to patients who wish to try unproven treatment methods 
that they find on the Web? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


Jo}TUIsuR.OINAU 'G 
‘asdeuds ‘p ‘yyeays ulpaAut ‘g ‘SuOXe ‘sayLIpUap ‘z ‘UOINEU *T 


KEY TERMS 


behavioral neuroscientists 
(or biopsychologists) p. 60 

neurons p. 61 

dendrite p. 61 


neurotransmitters p. 65 
excitatory message p. 66 
inhibitory message p. 66 
reuptake p. 66 


axon p. 61 

terminal buttons p. 61 
myelin sheath p. 62 
all-or-none law p. 62 


resting state p. 62 
action potential p. 63 
mirror neurons p. 64 
synapse p. 65 


MODULE 8 


How are the structures of 
the nervous system linked? 


How does the endocrine 
system affect behavior? 


The part of the nervous system that 
includes the brain and spinal cord. 


A bundle of neurons that 
leaves the brain and runs down the 
length of the back and is the main 
means for transmitting messages 
between the brain and the body. 


An automatic, involuntary 
response to an incoming stimulus. 


70 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


In light of the complexity of individual neurons and the neurotransmission process, 
it should come as no surprise that the connections and structures formed by the 
neurons are complicated. Because each neuron can be connected to 80,000 other 
neurons, the total number of possible connections is astonishing. For instance, esti- 
mates of the number of neural connections within the brain fall in the neighborhood 
of 10 quadrillion—a 1 followed by 16 zeros—and some experts put the number even 
higher. However, connections among neurons are not the only means of communi- 
cation within the body; as we'll see, the endocrine system, which secretes chemical 
messages that circulate through the blood, also communicates messages that influ- 
ence behavior and many aspects of biological functioning (Kandel, Schwartz, & 
Jessell, 2000; Forlenza & Baum, 2004; Boahen, 2005). 


The Nervous System: 
Linking Neurons 


Whatever the actual number of neural connections, the human nervous system has 
both logic and elegance. We turn now to a discussion of its basic structures. 





CENTRAL AND PERIPHERAL NERVOUS SYSTEMS 


As you can see from the schematic representation in Figure 1, the nervous system is 
divided into two main parts: the central nervous system and the peripheral nervous 
system. The central nervous system (CNS) is composed of the brain and spinal cord. 
The spinal cord, which is about the thickness of a pencil, contains a bundle of neu- 
rons that leaves the brain and runs down the length of the back (see Figure 2). As 
you can see in Figure 1, the spinal cord is the primary means for transmitting mes- 
sages between the brain and the rest of the body. 

However, the spinal cord is not just a communication channel. It also controls 
some simple behaviors on its own, without any help from the brain. An example is 
the way the knee jerks forward when it is tapped with a rubber hammer. This 
behavior is a type of reflex, an automatic, involuntary response to an incoming 
stimulus. A reflex is also at work when you touch a hot stove and immediately 
withdraw your hand. Although the brain eventually analyzes and reacts to the sit- 
uation (“Ouch—hot stove—pull away!”), the initial withdrawal is directed only by 
neurons in the spinal cord. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 8 The Nervous System and the Endocrine System: Communicating Within the Body 71 








FIGURE 1 A schematic diagram of the relationship of the parts of the nervous system. 


FIGURE 2 The central nervous system 
Peripheral nervous consists of the brain and spinal cord, 
system ; 

and the peripheral nervous system 
encompasses the network of nerves 
connecting the brain and spinal cord to 
other parts of the body. 






Central nervous 
system 


Spinal 
nerves 






Use Figures 1 and 2 to learn 

the components of the 
central and peripheral 
nervous systems. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


72 Chapter 3 Neuroscience and Behavior 


sensory (afferent) neurons Neurons 
that transmit information from the 
perimeter of the body to the central 
nervous system. 


motor (efferent) neurons Neurons 
that communicate information from 
the nervous system to muscles and 
glands. 


interneurons Neurons that connect 
sensory and motor neurons, carrying 
messages between the two. 


peripheral nervous system The part 
of the nervous system that includes the 
autonomic and somatic subdivisions; 
made up of neurons with long axons 
and dendrites, it branches out from the 
spinal cord and brain and reaches the 
extremities of the body. 


somatic division The part of the 
peripheral nervous system that 
specializes in the control of voluntary 
movements and the communication of 
information to and from the sense 
organs. 


autonomic division The part of the 
peripheral nervous system that 
controls involuntary movement of the 
heart, glands, lungs, and other organs. 


sympathetic division The part of the 
autonomic division of the nervous 
system that acts to prepare the body 
for action in stressful situations, engag- 
ing all the organism’s resources to 
respond to a threat. 


parasympathetic division The part of 
the autonomic division of the nervous 
system that acts to calm the body after 
an emergency has ended. 


Three kinds of neurons are involved in reflexes. Sensory (afferent) neurons 
transmit information from the perimeter of the body to the central nervous system. 
Motor (efferent) neurons communicate information from the nervous system to 
muscles and glands. Interneurons connect sensory and motor neurons, carrying mes- 
sages between the two. 

The importance of the spinal cord and reflexes is illustrated by the outcome of 
accidents in which the cord is injured or severed. In some cases, injury results in 
quadriplegia, a condition in which voluntary muscle movement below the neck is lost. 
In a less severe but still debilitating condition, paraplegia, people are unable to vol- 
untarily move any muscles in the lower half of the body. 

As suggested by its name, the peripheral nervous system branches out from the 
spinal cord and brain and reaches the extremities of the body. Made up of neurons 
with long axons and dendrites, the peripheral nervous system encompasses all the 
parts of the nervous system other than the brain and spinal cord. There are two major 
divisions—the somatic division and the autonomic division—both of which connect 
the central nervous system with the sense organs, muscles, glands, and other organs. 
The somatic division specializes in the control of voluntary movements—such as the 
motion of the eyes to read this sentence or those of the hand to turn this page—and 
the communication of information to and from the sense organs. The autonomic 
division controls the parts of the body that keep us alive—the heart, blood vessels, 
glands, lungs, and other organs that function involuntarily without our awareness. 
As you are reading at this moment, the autonomic division of the peripheral nervous 
system is pumping blood through your body, pushing your lungs in and out, and 
overseeing the digestion of your last meal. 


ACTIVATING THE DIVISIONS 
OF THE AUTONOMIC NERVOUS SYSTEM 


The autonomic division plays a particularly crucial role during emergencies. Suppose 
that as you are reading you suddenly sense that a stranger is watching you through 
the window. As you look up, you see the glint of something that might be a knife. 
As confusion clouds your mind and fear overcomes your attempts to think rationally, 
what happens to your body? If you are like most people, you react immediately on 
a physiological level. Your heart rate increases, you begin to sweat, and you develop 
goose bumps all over your body. 

The physiological changes that occur during a crisis result from the activation of 
one of the two parts of the autonomic nervous system: the sympathetic division. 
The sympathetic division acts to prepare the body for action in stressful situations 
by engaging all of the organism’s resources to run away or to confront the threat. 
This is often called the “fight or flight” response. 

In contrast, the parasympathetic division acts to calm the body after the emer- 
gency has ended. When you find, for instance, that the stranger at the window is 
actually your roommate, who has lost his keys and is climbing in the window to 
avoid waking you, your parasympathetic division begins to take over, lowering your 
heart rate, stopping your sweating, and returning your body to the state it was in 
before you became alarmed. The parasympathetic division also directs the body to 
store energy for use in emergencies. 

The sympathetic and parasympathetic divisions work together to regulate many 
functions of the body (see Figure 3). For instance, sexual arousal is controlled by the 
parasympathetic division, but sexual orgasm is a function of the sympathetic divi- 
sion. The sympathetic and parasympathetic divisions also are involved in a number 
of disorders. For example, one explanation of documented examples of “voodoo 
death”—in which a person is literally scared to death resulting from a voodoo 
curse—may be produced by overstimulation of the sympathetic division due to 
extreme fear (Sternberg, 2002). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 8 The Nervous System and the Endocrine System: Communicating Within the Body 


Parasympathetic 


Eyes RO} nK 


Contracts pupils 


Sympathetic 











Dilates pupils 
(enhanced vision) 








= ge 
Constricts bronchi Relaxes bronchi 
(increased air to lungs) 

Heart us wa - 

Slows heartbeat Accelerates, strengthens 

heartbeat (increased oxygen) 

Stomach, 
intestines 





i 
Inhibits activity 


Stimulates activit; 
y (blood sent to muscles) 


Blood vessels 

of internal 

organs 
Contracts vessels 


Dilates vessels (increased blood pressure) 


The Evolutionary Foundations 
of the Nervous System 


The complexities of the nervous system can be better understood if we take the 
course of evolution into consideration. The forerunner of the human nervous system 
is found in the earliest simple organisms to have a spinal cord. Basically, those organ- 
isms were simple input-output devices: When the upper side of the spinal cord was 
stimulated by, for instance, being touched, the organism reacted with a simple 
response, such as jerking away. Such responses were completely a consequence of 
the organism’s genetic makeup. 

Over millions of years, the spinal cord became more specialized, and organisms 
became capable of distinguishing between different kinds of stimuli and responding 
appropriately to them. Ultimately, a portion of the spinal cord evolved into what we 
would consider a primitive brain. 

Today, the nervous system is hierarchically organized, meaning that relatively 
newer (from an evolutionary point of view) and more sophisticated regions of the 
brain regulate the older, and more primitive, parts of the nervous system. As we 
move up along the spinal cord and continue upward into the brain, then, the func- 
tions controlled by the various regions become progressively more advanced. 

Why should we care about the evolutionary background of the human nervous 
system? The answer comes from researchers working in the area of evolutionary 
psychology, the branch of psychology that seeks to identify how behavior is influ- 
enced and produced by our genetic inheritance from our ancestors. 





73 


FIGURE 3 The major functions of 

the autonomic nervous system. The 
sympathetic division acts to prepare 
certain organs of the body for stressful 
situations, and the parasympathetic 
division acts to calm the body after the 
emergency has been passed. Can you 
explain why each response of the 
sympathetic division might be useful in 
an emergency? 


evolutionary psychology The branch 
of psychology that seeks to identify 
behavior patterns that are a result of 
our genetic inheritance from our 
ancestors. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


74 Chapter 3 Neuroscience and Behavior 


behavioral genetics The study of the 
effects of heredity on behavior. 


Evolutionary psychologists argue that the course of evolution is reflected in the 
structure and functioning of the nervous system and that evolutionary factors con- 
sequently have a significant influence on our everyday behavior. Their work, in 
conjunction with the research of scientists studying genetics, biochemistry, and med- 
icine, has led to an understanding of how our behavior is affected by heredity, our 
genetically determined heritage. In fact, evolutionary psychologists have spawned 
a new and increasingly influential field: behavioral genetics. 


BEHAVIORAL GENETICS 


Our evolutionary heritage manifests itself not only through the structure and func- 
tioning of the nervous system but through our behavior as well. In the view of a 
growing area of study, people’s personality and behavioral habits are affected in part 
by their genetic heritage. Behavioral genetics is the study of the effects of heredity 
on behavior. Behavioral genetics researchers are finding increasing evidence that cog- 
nitive abilities, personality traits, sexual orientation, and psychological disorders are 
determined to some extent by genetic factors (Ilies, Arvey, & Bouchard, 2006; Livesley 
& Jang, 2008; Vernon et al., 2008). 

Behavioral genetics lies at the heart of the nature-nurture question, one of the key 
issues in the study of psychology. Although no one would argue that our behavior is 
determined solely by inherited factors, evidence collected by behavioral geneticists 
does suggest that our genetic inheritance predisposes us to respond in particular ways 
to our environment, and even to seek out particular kinds of environments. For 
instance, research indicates that genetic factors may be related to such diverse behav- 
iors as level of family conflict, schizophrenia, learning disabilities, and general socia- 
bility (Ball et al., 2008; Davis, Haworth, & Plomin, 2009; Lakhan & Vieira, 2009). 

Furthermore, important human characteristics and behaviors are related to the 
presence (or absence) of particular genes, the inherited material that controls the trans- 
mission of traits. For example, researchers have found evidence that novelty-seeking 
behavior is determined, at least in part, by a certain gene (Golimbet et al., 2007). 

As we consider later in the book when we discuss human development, research- 
ers have identified some 25,000 individual genes, each of which appears in a specific 
sequence on a particular chromosome, a rod-shaped structure that transmits genetic 
information across generations. In 2003, after a decade of effort, researchers identified 
the sequence of the 3 billion chemical pairs that make up human DNA, the basic 
component of genes. Understanding the basic structure of the human genome—the 
“map” of humans’ total genetic makeup—brings scientists a giant step closer to 
understanding the contributions of individual genes to specific human structures and 
functioning (Andreasen, 2005; Dale & von Schantz, 2007; Plomin & Davis, 2009). 


Molecular Genetics and Psychological Disorders Despite its relative infancy, the field 
of behavioral genetics has already made substantial contributions to our understanding 
of behavior. One branch of behavioral genetics, molecular genetics, seeks to identify 
specific genes that are associated with behavior and, in particular, psychological dis- 
orders. Genes that are physically close to one another on a particular chromosome tend 
to be linked and inherited together. By finding genetic markers—genes with a known 
location—that are linked to a disorder, scientists are beginning to learn how disorders 
such as schizophrenia and depression develop and can potentially be treated. 
Molecular geneticists have already found that the risk of developing autism (a 
disorder that influences the development of language and effective social functioning) 
is increased in the presence of a gene related to early brain development. Children with 
this gene, a variation of the gene called HOXA1, are twice as likely to develop the 
disorder as children who do not have this variant (Hyman, 2003; Gregg et al., 2007). 
Yet having the variant gene does not always lead to autism. More than 99.5% of 
people with the variant do not develop the disorder, and 60% of those with autism 
do not have the variant. It is probable that autism, like other disorders with a genetic 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 8 The Nervous System and the Endocrine System: Communicating Within the Body 75 


basis, is not triggered by the presence or absence of a single, particular gene. More 
likely, it is produced by several genes in combination, as well as perhaps requiring 
the presence of certain environmental influences, such as infection or brain injury. 
The challenge for behavior geneticists, then, is not only to determine what genes are 
responsible for particular behaviors but also to identify the environmental triggers 
that activate those genes (Sen et al., 2007). 

In examining the genetic roots of various behaviors, the study of behavior genet- 
ics has stirred controversy. For instance, questions about the existence of genetic 
influences on criminality, intelligence, and homosexuality raise considerable emotion. 
Furthermore, it is unclear what the social and political consequences of discoveries 
in behavioral genetics would be. Would finding a strong genetic basis for criminal 
behavior lead to genetic screening and restricted civil rights for individuals having 
“criminal” genes? Clearly, behavioral genetic discoveries could have an impact on a 
number of important social issues (Wensley & King, 2008). 


Behavioral Genetics, Gene Therapy, and Genetic Counseling Behavioral genetics 
also holds the promise of developing new diagnostic and treatment techniques for 
genetic deficiencies that can lead to physical and psychological difficulties. In gene 
therapy, scientists inject into a patient’s bloodstream genes meant to cure a particular 
disease. When the genes arrive at the site of defective genes that are producing the 
illness, they trigger the production of chemicals that can treat the disease (Jaffé, 
Prasad, & Larcher, 2006; Eberling et al., 2008; Isacson & Kordower, 2008). 

The number of diseases that can be treated through gene therapy is growing, as 
we will see when we discuss human development. For example, gene therapy is now 
being used in experimental trials involving people with certain forms of cancer and 
blindness (Hirschler, 2007; Nakamura, 2004; Wagner et al., 2004). 

Advances in behavioral genetics also have led to the development of a profession 
that did not exist several decades ago: genetic counseling. Genetic counselors help 
people deal with issues related to inherited disorders. For example, genetic counselors 
provide advice to prospective parents about the potential risks in a future pregnancy, 
based on their family history of birth defects and hereditary illnesses. In addition, the 
counselor considers the parents’ age and problems with children they already have. 
They also can take blood, skin, and urine samples to examine specific chromosomes. 

Scientists have already developed genetic tests to determine whether someone is 
susceptible to certain types of cancer or heart disease, and it may not be long before 
analysis of a drop of blood can indicate whether a child—or potentially an unborn 
fetus—is susceptible to certain psychological disorders. How such knowledge will be 
used is a source of considerable speculation and controversy, controversy that is certain 
to grow as genetic testing becomes more common (Etchegary, 2004; Malpas, 2008). 


The Endocrine System: 
Of Chemicals and Glands 


Another of the body’s communication systems, the endocrine system is a chemical 
communication network that sends messages throughout the body via the blood- 
stream. Its job is to secrete hormones, chemicals that circulate through the blood and 
regulate the functioning or growth of the body. It also influences—and is influenced 
by—the functioning of the nervous system. Although the endocrine system is not 
part of the brain, it is closely linked to the hypothalamus. 

As chemical messengers, hormones are like neurotransmitters, although their 
speed and mode of transmission are quite different. Whereas neural messages are 
measured in thousandths of a second, hormonal communications may take minutes 
to reach their destination. Furthermore, neural messages move through neurons in 





endocrine system A chemical 
communication network that sends 
messages throughout the body via the 
bloodstream. 


hormones Chemicals that circulate 
through the blood and regulate the 
functioning or growth of the body. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


76 Chapter 3 Neuroscience and Behavior 





Anterior pituitary gland: 
Produces 6 hormones 
with diverse actions. 


Hypothalamus: 

Secretes several neurohormones that 
stimulate or inhibit anterior pituitary 
function. 


Posterior pituitary gland: 

Secretes oxytocin, which stimulates 
uterine contractions during birth; also 
secretes antidiuretic hormone, which 
increases water retention in the kidney. 








Heart: 
Makes atrial natriuretic peptide, 
which lowers blood sodium. 





Pineal: 
Makes melatonin, which regulates 
daily rhythms. 


Adrenal glands 


Medulla: 

Makes epinephrine and norepinephrine, 
which mediate the “fight-or-flight” 
response. 


Parathyroids (behind the thyroid): 
Make parathyroid hormone, which 
increases blood calcium. 

Cortex: 

Makes aldosterone, which regulates 
sodium and potassium balance in the 
blood; also makes glucocorticoids (such 
as cortisol), which regulate growth, 
metabolism, development, immune 
function, and the body’s response 

to stress. 


Thyroid: 
Regulates metabolic rate and growth. 


Stomach and small intestine: 
Secrete hormones that facilitate 
digestion and regulate 

pancreatic activity. 


Liver and kidneys: 
Secrete erythropoietin, which regulates 
production of red blood cells. 





Pancreas: 


Makes insulin. Ovaries: 


Produce estrogens such as 
progesterone, which control 
reproduction in females. 


Testes: 

Produce androgens, such as 
testosterone, which control 
reproduction in males. 


Adipose tissue: 
Produces adipokines (for example, leptin), 
which regulate appetite and metabolic rate. 





A3 


DF 








FIGURE 4 Location and function of the major endocrine glands. The pituitary gland controls 
the functioning of the other endocrine glands and, in turn, is regulated by the hypothalamus. 


specific lines (like a signal carried by wires strung along telephone poles), whereas 
hormones travel throughout the body, similar to the way radio waves are transmit- 
ted across the entire landscape. Just as radio waves evoke a response only when a 
radio is tuned to the correct station, hormones flowing through the bloodstream 
activate only those cells that are receptive and “tuned” to the appropriate hormonal 


message. 

pituitary gland The major component A key component of the endocrine system is the tiny pituitary gland, which is 
of the endocrine system, or “master found near—and regulated by—the hypothalamus. The pituitary gland has some- 
gland,” which secretes hormones that times been called the “master gland” because it controls the functioning of the rest 
control growth and other parts of the of the endocrine system. But the pituitary gland is more than just the taskmaster of 
endocrine system. other glands; it has important functions in its own right. For instance, hormones 


secreted by the pituitary gland control growth. Extremely short people and unusually 
tall ones usually have pituitary gland abnormalities. Other endocrine glands, shown 
in Figure 4, affect emotional reactions, sexual urges, and energy levels. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 8 The Nervous System and the Endocrine System: Communicating Within the Body 77 





Despite its designation as the “master gland,” the pituitary is actually a servant of 
the brain, because the brain is ultimately responsible for the endocrine system’s func- 
tioning. The brain maintains the internal balance of the body through the hypothalamus. 

Individual hormones can wear many hats, depending on circumstances. For 
example, the hormone oxytocin is at the root of many of life’s satisfactions and plea- 
sures. In new mothers, oxytocin produces an urge to nurse newborn offspring. The 
same hormone also seems to stimulate cuddling between species members. And—at 
least in rats—it encourages sexually active males to seek out females more passion- 
ately, and females to be more receptive to males’ sexual advances. There’s even evi- 
dence that oxytocin is related to the development of trust in others, helping to grease 
the wheels of effective social interaction (Kosfeld et al., 2005; Meinlschmidt & Heim, 
2007; Guastella, Mitchell, & Dadds, 2008). 

Although hormones are produced naturally by the endocrine system, the ingestion 
of artificial hormones has proved to be both beneficial and potentially dangerous. For 
example, before the early 2000s, physicians frequently prescribed hormone replacement 
therapy (HRT) to treat symptoms of menopause in older women. However, because 
recent research suggests that the treatment has potentially dangerous side effects, health 
experts now warn that the dangers outweigh the benefits (Herrington & Howard, 2003). 

The use of testosterone, a male hormone, and drugs known as steroids, which act 
like testosterone, is increasingly common. For athletes and others who want to bulk 
up their appearance, steroids provide a way to add muscle weight and increase 
strength. However, these drugs can lead to heart attacks, strokes, cancer, and even 
violent behavior, making them extremely dangerous. For example, in one infamous 
case, professional wrestler Chris Benoit strangled his wife, suffocated his son, and 
later hanged himself—acts that were attributed to his use of steroids (Klötz, 2006; 
Pagonis, Angelopoulos, & Koukoulis, 2006; Sandomir, 2007). 


Steroids can provide added muscle and 
strength, but they have dangerous side 
effects. A number of well-known 
athletes in a variety of sports have been 
accused of using the drugs illegally. 





dy Alert 


The endocrine system pro- 
duces hormones, chemicals 
that circulate through the 
body via the bloodstream. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


78 Chapter 3 Neuroscience and Behavior 

RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 

RECAP EVALUATE 

How are the structures of the nervous system linked? 1. If you put your hand on a red-hot piece of metal, the 


e The nervous system is made up of the central nervous 
system (the brain and spinal cord) and the peripheral 
nervous system. The peripheral nervous system is made 
up of the somatic division, which controls voluntary 
movements and the communication of information to 
and from the sense organs, and the autonomic division, 
which controls involuntary functions such as those of the 
heart, blood vessels, and lungs. (p. 70) 

e The autonomic division of the peripheral nervous 
system is further subdivided into the sympathetic and 
parasympathetic divisions. The sympathetic division 
prepares the body in emergency situations, and the 
parasympathetic division helps the body return to its 
typical resting state. (p. 72) 

e Evolutionary psychology, the branch of psychology that 
seeks to identify behavior patterns that are a result of 
our genetic inheritance, has led to increased under- 
standing of the evolutionary basis of the structure and 
organization of the human nervous system. Behavioral 
genetics extends this study to include the evolutionary 
and hereditary basis of human personality traits and 
behavior. (p. 73) 


How does the endocrine system affect behavior? 

e The endocrine system secretes hormones, chemicals 
that regulate the functioning of the body, via the 
bloodstream. The pituitary gland secretes growth 
hormones and influences the release of hormones by 
other endocrine glands, and in turn is regulated by the 
hypothalamus. (p. 75) 


immediate response of pulling it away would be an 
example of a(n) 


2. The central nervous system is composed of the 


3. In the peripheral nervous system, the 


and the 

division 
controls voluntary movements, whereas the 

division controls organs that keep us alive and function 
without our awareness. 


4, Maria saw a young boy run into the street and get hit by a 


5. The emerging field of 


car. When she got to the fallen child, she was in a state of 
panic. She was sweating, and her heart was racing. Her bi- 
ological state resulted from the activation of what division 
of the nervous system? 

a. parasympathetic 

b. central 

c. sympathetic 

studies ways in which 
our genetic inheritance predisposes us to behave in certain 
ways. 


RETHINK 


1. 


In what ways is the “fight-or-flight” response helpful to 
humans in emergency situations? 


2. From the perspective of a genetic counselor: How would you 


explain the pros and cons of genetic counseling to some- 
one who was interested in receiving genetic screening for 
various diseases and disorders? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


soouas [eIoTAvyagq *¢ 


Soneyyedurds 2p Oruouome yewos ‘g /prIOD TeuTds ‘uwaq 'z ‘Xapo I 


KEY TERMS 


central nervous system motor (efferent) autonomic division p.72 behavioral genetics p. 74 


(CNS) p. 70 neurons p. 72 sympathetic division p. 72 endocrine system p. 75 
spinal cord p. 70 interneurons p. 72 parasympathetic hormones p. 75 
reflex p. 70 peripheral nervous division p. 72 pituitary gland p. 76 
sensory (afferent) system p. 72 evolutionary 


neurons p. 72 somatic division p. 72 psychology p. 73 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


It is not much to look at. Soft, spongy, mottled, and pinkish-gray in color, it hardly 
can be said to possess much in the way of physical beauty. Despite its physical 
appearance, however, it ranks as the greatest natural marvel that we know and has 
a beauty and sophistication all its own. 

The object to which this description applies: the brain. The brain is responsible for 
our loftiest thoughts—and our most primitive urges. It is the overseer of the intricate 
workings of the human body. If one were to attempt to design a computer to mimic 
the range of capabilities of the brain, the task would be nearly impossible; in fact, it 
has proved difficult even to come close. The sheer quantity of nerve cells in the brain 
is enough to daunt even the most ambitious computer engineer. Many billions of 
neurons make up a structure weighing just 3 pounds in the average adult. However, 
it is not the number of cells that is the most astounding thing about the brain but its 
ability to allow the human intellect to flourish by guiding our behavior and thoughts. 

We turn now to a consideration of the particular structures of the brain and the 
primary functions to which they are related. However, a caution is in order. Although 
we'll discuss specific areas of the brain in relation to specific behaviors, this approach 
is an oversimplification. No straightforward one-to-one correspondence exists 
between a distinct part of the brain and a particular behavior. Instead, behavior is 
produced by complex interconnections among sets of neurons in many areas of the 
brain: Our behavior, emotions, thoughts, hopes, and dreams are produced by a vari- 
ety of neurons throughout the nervous system working in concert. 











The brain has posed a continual challenge to those who would study 
it. For most of history, its examination was possible only after an indi- 
vidual had died. Only then could the skull be opened and the brain 
cut into without serious injury. Although informative, this procedure 
could hardly tell us much about the functioning of the healthy brain. 

Today, however, brain-scanning techniques provide a window 
into the living brain. Using these techniques, investigators can take a 
“snapshot” of the internal workings of the brain without having to 
cut open a person’s skull. The most important scanning techniques, 
illustrated in Figure 1 on page 80, are the electroencephalogram (EEG), 
positron emission tomography (PET), functional magnetic resonance 
imaging (fMRI), and transcranial magnetic stimulation imaging (TMS). 

The electroencephalogram (EEG) records electrical activity in the 
brain through electrodes placed on the outside of the skull. Although 
traditionally the EEG could produce only a graph of electrical wave 


How do researchers identify 
the major parts and functions 
of the brain? 


What are the major parts of 
the brain, and for what 
behaviors is each part 
responsible? 


How do the two halves 
of the brain operate inter- 
dependently? 


How can an understanding of 
the nervous system help us 
find ways to alleviate disease 
and pain? 





The brain (shown here in cross-section) may not be much to 
look at, but it represents one of the great marvels of human 
development. Why do most scientists believe that it will be 
difficult, if not impossible, to duplicate the brain’s abilities? 


79 


80 





(a) EEG 





(b) fMRI 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 3 Neuroscience and Behavior 








‘eh 


(c) PET scan (d) TMS Apparatus 


dy Alert 


Remember that EEG, fMRI, 
PET, and TMS differ in terms 
of whether they examine 
brain structures or brain 
functioning. 


FIGURE 1 Brain scans produced by different techniques. (a) A computer-produced EEG 
image. (b) The fMRI scan uses a magnetic field to provide a detailed view of brain activity on 
a moment-by-moment basis. (c) The PET scan displays the functioning of the brain at a given 
moment. (d) Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), the newest type of scan, produces a 
momentary disruption in an area of the brain, allowing researchers to see what activities are 
controlled by that area. TMS also has the potential to treat some psychological disorders. 


patterns, new techniques are now used to transform the brain’s electrical activity into 
a pictorial representation of the brain that allows more precise diagnosis of disorders 
such as epilepsy and learning disabilities. 

Functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) scans provide a detailed, three- 
dimensional computer-generated image of brain structures and activity by aiming a 
powerful magnetic field at the body. With fMRI scanning, it is possible to produce 
vivid, detailed images of the functioning of the brain. 

Using fMRI scans, researchers are able to view features of less than a millimeter 
in size and view changes occurring in intervals of Yo of a second. For example, {MRI 
scans can show the operation of individual bundles of nerves by tracing the flow of 
blood, opening the way for improved diagnosis of ailments ranging from chronic back 
pain to nervous system disorders such as strokes, multiple sclerosis, and Alzheimer’s. 
Scans using fMRI are routinely used in planning brain surgery, because they can help 
surgeons distinguish areas of the brain involved in normal and disturbed functioning 
(Mazard et al., 2005; Quenot et al., 2005; D’Arcy et al., 2007). 

Positron emission tomography (PET) scans show biochemical activity within the 
brain at a given moment. PET scans begin with the injection of a radioactive (but 
safe) liquid into the bloodstream, which makes its way to the brain. By locating 
radiation within the brain, a computer can determine which are the more active 
regions, providing a striking picture of the brain at work. For example, PET scans 
may be used in cases of memory problems, seeking to identify the presence of brain 
tumors (Gronholm et al., 2005; McMurtray et al., 2007). 

Transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) is one of the newest types of scan. By 
exposing a tiny region of the brain to a strong magnetic field, TMS causes a momen- 
tary interruption of electrical activity. Researchers then are able to note the effects of 
this interruption on normal brain functioning. The procedure is sometimes called a 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


“virtual lesion,” because it produces effects analogous to what would occur if areas 
of the brain were physically cut. The enormous advantage of TMS, of course, is that 
the virtual cut is only temporary. In addition to identifying areas of the brain that 
are responsible for particular functions, TMS has the potential to treat certain kinds 
of psychological disorders, such as depression and schizophrenia, by shooting brief 
magnetic pulses through the brain (Fitzgerald & Daskalakis, 2008; Rado, Dowd, & 
Janicak, 2008; Pallanti & Bernardi, 2009). 

Future discoveries may yield even more sophisticated methods of examining the 
brain. For example, the emerging field of optogenetics involves genetic engineering 
and the use of special types of light to view individual circuits of neurons (Gradinaru 
et al., 2009; Miesenbock, 2008). 

Advances in our understanding of the brain also are paving the way for the 
development of new methods for harnessing the brain’s neural signals. We consider 
some of these intriguing findings in Applying Psychology in the 21st Century. 


The Central Core: 
Our “Old Brain” 


Although the capabilities of the human brain far exceed those of the brain of any 
other species, humans share some basic functions, such as breathing, eating, and 
sleeping, with more primitive animals. Not surprisingly, those activities are directed 
by a relatively primitive part of the brain. A portion of the brain known as the 
central core (see Figure 2) is quite similar in all vertebrates (species with back- 
bones). The central core is sometimes referred to as the “old brain,” because its 
evolution can be traced back some 500 million years to primitive structures found 
in nonhuman species. 

If we were to move up the spinal cord from the base of the skull to locate the 
structures of the central core of the brain, the first part we would come to would 
be the hindbrain, which contains the medulla, pons, and cerebellum (see Figure 3 on 
page 83). The medulla controls a number of critical body functions, the most impor- 
tant of which are breathing and heartbeat. The pons comes next, joining the two 
halves of the cerebellum, which lies adjacent to it. Containing large bundles of 
nerves, the pons acts as a transmitter of motor information, coordinating muscles 
and integrating movement between the right and left halves of the body. It is also 
involved in regulating sleep. 

The cerebellum is found just above the medulla and behind the pons. Without 
the help of the cerebellum we would be unable to walk a straight line without stag- 
gering and lurching forward, for it is the job of the cerebellum to control bodily 
balance. It constantly monitors feedback from the muscles to coordinate their place- 
ment, movement, and tension. In fact, drinking too much alcohol seems to depress 
the activity of the cerebellum, leading to the unsteady gait and movement charac- 
teristic of drunkenness. The cerebellum is also involved in several intellectual func- 
tions, ranging from the analysis and coordination of sensory information to problem 
solving (Bower & Parsons, 2003; Paquier & Marién, 2005; Vandervert, Schimpf, & 
Liu, 2007). 

The reticular formation extends from the medulla through the pons, passing 
through the middle section of the brain—or midbrain—and into the front-most part 
of the brain, called the forebrain. Like an ever-vigilant guard, the reticular formation 
is made up of groups of nerve cells that can activate other parts of the brain imme- 
diately to produce general bodily arousal. If, for example, we are startled by a loud 
noise, the reticular formation can prompt a heightened state of awareness to deter- 
mine whether a response is necessary. The reticular formation serves a different func- 
tion when we are sleeping, seeming to filter out background stimuli to allow us to 
sleep undisturbed. 





Module 9 The Brain 


Cerebral cortex 
(the “new brain”) 






81 


Central core 
(the “old brain”) 


FIGURE 2 The major divisions of the 


brain: the cerebral cortex and the 
central core. (Source: Seeley, Stephens, 


& Tate, 2000.) 


central core The “old brain,” which 
controls basic functions such as eating 


and sleeping and is common to all 


vertebrates. 


cerebellum (ser-uh-BELL-um) The 
part of the brain that controls bodily 


balance. 


reticular formation The part of the 


brain extending from the medulla 
through the pons and made up of 


groups of nerve cells that can immedi- 
ately activate other parts of the brain 


to produce general bodily arousal. 


81 


Applying Psychology in 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


the 21st Century 





Mind over Cursor: Harnessing 
Brainpower to Improve Lives 


Hans-Peter Salzmann, a lawyer suffering 
from Lou Gehrig’s disease, was locked 
within his own body. Paralyzed by the dis- 
ease and unable to eat, speak, or even 
breathe on his own, he had to rely on a res- 
pirator and a feeding tube to survive. Al- 
though his mind functioned normally, he 
was unable to communicate with the out- 
side world. 

All that changed, however, after Salz- 
mann obtained an experimental device that 
allows brain waves to be translated into 
written communication. Using EEG scan- 
ning techniques that react to the pattern of 
brain waves originating in the brain, Salz- 
mann learned to boost and curtail certain 
types of brain waves. After hundreds of 
hours of practice, he was able to select let- 
ters that appear on a video screen. By 
stringing letters together, he could spell out 
messages. The process, which makes use of 
brain waves called “slow cortical poten- 
tials,” permitted Salzmann to communicate 
effectively for the first time in years. Al- 
though the method is slow and tedious— 
Salzmann can produce only about two 
characters per minute—it holds great 
promise for people with spinal cord injuries 
and diseases that have left them paralyzed 
(Neumann & Birbaumer, 2004; Pollack, 
2006; Hatsopoulos & Donoghue, 2009). 

Neuroscience researchers are making re- 
markable progress at developing this tech- 
nology of thought-based interfaces. It’s not 
ready to be put into widespread use, but 
recent discoveries show how rapidly the 
technology is developing. In one extraordi- 
nary experiment, researchers implanted 
freckle-sized electrodes onto the brains of 
two monkeys at the motor cortex—the re- 
gion of the brain responsible for voluntary 
muscle movement. Each electrode con- 
nected with 100 individual neurons in this 
region. A computer wired to the electrodes 
interpreted the signals coming from these 
neurons and used the information to direct 
the movement of a mechanical arm while 
the monkeys’ own arms were restrained 
(Velliste et al., 2008). 


82 





Brain scanning techniques allow people to communicate via brain waves alone. 


The monkeys were initially “trained” 
in the manipulation of the arm, using joy- 
sticks at first and then using the brain in- 
terface with additional computer guidance 
(somewhat like using training wheels 
while learning to ride a bicycle). Within 
days, the monkeys were operating the 
arm using just their thoughts with no ad- 
ditional help. They sat in a chair and used 
the arm to reach out and grab treats such 
as grapes and marshmallows and bring 
them to their mouths. This task required 
them not only to move the arm through 
space but also to manipulate the grip on 
its end to seize the food and then to re- 
lease it into their mouths. 

While this much of a successful accom- 
plishment was extraordinary by itself, the 
progress didn’t stop there. The monkeys 
actually proceeded to show the scientists a 
new trick or two by becoming adept 
enough with the brain interface to impro- 
vise entirely unanticipated manipulations 
of the mechanical arm, such as using it to 


push food into their mouths as if it were 
their own hand. This ability is an impor- 
tant step forward, as any practical device 
for assisting the disabled would have to be 
able to be used in inventive ways in order 
to provide for their unique needs in a natu- 
ral way. No computer program alone can 
anticipate every possible movement that 
might be necessary, but the monkeys 
showed that they could adapt the device to 
serve their own goals (Carey, 2008; Velliste 
et al., 2008). 

Many hurdles remain before paralyzed 
people will be using thought-controlled 
devices routinely, particularly in terms of 
the need for stable electrodes, wireless 
communication, and portable translating 
computers. But achievements such as this 
give researchers hope that they are close to 
overcoming the remaining challenges such 
that one day—perhaps sooner than one 
might expect—their seemingly wild dream 
of giving new mobility to paralyzed people 
will be realized (Stix, 2008). 





e Why is it so important to develop a complex and very expensive means of com- 
munication for the relatively small number of people who are living with locked-in 


syndrome? 








www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 9 The Brain 83 





Thalamus 

Relay center for cortex; 
handles incoming and 
outgoing signals 


Midbrain 





Cerebral cortex 





Corpus callosum 
Bridge of fibers passing 
information between the 


two cerebral hemispheres Hypothalamus 


Responsible for regulating basic 
biological needs: hunger, thirst, 
temperature control 








Hippocampus 








Amygdala Olfactory bulb 


Pituitary gland 
“Master” gland that regulates 
other endocrine glands 





Cerebellum 
Controls bodily balance 





Spinal cord 
Responsible for communication 
between brain and rest of body; 
involved in simple reflexes 


Pons 
Involved in sleep and arousal 


Medulla 

Responsible for regulating largely 
unconscious functions such as 
breathing and circulation 





FIGURE 3 The major structures in the brain. (Source: Brooker, Widmaier, Graham, & Stiling, 2008.) 


Hidden within the forebrain, the thalamus acts primarily as a relay station for thalamus The part of the brain located 
information about the senses. Messages from the eyes, ears, and skin travel to the in the middle of the central core that 
thalamus to be communicated upward to higher parts of the brain. The thalamus acts primarily to relay information 
also integrates information from higher parts of the brain, sorting it out so that itcan about the senses. 
be sent to the cerebellum and medulla. 

The hypothalamus is located just below the thalamus. Although tiny—about the hypothalamus A tiny part of the 
size of a fingertip—the hypothalamus plays an extremely important role. One of its brain, located below the thalamus, that 


major functions is to maintain homeostasis, a steady internal environment for the body. maintains homeostasis and produces 
The hypothalamus helps provide a constant body temperature and monitors the and regulates vital behavior, such as 
amount of nutrients stored in the cells. A second major function is equally important: eating, drinking, and sexual behavior. 


the hypothalamus produces and regulates behavior that is critical to the basic sur- 
vival of the species, such as eating, self-protection, and sex. 


The Limbic System: 
Beyond the Central Core 


In an eerie view of the future, science fiction writers have suggested that people 
someday will routinely have electrodes implanted in their brains. Those electrodes 
will permit them to receive tiny shocks that will produce the sensation of pleasure 
by stimulating certain centers of the brain. When they feel upset, people will simply 
activate their electrodes to achieve an immediate high. 
Although far-fetched—and ultimately improbable—such a futuristic fantasy is limbic system The part of the brain 
based on fact. The brain does have pleasure centers in several areas, including some that controls eating, aggression, and 
in the limbic system. Consisting of a series of doughnut-shaped structures that reproduction. 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


84 Chapter 3 Neuroscience and Behavior 


Hippocampus 





Spinal cord 


Frontal lobe include the amygdala and hippocampus, the limbic system borders the top of 


the central core and has connections with the cerebral cortex (see Figure 4). 

The structures of the limbic system jointly control a variety of basic 
functions relating to emotions and self-preservation, such as eating, aggres- 
sion, and reproduction. Injury to the limbic system can produce striking 
changes in behavior. For example, injury to the amygdala, which is involved 
in fear and aggression, can turn animals that are usually docile and tame 
into belligerent savages. Conversely, animals that are usually wild and 
uncontrollable may become meek and obedient following injury to the 
amygdala (Bedard & Persinger, 1995; Gontkovsky, 2005). 

Research examining the effects of mild electric shocks to parts of the 
limbic system and other parts of the brain has produced some thought- 
provoking findings. In one experiment, rats that pressed a bar received 
mild electric stimulation through an electrode implanted in their brains, 
which produced pleasurable feelings. Even starving rats on their way to 
food would stop to press the bar as many times as they could. Some rats 


FIGURE 4 The limbic system is involved in self- would actually stimulate themselves literally thousands of times an hour— 
preservation, learning, memory, and the experience until they collapsed with fatigue (Routtenberg & Lindy, 1965; Olds & Fobes, 


of pleasure. 


cerebral cortex The “new brain,” 
responsible for the most sophisticated 
information processing in the brain; 
contains four lobes. 


1981; Fountas & Smith, 2007). 
The extraordinarily pleasurable quality of certain kinds of stimulation 
has also been experienced by humans, who, as part of the treatment for 
certain kinds of brain disorders, have received electrical stimulation to certain areas 
of the limbic system. Although at a loss to describe just what it feels like, these 
people report the experience to be intensely pleasurable, similar in some respects to 
sexual orgasm. 

The limbic system and hippocampus in particular play an important role in 
learning and memory, a finding demonstrated in patients with epilepsy. In an attempt 
to stop their seizures, such patients have had portions of the limbic system removed. 
One unintended consequence of the surgery is that individuals sometimes have dif- 
ficulty learning and remembering new information. In one case, a patient who had 
undergone surgery was unable to remember where he lived, although he had resided 
at the same address for eight years. Further, even though the patient was able to 
carry on animated conversations, he was unable, a few minutes later, to recall what 
had been discussed (Milner, 1966; Rich & Shapiro, 2007). 

The limbic system, then, is involved in several important functions, including 
self-preservation, learning, memory, and the experience of pleasure. These functions 
are hardly unique to humans; in fact, the limbic system is sometimes referred to as 
the “animal brain,” because its structures and functions are so similar to those of 
other mammals. To identify the part of the brain that provides the complex and 
subtle capabilities that are uniquely human, we need to turn to another structure— 
the cerebral cortex. 


The Cerebral Cortex: 
Our “New Brain” 


As we have proceeded up the spinal cord and into the brain, our discussion has 
centered on areas of the brain that control functions similar to those found in less 
sophisticated organisms. But where, you may be asking, are the portions of the 
brain that enable humans to do what they do best and that distinguish humans 
from all other animals? Those unique features of the human brain—indeed, the 
very capabilities that allow you to come up with such a question in the first 
place—are embodied in the ability to think, evaluate, and make complex judg- 
ments. The principal location of these abilities, along with many others, is the 
cerebral cortex. 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 








| Motor area 
Frontal lobe 5 





Broca’s area 


FIGURE 5 The cerebral cortex of the brain. The major physical structures of the cerebral cortex 
are called lobes. This figure also illustrates the functions associated with particular areas of the 
cerebral cortex. Are any areas of the cerebral cortex present in nonhuman animals? 


The cerebral cortex is referred to as the “new brain” because of its relatively 
recent evolution. It consists of a mass of deeply folded, rippled, convoluted tissue. 
Although only about 1⁄2 of an inch thick, it would, if flattened out, cover an area 
more than 2 feet square. This configuration allows the surface area of the cortex to 
be considerably greater than it would be if it were smoother and more uniformly 
packed into the skull. The uneven shape also permits a high level of integration of 
neurons, allowing sophisticated information processing. 

The cortex has four major sections called lobes. If we take a side view of the 
brain, the frontal lobes lie at the front center of the cortex and the parietal lobes lie 
behind them. The temporal lobes are found in the lower-center portion of the cortex, 
with the occipital lobes lying behind them. These four sets of lobes are physically 
separated by deep grooves called sulci. Figure 5 shows the four areas. 

Another way to describe the brain is in terms of the functions associated with 
a particular area. Figure 5 also shows the specialized regions within the lobes related 
to specific functions and areas of the body. Three major areas are known: the motor 
areas, the sensory areas, and the association areas. Although we will discuss these 
areas as though they were separate and independent, keep in mind that this is an 
oversimplification. In most instances, behavior is influenced simultaneously by several 
structures and areas within the brain, operating interdependently. To give one exam- 
ple, people use different areas of the brain when they create sentences (a verbal task) 
compared with when they improvise musical tunes. Furthermore, when people suffer 
brain injury, uninjured portions of the brain can sometimes take over the functions 
that were previously handled by the damaged area. (Also see PsychWork.) In short, 
the brain is extraordinarily adaptable (Sacks, 2003; Boller, 2004; Brown, Martinez, & 
Parsons, 2006). 


Somatosensory area 


Somatosensory association 
area 


Primary auditory area 


Visual area 


Visual association 
area 


Module 9 The Brain 


— Parietal lobe 


Wernicke’s area — Temporal lobe 


Auditory association 
area 


— Occipital lobe 





lobes The four major sections of the 
cerebral cortex: frontal, parietal, 
temporal, and occipital. 


85 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


86 Chapter 3 Neuroscience and Behavior 


Rehabilitation counselor Monique Tremaine helps individuals 
Psyc h Wo r k who have suffered severe brain injury regain as much normal 

functioning as possible. She does this by systematically assess- 
eee SEEOR ing patients’ problems, providing psychological and behavioral 
treatment, and ensuring that the treatment is state-of-the-art 
and is grounded in evidence gleaned from research. 

According to Tremaine, her work requires an understand- 
ing of the structure of the brain and nervous system as well 
as expertise in clinical psychology in order to understand how 
brain injury effects emotion, function, and behavior. 

“Sudden acquired or traumatic brain injury can impact many 
aspects of an individual's life, including their personality, cogni- 
tion, sense of physical well-being, family roles, and occupa- 
tional functioning,” she noted. “It is my role to communicate such changes to a 
multidisciplinary staff, to the patient, and to the family to develop a comprehensive 
treatment plan.” 





Name: Monique J. Tremaine 





Position: Rehabilitation Counselor 

Education: BS in Psychology & Natural Sciences, 
Ball State University; MA in Clinical Psychology, 
California School of Professional Psychology; 
PhD in Clinical Neuropsychology, California School 
of Professional Psychology 


THE MOTOR AREA OF THE CORTEX 


If you look at the frontal lobe in Figure 5, you will see a shaded portion labeled 


motor area The part of the cortex that motor area. This part of the cortex is largely responsible for the body’s voluntary 
is largely responsible for the body’s movement. Every portion of the motor area corresponds to a specific locale within 
voluntary movement. the body. If we were to insert an electrode into a particular part of the motor area of 


the cortex and apply mild electrical stimulation, there would be involuntary move- 
ment in the corresponding part of the body. If we moved to another part of the motor 
area and stimulated it, a different part of the body would move. 

The motor area is so well mapped that researchers have identified the amount 
and relative location of cortical tissue used to produce movement in specific parts of 
the human body. For example, the control of movements that are relatively large scale 
and require little precision, such as the movement of a knee or a hip, is centered in 
a very small space in the motor area. In contrast, movements that must be precise 
and delicate, such as facial expressions and finger movements, are controlled by a 
considerably larger portion of the motor area (Schwenkreis et al., 2007). 

In short, the motor area of the cortex provides a guide to the degree of complex- 
ity and the importance of the motor capabilities of specific parts of the body. In fact, 
it may do even more: Increasing evidence shows that not only does the motor cortex 
control different parts of the body, but it may also direct body parts into complex 
postures, such as the stance of a football center just before the ball is snapped to the 
quarterback or a swimmer standing at the edge of a diving board (Graziano, Taylor, 
& Moore, 2002; Dessing et al., 2005). 

Ultimately, movement, like other behavior, is produced through the coordinated 
firing of a complex variety of neurons in the nervous system. The neurons that pro- 
duce movement are linked in elaborate ways and work closely together. 


THE SENSORY AREA OF THE CORTEX 


Given the one-to-one correspondence between the motor area and body location, it 
is not surprising to find a similar relationship between specific portions of the cortex 


sensory area The site in the brain of and the senses. The sensory area of the cortex includes three regions: one that cor- 
the tissue that corresponds to each of responds primarily to body sensations (including touch and pressure), one relating 
the senses, with the degree of to sight, and a third relating to sound. For instance, the somatosensory area in the 
sensitivity related to the amount of parietal lobe encompasses specific locations associated with the ability to perceive 


tissue. touch and pressure in a particular area of the body. As with the motor area, the 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


amount of brain tissue related to a particular location on the 
body determines the degree of sensitivity of that location: The 
greater the area devoted to a specific area of the body within 
the cortex, the more sensitive is that area of the body. As you 
can see from the weird-looking individual in Figure 6, parts 
such as the fingers are related to a larger portion of the somato- 
sensory area and are the most sensitive. 

The senses of sound and sight are also represented in spe- 
cific areas of the cerebral cortex. An auditory area located in the 
temporal lobe is responsible for the sense of hearing. If the 
auditory area is stimulated electrically, a person will hear 
sounds such as clicks or hums. It also appears that particular 
locations within the auditory area respond to specific pitches 
(Hudspeth, 2000; Brown & Martinez, 2007; Hyde, Peretz, & 
Zatorre, 2008; Bizley et al., 2009). 

The visual area in the cortex, located in the occipital lobe, 
responds in the same way to electrical stimulation. Stimulation 
by electrodes produces the experience of flashes of light or 
colors, suggesting that the raw sensory input of images from 
the eyes is received in this area of the brain and transformed 
into meaningful stimuli. The visual area provides another 
example of how areas of the brain are intimately related to specific areas of the body: 
Specific structures in the eye are related to a particular part of the cortex—with, as 
you might guess, more area of the brain given to the most sensitive portions of the 
retina (Wurtz & Kandel, 2000; Stenbacka & Vanni, 2007). 


THE ASSOCIATION AREAS OF THE CORTEX 


In a freak accident in 1848, an explosion drove a 3-feet-long iron bar completely 
through the skull of railroad worker Phineas Gage, where it remained after the acci- 
dent. Amazingly, Gage survived and, despite the rod lodged through his head, a few 
minutes later seemed to be fine. 

But he wasn’t. Before the accident, Gage was hard-working and cautious. After- 
ward, he became irresponsible, drank heavily, and drifted from one wild scheme to 
another. In the words of one of his physicians, “he was ‘no longer Gage’” (Harlow, 
1869, p. 14). 

What had happened to the old Gage? Although there is no way of knowing for 
sure, we can speculate that the accident injured the region of Gage’s cerebral cortex 
known as the association areas, which generally are considered to be the site of 
higher mental processes such as thinking, language, memory, and speech (Rowe et 
al., 2000). 

The association areas make up a large portion of the cerebral cortex and consist 
of the sections that are not directly involved in either sensory processing or directing 
movement. The association areas control executive functions, which abilities are relat- 
ing to planning, goal setting, judgment, and impulse control. 

Much of our understanding of the association areas comes from patients who, 
like Phineas Gage, have suffered some type of brain injury. For example, when parts 
of the association areas are damaged, people undergo personality changes that affect 
their ability to make moral judgments and process emotions. At the same time, people 
with damage in those areas can still be capable of reasoning logically, performing 
calculations, and recalling information (Bechara et al., 1994). 

Injuries to the association areas of the brain can produce aphasia, problems with 
language. In Broca’s aphasia, speech becomes halting, laborious, and often ungram- 
matical, and a speaker is unable to find the right words. In contrast, Wernicke’s 
aphasia produces difficulties both in understanding others’ speech and in the produc- 
tion of language. The disorder is characterized by speech that sounds fluent but 


Module 9 The Brain 87 





FIGURE 6 The greater the amount of tissue in the somatosensory 
area of the brain that is related to a specific body part, the more 
sensitive is that body part. If the size of our body parts reflected 
the corresponding amount of brain tissue, we would look like this 
strange creature. 


association areas One of the major 
regions of the cerebral cortex; the site 
of the higher mental processes, such 
as thought, language, memory, and 
speech. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


88 Chapter 3 Neuroscience and Behavior 


neuroplasticity Changes in the brain 
that occur throughout the life span 
relating to the addition of new 
neurons, new interconnections 
between neurons, and the reorganiza- 
tion of information-processing areas. 


neurogenesis The creation of new 
neurons. 


y Alert 


Remember that neuroplas- 
ticity is the reorganization 
of existing neuronal con- 
nections, whereas neuro- 
genesis is the creation of 
new neurons. 


makes no sense, as in this example from a Wernicke’s patient: “Boy, I’m sweating, 
I’m awful nervous, you know, once in a while I get caught up, I can’t mention the 
tarripoi, a month ago, quite a little . . .” (Gardner, 1975: Kearns, 2005; Caplan, Waters, 
& Dede, 2007). 





Neuroplasticity and the Brain 


Shortly after he was born, Jacob Stark’s arms and legs started jerking every 20 minutes. 
Weeks later he could not focus his eyes on his mother’s face. The diagnosis: uncontrol- 
lable epileptic seizures involving his entire brain. 

His mother, Sally Stark, recalled: “When Jacob was 22 months old, they said he 
would never learn to sit up, would never be able to feed himself. . . . They told us to 
take him home, love him, and find an institution. (Blakeslee, 1992: C3) 


Instead, Jacob had brain surgery when he was 5 months old in which physicians 
removed 20% of his brain. The operation was a complete success. Three years later 
Jacob seemed normal in every way, with no sign of seizures. 

The surgery that helped Jacob was based on the premise that the diseased part 
of his brain was producing seizures throughout the brain. Surgeons reasoned that if 
they removed the misfiring portion, the remaining parts of the brain, which appeared 
intact in PET scans, would take over. They correctly bet that Jacob could still lead a 
normal life after surgery, particularly because the surgery was being done at so 
young an age. 

The success of Jacob’s surgery illustrates that the brain has the ability to shift 
functions to different locations after injury to a specific area or in cases of surgery. 
But equally encouraging are some new findings about the regenerative powers of the 
brain and nervous system. 

Scientists have learned in recent years that the brain continually reorganizes itself 
in a process termed neuroplasticity. Although for many years conventional wisdom 
held that no new brain cells are created after childhood, new research finds other- 
wise. Not only do the interconnections between neurons become more complex 
throughout life, but it now appears that new neurons are also created in certain areas 
of the brain during adulthood—a process called neurogenesis. Each day, thousands 
of new neurons are created, especially in areas of the brain related to learning and 
memory (Jang, You, & Ahn, 2007; Poo & Isaacson, 2007; Shors, 2009). 

The ability of neurons to renew themselves during adulthood has significant impli- 
cations for the potential treatment of disorders of the nervous system (see Figure 7). 
For example, drugs that trigger the development of new neurons might be used to 
counter such diseases as Alzheimer’s, which are produced when neurons die (Tsai, 
Tsai, & Shen, 2007; Eisch et al., 2008; Waddell & Shors, 2008). 

Furthermore, specific experiences can modify the way in which information is 
processed. For example, if you learn to read Braille, the amount of tissue in your 
cortex related to sensation in the fingertips will expand. Similarly, if you take up the 
violin, the area of the brain that receives messages from your fingers will grow—but 
only relating to the fingers that actually move across the violin’s strings (Kolb, Gibb, 
& Robinson, 2003; Schwartz & Begley, 2002). 

The future also holds promise for people who suffer from the tremors and loss 
of motor control produced by Parkinson’s disease, although the research is mired in 
controversy. Because Parkinson’s disease is caused by a gradual loss of cells that 
stimulate the production of dopamine in the brain, many investigators have reasoned 
that a procedure that would increase the supply of dopamine might be effective. 
They seem to be on the right track. When stem cells—immature cells from human 
fetuses that have the potential to develop into a variety of specialized cell types, 
depending on where they are implanted—are injected directly into the brains of 
Parkinson’s sufferers, they take root and stimulate dopamine production. Preliminary 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 9 The Brain 


The Plastic Brain 





FIGURE 7 The brain is incredibly plastic, meaning that it can change structurally (through 
changing connections between neurons) and chemically (through changing levels of 
neurotransmitters and hormones) throughout our lifetime. This plasticity can also be used to 
adapt to new situations. For example, it can reorganize how it represents the motor system 
when limbs are lost. (a) Here we see how the brain learns to respond and control a new 
artificial robotic hand. In the brain scans in (b) and (c), we see the areas responsible for hand 
movement. They appear in red and yellow. The first scan (b) shows the activation in the 
motor and parietal cortex before the person has used the robotic hand. The second scan (c) 
shows greater activation after the person has used it for a month, demonstrating the brain's 
ability to alter its functioning to better utilize the new prosthetic hand. (Source: Kato et al., 2009.) 





(b) (c) 


results have been promising, with some patients showing great improvement (Kor- 
ecka, Verhaagen, & Hol, 2007; Parish & Arenas, 2007; Newman & Bakay, 2008). 

Stem cells thus hold great promise. When a stem cell divides, each newly created 
cell has the potential to be transformed into more specialized cells that have the 
potential to repair damaged cells. Because many of the most disabling diseases, rang- 
ing from cancer to stroke, result from cell damage, the potential of stem cells to 
revolutionize medicine is significant. 

However, because the source of implanted stem cells typically is aborted fetuses, 
their use is controversial. Some critics have argued that the use of stem cells in research 
and treatment should be prohibited, while supporters argue that the potential benefits 
of the research are so great that stem cell research should be unrestricted. The issue has 
been politicized, and the question of whether and how stem cell research should be 
regulated is not clear (Rosen, 2005; Giacomini, Baylis, & Robert, 2007; Holden, 2007). 


The Specialization of the 
Hemispheres: Two Brains or One? 


The most recent development, at least in evolutionary terms, in the organization and 
operation of the human brain probably occurred in the last several million years: a 
specialization of the functions controlled by the left and right sides of the brain (Hopkins 
& Cantalupo, 2008; MacNeilage, Rogers, & Vallortigara, 2009; Tommasi, 2009). 

The brain is divided into two roughly mirror-image halves. Just as we have two hemispheres Symmetrical left and 
arms, two legs, and two lungs, we have a left brain and a right brain. Because of the right halves of the brain that control 
way nerves in the brain are connected to the rest of the body, these symmetrical left the side of the body opposite to their 
and right halves, called hemispheres, control motion in—and receive sensation location. 





89 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


90 Chapter 3 Neuroscience and Behavior 


lateralization The dominance of one 
hemisphere of the brain in specific 
functions, such as language. 


y Alert 


Although the hemispheres 
of the brain specialize in 
particular kinds of 
functions, the degree of 
specialization is not great, 
and the two hemispheres work 
interdependently. 





Human Diversity and the Brain 


from—the side of the body opposite their location. The left hemisphere of the brain, 
then, generally controls the right side of the body, and the right hemisphere controls 
the left side of the body. Thus, damage to the right side of the brain is typically 
indicated by functional difficulties in the left side of the body. 

Despite the appearance of similarity between the two hemispheres of the brain, 
they are somewhat different in the functions they control and in the ways they con- 
trol them. Certain behaviors are more likely to reflect activity in one hemisphere than 
in the other, or are lateralized. 

For example, for most people, language processing occurs more in the left side 
of the brain. In general, the left hemisphere concentrates more on tasks that require 
verbal competence, such as speaking, reading, thinking, and reasoning. In addition, 
the left hemisphere tends to process information sequentially, one bit at a time 
(Turkewitz, 1993; Banich & Heller, 1998; Hines, 2004). 

The right hemisphere has its own strengths, particularly in nonverbal areas such 
as the understanding of spatial relationships, recognition of patterns and drawings, 
music, and emotional expression. The right hemisphere tends to process information 
globally, considering it as a whole (Ansaldo, Arguin, & Roch-Locours, 2002; Holowka 
& Petitto, 2002). 

Keep in mind that the differences in specialization between the hemispheres are 
not great, and the degree and nature of lateralization vary from one person to another. 
If, like most people, you are right-handed, the control of language is probably con- 
centrated more in your left hemisphere. By contrast, if you are among the 10% of 
people who are left-handed or are ambidextrous (you use both hands interchange- 
ably), it is much more likely that the language centers of your brain are located more 
in the right hemisphere or are divided equally between the left and right hemispheres. 

Furthermore, the two hemispheres of the brain function in tandem. It is a mistake 
to think of particular kinds of information as being processed solely in the right or 
the left hemisphere. The hemispheres work interdependently in deciphering, inter- 
preting, and reacting to the world. 

In addition, people who suffer injury to the left side of the brain and lose lin- 
guistic capabilities often recover the ability to speak: The right side of the brain often 
takes over some of the functions of the left side, especially in young children; the 
extent of recovery increases the earlier the injury occurs (Gould et al., 1999; Kemper- 
mann & Gage, 1999; Johnston, 2004). 

Researchers also have unearthed evidence that there may be subtle differences 
in brain lateralization patterns between males and females and members of different 
cultures, as we see next. 


i The interplay of biology and environment in behavior is 
Exploring DIVERSITY 


especially clear when we consider evidence suggesting that 
even in brain structure and function there are both sex and 
cultural differences. Let’s consider sex differences first. 
Accumulating evidence seems to show intriguing differences in 
males’ and females’ brain lateralization and weight (Kosslyn 
et al., 2002; Boles, 2005; Clements, Rimvodt, & Abel, 2006). 

For instance, most males tend to show greater lateralization of language in the left 
hemisphere. For them, language is clearly relegated largely to the left side of the brain. In 
contrast, women display less lateralization, with language abilities apt to be more evenly 
divided between the two hemispheres. Such differences in brain lateralization may account, 
in part, for the superiority often displayed by females on certain measures of verbal skills, 
such as the onset and fluency of speech (Frings et al., 2006; Petersson et al., 2007). 

Other research suggests that men’s brains are somewhat bigger than women’s brains 
even after taking differences in body size into account. In contrast, part of the corpus 
callosum, a bundle of fibers that connects the hemispheres of the brain, is proportionally 
larger in women than in men (Cahill, 2005; Luders et al., 2006; Smith et al., 2007). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Men and women also may process information differently. For example, in one 
study, fMRI brain scans of men making judgments discriminating real from false 
words showed activation of the left hemisphere of the brain, whereas women used 
areas on both sides of the brain (Rossell et al., 2002). 

The meaning of such sex differences is far from clear. Consider one possibility 
related to differences in the proportional size of the corpus callosum. Its greater size 
in women may permit stronger connections to develop between the parts of the brain 
that control speech. In turn, this would explain why speech tends to emerge slightly 
earlier in girls than in boys. 

Before we rush to such a conclusion, though, we must consider an alternative 
hypothesis: The reason verbal abilities emerge earlier in girls may be that infant girls 
receive greater encouragement to talk than do infant boys. In turn, this greater early 
experience may foster the growth of certain parts of the brain. Hence, physical brain 
differences may be a reflection of social and environmental influences rather than a 
cause of the differences in men’s and women’s behavior. At this point, it is impos- 
sible to know which of these alternative hypotheses is correct. 

Culture also gives rise to differences in brain lateralization. Native speakers of 
Japanese seem to process information regarding vowel sounds primarily in the 
brain’s left hemisphere. In contrast, North and South Americans, Europeans, and 
individuals of Japanese ancestry who learn Japanese later in life handle vowel 
sounds principally in the right hemisphere. One explanation for this difference is 
that certain characteristics of the Japanese language, such as the ability to express 
complex ideas by using only vowel sounds, result in the development of a specific 
type of brain lateralization in native speakers (Isunoda, 1985; Kess & Miyamoto, 
1994; Lin et al., 2005). 


The Split Brain: Exploring 
the Two Hemispheres 





The patient, V. J., had suffered severe seizures. By cutting her corpus callosum, the 
fibrous portion of the brain that carries messages between the hemispheres, surgeons 
hoped to create a firebreak to prevent the seizures from spreading. The operation did 
decrease the frequency and severity of V. J.’s attacks. But V. J. developed an unexpected 
side effect: She lost the ability to write at will, although she could read and spell words 
aloud. (Strauss, 1998, p. 287) 


People like V. J., whose corpus callosum has been surgically cut to stop seizures and 
who are called split-brain patients, offer a rare opportunity for researchers investigat- 
ing the independent functioning of the two hemispheres of the brain. For example, 
psychologist Roger Sperry—who won the Nobel Prize for his work—developed a 
number of ingenious techniques for studying how each hemisphere operates (Sperry, 
1982; Gazzaniga, 1998; Savazzi et al., 2007). 

In one experimental procedure, blindfolded patients touched an object with their 
right hand and were asked to name it (see Figure 8). Because the right side of the 
body corresponds to the language-oriented left side of the brain, split-brain patients 
were able to name it. However, if blindfolded patients touched the object with their 
left hand, they were unable to name it aloud, even though the information had reg- 
istered in their brains: When the blindfold was removed, patients could identify the 
object they had touched. Information can be learned and remembered, then, using 
only the right side of the brain. (By the way, unless you've had split-brain surgery, 
this experiment won’t work with you, because the bundle of fibers connecting the 
two hemispheres of a normal brain immediately transfers the information from one 
hemisphere to the other.) 


Module 9 The Brain 


91 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


92 Chapter 3 Neuroscience and Behavior 





Left cerebral hemisphere 
Site where 
corpus 
callosum 
is severed 










Corpus 
callosum 


Right cerebral” 
hemisphere 











(a) 


(b) 


FIGURE 8 Hemispheres of the brain. (a) The corpus callosum 
connects the cerebral hemispheres of the brain as shown in this 
cross-section. (b) A split-brain patient is tested by touching objects 
behind a screen. Patients could name objects when they touched it 
with their right hand, but couldn't if they touched with their left 
hand. If a split-brain patient with her eyes closed was given a pencil 
to hold and called it a pencil, what hand was the pencil in? (Source: 
Brooker et al., 2008, p. 943.) 


It is clear from experiments like this one that the right and left hemispheres of 
the brain specialize in handling different sorts of information. At the same time, it is 
important to realize that both hemispheres are capable of understanding, knowing, 
and being aware of the world, in somewhat different ways. The two hemispheres, 
then, should be regarded as different in terms of the efficiency with which they 
process certain kinds of information, rather than as two entirely separate brains. The 
hemispheres work interdependently to allow the full range and richness of thought 
of which humans are capable. 


BECOMING AN INFORMED When Tammy DeMichael was involved in a horrific car accident 

CONSUMER that broke her neck and crushed her spinal cord, experts told 

her that she was doomed to be a quadriplegic for the rest of her 

of P S yc h O | O g y life, unable to move from the neck down. But they were wrong. 

Not only did she regain the use of her arms, but she was able to 

walk 60 feet with a cane (Hess, Houg, & Tammaro, 2007 Morrow 
& Wolf, 1991). 


Learning to Control Your Heart 
—and Mind—Through Biofeedback 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 9 The Brain 93 


The key to DeMichael’s astounding recovery: biofeedback. Biofeedback is a biofeedback A procedure in which a 
procedure in which a person learns to control through conscious thought internal person learns to control through 
physiological processes such as blood pressure, heart and respiration rate, skin conscious thought internal physiologi- 
temperature, sweating, and the constriction of particular muscles. Although it cal processes such as blood pressure, 
traditionally had been thought that the heart rate, respiration rate, blood pressure, and heart and respiration rate, skin temper- 
other bodily functions are under the control of parts of the brain over which we have ature, sweating, and the constriction of 
no influence, psychologists have discovered that these responses are actually particular muscles. 
susceptible to voluntary control (Nagai et al., 2004; Cho, Holyoak, & Cannon, 2007). 

In biofeedback, a person is hooked up to electronic devices that provide con- 
tinuous feedback relating to the physiological response in question. For instance, a 
person interested in controlling headaches through biofeedback might have elec- 
tronic sensors placed on certain muscles on her head and learn to control the con- 
striction and relaxation of those muscles. Later, when she felt a headache starting, 
she could relax the relevant muscles and abort the pain (Andrasik, 2007; Nestoriuc 
et al., 2008). 

In DeMichael’s case, biofeedback was effective because not all of the nervous 
system’s connections between the brain and her legs were severed. Through biofeed- 
back, she learned how to send messages to specific muscles, “ordering” them to 
move. Although it took more than a year, DeMichael was successful in restoring a 
large degree of her mobility. 

Although the control of physiological processes through the use of biofeedback 
is not easy to learn, it has been employed with success in a variety of ailments, 
including emotional problems (such as anxiety, depression, phobias, tension head- 
aches, insomnia, and hyperactivity), physical illnesses with a psychological compo- 
nent (such as asthma, high blood pressure, ulcers, muscle spasms, and migraine 
headaches), and physical problems (such as DeMichael’s injuries, strokes, cerebral 
palsy, and curvature of the spine) (Cho, Holyoak, & Cannon, 2007; Morone & Greco, 
2007; Reiner, 2008). 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP stasis, or body equilibrium, and regulates behavior re- 
lated to basic survival). The functions of the central core 
structures are similar to those found in other vertebrates. 
This central core is sometimes referred to as the “old 
brain.” (p. 81) 

e The cerebral cortex—the “new brain’”—has areas that 
control voluntary movement (the motor area); the senses 
(the sensory area); and thinking, reasoning, speech, and 
memory (the association areas). The limbic system, 


How do researchers identify the major parts and functions of 
the brain? 

e Brain scans take a “snapshot” of the internal workings of 
the brain without having to cut surgically into a person’s 
skull. Major brain-scanning techniques include the elec- 
troencephalogram (EEG), positron emission tomography 
(PET), functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), 


and transcranial magnetic stimulation imaging (TMS). foand Gn ihe border or ihe old al en bene ee 


(p. 79) sociated with eating, aggression, reproduction, and the 
What are the major parts of the brain, and for what behaviors experiences of pleasure and pain. (p. 84) 
is each part responsible? 

e The central core of the brain is made up of the medulla 
(which controls functions such as breathing and the 
heartbeat), the pons (which coordinates the muscles and 
the two sides of the body), the cerebellum (which con- 
trols balance), the reticular formation (which acts to 
heighten awareness in emergencies), the thalamus 
(which communicates sensory messages to and from the 
brain), and the hypothalamus (which maintains homeo- 


How do the two halves of the brain operate interdependently? 
e The brain is divided into left and right halves, or hemi- 

spheres, each of which generally controls the opposite 
side of the body. Each hemisphere can be thought of as 
being specialized in the functions it carries out: The left 
specializes in verbal tasks, such as logical reasoning, 
speaking, and reading; the right specializes in nonverbal 
tasks, such as spatial perception, pattern recognition, 
and emotional expression. (p. 89) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


94 Chapter 3 Neuroscience and Behavior 


How can an understanding of the nervous system help us to 
find ways to alleviate disease and pain? 

e Biofeedback is a procedure by which a person learns to 
control internal physiological processes. By controlling 
involuntary responses, people are able to relieve anxiety, 
tension, migraine headaches, and a wide range of other 
psychological and physical problems. (p. 92) 


EVALUATE 

1. Match the name of each brain scan with the appropriate 
description: 
a. EEG 1. By locating radiation within the 
b. fMRI brain, a computer can provide a 
@ PET striking picture of brain activity. 

2. Electrodes placed around the 
skull record the electrical signals 
transmitted through the brain. 

3. This technique provides a three- 
dimensional view of the brain 
by aiming a magnetic field at the 
body. 

2. Match the portion of the brain with its function: 
a. medulla 1. Maintains breathing and 
b. pons heartbeat. 
c. cerebellum 2. Controls bodily balance. 
d. reticular 3. Coordinates and integrates 
formation muscle movements. 


4. Activates other parts of the 
brain to produce general 
bodily arousal. 


KEY TERMS 


central core p. 81 hypothalamus p. 83 

cerebellum limbic system p. 83 
(ser-uh-BELL-um) p. 81 cerebral cortex p. 84 

reticular formation p. 81 lobes p. 85 


thalamus p. 83 


. A surgeon places an electrode on a portion of your brain 


and stimulates it. Immediately, your right wrist involun- 
tarily twitches. The doctor has most likely stimulated a 
portion of the ____ area of your brain. 


. Each hemisphere controls the side of the body. 
. Nonverbal realms, such as emotions and music, are con- 


trolled primarily by the ____ hemisphere of the 
brain, whereas the ______ hemisphere is more 
responsible for speaking and reading. 


RETHINK 


il 


Before sophisticated brain-scanning techniques were de- 
veloped, behavioral neuroscientists’ understanding of the 
brain was based largely on the brains of people who had 
died. What limitations would this pose, and in what areas 
would you expect the most significant advances once 
brain-scanning techniques became possible? 


. Could personal differences in people’s specialization of 


right and left hemispheres be related to occupational suc- 
cess? For example, might an architect who relies on spatial 
skills have a pattern of hemispheric specialization differ- 
ent from that of a writer? 


. From the perspective of an educator: How might you use dif- 


ferent techniques to teach reading to boys and girls based 
on the brain evidence? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


Yo 43u g ‘aytsoddo p ‘10jour*E ‘p-p Z- ‘E-q 'T-2 "TZ ‘1-9 E-G ‘TPT 


motor area p. 86 neurogenesis p. 88 
sensory area p. 86 hemispheres p. 89 
association areas p. 87 lateralization p. 90 
neuroplasticity p. 88 biofeedback p. 93 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Psychology on the Web 


1. Biofeedback research is continuously changing and being applied to new areas of 
human functioning. Find at least two websites that discuss recent research on 
biofeedback, and summarize the research and any findings it has produced. Include 
in your summary your best estimate of future applications of this technique. 

2. Find one or more websites on Parkinson's disease and learn more about this topic. 
Specifically, find reports of new treatments for Parkinson’s disease that do not involve 
the use of fetal tissue. Write a summary of your findings. 


E D | | OQ U e In our examination of neuroscience, we've traced the 


ways in which biological structures and functions of the 
body affect behavior. Starting with neurons, we considered each of the components of 
the nervous system, culminating in an examination of how the brain permits us to think, 
reason, speak, recall, and experience emotions—the hallmarks of being human. 

Before proceeding, turn back for a moment to the chapter prologue about Carol 
Poe, who underwent brain surgery to produce feelings of fullness in order to help her 
lose weight. Consider the following questions. 





1. In what region of Carol Poe's brain were the electrodes most likely inserted, and why? 

2. If a small region of Carol Poe's brain were damaged by the surgical procedure, would 
she necessarily lose forever the function associated with the damaged region? Why or 
why not? 

3. What other parts of the nervous or endocrine system might be involved in the 
regulation of hunger? 

4. Do you think that biofeedback techniques could be used to help a patient such as 
Carol Poe to regulate her own hunger? Why or why not? 





95 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


ie 

X 

SEN ©. 
tf. 


me Sere! TSO SS 


SS 

a 
L 
3 


X 
Ș NS \~ D ae 
WARY 7 
SHR) ee, cS ff 


NNA 


S Wa 


IDES 


ZB 
Zam 
ES 


~~ 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Key Concepts for Chapter 4 


MODULE 10 








What is sensation, and how do psychologists Sensing the World Around Us 


Absolute Thresholds: Detecting 
What's Out There 


Applying Psychology in the 21st Century: 
Driven to Distraction 


study it? @ What is the relationship between 
a physical stimulus and the kinds of sensory 


responses that result from it? Difference Thresholds: Noticing 


Distinctions Between Stimuli 


Sensory Adaptation: Turning Down 
Our Responses 







What basic processes underlie the sense of Vision: Shedding Light on the Eye 


dision? @ How de we see colors? Illuminating the Structure of the Eye 


Neuroscience in Your Life: 

Seeing Vision in the Brain 

Color Vision and Color Blindness: 
The 7-Million-Color Spectrum 


MODULE 12 







What role does the ear play in the senses of Hearing and the Other Senses 


; i d 
sound, motion, and balance? @ How do sensing sgun 
Smell and Taste 


smell and taste function? @ What are the The Skin Senses: Touch, Pressure, 


Temperature, and Pain 





skin senses, and how do they relate to the ceanna iir Anane 


experience of pain? of Psychology: Managing Pain 
How Our Senses Interact 





What principles underlie our organization of the Perceptual Organization: 


Constructing Our View of the World 


visual world and allow us to make sense of our = a 
he Gestalt Laws of Organization 





environment? @ How are we able to perceive the Top-Down and Bottom-Up Processing 


, , : . Depth Perception: Translating 2-D to 3-D 
world in three dimensions when our retinas are 5 
erceptual Constancy 
capable of sensing only two-dimensional images? Motion Perception: As the World Turns 
; r . R Perceptual Illusions: The Deceptions 
@ What clues do visual illusions give us about our af Percestions 
understanding of general perceptual mechanisms? r Diversity: Culture and 
erception 





97 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Prologue Never Forgetting a Face 





She never forgets a face. Literally. 

For a woman known as C. S., remembering people is not a 
problem. In fact, she—like a very few other individuals—can 
remember faces of people she met years ago, sometimes only in 
passing. These “super-recognizers,’ as they are called, excel at 
recalling faces. 

One super-recognizer said she-had identified another woman 
on the street who served her as a waitress five years earlier in a 
different city. Critically, she was able to confirm that the other 
woman had, in fact, been a waitress in the different city. Often, 
super-recognizers are able to recognize another person despite 


\ 


significant changes in appearance, such as aging or a different 
hair color. 

But being a super-recognizer is a mixed blessing. As one 
woman with this ability says, “It doesn’t matter how many years 
pass, if I've seen your face before | will be able to recall it” In 
fact, she sometimes pretends she doesn’t remember a person, 
“because it seems like | stalk them, or that they mean more to me 
than they do when | recall that we saw each other once walking 
on campus four years ago in front of the quad!” (Munger, 2009; 
Russell, Duchaine, & Nakayma, 2009) 





Most of us are reasonably good at recognizing people's faces, 
thanks in part to regions of the brain that specialize in detecting 
facial patterns. Super-recognizers represent a small minority of 
people who happen to be exceptionally good at facial recogni- 
tion. At the other extreme are people with “faceblindness,’ a rare 
disorder that makes it extremely difficult for them to recognize 
faces at all—even those of friends and family. 

Disorders such as super-recognition and faceblindness 
illustrate how much we depend on our senses to function 
normally. Our senses offer a window to the world, not only 
providing us with an awareness, understanding, and apprecia- 
tion of the world’s beauty, but alerting us to its dangers. Our 
senses enable us to feel the gentlest of breezes, see flickering 
lights miles away, and hear the soft murmuring of distant 
songbirds. 

In the next four modules, we focus on the field of psychology 
that is concerned with the ways our bodies take in information 
through the senses and the ways we interpret that information. 
We explore both sensation and perception. Sensation encom- 
passes the processes by which our sense organs receive informa- 
tion from the environment. Perception is the brain’s and the sense 
organs’ sorting out, interpretation, analysis, and integration of 
stimuli. 


98 


Although perception clearly represents a step beyond 
sensation, in practice it is sometimes difficult to find the precise 
boundary between the two. Indeed, psychologists—and 
philosophers as well—have argued for years over the distinction. 
The primary difference is that sensation can be thought of as an 
organism’s first encounter with a raw sensory stimulus, whereas 
perception is the process by which it interprets, analyzes, and 
integrates that stimulus with other sensory information. 

For example, if we were considering sensation, we might ask 
about the loudness of a ringing fire alarm. If we were considering 
perception, we might ask whether someone recognizes the 
ringing sound as an alarm and identifies its meaning. 

To a psychologist interested in understanding the causes of 
behavior, sensation and perception are fundamental topics, 
because so much of our behavior is a reflection of how we react 
to and interpret stimuli from the world around us. The areas of 
sensation and perception deal with a wide range of questions— 
among them, how we respond to the characteristics of physical 
stimuli; what processes enable us to see, hear, and experience 
pain; why visual illusions fool us; and how we distinguish one 
person from another. As we explore these issues, we'll see how 
the senses work together to provide us with an integrated view 
and understanding of the world. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 10 


As Isabel sat down to Thanksgiving dinner, her father carried the turkey in on a tray and 
placed it squarely in the center of the table. The noise level, already high from the talking 
and laughter of family members, grew louder still. As Isabel picked up her fork, the smell 
of the turkey reached her and she felt her stomach growl hungrily. The sight and sound 
of her family around the table, along with the smells and tastes of the holiday meal, 
made Isabel feel more relaxed than she had since starting school in the fall. 


Put yourself in this setting and consider how different it might be if any one of your 
senses were not functioning. What if you were blind and unable to see the faces of 
your family members or the welcome shape of the golden-brown turkey? What if 
you had no sense of hearing and could not listen to the conversations of family 
members or were unable to feel your stomach growl, smell the dinner, or taste the 
food? Clearly, you would experience the dinner very differently from someone whose 
sensory apparatus was intact. 

Moreover, the sensations mentioned above barely scratch the surface of sensory 
experience. Although perhaps you were taught, as I was, that there are just five 
senses—sight, sound, taste, smell, and touch—that enumeration is too modest. 
Human sensory capabilities go well beyond the basic five senses. For example, we 
are sensitive not merely to touch but to a considerably wider set of stimuli—pain, 
pressure, temperature, and vibration, to name a few. In addition, vision has two 
subsystems—relating to day and night vision—and the ear is responsive to informa- 
tion that allows us not only to hear but also to keep our balance. 

To consider how psychologists understand the senses and, more broadly, sensation 
and perception, we first need a basic working vocabulary. In formal terms, sensation 
is the activation of the sense organs by a source of physical energy. Perception is the 
sorting out, interpretation, analysis, and integration of stimuli carried out by the sense 
organs and brain. A stimulus is any passing source of physical energy that produces 
a response in a sense organ. 

Stimuli vary in both type and intensity. Different types of stimuli activate differ- 
ent sense organs. For instance, we can differentiate light stimuli (which activate the 
sense of sight and allow us to see the colors of a tree in autumn) from sound stimuli 
(which, through the sense of hearing, permit us to hear the sounds of an orchestra). 
In addition, stimuli differ in intensity, relating to how strong a stimulus needs to be 
before it can be detected. 

Questions of stimulus type and intensity are considered in a branch of psychol- 
ogy known as psychophysics. Psychophysics is the study of the relationship between 
the physical aspects of stimuli and our psychological experience of them. Psycho- 
physics played a central role in the development of the field of psychology. Many of 
the first psychologists studied issues related to psychophysics, and there is still an 
active group of psychophysics researchers (Chechile, 2003; Gardner, 2005; Hock & 
Ploeger, 2006). 


What is sensation, and how 
do psychologists study it? 


What is the relationship 
between a physical stimulus 
and the kinds of sensory 
responses that result from it? 


ly Alert 


Remember that sensation 
refers to the activation 
of the sense organs 
(a physical response), 
whereas perception 
refers to how stimuli are interpreted 
(a psychological response). 


The activation of the sense 
organs by a source of physical energy. 


The sorting out, interpre- 
tation, analysis, and integration of 
stimuli by the sense organs and brain. 


Energy that produces a 
response in a sense organ. 


The study of the 
relationship between the physical 
aspects of stimuli and our psychologi- 
cal experience of them. 


99 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


100 Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception 





Crowded conditions, sounds, and sights 
can all be considered as noise that 
interferes with sensation. Can you think 
of other examples of noise that is not 
auditory in nature? 


absolute threshold The smallest 
intensity of a stimulus that must be 
present for the stimulus to be detected. 


FIGURE 1 This test can shed some light 
on how sensitive the human senses are. 
(Source: Galanter, 1962.) 


Absolute Thresholds: 
Detecting What’s Out There 


Just when does a stimulus become strong enough to be detected by our sense organs? 
The answer to this question requires an understanding of the concept of absolute 
threshold. An absolute threshold is the smallest intensity of a stimulus that must be 
present for it to be detected (Aazh & Moore, 2007). 

Despite the “absolute” in absolute threshold, things are not so cut and dried. As 
the strength of a stimulus increases, the likelihood that it will be detected increases 
gradually. Technically, then, an absolute threshold is the stimulus intensity that is 
detected 50% of the time. 

It often takes a very small stimulus to produce a response in our senses. For 
example, the sense of touch is so sensitive that we can feel a bee’s wing falling on our 
cheeks when it is dropped from a distance of 1 centimeter. Test your knowledge of the 
absolute thresholds of other senses by completing the questionnaire in Figure 1. 

In fact, our senses are so fine-tuned that we might have problems if they were 
any more sensitive. For instance, if our ears were slightly more acute, we would be 
able to hear the sound of air molecules in our ears knocking into the eardrum—a 
phenomenon that would surely prove distracting and might even prevent us from 
hearing sounds outside our bodies. 

Of course, the absolute thresholds we have been discussing are measured under 
ideal conditions. Normally our senses cannot detect stimulation quite as well because 
of the presence of noise. Noise, as defined by psychophysicists, is background stimu- 
lation that interferes with the perception of other stimuli. Hence, noise refers not just 
to auditory stimuli, as the word suggests, but also to unwanted stimuli that interfere 
with other senses. 

For example, picture a talkative group of people crammed into a small, crowded, 
smoke-filled room at a party. The din of the crowd makes it hard to hear individual 
voices, and the smoke makes it difficult to see, or even taste, the food. In this case, 
the smoke and the crowded conditions would both be considered “noise,” because 





—Keahialiabaalilaalie PPPOE 


How Sensitive Are You? 
To test your awareness of the capabilities of your senses, answer the following questions: 


1. How far can a candle flame be seen on a clear, dark night: 
a. From a distance of 10 miles 
b. From a distance of 30 miles 
2. How far can the ticking of a watch be heard under quiet conditions? 
a. From 5 feet away 
b. From 20 feet away 
3. How much sugar is needed to allow it to be detected when dissolved in 2 gallons 
of water? 
a. 2 tablespoons 
b. 1 teaspoon 
4. Over what area can a drop of perfume be detected? 
a. A 5-foot by 5-foot area 
b. A 3-room apartment 


Scoring: ‘sasuas ino Jo Áyanısuəs SnOopUaWad} əy} Gulyessnj|! ‘q sı Jamsue au} ‘ased Yydea uj 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Driven to Distraction 


Twenty-four year old Aiden Quinn 
couldn’t believe his good fortune. At his 
young age, he had beaten the competition 
to land his dream job of conducting 
trolleys through the streets of Boston. But 
in just one moment his world came 
crashing down around him: It was the 





moment when he took his eyes off the 
tracks to send a text message to his Increasing research shows that texting while 
girlfriend and the trolley he was driving driving is extremely dangerous. 

crashed into the one ahead of it. 


If you've ever texted while driving, you’re 
not alone. A recent survey conducted by 
the AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety 
shows that nearly half of drivers aged 18 to 
24 admit to doing so, even though the ma- 
jority of them were aware that they were 
increasing their risk of having an accident. 
In fact, nearly 90% of survey respondents 
rated texting while driving as a very seri- 
ous threat to safety (AAA Foundation for 
Traffic Safety, 2008, 2009). 

Researchers are now confirming what 
intuition suggests is true: The distraction 
caused by texting while driving is ex- 


seemingly short period of time until you 
realize that a vehicle at highway speed 
travels about 150 yards in those few sec- 
onds (Virginia Tech Transportation Insti- 
tute, 2009). 

Another study using college students 
and a driving simulator showed that text- 
ing produced the same duration of about 
5 seconds of distraction and that the stu- 
dents were 8 times more likely to have a 
collision while texting. Although those 
may seem like better odds, they’re still 


tremely dangerous. One study used video RETHINK Žž  —  — > —— —————__—__ 


cameras to monitor truckers on long hauls e Most people assume that they are able to text and drive at the same time without 
and found that they were 23 times more declines in their driving ability. Why do you think that is the case, and how accu- 
likely to be in a collision while texting than rate is their assumption? 

while not texting. In collisions caused by e Why would frequent multitaskers be more distractible than those who multitask 
texting, the driver had typically taken his less frequently? 





eyes off the road for about 5 seconds—a 


much worse than the risk presented by 
other distractors such as changing the sta- 
tion on a car’s radio. Even though the 
study used students, who are likely skillful 
at texting and multitasking, and light ve- 
hicles that are more maneuverable than 
trucks, the results clearly show that texting 
while driving poses a terrific risk (Cooper 
& Strayer, 2008). 

Multitasking in general is difficult. For 
instance, in one study, researchers divided 
a group of college students into two 
groups, one composed of students who 
multitasked online regularly and a second 
that did not. The frequent multitaskers 
were more easily distracted, and they were 
unable to filter out stimuli that were irrele- 
vant to tasks that they were trying to com- 
plete (Ophir, Nass, & Wagner, 2009) 

If you, like many of the respondents to 
the AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety sur- 
vey, believe that you’re the exception to the 
rule, just keep one thing in mind: that’s 
what Aiden Quinn thought, too. He lost his 
job and is facing civil lawsuits and criminal 
prosecution. 








they are preventing sensation at more discriminating levels. Similarly, we have lim- 
ited ability to concentrate on several stimuli simultaneously, as we discuss in Apply- 
ing Psychology in the 21st Century. 


Difference Thresholds: Noticing 
Distinctions Between Stimuli 


Suppose you wanted to choose the six best apples from a supermarket display—the 
biggest, reddest, and sweetest apples. One approach would be to compare one apple 
with another systematically until you were left with a few so similar that you could 
not tell the difference between them. At that point, it wouldn’t matter which ones 
you chose. 

Psychologists have discussed this comparison problem in terms of the difference 
threshold, the smallest level of added (or reduced) stimulation required to sense that 





difference threshold (just noticeable 
difference) The smallest level of 
added or reduced stimulation required 
to sense that a change in stimulation 
has occurred. 


101 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


102 Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception 


Weber's law A basic law of psycho- 
physics stating that a just noticeable 
difference is a constant proportion to 
the intensity of an initial stimulus 
(rather than a constant amount). 


ly Alert 


Remember that Weber's law 
holds for every type of sen- 
sory stimuli: vision, sound, 
taste, and so on. 


adaptation An adjustment in sensory 
capacity after prolonged exposure to 
unchanging stimuli. 


a change in stimulation has occurred. Thus, the difference threshold is the minimum 
change in stimulation required to detect the difference between two stimuli, and so 
it also is called a just noticeable difference (Nittrouer & Lowenstein, 2007). 

The size of a stimulus that constitutes a just noticeable difference depends on 
the initial intensity of the stimulus. The relationship between changes in the original 
size of a stimulus and the degree to which a change will be noticed forms one of the 
basic laws of psychophysics: Weber’s law. Weber’s law (Weber is pronounced “VAY- 
ber”) states that a just noticeable difference is a constant proportion of the intensity of 
an initial stimulus (rather than a constant amount). 

For example, Weber found that the just noticeable difference for weight is 1:50. 
Consequently, it takes a 1-ounce increase in a 50-ounce weight to produce a notice- 
able difference, and it would take a 10-ounce increase to produce a noticeable differ- 
ence if the initial weight were 500 ounces. In both cases, the same proportional 
increase is necessary to produce a just noticeable difference—1:5 = 10:500. Similarly, 
the just noticeable difference distinguishing changes in loudness between sounds is 
larger for sounds that are initially loud than it is for sounds that are initially soft, 
but the proportional increase remains the same. 

Weber’s law helps explain why a person in a quiet room is more startled by the 
ringing of a telephone than is a person in an already noisy room. To produce the 
same amount of reaction in a noisy room, a telephone ring might have to approxi- 
mate the loudness of cathedral bells. Similarly, when the moon is visible during the 
late afternoon, it appears relatively dim—yet against a dark night sky, it seems quite 
bright. 


Sensory Adaptation: 
Turning Down Our Responses 


You enter a movie theater, and the smell of popcorn is everywhere. A few minutes 
later, though, you barely notice the smell. The reason you become accustomed to the 
odor is sensory adaptation. Adaptation is an adjustment in sensory capacity after 
prolonged exposure to unchanging stimuli. Adaptation occurs as people become 
accustomed to a stimulus and change their frame of reference. In a sense, our brain 
mentally turns down the volume of the stimulation that it’s experiencing (Calin- 
Jageman & Fischer, 2007). 

One example of adaptation is the decrease in sensitivity that occurs after repeated 
exposure to a strong stimulus. If you were to hear a loud tone over and over again, 
eventually it would begin to sound softer. Similarly, although jumping into a cold 
lake may be temporarily unpleasant, eventually you probably will get used to the 
temperature. 

This apparent decline in sensitivity to sensory stimuli is due to the inability of 
the sensory nerve receptors to fire off messages to the brain indefinitely. Because these 
receptor cells are most responsive to changes in stimulation, constant stimulation is 
not effective in producing a sustained reaction (Wark, Lundstrom, & Fairhall, 2007). 

Judgments of sensory stimuli are also affected by the context in which the judg- 
ments are made. This is the case because judgments are made not in isolation from 
other stimuli but in terms of preceding sensory experience. You can demonstrate this 
for yourself by trying a simple experiment: 

Take two envelopes, one large and one small, and put 15 nickels in each one. 
Now lift the large envelope, put it down, and lift the small one. Which seems to 
weigh more? Most people report that the small one is heavier, although, as you know, 
the weights are nearly identical. The reason for this misconception is that the visual 
context of the envelope interferes with the sensory experience of weight. Adaptation 
to the context of one stimulus (the size of the envelope) alters responses to another 
stimulus (the weight of the envelope) (Coren, 2004). 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 10 Sensing the World Around Us 103 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP EVALUATE 
What is sensation, and how do psychologists study it? 1, ______is the stimulation of the sense organs; 

e Sensation is the activation of the sense organs by any ——~ ~~~ isthe sorting out, interpretation, analysis, and 
source of physical energy. In contrast, perception is the integration of stimuli by the sense organs and the brain. 
process by which we sort out, interpret, analyze, and in- 2. The term absolute threshold refers to the ______ inten- 
tegrate stimuli to which our senses are exposed. (p. 99) sity of a stimulus that must be present for the stimulus to 


be detected. 
3. Weber discovered that for a difference between two stim- 
uli to be perceptible, the stimuli must differ by at least a 
proportion. 
4, After completing a very difficult rock climb in the morn- 
ing, Carmella found the afternoon climb unexpectedly 
easy. This example illustrates the phenomenon of 


What is the relationship between a physical stimulus and the 
kinds of sensory responses that result from it? 

e Psychophysics studies the relationship between the 
physical nature of stimuli and the sensory responses 
they evoke. (p. 99) 

e The absolute threshold is the smallest amount of physi- 
cal intensity at which a stimulus can be detected. Under 
ideal conditions absolute thresholds are extraordinarily 
sensitive, but the presence of noise (background stimuli 
that interfere with other stimuli) reduces detection capa- 


bilities. (p. 100) BENS 
e The difference threshold, or just noticeable difference, is 1. Do you think it is possible to have sensation without 
the smallest change in the level of stimulation required perception? Is it possible to have perception without 
to sense that a change has occurred. According to sensation? 
Weber’s law, a just noticeable difference is a constant 2. From the perspective of a manufacturer: How might you need 
proportion of the intensity of an initial stimulus. (p. 101) to take psychophysics into account when developing new 
e Sensory adaptation occurs when we become accustomed products or modifying existing ones? 
to a constant stimulus and change our evaluation of it. 
Repeated exposure to a stimulus results in an apparent Answers to Evaluate Questions 
decline in sensitivity to it. (p. 102) uoyejdepe ‘p 4uejsuod ‘g 4satpeuts ‘z ‘uoydaoied ‘uoHesuas ‘T 
sensation p. 99 absolute threshold p. 100 Weber’s law p. 102 
perception p. 99 difference threshold (just adaptation p. 102 


stimulus p. 99 noticeable difference) p. 101 
psychophysics p. 99 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 11 


If, as poets say, the eyes provide a window to the soul, they also provide us with a 
window to the world. Our visual capabilities permit us to admire and to react to 
scenes ranging from the beauty of a sunset, to the configuration of a lover’s face, to 
the words written on the pages of a book. 

Vision starts with light, the physical energy that stimulates the eye. Light is a 
form of electromagnetic radiation waves, which, as shown in Figure 1, are mea- 
sured in wavelengths. The sizes of wavelengths correspond to different types of 
energy. The range of wavelengths that humans are sensitive to—called the visual 
spectrum—is relatively small. Many nonhuman species have different capabilities. 
For instance, some reptiles and fish sense energies of longer wavelengths than 
humans do, and certain insects sense energies of shorter wavelengths than humans 
do. For example, bees are attracted to flowers that reflect ultraviolet rays that 
humans can’t detect. 

Light waves coming from some object outside the body (such as the tree in 
Figure 2) are sensed by the only organ that is capable of responding to the visible 
spectrum: the eye. Our eyes convert light to a form that can be used by the neurons 
that serve as messengers to the brain. The neurons themselves take up a relatively 
small percentage of the total eye. Most of the eye is a mechanical device that is 
similar in many respects to a nonelectronic camera that uses film, as you can see in 
Figure 2. 

Despite the similarities between the eye and a camera, vision involves processes 
that are far more complex and sophisticated than those of any camera. Furthermore, 
once an image reaches the neuronal receptors of the eye, the eye/camera analogy 
ends, for the processing of the visual image in the brain is more reflective of a com- 
puter than it is of a camera. 


What basic processes underlie 
the sense of vision? 


How do we see colors? 





Gamma Ultraviolet Shortwave 
Nays X-rays rey Infrared Radar FM TV AM AC 
| | | | rays | | | | electricity | 
| | | | | | | | | | | | | 
10-14 10-12 10-10 10-8 10-6 10-4 10-2 10! 102 104 106 108 
Wavelength in meters 
Visible light 
Violet Blue Green Yellow Red 
E EE )6hl(tkGa 
400 500 600 700 


Wavelength in nanometers (billionths of a meter) 

GURE 1 The visible spectrum—the range of wavelengths to which people are sensitive—is 
only a small part of the kinds of wavelengths present in our environment. Is it a benefit or 
disadvantage to our everyday lives that we aren't more sensitive to a broader range of visual 
stimuli? Why? 


104 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 11 Vision: Shedding Light on the Eye 105 


A camera’s lens focuses the 
inverted image on the film in 
the same way the eye’s lens 
focuses images on the retina. 
















Cornea Optic nerve 
lris Blind spot 
Pupil f | Retina 


Nonsensor cells 
of retina 


FIGURE 2 Although human vision is far more complicated than the most sophisticated 
camera, in some ways basic visual processes are analogous to those used in photography. 
Like the automatic lighting system on a traditional, nondigital camera, the human eye dilates 
to let in more light and contracts to block out light. 


Illuminating the Structure 
of the Eye 


The ray of light being reflected off the tree in Figure 2 first travels through the cornea, 
a transparent, protective window. The cornea, because of its curvature, bends (or 
refracts) light as it passes through, playing a primary role in focusing the light more 
sharply. After moving through the cornea, the light traverses the pupil. The pupil is 
a dark hole in the center of the iris, the colored part of the eye, which in humans 
ranges from a light blue to a dark brown. The size of the pupil opening depends on 
the amount of light in the environment. The dimmer the surroundings are, the more 
the pupil opens to allow more light to enter. 





Like the automatic lighting system on 
a camera, the pupil in the human eye 
expands to let in more light (left) and 
contracts to block out light (right). Can 
humans adjust their ears to let in more 
or less sound in a similar manner? 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


106 Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception 


retina The part of the eye that 
converts the electromagnetic energy of 
light to electrical impulses for trans- 
mission to the brain. 


rods Thin, cylindrical receptor cells in 
the retina that are highly sensitive to 
light. 


cones Cone-shaped, light-sensitive 
receptor cells in the retina that are 
responsible for sharp focus and color 
perception, particularly in bright light. 






udy Alert 
Remember that cones relate 
to color vision. 


Why shouldn’t the pupil be open completely all the time, allowing the greatest 
amount of light into the eye? The answer relates to the basic physics of light. A small 
pupil greatly increases the range of distances at which objects are in focus. With a 
wide-open pupil, the range is relatively small, and details are harder to discern. The 
eye takes advantage of bright light by decreasing the size of the pupil and thereby 
becoming more discriminating. In dim light the pupil expands to enable us to view 
the situation better—but at the expense of visual detail. (Perhaps one reason candle- 
light dinners are thought of as romantic is that the dim light prevents one from 
seeing a partner’s physical flaws.) 

Once light passes through the pupil, it enters the lens, which is directly behind 
the pupil. The lens acts to bend the rays of light so that they are properly focused 
on the rear of the eye. The lens focuses light by changing its own thickness, a process 
called accommodation: It becomes flatter when viewing distant objects and rounder 
when looking at closer objects. 


REACHING THE RETINA 


Having traveled through the pupil and lens, the image of the tree finally reaches its 
ultimate destination in the eye—the retina. It is within the retina that the electromag- 
netic energy of light is converted to electrical impulses for transmission to the brain. 
Note that, because of the physical properties of light, the image has reversed itself 
in traveling through the lens, and it reaches the retina upside down (relative to its 
original position). Although it might seem that this reversal would cause difficulties 
in understanding and moving about the world, this is not the case. The brain inter- 
prets the image in terms of its original position. 

The retina consists of a thin layer of nerve cells at the back of the eyeball (see 
Figure 3). There are two kinds of light-sensitive receptor cells in the retina. The names 
they have been given describe their shapes: rods and cones. Rods are thin, cylindri- 
cal receptor cells that are highly sensitive to light. Cones are typically cone-shaped, 
light-sensitive receptor cells that are responsible for sharp focus and color perception, 
particularly in bright light. The rods and cones are distributed unevenly throughout 
the retina. Cones are concentrated on the part of the retina called the fovea. The fovea 
is a particularly sensitive region of the retina. If you want to focus on something of 
particular interest, you will automatically try to center the image on the fovea to see 
it more sharply. 

The rods and cones not only are structurally dissimilar but they also play dis- 
tinctly different roles in vision. Cones are primarily responsible for the sharply 
focused perception of color, particularly in brightly lit situations; rods are related to 
vision in dimly lit situations and are largely insensitive to color and to details as 
sharp as those the cones are capable of recognizing. The rods play a key role in 
peripheral vision—seeing objects that are outside the main center of focus—and in 
night vision. 

Rods and cones also are involved in dark adaptation, the phenomenon of adjusting 
to dim light after being in brighter light. (Think of the experience of walking into a 
dark movie theater and groping your way to a seat but a few minutes later seeing 
the seats quite clearly.) The speed at which dark adaptation occurs is a result of the 
rate of change in the chemical composition of the rods and cones. Although the cones 
reach their greatest level of adaptation in just a few minutes, the rods take 20 to 30 
minutes to reach the maximum level. The opposite phenomenon—light adaptation, or 
the process of adjusting to bright light after exposure to dim light—occurs much 
faster, taking only a minute or so. 


SENDING THE MESSAGE FROM THE EYE TO THE BRAIN 


When light energy strikes the rods and cones, it starts a chain of events that trans- 
forms light into neural impulses that can be communicated to the brain. Even before 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 11 Vision: Shedding Light on the Eye 107 


Cornea 


Light waves 





Impulses to Lens 
optic nerve 


f Retina 


Nerve fibers 


Light 


waves »_ a 


Front of eye 


Ganglion cell Bipolar cell Cone Rod 
N J 








Fovea 


Back of eye 








y Wr 
Layer of connecting neurons Receptor cells 


FIGURE 3 The basic cells of the eye. Light entering the eye travels through the ganglion and 
bipolar cells and strikes the light-sensitive rods and cones located at the back of the eye. The 
rods and cones then transmit nerve impulses to the brain via the bipolar and ganglion cells. 
(Source: Shier, Butler, & Lewis, 2000.) 


the neural message reaches the brain, however, some initial coding of the visual 
information takes place. 

What happens when light energy strikes the retina depends in part on whether 
it encounters a rod or a cone. Rods contain rhodopsin, a complex reddish-purple 
substance whose composition changes chemically when energized by light. The sub- 
stance in cone receptors is different, but the principles are similar. Stimulation of the 
nerve cells in the eye triggers a neural response that is transmitted to other nerve 
cells in the retina called bipolar cells and ganglion cells. 

Bipolar cells receive information directly from the rods and cones and commu- 
nicate that information to the ganglion cells. The ganglion cells collect and summa- 
rize visual information, which is then moved out the back of the eyeball and sent to 
the brain through a bundle of ganglion axons called the optic nerve. 

Because the opening for the optic nerve passes through the retina, there are no 
rods or cones in the area, and that creates a blind spot. Normally, however, this 


optic nerve A bundle of ganglion 
axons that carry visual information to 
the brain. 


108 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception 








FIGURE 4 To find your blind spot, close your right eye and look at the haunted house 
with your left eye. You will see the ghost on the periphery of your vision. Now, while 
staring at the house, move the page toward you. When the book is about a foot from your 
eye, the ghost will disappear. At this moment, the image of the ghost is falling on your 
blind spot. 

But also notice how, when the page is at that distance, not only does the ghost seem 
to disappear, but the line seems to run continuously through the area where the ghost 
used to be. This simple experiment shows how we automatically compensate for missing 
information by using nearby material to complete what is unseen. That's the reason you 
never notice the blind spot. What is missing is replaced by what is seen next to the blind 
spot. Can you think of any advantages that this tendency to provide missing information 
gives humans as a species? 


absence of nerve cells does not interfere with vision because you automatically com- 
pensate for the missing part of your field of vision. (To find your blind spot, see 
Figure 4.) 

Once beyond the eye itself, the neural impulses relating to the image move 
through the optic nerve. As the optic nerve leaves the eyeball, its path does not take 
the most direct route to the part of the brain right behind the eye. Instead, the optic 
nerves from each eye meet at a point roughly between the two eyes—called the optic 
chiasm (pronounced KI-asm)—where each optic nerve then splits. 

When the optic nerves split, the nerve impulses coming from the right half of 
each retina are sent to the right side of the brain, and the impulses arriving from the 
left half of each retina are sent to the left side of the brain. Because the image on the 
retinas is reversed and upside down, however, those images coming from the right 
half of each retina actually originated in the field of vision to the person’s left, and 
the images coming from the left half of each retina originated in the field of vision 
to the person’s right (see Figure 5). 


PROCESSING THE VISUAL MESSAGE 


By the time a visual message reaches the brain, it has passed through several stages 
of processing. One of the initial sites is the ganglion cells. Each ganglion cell gathers 
information from a group of rods and cones in a particular area of the eye and com- 
pares the amount of light entering the center of that area with the amount of light 
in the area around it. Some ganglion cells are activated by light in the center (and 
darkness in the surrounding area). Other ganglion cells are activated when there is 
darkness in the center and light in the surrounding areas. The outcome of this process 
is to maximize the detection of variations in light and darkness. The image that is 
passed on to the brain, then, is an enhanced version of the actual visual stimulus 
outside the body (Kubovy, Epstein, & Gepshtein, 2003; Pearson & Clifford, 2005; 
Lascaratos, Ji, & Wood, 2007). 

The ultimate processing of visual images takes place in the visual cortex of the 
brain, and it is here that the most complex kinds of processing occur. Psychologists 
David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel won the Nobel Prize in 1981 for their discovery that 
many neurons in the cortex are extraordinarily specialized, being activated only by 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 11 Vision: Shedding Light on the Eye 109 


Primary visual area 
of cerebral cortex 










Optic tract 





Optic chiasm 


Optic nerve 


Right 


visual field Left visual field 


visual stimuli of a particular shape or pattern—a process known as feature detection. 
They found that some cells are activated only by lines of a particular width, shape, 
or orientation. Other cells are activated only by moving, as opposed to stationary, 
stimuli (Hubel & Wiesel, 2004; Pelli, Burns, & Farell, 2006). 

More recent work has added to our knowledge of the complex ways in which 
visual information coming from individual neurons is combined and processed. Dif- 
ferent parts of the brain process nerve impulses in several individual systems simul- 
taneously. For instance, one system relates to shapes, one to colors, and others to 
movement, location, and depth. Furthermore, different parts of the brain are involved 
in the perception of specific kinds of stimuli, showing distinctions, for example, 
between the perception of human faces, animals, and inanimate stimuli (Bindemann 
et al., 2008; Platek & Kemp, 2009; Werblin & Roska, 2007; Winston, O’Doherty, & 
Kilner, 2006). 

If separate neural systems exist for processing information about specific 
aspects of the visual world, how are all these data integrated by the brain? The 
brain makes use of information regarding the frequency, rhythm, and timing of 
the firing of particular sets of neural cells. Furthermore, the brain’s integration of 
visual information does not occur in any single step or location in the brain but 
rather is a process that occurs on several levels simultaneously. The ultimate out- 
come, though, is indisputable: a vision of the world around us (de Gelder, 2000; 
Macaluso, Frith, & Driver, 2000; Werner, Pinna, & Spillmann, 2007; also see Figure 6 
on page 110). 


FIGURE 5 Because the optic nerve 
coming from the eye splits at the optic 
chiasm, the image to a person’s right 
eye is sent to the left side of the brain 
and the image to the person's left is 
transmitted to the right side of the 
brain. (Source: Mader, 2000.) 


feature detection The activation of 
neurons in the cortex by visual stimuli 
of specific shapes or patterns. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


110 Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception 


Seeing Vision in the Brain 


FIGURE 6 In addition to having specialized neurons that respond more strongly to particular 
shapes, orientations, and widths, our brains process information coming from each eye 
separately, creating what are called ocular dominance columns. With new MRI techniques, 
researchers can note the activity of the each eye more precisely. In these scans we can see the 
variation in ocular dominance columns for three individuals. The red areas show the response 
of one eye, and blue areas show the response of the other. (Source: Shmuel et al., 2010.) 





Participant 1 Participant 2 Participant 3 


Color Vision and Color Blindness: 
The _7-Million-Color Spectrum 


Although the range of wavelengths to which humans are sensitive is relatively nar- 
row, at least in comparison with the entire electromagnetic spectrum, the portion to 
which we are capable of responding allows us great flexibility in sensing the world. 
Nowhere is this clearer than in terms of the number of colors we can discern. A 
person with normal color vision is capable of distinguishing no less than 7 million 
different colors (Bruce, Green, & Georgeson, 1997; Rabin, 2004). 

Although the variety of colors that people are generally able to distinguish is 
vast, there are certain individuals whose ability to perceive color is quite limited—the 
color-blind. Interestingly, the condition of these individuals has provided some of the 
most important clues to understanding how color vision operates (Bonnardel, 2006; 
Neitz, Neitz, & Kainz, 1996). 

Approximately 7% of men and .4% of women are color-blind. For most people 
with color-blindness, the world looks quite dull (see Figure 7). Red fire engines 
appear yellow, green grass seems yellow, and the three colors of a traffic light all 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 11 Vision: Shedding Light on the Eye 





(a) (b) (c) 

FIGURE 7 To someone with normal vision, the hot-air balloon in the foreground appears 
with regions of very pure red, orange, yellow, green, blue, and violet, as well as off-white; and 
the balloon in the rear is a bright shade of red-orange. (b) A person with red-green color 
blindness would see the scene in part (a) like this, in hues of blue and yellow. (c) A person 
who is blue-yellow blind, conversely, would see it in hues of red and green. 


look yellow. In fact, in the most common form of color-blindness, all red and green 
objects are seen as yellow. In other forms of color-blindness, people are unable to tell 
the difference between yellow and blue. In the most extreme cases of color-blindness, 
which are quite rare, people perceive no color at all. To such individuals, the world 
looks something like the picture on an old black-and-white television set. 


EXPLAINING COLOR VISION 


To understand why some people are color-blind, we need to consider the basics of color 
vision. Two processes are involved. The first process is explained by the trichromatic 
theory of color vision, which was first proposed by Thomas Young and extended by 
Hermann von Helmholtz in the first half of the 1800s. This theory suggests that there 
are three kinds of cones in the retina, each of which responds primarily to a specific 
range of wavelengths. One is most responsive to blue-violet colors, one to green, and 
the third to yellow-red (Brown & Wald, 1964). According to trichromatic theory, per- 
ception of color is influenced by the relative strength with which each of the three 
kinds of cones is activated. If we see a blue sky, the blue-violet cones are primarily 
triggered, and the others show less activity. 

However, there are aspects of color vision that the trichromatic theory is less 
successful at explaining. For example, the theory does not explain what happens after 
you stare at something like the flag shown in Figure 8 on page 112 for about a min- 
ute. Try this yourself and then look at a blank white page: You'll see an image of the 
traditional red, white, and blue U.S. flag. Where there was yellow, you'll see blue, 
and where there were green and black, you'll see red and white. 

The phenomenon you have just experienced is called an afterimage. It occurs 
because activity in the retina continues even when you are no longer staring at the 
original picture. However, it also demonstrates that the trichromatic theory does not 
explain color vision completely. Why should the colors in the afterimage be different 
from those in the original? 


111 





trichromatic theory of color vision 
The theory that there are three kinds 
of cones in the retina, each of which 
responds primarily to a specific range 
of wavelengths. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


112 Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception 


FIGURE 8 Stare at the dot in this flag 
for about a minute and then look at a 
piece of plain white paper. What do you 


see? Most people see an afterimage that 
converts the colors in the figure into the 


traditional red, white, and blue U.S. flag. 
If you have trouble seeing it the first 
time, blink once and try again. 


opponent-process theory of color 
vision The theory that receptor cells 
for color are linked in pairs, working 
in opposition to each other. 







y Alert 


Keep in mind that there are 
two explanations for color 
vision: trichromatic and 
opponent-process 

theories. 





Because trichromatic processes do not provide a full explanation of color vision, 
alternative explanations have been proposed. According to the opponent-process 
theory of color vision, first proposed by German physiologist Ewald Hering in the 
19th century, receptor cells are linked in pairs, working in opposition to each other. 
Specifically, there are a blue-yellow pairing, a red-green pairing, and a black-white 
pairing. If an object reflects light that contains more blue than yellow, it will stimulate 
the firing of the cells sensitive to blue, simultaneously discouraging or inhibiting the 
firing of receptor cells sensitive to yellow—and the object will appear blue. If, in 
contrast, a light contains more yellow than blue, the cells that respond to yellow will 
be stimulated to fire while the blue ones are inhibited, and the object will appear 
yellow (D. N. Robinson, 2007). 

The opponent-process theory provides a good explanation for afterimages. When 
we stare at the yellow in the figure, for instance, our receptor cells for the yellow 
component of the yellow-blue pairing become fatigued and are less able to respond 
to yellow stimuli. In contrast, the receptor cells for the blue part of the pair are not 
tired, because they are not being stimulated. When we look at a white surface, the 
light reflected off it would normally stimulate both the yellow and the blue receptors 
equally. But the fatigue of the yellow receptors prevents this from happening. They 
temporarily do not respond to the yellow, which makes the white light appear to be 
blue. Because the other colors in the figure do the same thing relative to their specific 
opponents, the afterimage produces the opponent colors—for a while. The afterimage 
lasts only a short time, because the fatigue of the yellow receptors is soon overcome, 
and the white light begins to be perceived more accurately. 

We now know that both opponent processes and trichromatic mechanisms are 
at work in producing the perception of color vision, but in different parts of the visual 
sensing system. Trichromatic processes work within the retina itself, whereas oppo- 
nent mechanisms operate both in the retina and at later stages of neuronal processing 
(Baraas, Foster, & Amano, 2006; Chen, Zhou, & Gong, 2004; Gegenfurtner, 2003). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


RECAP 


What basic processes underlie the sense of vision? 

e Vision depends on sensitivity to light, electromagnetic 
waves in the visible part of the spectrum that are either 
reflected off objects or produced by an energy source. 
The eye shapes the light into an image that is trans- 
formed into nerve impulses and interpreted by the 
brain. (p. 104) 

e As light enters the eye, it passes through the cornea, 
pupil, and lens and ultimately reaches the retina, where 


Module 11 Vision: Shedding Light on the Eye 113 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


2. 


The structure that converts light into usable neural 
messages is called the 


. A woman with blue eyes could ke described as having 


blue pigment in her 


. What is the process by which te thickness of the lens is 


changed in order to focus light properly? 


. The proper sequence of structures that light passes 





through in the eye is the 4 j 
, and 





. Match each type of visual receptor with its function. 


à yas a. rods 1. used for dim light, largely 
the electromagnetic energy of light is converted to nerve : not 
; mS : z b. cones insensitive to color 
impulses for transmission to the brain. These impulses eom 
l th eed (p. 105) 2. detect color, good in bright light 
E A T We theory states that there are three types of 


e The visual information gathered by the rods and cones is 
transferred via bipolar and ganglion cells through the 
optic nerve, which leads to the optic chiasm—the point 
where the optic nerve splits. (p. 106) 


How do we see colors? 

e Color vision seems to be based on two processes 
described by the trichromatic theory and the opponent- 
process theory. (p. 111) 

e The trichromatic theory suggests that there are three 
kinds of cones in the retina, each of which is responsive 
to a certain range of colors. The opponent-process theory 
presumes pairs of different types of cells in the eye that 
work in opposition to each other. (p. 111) 


cones in the retina, each of which responds primarily to a 
different color. 


RETHINK 


1. 


If the eye had a second lens that “unreversed” the image 
hitting the retina, do you think there would be changes in 
the way people perceive the world? 


. From the perspective of an advertising specialist: How might 


you market your products similarly or differently to those 
who are color-blind versus those who have normal color 
vision? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


OeUIOIYIAL, *Z ‘Z-q T- 9 ‘euTjoI 


EVALUATE 


‘suaj ‘Tidnd ‘eauroo 'g tuoyepowwoo ‘p ‘SHL’ ‘eUTJOI*Z feU I 
1. Light entering the eye first passes through the ____, 
a protective window. 


KEY TERMS 


retina p. 106 
rods p. 106 
cones p. 106 


trichromatic theory of color 
vision p. 111 


opponent-process theory of 
color vision p. 112 


optic nerve p. 107 
feature detection p. 109 


What role does the ear play in 
the senses of sound, motion, 
and balance? 


How do smell and taste 
function? 


What are the skin senses, and 
how do they relate to the 
experience of pain? 


The movement of air molecules 
brought about by a source of vibration. 


The part of the ear that 
vibrates when sound waves hit it. 


(KOKE-lee-uh) A coiled tube 
in the ear filled with fluid that vibrates 
in response to sound. 


A vibrating 
structure that runs through the center 
of the cochlea, dividing it into an upper 
chamber and a lower chamber and 
containing sense receptors for sound. 


114 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


The blast-off was easy compared with what the astronaut was experiencing now: space 
sickness. The constant nausea and vomiting were enough to make him wonder why he 
had worked so hard to become an astronaut. Even though he had been warned that 
there was a two-thirds chance that his first experience in space would cause these 
symptoms, he wasn’t prepared for how terribly sick he really felt. 


Whether or not the astronaut wishes he could head right back to earth, his experi- 
ence, a major problem for space travelers, is related to a basic sensory process: the 
sense of motion and balance. This sense allows people to navigate their bodies 
through the world and keep themselves upright without falling. Along with hearing— 
the process by which sound waves are translated into understandable and meaning- 
ful forms—the sense of motion and balance resides in the ear. 





Sensing Sound 


Although many of us think primarily of the outer ear when we speak of the ear, that 
structure is only one simple part of the whole. The outer ear acts as a reverse mega- 
phone, designed to collect and bring sounds into the internal portions of the ear (see 
Figure 1). The location of the outer ears on different sides of the head helps with 
sound localization, the process by which we identify the direction from which a sound 
is coming. Wave patterns in the air enter each ear at a slightly different time, and 
the brain uses the discrepancy as a clue to the sound’s point of origin. In addition, 
the two outer ears delay or amplify sounds of particular frequencies to different 
degrees. 

Sound is the movement of air molecules brought about by a source of vibration. 
Sounds travel through the air in wave patterns similar in shape to those made in 
water when a stone is thrown into a still pond. Sounds, arriving at the outer ear in 
the form of wavelike vibrations, are funneled into the auditory canal, a tubelike pas- 
sage that leads to the eardrum. The eardrum is aptly named because it operates as 
a miniature drum, vibrating when sound waves hit it. The more intense the sound, 
the more the eardrum vibrates. These vibrations are then transferred into the middle 
ear, a tiny chamber containing three bones (the hammer, the anvil, and the stirrup) that 
transmit vibrations to the oval window, a thin membrane leading to the inner ear. 
Because the hammer, anvil, and stirrup act as a set of levers, they not only transmit 
vibrations but also increase their strength. Moreover, because the opening into the 
middle ear (the eardrum) is considerably larger than the opening out of it (the oval 
window), the force of sound waves on the oval window becomes amplified. The 
middle ear, then, acts as a tiny mechanical amplifier. 

The inner ear is the portion of the ear that changes the sound vibrations into a 
form in which they can be transmitted to the brain. (As you will see, it also contains 
the organs that allow us to locate our position and determine how we are moving 
through space.) When sound enters the inner ear through the oval window, it moves 
into the cochlea, a coiled tube that looks something like a snail and is filled with 
fluid that vibrates in response to sound. Inside the cochlea is the basilar membrane, 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Outer ear Middle ear Inner ear 











Module 12 Hearing and the Other Senses 115 
Semicircular canals ii 
os Vestibular system 
Auditory 








Eustachian 
tube 


Oval window 
(under Stirrup) 


FIGURE 1 The major parts of the ear. (Source: Brooker et al., 2008, Figure 45.6.) 


a structure that runs through the center of the cochlea, dividing it into an upper 
chamber and a lower chamber. The basilar membrane is covered with hair cells. 
When the hair cells are bent by the vibrations entering the cochlea, the cells send a 
neural message to the brain (Cho, 2000; Zhou, Liu, & Davis, 2005). 


THE PHYSICAL ASPECTS OF SOUND 


As we mentioned earlier, what we refer to as sound is actually the physical move- 
ment of air molecules in regular, wavelike patterns caused by a vibrating source. 
Sometimes it is even possible to see these vibrations: If you have ever seen an audio 
speaker that has no enclosure, you know that, at least when the lowest notes are 
playing, you can see the speaker moving in and out. Less obvious is what happens 
next: The speaker pushes air molecules into waves with the same pattern as its move- 
ment. Those wave patterns soon reach your ear, although their strength has been 
weakened considerably during their travels. All other sources that produce sound 
work in essentially the same fashion, setting off wave patterns that move through 
the air to the ear. Air—or some other medium, such as water—is necessary to make 
the vibrations of objects reach us. This explains why there can be no sound in a 
vacuum. 

We are able to see the audio speaker moving when low notes are played because 
of a primary characteristic of sound called frequency. Frequency is the number of 
wave cycles that occur in a second. At very low frequencies there are relatively few 
wave cycles per second (see Figure 2). These cycles are visible to the naked eye as 
vibrations in the speaker. Low frequencies are translated into a sound that is very 
low in pitch. (Pitch is the characteristic that makes sound seem “high” or “low.”) For 


Eustachian tube Cochlea 




















“unrolled” 











hair cells Tiny cells covering the 
basilar membrane that, when bent by 
vibrations entering the cochlea, 
transmit neural messages to the brain. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


116 Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception 


FIGURE 2 The sound waves produced by 
different stimuli are transmitted—usually 
through the air—in different patterns, 
with lower frequencies indicated by fewer 
peaks and valleys per second. (Source: 
Seeley, Stephens, & Tate, 2000.) 


place theory of hearing The theory 
that different areas of the basilar 
membrane respond to different 
frequencies. 


frequency theory of hearing The 
theory that the entire basilar mem- 
brane acts like a microphone, vibrating 
as a whole in response to a sound. 








Lower frequency Higher frequency 
(lower pitch) (higher pitch) 
| | 
l | | 
g E beca 
= 
= 
E 
< 
Time 


example, the lowest frequency that humans are capable of hearing is 20 cycles per 
second. Higher frequencies are heard as sounds of higher pitch. At the upper end of 
the sound spectrum, people can detect sounds with frequencies as high as 20,000 
cycles per second. 

Amplitude is a feature of wave patterns that allows us to distinguish between 
loud and soft sounds. Amplitude is the spread between the up-and-down peaks and 
valleys of air pressure in a sound wave as it travels through the air. Waves with small 
peaks and valleys produce soft sounds; those with relatively large peaks and valleys 
produce loud sounds. 

We are sensitive to broad variations in sound amplitudes. The strongest sounds 
we are capable of hearing are over a trillion times as intense as the very weakest 
sound we can hear. This range is measured in decibels. When sounds get higher than 
120 decibels, they become painful to the human ear. 

Our sensitivity to different frequencies changes as we age. For instance, as we 
get older, the range of frequencies we can detect declines, particularly for high- 
pitched sounds. This is why high school students sometimes choose high-pitched 
ring tones for their cell phones in settings where cell phone use is forbidden: the 
ringing sound goes undetected by their aging teachers (Vitello, 2006) (see Figure 3). 


Sorting Out Theories of Sound How are our brains able to sort out wavelengths of 
different frequencies and intensities? One clue comes from studies of the basilar 
membrane, the area in the cochlea that translates physical vibrations into neural 
impulses. It turns out that sounds affect different areas of the basilar membrane, 
depending on the frequency of the sound wave. The part of the basilar membrane 
nearest to the oval window is most sensitive to high-frequency sounds, and the part 
nearest to the cochlea’s inner end is most sensitive to low-frequency sounds. This 
finding has led to the place theory of hearing, which states that different areas of 
the basilar membrane respond to different frequencies. 

However, place theory does not tell the full story of hearing, because very low 
frequency sounds trigger neurons across such a wide area of the basilar membrane 
that no single site is involved. Consequently, an additional explanation for hearing 
has been proposed: frequency theory. The frequency theory of hearing suggests that 
the entire basilar membrane acts as a microphone, vibrating as a whole in response 
to a sound. According to this explanation, the nerve receptors send out signals that 
are tied directly to the frequency (the number of wave crests per second) of the 
sounds to which we are exposed, with the number of nerve impulses being a direct 
function of a sound’s frequency. Thus, the higher the pitch of a sound (and therefore 
the greater the frequency of its wave crests), the greater the number of nerve impulses 
that are transmitted up the auditory nerve to the brain. 

Neither place theory nor frequency theory provides the full explanation for hear- 
ing. Place theory provides a better explanation for the sensing of high-frequency 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 12 Hearing and the Other Senses 117 


Range by age group 
Audible frequencies for 
sound at 60 decibels 

SPL (sound pressure level) 





50-59 years old ————‘- 
40-49 ———_______ 














30-39 | 
18-24 | 
pEpEpAAA MMB Beis m 7 s 19 20 
Low pitch L | High pitch 
Highest note New 
on a piano text-message 
Frequency (in kilohertz) ringtone 


FIGURE 3 Some teenagers set their text-message ring tone to a frequency too high for most 
adults to hear, allowing them to use cell phones where they are prohibited. (Source: Vitello, 2006.) 


sounds, whereas frequency theory explains what happens when low-frequency 
sounds are encountered. Medium-frequency sounds incorporate both processes 
(Hirsh & Watson, 1996; Hudspeth, 2000). 

After an auditory message leaves the ear, it is transmitted to the auditory cortex 
of the brain through a complex series of neural interconnections. As the message is 
transmitted, it is communicated through neurons that respond to specific types of 
sounds. Within the auditory cortex itself, there are neurons that respond selectively 
to very specific sorts of sound features, such as clicks and whistles. Some neurons 
respond only to a specific pattern of sounds, such as a steady tone but not an inter- 
mittent one. Furthermore, specific neurons transfer information about a sound’s loca- 
tion through their particular pattern of firing (Middlebrooks et al., 2005; Alho et al., 
2006; Wang et al., 2005). 

If we were to analyze the configuration of the cells in the auditory cortex, we 
would find that neighboring cells are responsive to similar frequencies. The auditory 
cortex, then, provides us with a “map” of sound frequencies, just as the visual cortex 
furnishes a representation of the visual field. In addition, because of the asymmetry 
in the two hemispheres of the brain (which we discussed in the last chapter), the left 
and right ears process sound differently. The right ear reacts more to speech, while 
the left ear responds more to music (Sininger & Cone-Wesson, 2004, 2006). 

Speech perception requires that we make fine discriminations among sounds that 
are quite similar in terms of their physical properties. Furthermore, not only are we 
able to understand what is being said from speech, we can use vocal cues to deter- 
mine who is speaking, if they have an accent and where they may be from, and even 
their emotional state. Such capabilities illustrate the sophistication of our sense of 
hearing (Fowler & Galantucci, 2008; Massaro & Chen, 2008; Pell et al., 2009). 


Balance: The Ups and Downs of Life Several structures of the ear are related more 
to our sense of balance than to our hearing. Collectively, these structures are known 
as the vestibular system, which responds to the pull of gravity and allows us to main- 
tain our balance, even when standing in a bus in stop-and-go traffic. 

The main structure of the vestibular system is formed by the semicircular canals 
of the inner ear (refer to Figure 1 on page 115), which consist of three tubes contain- 
ing fluid that sloshes through them when the head moves, signaling rotational or 
angular movement to the brain. The pull on our bodies caused by the acceleration 
of forward, backward, or up-and-down motion, as well as the constant pull of grav- 
ity, is sensed by the otoliths, tiny, motion-sensitive crystals in the semicircular canals. 
When we move, these crystals shift as sands do on a windy beach, contacting the 


y Alert 


Be sure to understand the dif- 
ferences between the place 
and frequency theories of 
hearing. 


semicircular canals Three tubelike 
structures of the inner ear containing 
fluid that sloshes through them when 
the head moves, signaling rotational or 
angular movement to the brain. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


118 Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception 


The weightlessness of the ear’s otoliths 
produces space sickness in most 
astronauts. 


More than 1,000 receptor cells, known 
as olfactory cells, are spread across the 
nasal cavity. The cells are specialized to 
react to particular odors. Do you think it 
is possible to “train” the nose to pick up 
a greater number of odors? 








specialized receptor hair cells in the semicircular canals. The brain’s inexperience in 
interpreting messages from the weightless otoliths is the cause of the space sickness 
commonly experienced by two-thirds of all space travelers, mentioned at the start of 
this module (Flam, 1991; Stern & Koch, 1996). 


Smell and Taste 


Until he bit into a piece of raw cabbage on that February evening . . . , Raymond 





Fowler had not thought much about the sense of taste. The cabbage, part of a pasta 
dish he was preparing for his family’s dinner, had an odd, burning taste, but he did not 
pay it much attention. Then a few minutes later, his daughter handed him a glass of 
cola, and he took a swallow. “It was like sulfuric acid,” he said. “It was like the hottest 
thing you could imagine boring into your mouth.” (Goode, 1999, pp. D1-D2) 


It was evident that something was very wrong with Fowler’s sense of taste. After 
extensive testing, it became clear that he had damaged the nerves involved in his 
sense of taste, probably because of a viral infection or a medicine he was taking. 
(Luckily for him, a few months later his sense of taste returned to normal.) 

Even without disruptions in our ability to perceive the world such as those 
experienced by Fowler, we all know the important roles that taste and smell play. 
We'll consider these two senses next. 


SMELL 


Although many animals have keener abilities to detect odors than we do, the human 
sense of smell (olfaction) permits us to detect more than 10,000 separate smells. We also 
have a good memory for smells, and long-forgotten events and memories—good and 
bad—can be brought back with the mere whiff of an odor associated with a memory 
(Schroers, Prigot, & Fagen, 2007; Stevenson & Case, 2005; Willander & Larsson, 2006). 

Results of “sniff tests” have shown that women generally have a better sense of 
smell than men do (Engen, 1987). People also have the ability to distinguish males 
from females on the basis of smell alone. In one experiment, blindfolded students 
who were asked to sniff the breath of a female or male volunteer who was hidden 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 12 Hearing and the Other Senses 119 


from view were able to distinguish the sex of the donor at better than chance levels. 
People can also distinguish happy from sad emotions by sniffing under-arm smells, 
and women are able to identify their babies solely on the basis of smell just a few 
hours after birth (Doty et al., 1982; Fusari & Ballesteros, 2008; Haviland-Jones & 
Chen, 1999). 

The sense of smell is sparked when the molecules of a substance enter the nasal 
passages and meet olfactory cells, the receptor neurons of the nose, which are spread 
across the nasal cavity. More than 1,000 separate types of receptors have been identified 
on those cells so far. Each of these receptors is so specialized that it responds only to 
a small band of different odors. The responses of the separate olfactory cells are then 
transmitted to the brain, where they are combined into recognition of a particular smell 
(Marshall, Laing, & Jinks, 2006; Murphy et al., 2004; Zhou & Buck, 2006). 

Smell may also act as a hidden means of communication for humans. It has long 
been known that nonhumans release pheromones, chemicals they secrete into the envi- 
ronment that produce a reaction in other members of the same species, permitting 
the transmission of messages such as sexual availability. For instance, the vaginal 
secretions of female monkeys contain pheromones that stimulate the sexual interest 
of male monkeys (Hawkes & Doty, 2009; Holy, Dulac, & Meister, 2000; Touhara, 2007). 

The degree to which pheromones are part of the human experience remains an 
open question. Some psychologists believe that human pheromones affect emotional 
responses, although the evidence is inconclusive. For one thing, it is not clear what 
specific sense organ is receptive to pheromones. In non-humans, it is the vomeronasal 
organ in the nose, but in humans the organ appears to recede during fetal develop- 
ment (Haviland-Jones & Wilson, 2008; Hummer & McClintock, 2009). 


TASTE 


The sense of taste (gustation) involves receptor cells that respond to four basic 
stimulus qualities: sweet, sour, salty, and bitter. A fifth category also exists, a flavor 
called umami, although there is controversy about whether it qualifies as a funda- 
mental taste. Umami is a hard-to-translate Japanese word, although the English 
“meaty” or “savory” comes close. Chemically, umami involves food stimuli that 
contain amino acids (the substances that make up proteins) (McCabe & Rolls, 2007; 
Erickson, 2008). 

Although the specialization of the receptor cells leads them to respond most 
strongly to a particular type of taste, they also are capable of responding to other 
tastes as well. Ultimately, every taste is simply a combination of the basic flavor 
qualities, in the same way that the primary colors blend into a vast variety of shades 
and hues (Dilorenzo & Youngentob, 2003; Yeomans, Tepper, & Ritezschel, 2007). 

The receptor cells for taste are located in roughly 10,000 taste buds, which are 
distributed across the tongue and other parts of the mouth and throat. The taste buds 
wear out and are replaced every 10 days or so. That’s a good thing, because if our 
taste buds weren't constantly reproducing, we’d lose the ability to taste after we'd 
accidentally burned our tongues. 

The sense of taste differs significantly from one person to another, largely as a 
result of genetic factors. Some people, dubbed “supertasters,” are highly sensitive to 
taste; they have twice as many taste receptors as “nontasters,” who are relatively 
insensitive to taste. Supertasters (who, for unknown reasons, are more likely to be 
female than male) find sweets sweeter, cream creamier, and spicy dishes spicier, and 
weaker concentrations of flavor are enough to satisfy any cravings they may have. 
In contrast, because they aren’t so sensitive to taste, nontasters may seek out rela- 
tively sweeter and fattier foods in order to maximize the taste. As a consequence, 
they may be prone to obesity (Bartoshuk, 2000; Pickering & Gordon, 2006; Snyder, 
Fast, & Bartoshuk, 2004). 

Are you a supertaster? To find out, complete the questionnaire in Figure 4 on 
page 120. 





There are 10,000 taste buds on the 
tongue and other parts of the mouth. 
Taste buds wear out and are replaced 
every 10 days. What would happen if 
taste buds were not regenerated? 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


120 Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception 


FIGURE 4 All tongues are not created 
equal, according to taste researchers 
Linda Bartoshuk and Laurie Lucchina. 
Instead they suggest that the intensity 
of a flavor experienced by a given 
person is determined by that person's 
genetic background. This taste test can 
help determine if you are a nontaster, 
average taster, or supertaster. (Source: 
Bartoshuk & Lucchina, 1997.) 


nan n a a ODL 


Take a Taste Test 


|. Taste Bud Count 
Punch a hole with a standard hole punch in a square of wax paper. Paint the front of your 
tongue with a cotton swab dipped in blue food coloring. Put wax paper on the tip of your 
tongue, just to the right of center With a flashlight and magnifying glass, count the number 
of pink, unstained circles. They contain taste buds. 

2. Sweet Taste 
Rinse your mouth with water before tasting each sample. Put /2 cup sugar in a measuring 
cup, and then add enough water to make | cup. Mix. Coat front half of your tongue, 
including the tip, with a cotton swab dipped in the solution. Wait a few moments. Rate the 
sweetness according to the scale shown below. 

3. Salt Taste 
Put 2 teaspoons of salt in a measuring cup and add enough water to make | cup. Repeat the 
steps listed above, rating how salty the solution is. 

4. Spicy Taste 
Add | teaspoon of Tabasco sauce to | cup of water Apply with a cotton swab to first 2 inch 
of the tongue, including the tip. Keep your tongue out of your mouth until the burn reaches 
a peak, then rate the burn according to the scale. 





TASTE SCALE 
Barely Moderate Strongest 
Detectable Strong Very Strong Imaginable Sensation 


laa ATT ait a 


| | | | | l | | | | l 
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 


SUPERTASTERS NONTASTERS 
Number of taste buds 25 on Average 10 
Sweet rating 56 on Average 52 
Tabasco 64 on Average 3l 


Average tasters lie in between supertasters and nontasters. Bartoshuk and Lucchina lack the 
data at this time to rate salt reliably, but you can compare your results with others taking the 
test. 


The Skin Senses: Touch, Pressure, 
Temperature, and Pain 





It started innocently when Jennifer Darling hurt her right wrist during gym class. At 
first it seemed like a simple sprain. But even though the initial injury healed, the 
excruciating, burning pain accompanying it did not go away. Instead, it spread to her 
other arm and then to her legs. The pain, which Jennifer described as similar to “a hot 
iron on your arm,” was unbearable—and never stopped. 


The source of Darling’s pain turned out to be a rare condition known as “reflex 
sympathetic dystrophy syndrome,” or RSDS. For a victim of RSDS, a stimulus as 
mild as a gentle breeze or the touch of a feather can produce agony. Even bright 
sunlight or a loud noise can trigger intense pain. 

Pain such as Darling’s can be devastating, yet a lack of pain can be equally bad. 
If you never experienced pain, for instance, you might not notice that your arm had 
brushed against a hot pan, and you would suffer a severe burn. Similarly, without 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 12 Hearing and the Other Senses 121 


the warning sign of abdominal pain that typically accompanies an inflamed appendix, 
your appendix might eventually rupture, spreading a fatal infection throughout your 
body. 

In fact, all our skin senses—touch, pressure, temperature, and pain—play a 
critical role in survival, making us aware of potential danger to our bodies. Most of 
these senses operate through nerve receptor cells located at various depths through- 
out the skin, distributed unevenly throughout the body. For example, some areas, 
such as the fingertips, have many more receptor cells sensitive to touch and as a 
consequence are notably more sensitive than other areas of the body (Gardner & 
Kandel, 2000) (see Figure 5). 

Probably the most extensively researched skin sense is pain, and with good rea- 
son: People consult physicians and take medication for pain more than any other 
symptom or condition. Pain costs $100 billion a year in the United States alone (Kalb, 
2003; Pesmen, 2006). 

Pain is a response to a great variety of different kinds of stimuli. A light that is 
too bright can produce pain, and sound that is too loud can be painful. One explana- 
tion is that pain is an outcome of cell injury; when a cell is damaged, regardless of 
the source of damage, it releases a chemical called substance P that transmits pain 
messages to the brain. 

Some people are more susceptible to pain than others. For example, women 
experience painful stimuli more intensely than men. These gender differences are 
associated with the production of hormones related to menstrual cycles. In addition, 
certain genes are linked to the experience of pain, so that we may inherit our sensi- 
tivity to pain (Edwards & Fillingim, 2007; Kim, Clark, & Dionne, 2009; Nielsen et al., 
2008; Nielsen, Staud, & Price, 2009). 


Forehead | yy 
A Nose |) 
Cheek |) 
E Upper lip | 
Shoulder 
Upper arm 


Forearm iis 


Breast 
Palm (i is 
Thumb 
I 






































0 5 10 IS 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 


Mean threshold (mm) 


skin senses The senses of touch, 
pressure, temperature, and pain. 





Remember that there are 

multiple skin senses, 
including touch, pressure, 
temperature, and pain. 


FIGURE 5 Skin sensitivity in various 
areas of the body. The lower the average 
threshold is, the more sensitive a body 
part is. The fingers and thumb, lips, 
nose, cheeks, and big toe are the most 
sensitive. Why do you think certain areas 
are more sensitive than others? 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


122 Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception 





The ancient practice of acupuncture is still used in the 21st century. 
How does the gate-control theory of pain explain how acupuncture 
works? 


But the experience of pain is not determined by biological 
factors alone. For example, women report that the pain expe- 
rienced in childbirth is moderated to some degree by the 
joyful nature of the situation. In contrast, even a minor stim- 
ulus can produce the perception of strong pain if it is accom- 
panied by anxiety (for example, during a visit to the dentist). 
Clearly, then, pain is a perceptual response that depends 
heavily on our emotions and thoughts (Hadjistavropoulos, 
Craig, & Fuchs-Lacelle, 2004; Lang, Sorrell, & Rodgers, 2006; 
Rollman, 2004). 

According to the gate-control theory of pain, particular 
nerve receptors in the spinal cord lead to specific areas of the 
brain related to pain. When these receptors are activated 
because of an injury or problem with a part of the body, a 
“gate” to the brain is opened, allowing us to experience the 
sensation of pain (Melzack & Katz, 2004). 

However, another set of neural receptors can, when stim- 
ulated, close the “gate” to the brain, thereby reducing the 
experience of pain. The gate can be shut in two different 


ways. First, other impulses can overwhelm the nerve pathways relating to pain, 
which are spread throughout the brain. In this case, nonpainful stimuli compete with 


gate-control theory of pain The 
theory that particular nerve receptors 
in the spinal cord lead to specific areas 
of the brain related to pain. 


and sometimes displace the neural message of pain, thereby shutting off the painful 
stimulus. This explains why rubbing the skin around an injury (or even listening to 
distracting music) helps reduce pain. The competing stimuli can overpower the pain- 
ful ones (Villemure, Slotnick, & Bushnell, 2003). 


Psychological factors account for the second way a gate can be shut. Depend- 
ing on an individual’s current emotions, interpretation of events, and previous 
experience, the brain can close a gate by sending a message down the spinal cord 
to an injured area, producing a reduction in or relief from pain. Thus, soldiers who 
are injured in battle may experience no pain—the surprising situation in more than 
half of all combat injuries. The lack of pain probably occurs because a soldier 
experiences such relief at still being alive that the brain sends a signal to the injury 
site to shut down the pain gate (Gatchel & Weisberg, 2000; Pincus & Morley, 2001; 


Turk, 1994). 


Gate-control theory also may explain cultural differences in the experience of 
pain. Some of these variations are astounding. For example, in India people who 
participate in the “hook-swinging” ritual to celebrate the power of the gods have 
steel hooks embedded under the skin and muscles of their backs. During the ritual, 
they swing from a pole, suspended by the hooks. What would seem likely to 
induce excruciating pain instead produces a state of celebration and near euphoria. 
In fact, when the hooks are later removed, the wounds heal quickly, and after two 
weeks almost no visible marks remain (Kosambi, 1967; Melzack & Katz, 2001). 

Gate-control theory suggests that the lack of pain is due to a message from the 
participant’s brain, which shuts down the pain pathways. Gate-control theory also 
may explain the effectiveness of acupuncture, an ancient Chinese technique in which 
sharp needles are inserted into various parts of the body. The sensation from the 
needles may close the gateway to the brain, reducing the experience of pain. It is 
also possible that the body’s own painkillers—called endorphins—as well as positive 
and negative emotions, play a role in opening and closing the gate (Cabioglu, Ergene, 
& Tan, 2007; Fee et al., 2002; Witt, Jena, & Brinkhaus, 2006). 

Although the basic ideas behind gate-control theory have been supported by 
research, other processes are involved in the perception of pain. For instance, it 
appears that there are multiple neural pathways involved in the experience of pain. 
Furthermore, it is clear that the suppression of pain can occur through the natural 
release of endorphins and other compounds that produce a reduction of discomfort 
and a sense of well-being (Grahek, 2007). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 12 Hearing and the Other Senses 


Are you one of the 50 million people in the United States who BECOMING AN 
suffer from chronic pain? Psychologists and medical specialists INFORMED CONSUMER 
have devised several strategies to fight pain. Among the most 


important approaches are these: of Ps YC h O | O g y 


e Medication. Painkilling drugs are the most popular Managing Pain 
treatment in fighting pain. Drugs range from those that 
directly treat the source of the pain—such as reducing 
swelling in painful joints—to those that work on the symptoms. Medication can 
be in the form of pills, patches, injections, or liquids. In a recent innovation, 
drugs are pumped directly into the spinal cord (Kalb, 2003; Pesmen, 2006). 

e Nerve and brain stimulation. Pain can sometimes be relieved when a low-voltage 
electric current is passed through the specific part of the body that is in pain. 
In even more severe cases, electrodes can be implanted surgically directly into 
the brain, or a handheld battery pack can stimulate nerve cells to provide 
direct relief (Campbell & Ditto, 2002; Ross, 2000; Tugay et al., 2007). 

e Light therapy. One of the newest forms of pain reduction involves exposure to 
specific wavelengths of red or infrared light. Certain kinds of light increase the 
production of enzymes that may promote healing (Evcik et al., 2007; Under- 
wood, 2005). 

e Hypnosis. For people who can be hypnotized, hypnosis can greatly relieve pain 
(Accardi & Milling, 2009; Neron & Stephenson, 2007; Walker, 2008). 

e Biofeedback and relaxation techniques. Using biofeedback, people learn to control 
“involuntary” functions such as heartbeat and respiration. If the pain involves 
muscles, as in tension headaches or back pain, sufferers can be trained to relax 
their bodies systematically (Nestoriuc & Martin, 2007; Vitiello, Bonello, & 
Pollard, 2007). 

e Surgery. In one of the most extreme methods, nerve fibers that carry pain 
messages to the brain can be cut surgically. Still, because of the danger that 
other bodily functions will be affected, surgery is a treatment of last resort, 
used most frequently with dying patients (Cullinane, Chu, & Mamelak, 
2002). 

e Cognitive restructuring. Cognitive treatments are effective for people who 
continually say to themselves, “This pain will never stop,” “The pain is 
ruining my life,” or “I can’t take it anymore” and are thereby likely to make 
their pain even worse. By substituting more positive ways of thinking, people 
can increase their sense of control—and actually reduce the pain they experi- 
ence (Bogart et al., 2007; Spanos, Barber, & Lang, 2005). 


How Our Senses Interact 





When Matthew Blakeslee shapes hamburger patties with his hands, he experiences a 
vivid bitter taste in his mouth. Esmerelda Jones (a pseudonym) sees blue when she 
listens to the note C sharp played on the piano; other notes evoke different hues—so 
much so that the piano keys are actually color-coded, making it easier for her to 
remember and play musical scales. (Ramachandran & Hubbard, 2004, p. 53) 


The explanation? Both of these people have an unusual condition known as synes- 
thesia, in which exposure to one sensation (such as sound) evokes an additional one 
(such as vision). 

The origins of synesthesia are a mystery. It is possible that people with synes- 
thesia have unusually dense neural linkages between the different sensory areas of 
the brain. Another hypothesis is that they lack neural controls that usually inhibit 
connections between sensory areas (Kadosh, Henik, & Walsh, 2009; Pearce, 2007; 
Ramachandran, 2004). 


123 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


124 Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception 


FIGURE 6 (a) Try to pick out the 2s in 
the display. Most people take several 
seconds to find them buried among the 


5s and to see that the 2s form a triangle. 


For people with certain forms of synes- 
thesia, however, it’s easy, because they 
perceive the different numbers in 
contrasting colors as in (b). (Source: From 
“Hearing Colors, Tasting Shapes” by Vilayanur 
S. Ramachandran and Edward M. Hubbard. 
Copyright © 2003 by Scientific American, Inc. 
All rights reserved.) 














5.49 g 5 5 5.7 45 5 5 
5. 5 15 5 5.5.75 5 
5 

5 5 -5 5 F 
g gta 5 p72 g53 as 
5 575 5 5 575 "g 
5 A 5 5 a 5 
4 5 5 gZ 5f |5 5 E5 Rg 
5 5 5 5 

ga 2 gp P B F 
5575355 5//5575 75456 








Whatever the reason for synesthesia, it is a rare condition. (If you'd like to check 
out this phenomenon, see Figure 6.) Even so, the senses of all of us do interact and 
integrate in a variety of ways. For example, the taste of food is influenced by its 
texture and temperature. We perceive food that is warmer as sweeter (think of the 
sweetness of steamy hot chocolate compared with cold chocolate milk). Spicy foods 
stimulate some of the same pain receptors that are also stimulated by heat—making 
the use of “hot” as a synonym for “spicy” quite accurate (Balaban, McBurney, & 
Affeltranger, 2005; Cruz & Green, 2000; Green & George, 2004). 

It’s important, then, to think of our senses as interacting with one another. For 
instance, brain imaging studies show that the senses work in tandem to build our 
understanding of the world around us. We engage in multimodal perception, in which 
the brain collects the information from the individual sensory systems and integrates 
and coordinates it (Macaluso & Driver, 2005; Paulmann, Jessen, & Kotz, 2009). 

Moreover, despite the fact that very different sorts of stimuli activate our indi- 
vidual senses, they all react according to the same basic principles that we discussed 
at the start of this chapter. For example, our responses to visual, auditory, and taste 
stimuli all follow Weber’s law involving our sensitivity to changes in the strength of 
stimuli. 

In short, in some ways our senses are more similar to one another than different. 
Each of them is designed to pick up information from the environment and translate 
it into useable information. Furthermore, individually and collectively, our senses 
help us to understand the complexities of the world around us, allowing us to nav- 
igate through the world effectively and intelligently. 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 


What role does the ear play in the senses of sound, motion, 


and balance? 


e Sound, motion, and balance are centered in the ear. 
Sounds, in the form of vibrating air waves, enter through 
the outer ear and travel through the auditory canal until 


they reach the eardrum. (p. 114) 


e The vibrations of the eardrum are transmitted into the 
middle ear, which consists of three bones: the hammer, 
the anvil, and the stirrup. These bones transmit 
vibrations to the oval window. (p. 114) 


e In the inner ear, vibrations move into the cochlea, which 
encloses the basilar membrane. Hair cells on the basilar 
membrane change the mechanical energy of sound 
waves into nerve impulses that are transmitted to the 
brain. The ear is also involved in the sense of balance 
and motion. (p. 114) 

e Sound has a number of physical characteristics, 
including frequency and amplitude. The place theory of 
hearing and the frequency theory of hearing explain the 
processes by which we distinguish sounds of varying 
frequency and intensity. (p. 115) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 12 Hearing and the Other Senses 125 





How do smell and taste function? 5. The three fluid-filled tubes in the inner ear that are 
e Smell depends on olfactory cells (the receptor cells of responsible for our sense of balance are known as the 
the nose), and taste is centered in the tongue’s taste ; 
buds. (p. 118) 6. The theory states that when certain 





skin receptors are activated as a result of an injury, a “path- 


What are the skin senses, and how do they relate to the experi- e 1 : $ 
y pa way” to the brain is opened, allowing pain to be experienced. 


ence of pain? 

e The skin senses are responsible for the experiences of 
touch, pressure, temperature, and pain. Gate-control 
theory suggests that particular nerve receptors, when RETHINK 
activated, open a “gate” to specific areas of the brain 
related to pain, and that another set of receptors closes 
the gate when stimulated. (p. 120) 

e Among the techniques used frequently to alleviate pain 
are medication, hypnosis, biofeedback, relaxation 
techniques, surgery, nerve and brain stimulation, and 
cognitive therapy. (p. 123) 


1. Much research is being conducted on repairing faulty sen- 
sory organs through devices such as personal guidance 
systems and eyeglasses, among others. Do you think that 
researchers should attempt to improve normal sensory 
capabilities beyond their “natural” range (for example, 
make human visual or audio capabilities more sensitive 
than normal)? What benefits might this ability bring? 
What problems might it cause? 

EVALUATE 2. From the perspective of a social worker: How would you han- 

dle the case of a deaf child whose hearing could be re- 

stored with a cochlear implant—but different family 
members had conflicting views on whether the procedure 
should be done? 


1. The tubelike passage leading from the outer ear to the 
eardrum is known as the : 

2. The purpose of the eardrum is to protect the sensitive 
nerves underneath it. It serves no purpose in actual 
hearing. True or false? 

3. The three middle ear bones transmit their sound to the 





Answers to Evaluate Questions 
[O1}U0d-97e3 *g ‘speuRd 
=e Tepnoqorutas *g ‘Aouanbayy p ‘mopu [RAO “Eg ‘punos əy} s}TUsURy 
4. The ______ theory of hearing states that the entire pur ‘jr Ty S9AVM punos UaYM saqeaqra —asyey "Z ‘Teue Aoppne "T 
basilar membrane responds to a sound, vibrating more or 
less, depending on the nature of the sound. 


KEY TERMS 


sound p. 114 basilar membrane p. 114 frequency theory of gate-control theory 
eardrum p. 114 hair cells p. 115 hearing p. 116 of pain p. 122 
cochlea place theory of semicircular canals p. 117 


(KOKE-lee-uh) p. 114 hearing p. 116 skin senses p. 121 


What principles underlie our 
organization of the visual 
world and allow us to make 
sense of our environment? 


How are we able to perceive 
the world in three dimensions 
when our retinas are capable of 
sensing only two-dimensional 
images? 


What clues do visual illusions 
give us about our under- 
standing of general percep- 
tual mechanisms? 


gestalt laws of organization A series 
of principles that describe how we 
organize bits and pieces of information 
into meaningful wholes. 


FIGURE 1 When the usual cues we use 
to distinguish figure from ground are 
absent, we may shift back and forth 
between different views of the same 
figure. If you look at each of these 
objects long enough, you'll probably 
experience a shift in what you're seeing. 
In (a), you can see either a vase or the 
profiles of two people. In (b), the shaded 
portion of the figure, called a Necker 
cube, can appear to be either the front 
or the back of the cube. 


126 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Consider the vase shown in Figure la for a moment. Or is it a vase? Take another 
look, and instead you may see the profiles of two people. 

Now that an alternative interpretation has been pointed out, you will probably 
shift back and forth between the two interpretations. Similarly, if you examine the 
shapes in Figure 1b long enough, you will probably experience a shift in what you’re 
seeing. The reason for these reversals is this: Because each figure is two-dimensional, 
the usual means we employ for distinguishing the figure (the object being perceived) 
from the ground (the background or spaces within the object) do not work. 

The fact that we can look at the same figure in more than one way illustrates an 
important point. We do not just passively respond to visual stimuli that happen to fall 
on our retinas. Rather, we actively try to organize and make sense of what we see. 

We turn now from a focus on the initial response to a stimulus (sensation) to 
what our minds make of that stimulus—perception. Perception is a constructive pro- 
cess by which we go beyond the stimuli that are presented to us and attempt to 
construct a meaningful situation. 


The Gestalt Laws of Organization 


Some of the most basic perceptual processes can be described by a series of principles 
that focus on the ways we organize bits and pieces of information into meaningful 
wholes. Known as gestalt laws of organization, these principles were set forth in the 
early 1900s by a group of German psychologists who studied patterns, or gestalts 
(Wertheimer, 1923). Those psychologists discovered a number of important principles 
that are valid for visual (as well as auditory) stimuli, illustrated in Figure 2: closure, 
proximity, similarity, and simplicity. 

Figure 2a illustrates closure: We usually group elements to form enclosed or com- 
plete figures rather than open ones. We tend to ignore the breaks in Figure 2a and 











(a) (b) 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 13 Perceptual Organization: Constructing Our View of the World 127 

eeeeeee0 

HEEEEEESE 

@eeeee0e0 

HEEREEESE 

eeeeeede 

i ; HEEREEESE 
00 00 00 00O 868 808 68 68 08 @o@e@ee0e0@ 

HEEREEEE 

(a) Closure (b) Proximity (c) Similarity (d) Simplicity 


FIGURE 2 Organizing these various bits and pieces of information into meaningful wholes 
constitutes some of the most basic processes of perception, which are summed up in the 
gestalt laws of organization. Do you think any other species share this organizational 
tendency? How might we find out? 


concentrate on the overall form. Figure 2b demonstrates the 
principle of proximity: We perceive elements that are closer 
together as grouped together. As a result, we tend to see pairs 
of dots rather than a row of single dots in Figure 2b. 

Elements that are similar in appearance we perceive as 
grouped together. We see, then, horizontal rows of circles and 
squares in Figure 2c rather than vertical mixed columns. 
Finally, in a general sense, the overriding gestalt principle is 
simplicity: When we observe a pattern, we perceive it in the 
most basic, straightforward manner that we can. For example, 
most of us see Figure 2d as a square with lines on two sides, 
rather than as the block letter W on top of the letter M. If we 
have a choice of interpretations, we generally opt for the sim- 
pler one. 

Although gestalt psychology no longer plays a prominent 
role in contemporary psychology, its legacy endures. One fun- 
damental gestalt principle that remains influential is that two 
objects considered together form a whole that is different 
from the simple combination of the objects. Gestalt psycholo- “I’m turning into my mother.” 
gists argued that the perception of stimuli in our environment 
goes well beyond the individual elements that we sense. 
Instead, it represents an active, constructive process carried 
out within the brain (Humphreys & Miiller, 2000; Lehar, 2003; 
van der Helm, 2006) (see Figure 3). 





Understanding this cartoon involves the separation of the figure 
and ground. If you're having trouble appreciating the humor, stare 
at the woman on the right, who eventually will be transformed. 


Alert 


The gestalt laws of organiza- 
tion are classic principles in 
the field of psychology. 
Figure 2 can help you re- 
member them. 


Top-Down and Bottom-Up 
Processing 


Ca- yo- re-d t-is -en-en-e, w-ic- ha- ev-ry -hi-d |-tt-r m-ss-ng? It probably won’t take 
you too long to figure out that it says, “Can you read this sentence, which has every 
third letter missing?” 

If perception were based primarily on breaking down a stimulus into its most 
basic elements, understanding the sentence, as well as other ambiguous stimuli, 
would not be possible. The fact that you were probably able to recognize such an 
imprecise stimulus illustrates that perception proceeds along two different avenues, 
called top-down processing and bottom-up processing. 

In top-down processing, perception is guided by higher-level knowledge, expe- top-down processing Perception that 
rience, expectations, and motivations. You were able to figure out the meaning of the is guided by higher-level knowledge, 
sentence with the missing letters because of your prior reading experience and experience, expectations, and 
because written English contains redundancies. Not every letter of each word is motivations. 








www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


128 Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception 


FIGURE 3 Although at first it is difficult 
to distinguish anything in this drawing, 
keep looking, and eventually you'll 
probably be able to see the figure of a 
dog. The dog represents a gestalt, or 
perceptual, whole, which is something 
greater than the sum or the individual 
elements. 


bottom-up processing Perception 
that consists of the progression of 
recognizing and processing information 
from individual components of a 
stimuli and moving to the perception 
of the whole. 





necessary to decode its meaning. Moreover, your expectations played a role in your 
being able to read the sentence. You were probably expecting a statement that had 
something to do with psychology, not the lyrics to a Lady Gaga song. 

Top-down processing is illustrated by the importance of context in determining 
how we perceive objects. Look, for example, at Figure 4. Most of us perceive that the 
first row consists of the letters A through F, while the second contains the numbers 
10 through 14. But take a more careful look and you'll see that the “B” and the “13” 
are identical. Clearly, our perception is affected by our expectations about the two 
sequences—even though the two stimuli are exactly the same. 

However, top-down processing cannot occur on its own. Even though top-down 
processing allows us to fill in the gaps in ambiguous and out-of-context stimuli, we 
would be unable to perceive the meaning of such stimuli without bottom-up process- 
ing. Bottom-up processing consists of the progression of recognizing and processing 
information from individual components of a stimuli and moving to the perception 
of the whole. We would make no headway in our recognition of the sentence without 
being able to perceive the individual shapes that make up the letters. Some perception, 
then, occurs at the level of the patterns and features of each of the separate letters. 

Top-down and bottom-up processing occur simultaneously, and interact with 
each other, in our perception of the world around us. Bottom-up processing permits 
us to process the fundamental characteristics of stimuli, whereas top-down process- 
ing allows us to bring our experience to bear on perception. As we learn more about 
the complex processes involved in perception, we are developing a better under- 
standing of how the brain continually interprets information from the senses and 
permits us to make responses appropriate to the environment (Folk & Remington, 
2008; Sobel et al., 2007; Westerhausen et al., 2009). 


ABCDEF 
IO Il 12 13 14 


FIGURE 4 The power of context is shown in this figure. Note how the B and the 13 are 
identical. (Source: Coren & Ward, 1989.) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 13 Perceptual Organization: Constructing Our View of the World 129 


Depth Perception: 
Translating 2-D to 3-D 


As sophisticated as the retina is, the images projected onto it are flat and two- 
dimensional. Yet the world around us is three-dimensional, and we perceive it that 
way. How do we make the transformation from 2-D to 3-D? 

The ability to view the world in three dimensions and to perceive distance—a 
skill known as depth perception—is due largely to the fact that we have two eyes. 
Because there is a certain distance between the eyes, a slightly different image reaches 
each retina. The brain integrates the two images into one view, but it also recognizes 
the difference in images and uses this difference to estimate the distance of an object 
from us. The difference in the images seen by the left eye and the right eye is known 
as binocular disparity (Hibbard, 2007; Kara & Boyd, 2009). 

To get a sense of binocular disparity, hold a pencil at arm’s length and look at 
it first with one eye and then with the other. There is little difference between the 
two views relative to the background. Now bring the pencil just 6 inches away from 
your face, and try the same thing. This time you will perceive a greater difference 
between the two views. 

The fact that the discrepancy between the images in the two eyes varies accord- 
ing to the distance of objects that we view provides us with a means of determining 
distance. If we view two objects and one is considerably closer to us than the other 
is, the retinal disparity will be relatively large and we will have a greater sense of 
depth between the two. However, if the two objects are a similar distance from us, 
the retinal disparity will be minor, and we will perceive them as being a similar 
distance from us. 

In some cases, certain cues permit us to obtain a sense of depth and distance 
with just one eye. These cues are known as monocular cues. One monocular cue— 
motion parallax—is the change in position of an object on the retina caused by move- 
ment of your body relative to the object. For example, suppose you are a passenger 
in a moving car, and you focus your eye on a stable object such as a tree. Objects 
that are closer than the tree will appear to move backward, and the nearer the object 








depth perception The ability to view 
the world in three dimensions and to 
perceive distance. 


Railroad tracks that seem to join 


together in the distance are an example 


of linear perspective. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


130 Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception 


When the moon is near the horizon, we 
do not see it by itself, and perceptual 
constancy leads us to take into account 
a misleading sense of distance. 


perceptual constancy The phenom- 
enon in which physical objects are 
perceived as unvarying and consistent 
despite changes in their appearance or 
in the physical environment. 





is, the more quickly it will appear to move. In contrast, objects beyond the tree will 
seem to move at a slower speed, but in the same direction as you are. Your brain is 
able to use these cues to calculate the relative distances of the tree and other objects. 

Similarly, experience has taught us that if two objects are the same size, the one 
that makes a smaller image on the retina is farther away than is the one that provides 
a larger image—an example of the monocular cue of relative size. But it’s not just size 
of an object that provides information about distance; the quality of the image on 
the retina helps us judge distance. The monocular cue of texture gradient provides 
information about distance, because the details of things that are far away are less 
distinct (Proffitt, 2006). 

Finally, anyone who has ever seen railroad tracks that seem to come together in 
the distance knows that distant objects appear to be closer together than are nearer 
ones, a phenomenon called linear perspective. People use linear perspective as a mon- 
ocular cue in estimating distance, allowing the two-dimensional image on the retina 
to record the three-dimensional world (Bruce, Green, & Georgeson, 1997; Bruggeman, 
Yonas, & Konczak, 2007; Dobbins et al., 1998; Shimono & Wade, 2002). 


Perceptual Constancy 


Consider what happens as you finish a conversation with a friend and he begins to 
walk away from you. As you watch him walk down the street, the image on your 
retina becomes smaller and smaller. Do you wonder why he is shrinking? 

Of course not. Despite the very real change in the size of the retinal image, 
because of perceptual constancy you factor into your thinking the knowledge that 
your friend is moving farther away from you. Perceptual constancy is a phenomenon 
in which physical objects are perceived as unvarying and consistent despite changes 
in their appearance or in the physical environment. Perceptual constancy leads us to 
view objects as having an unvarying size, shape, color, and brightness, even if the 
image on our retina varies. For example, despite the varying images on the retina as 
an airplane approaches, flies overhead, and disappears, we do not perceive the air- 
plane as changing shape (Garrigan & Kellman, 2008; Redding, 2002; Wickelgren, 2004). 

In some cases, though, our application of perceptual constancy can mislead us. 
One good example of this involves the rising moon. When the moon first appears at 
night, close to the horizon, it seems to be huge—much larger than when it is high 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 13 Perceptual Organization: Constructing Our View of the World 


in the sky later in the evening. You may have thought that the apparent change in 
the size of the moon was caused by the moon’s being physically closer to the earth 
when it first appears. In fact, though, this is not the case at all: the actual image of 
the moon on our retina is the same, whether it is low or high in the sky. 

There are several explanations for the moon illusion. One suggests that the moon 
appears to be larger when it is close to the horizon primarily because of perceptual 
constancy. When the moon is near the horizon, the perceptual cues of intervening 
terrain and objects such as trees on the horizon produce a misleading sense of dis- 
tance, leading us to misperceive the moon as relatively large. 

In contrast, when the moon is high in the sky, we see it by itself, and we don’t 
try to compensate for its distance from us. In this case, then, perceptual constancy 
leads us to perceive it as relatively small. To experience perceptual constancy, try 
looking at the moon when it is relatively low on the horizon through a paper-towel 
tube; the moon suddenly will appear to “shrink” back to normal size (Coren, 1992; 
Imamura & Nakamizo, 2006; Kaufman, Johnson, & Liu, 2008; Ross & Plug, 2002). 

Perceptual constancy is not the only explanation for the moon illusion, and it 
remains a puzzle to psychologists. It may be that several different perceptual pro- 
cesses are involved in the illusion (Gregory, 2008; Kim, 2008). 


Motion Perception: 
As the World Turns 


When a batter tries to hit a pitched ball, the most important factor is the motion of 
the ball. How is a batter able to judge the speed and location of a target that is mov- 
ing at some 90 miles per hour? 

The answer rests in part on several cues that provide us with relevant informa- 
tion about the perception of motion. For one thing, the movement of an object across 
the retina is typically perceived relative to some stable, unmoving background. More- 
over, if the stimulus is heading toward us, the image on the retina expands in size, 
filling more and more of the visual field. In such cases, we assume that the stimulus 
is approaching—not that it is an expanding stimulus viewed at a constant distance. 

It is not, however, just the movement of images across the retina that brings 
about the perception of motion. If it were, we would perceive the world as moving 
every time we moved our heads. Instead, one of the critical things we learn about 
perception is to factor information about our own head and eye movements along 
with information about changes in the retinal image. 

Sometimes we perceive motion when it doesn’t occur. Have you ever been on a 
stationary train that feels as if it is moving, because a train on an adjacent track 
begins to slowly move past? Or have you been in an IMAX movie theater, in which 
you feel as if you were falling as a huge image of plane moves across the screen? In 
both cases, the experience of motion is convincing. Apparent movement is the percep- 
tion that a stationary object is moving. It occurs when different areas of the retina 
are quickly stimulated, leading us to interpret motion (Ekroll & Scherzer, 2009; 
Lindemann & Bekkering, 2009). 





Perceptual Illusions: 
The Deceptions of Perceptions 


If you look carefully at the Parthenon, one of the most famous buildings of ancient 
Greece, still standing at the top of an Athens hill, you'll see that it was built with a 
bulge on one side. If it didn’t have that bulge—and quite a few other architectural 
“tricks” like it, such as columns that incline inward—it would look as if it were 





131 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


132 Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception 





visual illusions Physical stimuli that 
consistently produce errors in 
perception. 






Alert 


The explanation for the 

Miller-Lyer illusion is 
complicated. Figure 6 will 
help you master it. 














(b) 








FIGURE 5 (a) In building the Parthenon, the Greeks constructed an architectural wonder 
that looks perfectly straight, with right angles at every corner as in. (b) However, if it had 
been built with completely true right angles, it would have looked as it does here. (c) To 
compensate for this illusion, the Parthenon was designed to have a slight upward curvature, 
as shown here. (Source: Coren & Ward, 1989, p. 5.) 


crooked and about to fall down. Instead, it appears to stand completely straight, at 
right angles to the ground. 

The fact that the Parthenon appears to be completely upright is the result of a 
series of visual illusions. Visual illusions are physical stimuli that consistently pro- 
duce errors in perception. In the case of the Parthenon, the building appears to be 
completely square, as illustrated in Figure 5a. However, if it had been built that way, 
it would look to us as it does in Figure 5b. The reason for this is an illusion that makes 
right angles placed above a line appear as if they were bent. To offset the illusion, the 
Parthenon was constructed as in Figure 5c, with a slight upward curvature. 

The Miiller-Lyer illusion (illustrated in Figure 6) has fascinated psychologists for 
decades. Although the two lines are the same length, the one with the arrow tips 
pointing outward, away from the vertical line (Figure 6a, left) appears to be shorter 
than the one with the arrow tips pointing inward (Figure 6a, right). 

Although all kinds of explanations for visual illusions have been suggested, most 
concentrate either on the physical operation of the eye or on our misinterpretation 
of the visual stimulus. For example, one explanation for the Miiller-Lyer illusion is 
that eye movements are greater when the arrow tips point inward, making us per- 
ceive the line as longer than it is when the arrow tips face outward. In contrast, a 
different explanation for the illusion suggests that we unconsciously attribute par- 
ticular significance to each of the lines (Gregory, 1978; Redding & Hawley, 1993). 
When we see the left line in Figure 6a we tend to perceive it as if it were the relatively 
close outside corner of a rectangular object, such as the outside corner of the room 
illustrated in Figure 6b. In contrast, when we view the line on the right in Figure 6a, 
we perceive it as the relatively more distant inside corner of a rectangular object, 
such as the inside room corner in Figure 6c. Because previous experience leads us to 
assume that the outside corner is closer than the inside corner, we make the further 
assumption that the inside corner must therefore be longer. 

Despite the complexity of the latter explanation, a good deal of evidence sup- 
ports it. For instance, cross-cultural studies show that people raised in areas where 
there are few right angles—such as the Zulu in Africa—are much less susceptible to 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 13 Perceptual Organization: Constructing Our View of the World 





(a) (b) (c) 
FIGURE 6 In the Miller-Lyer illusion (a), the vertical line on the left appears shorter than the 
one on the right, even though they are identical in length. One explanation for the Müller- 
Lyer illusion suggests that the line on the left (with arrow points directed outward) is 
perceived as the relatively close corner of a rectangular object, such as the building corner in 
(b), and the line on the right (with the arrow points directed inward) is interpreted as the 
inside corner of a rectangular object, such as the room extending away from us (c). Our 
previous experience with distance cues leads us to assume that the outside corner is closer 
than the inside corner and, consequently, that the inside corner must therefore be longer. 


the illusion than are people who grow up where most structures are built using right 
angles and rectangles (Segall, Campbell, & Herskovits, 1966). 


As the example of the Zulu indicates, the culture in which we Ex | O ri Nn fa 
are raised has clear consequences for how we perceive the p g DIVERSITY ~N 


world. Consider the drawing in Figure 7. Sometimes called the 
“devil's tuning fork,” it is likely to produce a mind-boggling 
effect, as the center tine of the fork alternates between appearing 
and disappearing. 

Now try to reproduce the drawing on a piece of paper. Chances are that the 
task is nearly impossible for you—unless you are a member of an African tribe with 
little exposure to Western cultures. For such individuals, the task is simple; they 
have no trouble reproducing the figure. The reason is that Westerners automatically 
interpret the drawing as something that cannot exist in three dimensions, and they 
therefore are inhibited from reproducing it. The African tribal members, in contrast, 
do not make the assumption that the figure is “impossible” and instead view it in 
two dimensions, a perception that enables them to copy the figure with ease 
(Deregowski, 1973). 

Cultural differences are also reflected in depth perception. A Western viewer of 
Figure 8 would interpret the hunter in the drawing as aiming for the antelope in the 
foreground, while an elephant stands under the tree in the background. A member 
of an isolated African tribe, however, interprets the scene very differently by assum- 
ing that the hunter is aiming at the elephant. Westerners use the difference in sizes 
between the two animals as a cue that the elephant is farther away than the antelope 
(Hudson, 1960). 

Does this mean that basic perceptual processes differ among people of different 
cultures? No. Variations in learning and experience produce cross-cultural differences 
in perception, and the underlying psychological processes involved in perception are 
similar (McCauley & Henrich, 2006). 

Although visual illusions may seem like mere psychological curiosities, they 


Culture and Perception ~ 


(= 
> 


133 





2 Ri 
pay 
oy 


D 


actually illustrate something fundamental about perception. There is a basic connec- FIGURE 7 The “devil's tuning fork” has 
tion between our prior knowledge, needs, motivations, and expectations about how three prongs . . . or does it have two? 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


134 Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception 


FIGURE 8 Is the man aiming for the 
elephant or the antelope? Westerners 
assume that the difference in size 
between the two animals indicates that 
the elephant is farther away, and 
therefore the man is aiming for the 
antelope. In contrast, members of some 
African tribes, not used to depth cues in 
two-dimensional drawings, assume that 
the man is aiming for the elephant. (The 
drawing is based on Deregowski, 1973.) 
Do you think Westerners, who view the 
picture in three dimensions, could 
explain what they see to someone who 
views the scene in two dimensions and 
eventually get that person to view it in 
three dimensions? 





the world is put together and the way we perceive it. Our view of the world is very 
much an outcome, then, of fundamental psychological factors. Furthermore, each 
person perceives the environment in a way that is unique and special (Knoblich & 
Sebanz, 2006; Repp & Knoblich, 2007). 


SUBLIMINAL PERCEPTION 


Can stimuli that we’re not consciously aware of change our behavior? In some 
ways, yes. 

Subliminal perception refers to the perception of messages about which we have 
no awareness. The stimulus could be a written word, a sound, or even a smell that 
activates the sensory system but that is not intense enough for a person to report 
having experienced it. For example, in some studies people are exposed to a descrip- 
tive label—called a prime—about a person (such as the word smart or happy) so briefly 
that they cannot report seeing the label. Later, however, they form impressions that 
are influenced by the content of the prime. Somehow, they have been influenced by 
the prime that they say they couldn’t see, providing some evidence for subliminal 
perception (Greenwald, Draine, & Abrams, 1996; Key, 2003). 

Although subliminal messages (which social psychologists refer to as priming) 
can influence behavior in subtle ways, there’s little evidence that it can lead to 
major changes in attitudes or behavior. Most research suggests that they cannot. 
For example, people who are subliminally exposed to an image of a Coke can and 
the word “thirst” do later rate themselves as thirstier, and they actually do drink 
more when given the opportunity. However, they don’t particularly care if they 
drink Coke or some other liquid to quench their thirst (Dijksterhuis, Chartrand, 
& Aarts, 2007). 

In short, although we are able to perceive at least some kinds of information of 
which we are unaware, there’s little evidence that subliminal messages can change 
our attitudes or behavior in substantial ways. At the same time, subliminal percep- 
tion does have at least some consequences. If our motivation to carry out a behav- 
ior is already high and the appropriate stimuli are presented subliminally, 
subliminal perception may have at least some effect on our behavior (Abrams, 
Klinger, & Greenwald, 2002; Pratkanis, Epley, & Savitsky, 2007; Randolph-Seng & 
Nielsen, 2009). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 13 Perceptual Organization: Constructing Our View of the World 135 


EXTRASENSORY PERCEPTION (ESP) 


Given the lack of evidence that subliminal perception can alter our behavior in sub- 
stantial ways, psychologists are particularly skeptical of reports of extrasensory percep- 
tion, or ESP—perception that does not involve our known senses. Although half of 
the general population of the United States believes it exists, most psychologists reject 
the existence of ESP, asserting that there is no sound documentation of the phenom- 
enon (Gallup Poll, 2001; Hyman, 1994; Swets & Bjork, 1990). 

However, a debate in one of the most prestigious psychology journals, Psycho- 
logical Bulletin, heightened interest in ESP. According to proponents of ESP, reliable 
evidence exists for an “anomalous process of information transfer,” or psi. These 
researchers, who painstakingly reviewed considerable evidence, argue that a cumula- 
tive body of research shows reliable support for the existence of psi (Bem & Honorton, 
1994; Parra & Argibay, 2007; Storm & Ertel, 2001). 

Their conclusion has been challenged on several counts. For example, critics 
suggest that the research methodology was inadequate and that the experiments 
supporting psi are flawed (Hyman, 1994; Kennedy, 2004; Milton & Wiseman, 1999). 

Because of questions about the quality of the research, as well as a lack of any 
credible theoretical explanation for how extrasensory perception might take place, 
the vast majority of psychologists continue to believe that there is no reliable scien- 
tific support for ESP (Rose & Blackmore, 2002; Wiseman & Greening, 2002). Still, the 
exchanges in Psychological Bulletin are likely to heighten the debate. More important, 
the renewed interest in ESP among psychologists is likely to inspire more research, 
which is the only way the issue can be resolved. 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP parallax, the relative size of images on the retina, and 
linear perspective. (p. 129) 

e Perceptual constancy permits us to perceive stimuli as 
unvarying in size, shape, and color despite changes in 
the environment or the appearance of the objects being 
perceived. (p. 130) 

e Motion perception depends on cues such as the 
perceived movement of an object across the retina and 
information about how the head and eyes are moving. 


(p. 131) 


What clues do visual illusions give us about our understanding 
of general perceptual mechanisms? 
e Visual illusions are physical stimuli that consistently 


What principles underlie our organization of the visual world 
and allow us to make sense of our environment? 

e Perception is a constructive process in which people go 
beyond the stimuli that are physically present and try to 
construct a meaningful interpretation. (p. 126) 

e The gestalt laws of organization are used to describe the 
way in which we organize bits and pieces of information 
into meaningful wholes, known as gestalts, through 
closure, proximity, similarity, and simplicity. (p. 126) 

e In top-down processing, perception is guided by 
higher-level knowledge, experience, expectations, and 
motivations. In bottom-up processing, perception 


consists of the progression of recognizing and processing 
information from individual components of a stimuli 
and moving to the perception of the whole. (p. 127) 


produce errors in perception, causing judgments that do 
not reflect the physical reality of a stimulus accurately. 
One of the best-known illusions is the Miiller-Lyer 


illusion. (p. 132) 

e Visual illusions are usually the result of errors in the 
brain’s interpretation of visual stimuli. Furthermore, 
culture clearly affects how we perceive the world. 


How are we able to perceive the world in three dimensions 
when our retinas are capable of sensing only two-dimensional 
images? 

e Depth perception is the ability to perceive distance and 


view the world in three dimensions even though the 
images projected on our retinas are two-dimensional. 
We are able to judge depth and distance as a result of 
binocular disparity and monocular cues, such as motion 


(p. 132) 

Subliminal perception refers to the perception of 
messages about which we have no awareness. The 
reality of the phenomenon, as well as of ESP, is open to 
question and debate. (p. 134) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


136 Chapter 4 Sensation and Perception 


EVALUATE 5. is the ability to view the world in 
three dimensions instead of two. 
6. The brain makes use of a phenomenon known as 





1. Match each of the following organizational laws with its 





SR , or the difference in the images the 
ao (lost po men close tee era. two eyes see, to give three dimensions to sight. 
b. proximity grouped together. 
c. similarity 2. Patterns are perceived in the 
d. simplicity most basic, direct manner RETHINK 
3 Bees are f 1. In what ways do painters represent three-dimensional 
í BR ae a scenes in two dimensions on a canvas? Do you think art- 
4 ae ie ee ists in non-Western cultures use the same or different prin- 
pe eee ere z A PERETE OSE ciples to represent three-dimensionality? Why? 
2 reer tes aia F rebe 2. From the perspective of a corporate executive: What arguments 
i : Glue afte ee es ee might you make if a member of your staff proposed a sub- 
an object down into its component pieces in order to un- lawinabadveruel ion? D fink l 
IA. iminal advertising campaign? Do you think your expla- 
KN ; r nation would be enough to convince them? Why? 
3. Processing that involves higher functions such as expecta- 
tions and motivations is known as ____, whereas Answers to Evaluate Questions 
processing that recognizes the individual components of a Ayredstp repndourg “9 ‘uoydaorzad yydaq 'g ‘Aoueysu0s 
stimulus is known as —____ [enjdaorad “% ‘dn-woyog ‘umop-do} ‘g ‘amnyea `Z ‘T-P f-9 ‘T-q ‘g-e 'T 


4, When a car passes you on the road and appears to shrink 
as it gets farther away, the phenomenon of 
permits you to realize that the car is not in fact 
getting smaller. 


KEY TERMS 


gestalt laws of top-down processing p.127 depth perception p. 129 visual illusions p. 132 
organization p. 126 bottom-up processing p.128 perceptual constancy p. 130 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Psychology on the Web 


1. Select one topic of personal interest to you that was mentioned in this set of 
modules (for instance, cochlear implants, visual illusions, psi). Find one “serious” or 
scientific website and one “popular” or commercial website with information about 
the chosen topic. Compare the type, level, and reliability of the information that you 
find on each site. Write a summary of your findings. 

2. Are there more gestalt laws of organization than the four we've considered (closure, 
proximity, similarity, and simplicity)? Find the answer to this question on the Internet 
and write a summary of any additional gestalt laws you find. 


È D | log U e We have noted the important distinction between 


sensation and perception, and we have examined the 
processes that underlie both of them. We've seen how external stimuli evoke sensory 
responses and how our different senses process the information contained in those 
responses. We also have focused on the physical structure and internal workings of the 
individual senses, including vision, hearing, balance, smell, taste, and the skin senses, 
and we've explored how our brains organize and process sensory information to 
construct a consistent, integrated picture of the world around us. 

To complete our investigation of sensation and perception, let’s reconsider super- 
recognizers like C. S., who are able to recognize faces years after they have met people, 
often only in passing. Using your knowledge of sensation and perception, answer these 
questions: 





1. Why might some people be extremely capable at recognizing faces? 

2. Is having extremely sensitive perception always a good thing? What might be some 
drawbacks to being a super-recognizer? 

3. Does the ability of super-recognizers seem to be a matter of sensation or of percep- 
tion? Why do you think so? 





137 








States of Consciousness 


Ul 

















www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Key Concepts for Chapter 5 


MODULE 14 






What are the different states of Sleep and Dreams 


The Stages of Sleep 
REM Sleep: The Paradox of Sleep 
Applying Psychology in the 21st Century: 


Let Me Sleep on It: The Role of Sleep 
in Memory and Thinking 


Why Do We Sleep, and How 
Much Sleep Is Necessary? 


@ How much do we dayd ream? Neuroscience in Your Life: Why Are You 
Cranky? Your Brain Is Too Awake 


consciousness? @ What happens when we 
sleep, and what are the meaning and 
function of dreams? @ What are the major 


sleep disorders, and how can they be treated? 


The Function and Meaning of Dreaming 
Sleep Disturbances: Slumbering Problems 
PsychWork: Sleep Technologist 

Circadian Rhythms: Life Cycles 
Daydreams: Dreams without Sleep 


Becoming an Informed Consumer 
of Psychology: Sleeping Better 





1" ei >a Ut n = I) 







What is hypnosis, and are hypnotized people in a Hypnosis and Meditation 
Hypnosis: A Trance-Forming Experience? 


different state of consciousness? @ What are the ne 
Meditation: Regulating Our Own 


effects of meditation? State of Consciousness 
Exploring Diversity: Cross-Cultural Routes 
to Altered States of Consciousness 


MODULE 16 





What are the major classifications of drugs, Drug Use: The Highs 


n ace hereta and Lows of Consciousness 
` Stimulants: Drug Highs 
Depressants: Drug Lows 
Narcotics: Relieving Pain and Anxiety 


Becoming an Informed Consumer 
of Psychology: Identifying Drug 
and Alcohol Problems 


139 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Prologue Out of Control 


Annie Fuller knew she was in.trouble a year ago, when in the 
space of a few hours she managed to drink a male co-worker 
more than twice her size under the table. Of course, she'd 
been practicing for a quarter of her life by then; at 47, she was 
pouring a pint of bourbon, a 12-pack of beer, anda couple of 
bottles of wine into her 115-pound body each day. She had 


come to prefer alcohol to food, sex, or the company of friends 
and loved ones. Her marriage had ended; she had virtually 
stopped leaving the house, except to work and to drink. Fuller 
had tried and failed enough times over the years to know that 
she would not be able to sober up on her own. (Interlandi, 
2008, p. 36) 





Annie Fuller was an alcoholic—a person who abuses alcohol to 
the point where it causes serious life problems. A common 
experience of alcoholics is that they develop a resistance to 
alcohol, having to consume ever greater quantities of it to 
achieve the same effects, as Annie Fuller experienced. What those 
effects are, why people find them so pleasurable, and why some 
people become alcoholics are some of the questions we will 
address as we turn our attention to the study of consciousness. 

Consciousness is the awareness of the sensations, thoughts, 
and feelings we experience at a given moment. Consciousness is 
our subjective understanding of both the environment around us 
and our private internal world, unobservable to outsiders. 

In waking consciousness, we are awake and aware of our 
thoughts, emotions, and perceptions. All other states of con- 
sciousness are considered altered states of consciousness. Among 
these, sleeping and dreaming occur naturally; drug use and 
hypnosis, in contrast, are methods of deliberately altering one’s 
state of consciousness. 

In the past, because consciousness is so personal a phenom- 
enon, psychologists were sometimes reluctant to study it. After 
all, who can say that your consciousness is similar to or, for that 
matter, different from anyone else's? Although the earliest 
psychologists, including William James (1890), saw the study of 
consciousness as central to the field, later psychologists sug- 
gested that it was out of bounds for the discipline. They argued 
that consciousness could be understood only by relying “unscien- 
tifically” on what experimental participants said they were 
experiencing. In this view, it was philosophers—not psycholo- 
gists—who should speculate on such knotty issues as whether 
consciousness is separate from the physical body, how people 
know they exist, and how the body and mind are related to each 
other (Barresi, 2007; Gennaro, 2004; Rychlak, 1997). 


140 


Contemporary psychologists reject the view that the study of 
consciousness is unsuitable for the field of psychology. Instead, 
they argue that several approaches permit the scientific study of 
consciousness. For example, behavioral neuroscientists can 
measure brain-wave patterns under conditions of consciousness 
ranging from sleep to waking to hypnotic trances. And new 
understanding of the chemistry of drugs such as marijuana and 
alcohol has provided insights into the way they produce their 
pleasurable—as well as adverse—effects (Baars & Seth, 2009; 
Damasio, 2003; Mosher & Akins, 2007). 

Yet how humans experience consciousness remains an open 
question. Some psychologists believe that the experience of 
consciousness is produced by a quantitative increase in neuronal 
activity that occurs throughout the brain. For example, an alarm 
clock moves us from sleep to waking consciousness by its loud 
ringing, which stimulates neurons throughout the brain as a 
whole (Greenfield, 2002; Koch & Greenfield, 2007). 

In contrast, others believe that states of consciousness are 
produced by particular sets of neurons and neuronal pathways 
that are activated in specific ways. In this view, an alarm clock 
wakes us from sleep into consciousness, because specific 
neurons related to the auditory nerve are activated; the auditory 
nerve then sends a message to other neurons to release particu- 
lar neurotransmitters that produce awareness of the alarm 
(Tononi & Koch, 2008). 

Although we don't know yet which of these views is correct, it 
is clear that whatever state of consciousness we are in—be it 
waking, sleeping, hypnotic, or drug-induced—the complexities 
of consciousness are profound. 


consciousness The awareness of the sensations, thoughts, and 
feelings being experienced at a given moment. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 14 


Mike Trevino, 29, slept nine hours in nine days in his quest to win a 3,000-mile, 


cross-country bike race. For the first 38 hrs. and 646 miles, he skipped sleep entirely. 
Later he napped—with no dreams he can remember—for no more than 90 minutes a 
night. Soon he began to imagine that his support crew was part of a bomb plot. “It was 
almost like riding in a movie. I thought it was a complex dream, even though I was 
conscious,” says Trevino, who finished second. (Springen, 2004, p. 47) 


Trevino’s case is unusual—in part because he was able to function with so little sleep 


What are the different states 
of consciousness? 


What happens when we sleep, 
and what are the meaning 
and function of dreams? 


for so long—and it raises a host of questions about sleep and dreams. Can we live 


without sleep? What is the meaning of dreams? More generally, what is sleep? 
Although sleeping is a state that we all experience, there are still many unan- 

swered questions about sleep that remain, along with a considerable number of myths. 

Test your knowledge of sleep and dreams by answering the questionnaire in Figure 1. 


~ Feim i 


Sleep Quiz 


What are the major sleep 
disorders, and how can they 
be treated? 


How much do we daydream? 


There are many unanswered 
questions about sleep. Taking this quiz 
can help you clear up some of the myths. 


Although sleeping is something we all do for a significant part of our lives, myths and 
misconceptions about the topic abound. To test your own knowledge of sleep and 
dreams, try answering the following questions before reading further. 





1. Some people never dream 
True or false? 


6. 


If we lose some sleep we will 
eventually make up all the 
lost sleep the next night or 
another night. True or false? 





2. Most dreams are caused by 
bodily sensations such as an 
upset stomach. True or false? 


. No one has been able to go 


for more than 48 hours with- 
out sleep. True or false? 





3. It has been proved that 
people need eight hours 
of sleep to maintain men- 
tal health. True or false? 


4. When people do not recall 
their dreams, it is probably 
because they are secretly 
trying to forget them. True 
or false? 


. Our muscles are the most 


relaxed of the night when 
we are dreaming. True or 
false? 





. Sleep enables the brain to 


rest because little brain activ- 
ity takes place during sleep. 
True or false? 





5. Depriving someone of sleep 
will invariably cause the indi- 
vidual to become mentally 
imbalanced. True or false? 





10. 


Drugs have been proved to 
provide a long term cure 
for sleeplessness. True or 
false? 








Scoring: This is an easy set of questions to score for every item is false. But don't lose any sleep if 
you missed them; they were chosen to represent the most common myths regarding sleep. 





141 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


142 Chapter 5 States of Consciousness 


stage 1 sleep The state of transition 
between wakefulness and sleep, 
characterized by relatively rapid, 
low-amplitude brain waves. 


stage 2 sleep Asleep deeper than that 
of stage 1, characterized by a slower, 
more regular wave pattern, along with 
momentary interruptions of “sleep 
spindles.” 


stage 3 sleep Asleep characterized by 
slow brain waves, with greater peaks 
and valleys in the wave pattern than in 
stage 2 sleep. 


stage 4 sleep The deepest stage of 
sleep, during which we are least 
responsive to outside stimulation. 





Stage | 
(non-REM) 





The Stages of Sleep 


Most of us consider sleep a time of tranquility when we set aside the tensions of the 
day and spend the night in uneventful slumber. However, a closer look at sleep 
shows that a good deal of activity occurs throughout the night. 

Measures of electrical activity in the brain show that the brain is quite active 
during the night. It produces electrical discharges with systematic, wavelike patterns 
that change in height (or amplitude) and speed (or frequency) in regular sequences. 
There is also significant physical activity in muscle and eye movements. 

People progress through a series of distinct stages of sleep during a night’s rest— 
known as stage 1 through stage 4 and REM sleep—moving through the stages in cycles 
lasting about 90 minutes. Each of these sleep stages is associated with a unique pat- 
tern of brain waves, which you can see in Figure 2. 

When people first go to sleep, they move from a waking state in which they are 
relaxed with their eyes closed into stage 1 sleep, which is characterized by relatively 
rapid, low-amplitude brain waves. This is actually a stage of transition between 
wakefulness and sleep and lasts only a few minutes. During stage 1, images some- 
times appear, as if we were viewing still photos, although this is not true dreaming, 
which occurs later in the night. 

As sleep becomes deeper, people enter stage 2 sleep, which makes up about 
half of the total sleep of those in their early 20s and is characterized by a slower, 
more regular wave pattern. However, there are also momentary interruptions of 
sharply pointed, spiky waves that are called, because of their configuration, sleep 
spindles. It becomes increasingly difficult to awaken a person from sleep as stage 2 
progresses. 

As people drift into stage 3 sleep, the brain waves become slower, with higher 
peaks and lower valleys in the wave pattern. By the time sleepers arrive at stage 4 
sleep, the pattern is even slower and more regular, and people are least responsive 
to outside stimulation. 


As sleep becomes 
deeper, brain waves 


take on a slower 
wave pattern 


Sleep spindle 
| 








REM 


Stage 2 
(non-REM) 


Stage 3 
(non-REM) 


Stage 4 
(non-REM) 


FIGURE 2 Brain-wave patterns (measured by an EEG apparatus) vary significantly during the 
different stages of sleep (Hobson, 1989). As sleep moves from stage 1 through stage 4, brain 
waves become slower. During REM sleep, however, the fast wave patterns are similar to 
relaxed wakefulness. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Sleep stage 


REM sleep 














Hours slept 


As you can see in Figure 3, stage 4 sleep is most likely to occur during the early 
part of the night. In the first half of the night, sleep is dominated by stages 3 and 4. The 
second half is characterized by stages 1 and 2—as well as a fifth stage during which 
dreams occur. 


REM Sleep: The Paradox of Sleep 


Several times a night, when sleepers have cycled back to a shallower state of sleep, 
something curious happens. Their heart rate increases and becomes irregular, their 
blood pressure rises, and their breathing rate increases. Most characteristic of this 
period is the back-and-forth movement of their eyes, as if they were watching an 
action-filled movie. This period of sleep is called rapid eye movement, or REM sleep, 
and it contrasts with stages 1 through 4, which are collectively labeled non-REM (or 
NREM) sleep. REM sleep occupies a little more than 20% of adults’ total sleeping time. 








People progress through four distinct stages of sleep during a night's rest spread over cycles 
lasting about 90 minutes. REM sleep, which occupies only 20% of adults’ sleeping time, occurs 
in stage 1 sleep. These photos, taken at different times of night, show the synchronized 
patterns of a couple accustomed to sleeping in the same bed. 


Module 14 Sleep and Dreams 143 


FIGURE 3 During the night, the typical 
sleeper passes through all four stages of 
sleep and several REM periods. (Source: 
From Ernest Hartmann, The Biology of 
Dreaming (1967), p. 6. Courtesy of Charles C 
Thomas Publisher, Ltd., Springfield, Illinois.) 


rapid eye movement (REM) sleep 
Sleep occupying 20% of an adult’s 
sleeping time, characterized by 
increased heart rate, blood pressure, 
and breathing rate; erections; eye 
movements; and the experience of 
dreaming. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Let Me Sleep on It: The Role of 
Sleep in Memory and Thinking 


“Let me sleep on it. And I'll give you an 
answer in the morning.” 

As the popular song lyric suggests, lay- 
people and scientists alike have long sus- 
pected that sleep plays an important role in 
thinking and memory—but what that role 
might be has been less than clear. One idea is 
that a process called memory consolidation 
occurs during sleep. While we're awake, our 
brains store information from our experi- 
ences throughout the day in the hippocam- 
pus region. But for long term storage, those 
memories need to be relocated to the cortex, 
where connections are made between the 
new information and what we already 
know. This process helps us to make better 
sense of the new information and to retain 
it—and it may be facilitated by sleep, when 
the influx of new information slows to a 
trickle (Cai et al., 2009; Gilestro, Tononi, & 
Cirelli, 2009). 

Recent studies are shedding light on 
how memory consolidation works. In one, 
participants memorized a long list of word 
pairs immediately before they went to 
sleep. As soon as they reached the deepest 
stages of sleep, researchers used electrodes 
to stimulate very slow brain waves in 
some of the participants. The participants 
whose brain waves were altered in this 
way showed better recall for the word 


y Alert 


Differentiate the five stages 
of sleep (stage 1, stage 2, 
stage 3, stage 4, and REM 
sleep), which produce dif- 
ferent brain-wave patterns. 


144 





Does sleeping help us to remember more 
effectively? 


pairs the next morning. The researchers 
theorize that these slow brain waves im- 
proved memory storage by strengthening 
the connections between neurons (Ver- 
leger et al., 2008). 

Another study showed how sleep is in- 
volved in thinking as well as memory. The 
researchers gave participants a puzzle that 
involved performing a set of seven opera- 
tions on a set of numbers, with the outcome 


of the seventh operation as the final answer. 
The participants were given a series of 
these puzzles using different number sets, 
and they were instructed to find the an- 
swers quickly. What the participants didn’t 
know was that there was a “trick” to the 
puzzles: the final answer was always the 
same as the outcome of the second opera- 
tion. If they realized this trick, they could 
skip most of the work and give the final an- 
swer much more quickly. 

The question was whether “sleeping on 
it” would help participants achieve this 
insight—and it did. Participants who 
worked a set of puzzles before sleeping and 
then worked another set the next morning 
were almost three times more likely to dis- 
cover the trick than were participants who 
took a long break between puzzle sessions 
but did not sleep (Stickgold & Wehrwein, 
2009; Yordanova et al., 2008). 

This, and other findings like it, suggest 
that sleep plays an important role in help- 
ing us to analyze and make meaning of 
our waking experiences. “Sleeping on it,” 
then, may turn out to be a reasonable 
strategy for coming to solutions for our 
problems. 





e Why do you think memory and thinking may be improved by sleeping? 
e How could students make use of memory consolidation while sleeping to improve 
their test performance? 





Paradoxically, while all this activity is occurring, the major muscles of the body 
appear to be paralyzed. In addition, and most important, REM sleep is usually accom- 
panied by dreams, which—whether or not people remember them—are experienced 
by everyone during some part of their night’s sleep. Although some dreaming occurs 
in non-REM stages of sleep, dreams are most likely to occur in the REM period, where 
they are the most vivid and easily remembered (Conduit, Crewther, & Coleman, 2004; 
Lu et al., 2006; Titone, 2002). 

There is good reason to believe that REM sleep plays a critical role in everyday 
human functioning. People deprived of REM sleep—by being awakened every time 
they begin to display the physiological signs of that stage—show a rebound effect 
when allowed to rest undisturbed. With this rebound effect, REM-deprived sleepers 
spend significantly more time in REM sleep than they normally would. In addition, 
REM sleep may play a role in learning and memory, allowing us to rethink and 
restore information and emotional experiences that we’ve had during the day 
(Nishida et al., 2009; Walker & van der Helm, 2009). (Also see Applying Psychology in 
the 21st Century.) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Why Do We Sleep, and How 
Much Sleep Is Necessary? 


Sleep is a requirement for normal human functioning, although, surprisingly, we 
don’t know exactly why. It is reasonable to expect that our bodies would require a 
tranquil “rest and relaxation” period to revitalize themselves, and experiments with 
rats show that total sleep deprivation results in death. But why? 

One explanation, based on an evolutionary perspective, suggests that sleep permit- 
ted our ancestors to conserve energy at night, a time when food was relatively hard 
to come by. Consequently, they were better able to forage for food when the sun is up. 

A second explanation for why we sleep is that sleep restores and replenishes our 
brains and bodies. For instance, the reduced activity of the brain during non-REM 
sleep may give neurons in the brain a chance to repair themselves. Furthermore, the 
onset of REM sleep stops the release of neurotransmitters called monoamines and so 
permits receptor cells to get some necessary rest and to increase their sensitivity 
during periods of wakefulness (McNamara, 2004; Siegel, 2003; Steiger, 2007). 

Finally, sleep may be essential, because it assists physical growth and brain 
development in children. For example, the release of growth hormones is associated 
with deep sleep (Peterfi et al., 2010). 

Still, these explanations remain speculative, and there is no definitive answer as 
to why sleep is essential. Furthermore, scientists have been unable to establish just 
how much sleep is absolutely required. Most people today sleep between seven and 
eight hours each night, which is three hours a night less than people slept a hundred 
years ago. In addition, there is wide variability among individuals, with some people 
needing as little as three hours of sleep (see Figure 4). Sleep requirements also vary 
over the course of a lifetime: As they age, people generally need less and less sleep. 

People who participate in sleep deprivation experiments, in which they are kept 
awake for stretches as long as 200 hours, show no lasting effects. It’s no fun—they feel 
weary and irritable, can’t concentrate, and show a loss of creativity, even after only 
minor deprivation. They also show a decline in logical reasoning ability. However, after 
being allowed to sleep normally, they bounce back quickly and are able to perform at 
predeprivation levels after just a few days (Babson et al., 2009; Mograss et al., 2009). 

In short, as far as we know, most people suffer no permanent consequences of 
such temporary sleep deprivation. But—and this is an important but—a lack of sleep 





50 


40 


30 


20 


Percentage of people 











— os 
4 5 6 7 8 9 10 II 


Number of hours of sleep 


Module 14 Sleep and Dreams 145 


FIGURE 4 Although most people report 
sleeping between eight and nine hours 
per night, the amount that individuals 
needs varies a great deal (Borbely, 1986). 
Where would you place yourself on this 
graph, and why do you think you need 
more or less sleep than others? 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


146 Chapter 5 States of Consciousness 


FIGURE 6 Although dreams tend to be 
subjective to the person having them, 
there are common elements that 
frequently occur in everyone's dreams. 
Why do you think so many common 
dreams are unpleasant and so few are 
pleasant? Do you think this tells us 
anything about the function of dreams? 
(Source: Schneider & Domhoff, 2002.) 


Why Are You Cranky? 


Your Brain Is Too Awake 





FIGURE 5 One explanation about why we need sleep is that we need to restore and repair 
various systems in our brains. One such system is the one that regulates our emotions. In the 
MRI scan, the amygdala, which helps process emotions, shows less activation (seen in reds and 
oranges) when viewing emotional pictures when participants had enough sleep compared to 
when they were sleep deprived, suggesting that participants responded more emotionally 
when they had less sleep. (Source: Walker and van der Helm 2009.) 


No Sleep 


t-score 
0 mo 





can make us feel edgy, slow our reaction time, and lower our performance on aca- 
demic and physical tasks. In addition, we put ourselves, and others, at risk when we 
carry out routine activities, such as driving, when we're very sleepy (Anderson & 
Home, 2006; Morad et al., 2009; Philip et al., 2005) (also see Figure 5). 


The Function and Meaning 
of Dreaming 





I was sitting at my desk when I remembered that this was the day of my chemistry final! I 
was terrified, because I hadn’t studied a bit for it. In fact, I had missed every lecture all 
semester. In a panic, I began running across campus desperately searching for the class- 
room, to which I’d never been. It was hopeless; I knew I was going to fail and flunk out 





of college. 

Percentage of Dreams 

Reporting at Least One Event 

Thematic Event Males Females 
Aggression 47% 44% 
Friendliness 38 42 
Sexuality 12 04 
Misfortune 36 33 
Success 15 08 


Failure 15 10 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Theory Basic Explanation 
Unconscious wish 


fulfillment theory (Freud) 


Psychoanalytical explanation where 
dreams represent unconscious 
wishes the dreamer wants to fulfill 


Dreams-for-survival 
theory 


Evolutionary explanation where 
information relevant to daily survival 


is reconsidered and reprocessed survival 


Activation-synthesis 
theory 


Neuroscience explanation where 
dreams are the result of random 
activation of various memories, 
which are tied together in a logical 
story line 


FIGURE 7 Three theories of dreams. As researchers have yet to agree on the fundamental 
meaning of dreams, several theories about dreaming have emerged. 


If you have had a similar dream—a surprisingly common dream among people involved 
in academic pursuits—you know how utterly convincing are the panic and fear that the 
events in the dream can bring about. Nightmares, unusually frightening dreams, occur 
fairly often. In one survey, almost half of a group of college students who kept records 
of their dreams over a two-week period reported having at least one nightmare. This 
works out to some 24 nightmares per person each year, on average (Levin & Nielsen, 
2009; Nielson, Stenstrom, & Levin, 2006; Schredl et al., 2009). 

However, most of the 150,000 dreams the average person experiences by the age 
of 70 are much less dramatic. They typically encompass everyday events such as 
going to the supermarket, working at the office, and preparing a meal. Students 
dream about going to class; professors dream about lecturing. Dental patients dream 
of getting their teeth drilled; dentists dream of drilling the wrong tooth. The English 
have tea with the queen in their dreams; in the United States, people go to a bar with 
the president (Domhoff, 1996; Schredl & Piel, 2005; Taylor & Bryant, 2007). Figure 6 
shows the most common themes found in people’s dreams. 

But what, if anything, do all these dreams mean? Whether dreams have a specific 
significance and function is a question that scientists have considered for many years, 
and they have developed the three alternative theories we discuss below (and sum- 
marized in Figure 7). 


PSYCHOANALYTIC EXPLANATIONS OF DREAMS: 
DO DREAMS REPRESENT UNCONSCIOUS WISH FULFILLMENT? 


Using psychoanalytic theory, Sigmund Freud viewed dreams as a guide to the uncon- 
scious (Freud, 1900). In his unconscious wish fulfillment theory, he proposed that 
dreams represent unconscious wishes that dreamers desire to see fulfilled. However, 
because these wishes are threatening to the dreamer’s conscious awareness, the 
actual wishes—called the latent content of dreams—are disguised. The true subject 
and meaning of a dream, then, may have little to do with its apparent story line, 
which Freud called the manifest content of dreams. 

To Freud, it was important to pierce the armor of a dream’s manifest content to 
understand its true meaning. To do this, Freud tried to get people to discuss their 
dreams, associating symbols in the dreams with events in the past. He also suggested 
that certain common symbols with universal meanings appear in dreams. For exam- 
ple, to Freud, dreams in which a person is flying symbolize a wish for sexual inter- 
course. (See Figure 8 for other common symbols.) 

Many psychologists reject Freud’s view that dreams typically represent uncon- 
scious wishes and that particular objects and events in a dream are symbolic. Rather, 


Meaning of Dreams 


Latent content reveals 
unconscious wishes 


Clues to everyday 
concerns about 


Dream scenario that is 
constructed is related 
to dreamer’'s concerns 


Module 14 Sleep and Dreams 147 


Is Meaning of Dream 
Disguised? 

Yes, by manifest 
content of dreams 


Not necessarily 


Not necessarily 


y Alert 


Use Figure 7 to learn the 
differences between the 
three main explanations of 
dreaming. 


unconscious wish fulfillment 

theory Sigmund Freud’s theory that 
dreams represent unconscious wishes 
that dreamers desire to see fulfilled. 


latent content of dreams According to 
Freud, the “disguised” meanings of 
dreams, hidden by more obvious 
subjects. 


manifest content of dreams Accord- 
ing to Freud, the apparent story line of 
dreams. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


148 Chapter 5 States of Consciousness 





dreams-for-survival theory The 
theory suggesting that dreams permit 
information that is critical for our daily 
survival to be reconsidered and 
reprocessed during sleep. 


Symbol (Manifest Content of Dream) Interpretation (Latent Content) 


Climbing up a stairway, crossing a bridge, riding Sexual intercourse 
an elevator, flying in an airplane, walking down a 

long hallway, entering a room, train traveling 

through a tunnel 


Apples , peaches grapefruits Breasts 

Bullets, fire, snakes, sticks, umbrellas, guns, hoses, Male sex organs 
knives 

Ovens, boxes, tunnels, closets, caves, bottles, ship Female sex organs 


FIGURE 8 According to Freud, dreams contain common symbols with universal meanings. 


they believe that the direct, overt action of a dream is the focal point of its meaning. 
For example, a dream in which we are walking down a long hallway to take an exam 
for which we haven't studied does not relate to unconscious, unacceptable wishes. 
Instead, it simply may mean that we are concerned about an impending test. Even 
more complex dreams can often be interpreted in terms of everyday concerns and 
stress (Picchioni et al., 2002; Cartwright, Agargum, & Kirkby, 2006). 

Moreover, some dreams reflect events occurring in the dreamer’s environment 
as he or she is sleeping. For example, sleeping participants in one experiment were 
sprayed with water while they were dreaming. Those unlucky volunteers reported 
more dreams involving water than did a comparison group of participants who were 
left to sleep undisturbed (Dement & Wolpert, 1958). Similarly, it is not unusual to 
wake up to find that the doorbell that was heard ringing in a dream is actually an 
alarm clock telling us it is time to get up. 

However, PET brain scan research does lend a degree of support for the wish 
fulfillment view. For instance, the limbic and paralimbic regions of the brain, which 
are associated with emotion and motivation, are particularly active during REM 
sleep. At the same time, the association areas of the prefrontal cortex, which control 
logical analysis and attention, are inactive during REM sleep. The high activation of 
emotional and motivational centers of the brain during dreaming makes it more 
plausible that dreams may reflect unconscious wishes and instinctual needs, as Freud 
suggested (Braun et al., 1998; Occhionero, 2004; Wehrle et al., 2007). 


EVOLUTIONARY EXPLANATIONS OF DREAMS: 
DREAMS-FOR-SURVIVAL THEORY 


According to the dreams-for-survival theory, which is based in the evolutionary 
perspective, dreams permit us to reconsider and reprocess during sleep information 
that is critical for our daily survival. Dreaming is considered an inheritance from our 
animal ancestors, whose small brains were unable to sift sufficient information dur- 
ing waking hours. Consequently, dreaming provided a mechanism that permitted 
the processing of information 24 hours a day. 

According to this theory, dreams represent concerns about our daily lives, illus- 
trating our uncertainties, indecisions, ideas, and desires. Dreams are seen, then, as 
consistent with everyday living. Rather than being disguised wishes, as Freud sug- 
gested, they represent key concerns growing out of our daily experiences (Ross, 2006; 
Winson, 1990). 

Research supports the dreams-for-survival theory, suggesting that certain dreams 
permit people to focus on and to consolidate memories, particularly dreams that per- 
tain to “how-to-do-it” memories related to motor skills. For example, rats seem to 
dream about mazes that they learned to run through during the day, at least accord- 
ing to the patterns of brain activity that appear while they are sleeping (Kenway & 
Wilson, 2001; Kuriyama, Stickgold, & Walker, 2004; Smith, 2006; Stickgold et al., 2001). 

A similar phenomenon appears to work in humans. For instance, in one experi- 
ment, participants learned a visual memory task late in the day. They were then sent 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


to bed, but awakened at certain times during the night. THE FAR SIDE” 
When they were awakened at times that did not interrupt 
dreaming, their performance on the memory task typically 
improved the next day. But when they were awakened dur- 
ing rapid eye movement (REM) sleep—the stage of sleep 
when people dream—their performance declined. The impli- 
cation is that dreaming, at least when it is uninterrupted, can 
play a role in helping us remember material to which we 
have been previously exposed (Karni et al., 1994; Marshall 
& Born, 2007; Nishida et al., 2009). 


NEUROSCIENCE EXPLANATIONS OF DREAMS: 
ACTIVATION-SYNTHESIS THEORY 


Using the neuroscience perspective, psychiatrist J. Allan 
Hobson has proposed the activation-synthesis theory of 
dreams. The activation-synthesis theory focuses on the 
random electrical energy that the brain produces during 
REM sleep, possibly as a result of changes in the production 
of particular neurotransmitters. This electrical energy ran- 
domly stimulates memories stored in the brain. Because we 
have a need to make sense of our world even while asleep, 
the brain takes these chaotic memories and weaves them into 
a logical story line, filling in the gaps to produce a rational 
scenario (Hobson, 2005; Porte & Hobson, 1996). 
Activation-synthesis theory has been refined by the acti- 
vation information modulation (AIM) theory. According to AIM, 
dreams are initiated in the brain’s pons, which sends random 
signals to the cortex. Areas of the cortex that are involved in 
particular waking behaviors are related to the content of 
dreams. For example, areas of the brain related to vision are 
involved in the visual aspects of the dream, while areas of the brain related to move- 
ment are involved in aspects of the dream related to motion (Hobson, 2007). 
Activation-synthesis and AIM theories do not entirely reject the view that dreams 
reflect unconscious wishes. They suggest that the particular scenario a dreamer produces 
is not random but instead is a clue to the dreamer’s fears, emotions, and concerns. 
Hence, what starts out as a random process culminates in something meaningful. 


Sleep Disturbances: 
Slumbering Problems 


At one time or another, almost all of us have difficulty sleeping—a condition known 
as insomnia. It could be due to a particular situation, such as the breakup of a rela- 
tionship, concern about a test score, or the loss of a job. Some cases of insomnia, 
however, have no obvious cause. Some people are simply unable to fall asleep easily, 
or they go to sleep readily but wake up frequently during the night. Insomnia is a 
problem that afflicts as many as one-third of all people. Women and older adults are 
more likely to suffer from insomnia, as well as people who are unusually thin or are 
depressed (Bains, 2006; Cooke & Ancoli-Israel, 2006; Henry et al., 2008). 

Some people who think they have sleeping problems actually are mistaken. For 
example, researchers in sleep laboratories have found that some people who report 
being up all night actually fall asleep in 30 minutes and stay asleep all night. Fur- 
thermore, some people with insomnia accurately recall sounds that they heard while 
they were asleep, which gives them the impression that they were awake during the 
night (Semler & Harvey, 2005; Yapko, 2006). (Also see PsychWork.) 








Module 14 Sleep and Dreams 149 


By GARY LARSON 


The Far Side” by Gary Larson © 1983 FarWorks, Inc. All Rights Reserved. The Far Side* and 
the Larson" signature are registered trademarks of FarWorks, Inc. Used with permission. 


“I’ve got it again, Larry ... an eerie feeling like 
there’s something on top of the bed.” 


activation-synthesis theory Hobson's 
theory that the brain produces random 
electrical energy during REM sleep 
that stimulates memories stored in the 
brain. 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


150 Chapter 5 States of Consciousness 


Name: Brandon Liebig 


Diagnostics, Northbrook, IL 





Sleepwalking (along with sleeptalking) 
are more common in children than 
adults, and they both occur during 
stage 4 of sleep. 





> Although each of us spends the majority of our time sleeping, 
Ea» PsychWork 
SLEEP TECHNOLOGIST 


sleep—or rather a lack of it—is a state that is problematic for 
many people. For those seeking treatment for sleep disorders, 
sleep technologist Brandon Liebig is on the front lines, assisting 
in clinical assessments, helping to monitor and test patients, 


Position: Sleep Technologist, Central Sleep and participating in the development of treatment procedures. 


As Liebig notes, “Patients seen in the sleep lab often have 


Education: BFA, Studio Art, University of Nebraska complicated medical backgrounds and health needs, and some 


at Omaha; Graduate of Accredited Sleep may have cognitive limitations/disabilities or coexisting psy- 
Technology Education Program, University of chological conditions in addition to their sleep symptoms.” 

Massachusetts Memorial Hospital Sleep Center, “Sleep technologists must recognize the particular needs of 
Worcester, MA; Certified by the Board of a patient and adjust their style of providing care to best suit 
Registered Polysomnographic Technologists the patient and promote the best possible outcomes, both for 


the patient’s experience in the sleep lab and the data collected 
in research studies,” he added. 

“Often, patients may find it stressful, unfamiliar, and some- 
times uncomfortable to sleep in a lab setting with the sensors and other equipment 
attached to their bodies. Sleep technologists use their knowledge of psychology to pro- 
vide the patient with understanding, reassurance, respect, and patience,” said Liebig. 


Other sleep problems are less common than insomnia, although they are still 
widespread. For instance, some 20 million people suffer from sleep apnea. Sleep 
apnea is a condition in which a person has difficulty breathing while sleeping. The 
result is disturbed, fitful sleep, and a significant loss of REM sleep, as the person 
is constantly reawakened when the lack of oxygen becomes great enough to trig- 
ger a waking response. Some people with apnea wake as many as 500 times dur- 
ing the course of a night, although they may not even be aware that they have 
wakened. Not surprisingly, such disturbed sleep results in extreme fatigue the next 
day. Sleep apnea also may play a role in sudden infant death syndrome (SIDS), a 
mysterious killer of seemingly normal infants who die while sleeping (Aloia, 
Smith, & Arnedt, 2007; Gami et al., 2005; Tippin, Sparks, & Rizzo, 2009). 

Night terrors are sudden awakenings from non-REM sleep that are accompanied 
by extreme fear, panic, and strong physiological arousal. Usually occurring in stage 
4 sleep, night terrors may be so frightening that a sleeper awakens with a shriek. 
Although night terrors initially produce great agitation, victims usually can get 
back to sleep fairly quickly. They are far less frequent than nightmares, and, unlike 
nightmares, they typically occur during slow-wave, non-REM sleep. They occur 
most frequently in children between the ages of 3 and 8 (Lowe, Humphreys, & 
Williams, 2007). 

Narcolepsy is uncontrollable sleeping that occurs for short periods while a person 
is awake. No matter what the activity—holding a heated conversation, exercising, or 
driving—a narcoleptic will suddenly fall asleep. People with narcolepsy go directly 
from wakefulness to REM sleep, skipping the other stages. The causes of narcolepsy 
are not known, although there could be a genetic component, because narcolepsy 
runs in families (Billiard, 2008; Ervik, Abdelnoor, & Heier, 2006; Mahmood & Black, 
2005; Nishino, 2007). 

We know relatively little about sleeptalking and sleepwalking, two sleep distur- 
bances that are usually harmless. Both occur during stage 4 sleep and are more 
common in children than in adults. Sleeptalkers and sleepwalkers usually have a 
vague consciousness of the world around them, and a sleepwalker may be able to 
walk with agility around obstructions in a crowded room. Unless a sleepwalker wan- 
ders into a dangerous environment, sleepwalking typically poses little risk. And the 
common idea that it’s dangerous to wake a sleepwalker? It’s just superstition (Baruss, 
2003; Guilleminault et al., 2005; Lee-Chiong, 2006). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Circadian Rhythms: Life Cycles 


The fact that we cycle back and forth between wakefulness and sleep is one example 
of the body’s circadian rhythms. Circadian rhythms (from the Latin circa diem, or 
“about a day”) are biological processes that occur regularly on approximately a 
24-hour cycle. Sleeping and waking, for instance, occur naturally to the beat of an 
internal pacemaker that works on a cycle of about 24 hours. Several other bodily 
functions, such as body temperature, hormone production, and blood pressure, also 
follow circadian rhythms (Beersma & Gordijn, 2007; Blatter & Cajochen, 2007; Saper 
et al., 2005). 

Circadian cycles are complex, and they involve a variety of behaviors. For 
instance, sleepiness occurs not just in the evening but throughout the day in reg- 
ular patterns, with most of us getting drowsy in mid-afternoon—regardless of 
whether we have eaten a heavy lunch. By making an afternoon siesta part of their 
everyday habit, people in several cultures take advantage of the body’s natural 
inclination to sleep at this time (Reilly & Waterhouse, 2007; Takahashi et al., 2004; 
Wright, 2002). 

The brain’s suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN) controls circadian rhythms. However, 
the relative amount of light and darkness, which varies with the seasons of the 
year, also plays a role in regulating circadian rhythms. In fact, some people experi- 
ence seasonal affective disorder, a form of severe depression in which feelings of 
despair and hopelessness increase during the winter and lift during the rest of the 
year. The disorder appears to be a result of the brevity and gloom of winter days. 
Daily exposure to bright lights is sometimes sufficient to improve the mood of 
those with this disorder (Golden et al., 2005; Kasof, 2009; Rohan, Roecklein, & 
Tierney Lindsey, 2007). 





Daydreams: Dreams 
Without Sleep 


It is the stuff of magic: Our past mistakes can be wiped out and the future filled with 
noteworthy accomplishments. Fame, happiness, and wealth can be ours. In the next 
moment, though, the most horrible tragedies can occur, leaving us devastated, alone, 
and penniless. 

The source of these scenarios is daydreams, fantasies that people construct while 
awake. Unlike dreaming that occurs during sleep, daydreams are more under peo- 
ple’s control. Therefore, their content is often more closely related to immediate 
events in the environment than is the content of the dreams that occur during sleep. 
Although they may include sexual content, daydreams also pertain to other activities 
or events that are relevant to a person’s life. 

Daydreams are a typical part of waking consciousness, even though our aware- 
ness of the environment around us declines while we are daydreaming. People vary 
considerably in the amount of daydreaming they do. For example, around 2% to 4% 
of the population spend at least half their free time fantasizing. Although most peo- 
ple daydream much less frequently, almost everyone fantasizes to some degree. Stud- 
ies that ask people to identify what they are doing at random times during the day 
have shown that they are daydreaming about 10% of the time (Holler, 2006; Lynn et 
al., 1996; Singer, 2006). 

The brain is surprisingly active during daydreaming. For example, several areas 
of the brain that are associated with complex problem solving become activated dur- 
ing daydreaming. In fact, daydreaming may be the only time these areas are activated 
simultaneously, suggesting that daydreaming may lead to insights about problems 
that we are grappling with (Fleck et al., 2008; Kounios et al., 2008). 





Module 14 Sleep and Dreams 151 





Circadian rhythms produce jet lag. 


circadian rhythms Biological 
processes that occur regularly on 
approximately a 24-hour cycle. 


daydreams Fantasies that people 
construct while awake. 





Daydreams are fantasies that people 
construct while they are awake. What 
are the similarities and differences 
between daydreams and night dreams? 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


152 Chapter 5 States of Consciousness 


A Co peer sO COILED? 
IN eer ees ae TOM 


NE? CAp- 


THEM o 
m AUER, 
a g pet 





© Drew Dernavich/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


“When I can’t sleep, I find that it sometimes helps to get up and jot down my anxieties.” 


BECOMING AN INFORMED Do you have trouble sleeping? You’re not alone—70 million 

CONSUMER people in the United States have sleep problems. For those of us 

who spend hours tossing and turning in bed, psychologists 

of Psyc h O | og y studying sleep disturbances have a number of suggestions for 

Sleeping Better overcoming insomnia (Benca, 2005; Edinger et al., 2001; Finley 
& Cowley, 2005). Here are some ideas. 


e Exercise during the day (at least six hours before bedtime) and 
avoid naps. Not surprisingly, it helps to be tired before going to sleep! Moreover, 
learning systematic relaxation techniques and biofeedback can help you unwind 
from the day’s stresses and tensions. 

e Choose a regular bedtime and stick to it. Adhering to a habitual schedule helps your 
internal timing mechanisms regulate your body mote effectively. 

e Avoid drinks with caffeine after lunch. The effects of beverages such as coffee, tea, and 
some soft drinks can linger for as long as 8 to 12 hours after they are consumed. 

e Drink a glass of warm milk at bedtime. Your grandparents were right when they 
dispensed this advice: Milk contains the chemical tryptophan, which helps 
people fall asleep. 

e Avoid sleeping pills. Even though 25% of U.S. adults report having taken medica- 
tion for sleep in the previous year, in the long run sleep medications can do more 
harm than good, because they disrupt the normal sleep cycle. 

e Try not to sleep. This approach works because people often have difficulty falling 
asleep, because they are trying so hard. A better strategy is to go to bed only 
when you feel tired. If you don’t get to sleep within 10 minutes, leave the 
bedroom and do something else, returning to bed only when you feel sleepy. 
Continue this process all night if necessary. But get up at your usual hour in the 
morning, and don’t take any naps during the day. After three or four weeks, most 
people become conditioned to associate their beds with sleep—and fall asleep 
rapidly at night (Sloan et al., 1993; Smith, 2001; Ubell, 1993). 


For long-term problems with sleep, you might consider visiting a sleep disorders 
center. For information on accredited clinics, consult the American Academy of Sleep 
Medicine at www.aasmnet.org. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


RECAP 


What are the different states of consciousness? 
e Consciousness is a person’s awareness of the sensations, 
thoughts, and feelings at a given moment. Waking 


Module 14 Sleep and Dreams 153 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


EVALUATE 


1. 


is the term used to describe our understand- 
ing of the world external to us, as well as our own internal 
world. 








consciousness can vary from more active to more passive 2. A great deal of neural activity goes on during sleep. True 
states. (p. 140) or false? 
e Altered states of consciousness include naturally 3. Dreams most often occur in sleep. 
occurring sleep and dreaming, as well as hypnotic and 4, are internal bodily processes that 
drug-induced states. (p. 140) occur on a daily cycle. 
: 5. Freud’s theory of unconscious 
What happens when we sleep, and what are the meaning and ee ear : 
: states that the actual wishes an individual expresses in 
function of dreams? aa 
T : dreams are disguised, because they are threatening to the 
e The brain is active throughout the night, and sleep i : 
; : as ; person's conscious awareness. 
proceeds through a series of stages identified by unique Pee the theory of dreaming with ite definidon. 
paliem orarin waye (Bie) 1. activation-synthesis theory 
e REM (rapid eye movement) sleep is characterized by an 
í ; A 2. dreams-for-survival theory 
increase in heart rate, a rise in blood pressure, an à : 
‘ : : i i 3. dreams as wish fulfillment 
increase in the rate of breathing, and, in males, erections. he i A 
À a. Dreams permit important information to be repro- 
Dreams most often occur during this stage. (p. 143) : 
: ; : cessed during sleep. 
e According to Freud’s psychoanalytic approach, dreams : TRE 
; ‘ b. The manifest content of dreams disguises the latent 
have both a manifest content (an apparent story line) and a 
: content of the dreams. 
latent content (a true meaning). He suggested that the la- : : ; 3 
2 , ; A c. Electrical energy stimulates random memories, which 
tent content provides a guide to a dreamer’s unconscious, BES ee pd et en hae ele 
revealing unfulfilled wishes or desires. (p. 147) 8 p j 
e The dreams-for-survival theory, grounded in an 
evolutionary perspective, suggests that information RETHINK 
relevant to daily survival is reconsidered and repro- 1. Suppose that a new “miracle pill” allows a person to func- 
cessed in dreams. Taking a neuroscience approach, the tion with only one hour of sleep per night. However, be- 
activation-synthesis theory proposes that dreams are a cause a night's sleep is so short, a person who takes the 
result of random electrical energy that stimulates pill will never dream again. Knowing what you do about 
different memories, which then are woven into a the functions of sleep and dreaming, what would be some 
coherent story line. (p. 148) advantages and drawbacks of such a pill from a personal 
What are the major sleep disorders, and how can they be standpoint? Would you take such a pill? 
treated? 2. From the perspective of an educator: How might you use 


e Insomnia is a sleep disorder characterized by difficulty 
sleeping. Sleep apnea is a condition in which people 
have difficulty sleeping and breathing at the same time. 
People with narcolepsy have an uncontrollable urge to 
sleep. Sleepwalking and sleeptalking are relatively 
harmless.(p. 149) 


How much do we daydream? 
e Wide individual differences exist in the amount of time 
devoted to daydreaming. Almost everyone daydreams 
or fantasizes to some degree. (p. 151) 


the findings in sleep research to maximize student 
learning? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


gre “L-g -1'9 Fuon] 
USIM ‘G 'SWYJÁYI ULIPO P NAA CE ‘ond 7 ‘ssausnosuosd ‘I 


KEY TERMS 


consciousness p. 140 rapid eye movement (REM) manifest content of activation-synthesis 


stage 1 sleep p. 142 sleep p. 143 dreams p. 147 theory p. 149 
stage 2 sleep p. 142 unconscious wish fulfillment dreams-for-survival circadian rhythms p. 151 
stage 3 sleep p. 142 theory p. 147 theory p. 148 daydreams p. 151 


latent content of 
dreams p. 147 


stage 4 sleep p. 142 


MODULE 15 


What is hypnosis, and are 
hypnotized people ina 
different state of 
consciousness? 


What are the effects of 
meditation? 


A trancelike state of 
heightened susceptibility to the 
suggestions of others. 


154 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


You are feeling relaxed and drowsy. You are getting sleepier. Your body is becoming 
limp. Your eyelids are feeling heavier. Your eyes are closing; you can’t keep them open 
anymore. You are totally relaxed. Now, place your hands above your head. But you will 
find they are getting heavier and heavier—so heavy you can barely keep them up. In 
fact, although you are straining as hard as you can, you will be unable to hold them up 
any longer. 


An observer watching this scene would notice a curious phenomenon. Many of the 
people listening to the voice are dropping their arms to their sides. The reason for 
this strange behavior? Those people have been hypnotized. 


Hypnosis: A Trance-Forming 
Experience? 


People under hypnosis are in a trancelike state of heightened susceptibility to the 
suggestions of others. In some respects, it appears that they are asleep. Yet other 
aspects of their behavior contradict this notion, for people are attentive to the hyp- 
notist’s suggestions and may carry out bizarre or silly suggestions. 

How is someone hypnotized? Typically, the process follows a series of four steps. 
First, a person is made comfortable in a quiet environment. Second, the hypnotist 
explains what is going to happen, such as telling the person that he or she will 
experience a pleasant, relaxed state. Third, the hypnotist tells the person to concen- 
trate on a specific object or image, such as the hypnotist’s moving finger or an image 
of a calm lake. The hypnotist may have the person concentrate on relaxing different 
parts of the body, such as the arms, legs, and chest. Fourth, once the subject is in a 
highly relaxed state, the hypnotist may make suggestions that the person interprets 
as being produced by hypnosis, such as “Your arms are getting heavy” and “Your 
eyelids are more difficult to open.” Because the person begins to experience these 
sensations, he or she believes they are caused by the hypnotist and becomes suscep- 
tible to the suggestions of the hypnotist. 

Despite their compliance when hypnotized, people do not lose all will of their 
own. They will not perform antisocial behaviors, and they will not carry out self- 
destructive acts. People will not reveal hidden truths about themselves, and they are 
capable of lying. Moreover, people cannot be hypnotized against their will—despite 
popular misconceptions (Gwynn & Spanos, 1996; Raz, 2007). 

There are wide variations in people’s susceptibility to hypnosis. About 5% to 
20% of the population cannot be hypnotized at all, and some 15% are very easily 
hypnotized. Most people fall somewhere in between. Moreover, the ease with 
which a person is hypnotized is related to a number of other characteristics. Peo- 
ple who are readily hypnotized are also easily absorbed while reading books or 
listening to music, becoming unaware of what is happening around them, and 
they often spend an unusual amount of time daydreaming. In sum, then, they 
show a high ability to concentrate and to become completely absorbed in what 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 15 Hypnosis and Meditation 155 


Despite common misconceptions, 
people cannot be hypnotized against 
their will, nor do they lose all will of 
their own when they are hypnotized. 
Why, then, do people sometimes behave 
so unusually when asked to by a 
hypnotist? 





they are doing (Benham, Woody, & Wilson, 2006; Kirsch & Braffman, 2001; Rubichi 
et al., 2005). 


A DIFFERENT STATE OF CONSCIOUSNESS? 


The question of whether hypnosis is a state of consciousness that is qualitatively 
different from normal waking consciousness is controversial. Some psychologists 
believe that hypnosis represents a state of consciousness that differs significantly 
from other states. In this view, high suggestibility, increased ability to recall and 
construct images, and acceptance of suggestions that clearly contradict reality suggest 
it is a different state. Moreover, changes in electrical activity in the brain are associ- 
ated with hypnosis, supporting the position that hypnosis is a state of consciousness 
different from normal waking (Fingelkurts, Fingelkurts, & Kallio, 2007; Hilgard, 1992; 
Kallio & Revonsuo, 2003). 

In this view, hypnosis represents a state of divided consciousness. According to 
famed hypnosis researcher Ernest Hilgard, hypnosis brings about a dissociation, or 
division, of consciousness into two simultaneous components. In one stream of con- 
sciousness, hypnotized people are following the commands of the hypnotist. Yet on 
another level of consciousness, they are acting as “hidden observers,” aware of what 
is happening to them. For instance, hypnotic subjects may appear to be following 
the hypnotist’s suggestion about feeling no pain, yet in another stream of conscious- 
ness they may be actually aware of the pain. 

On the other side of the controversy are psychologists who reject the notion 
that hypnosis is a state significantly different from normal waking consciousness. 
They argue that altered brain-wave patterns are not sufficient to demonstrate a 
qualitative difference, because no other specific physiological changes occur when 
people are in trances. Furthermore, little support exists for the contention that 
adults can recall memories of childhood events accurately while hypnotized. That 
lack of evidence suggests that there is nothing qualitatively special about the hyp- 
notic trance (Hongchun & Ming, 2006; Lynn et al., 2003; Lynn, Fassler, & Knox, 
2005; Wagstaff, 2009). 

There is increasing agreement that the controversy over the nature of hypnosis 
has led to extreme positions on both sides of the issue. More recent approaches sug- 
gest that the hypnotic state may best be viewed as lying along a continuum in which 
hypnosis is neither a totally different state of consciousness nor totally similar to 
normal waking consciousness (Jamieson, 2007; Lynn et al., 2000; Kihlstrom, 2005b). 


Alert 


The question of whether hyp- 

nosis represents a different 
state of consciousness or is 
similar to normal waking 

consciousness is a key issue. 






www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


156 Chapter 5 States of Consciousness 


As arguments about the true nature of hypnosis continue, though, one thing 
is clear: Hypnosis has been used successfully to solve practical human problems. 
In fact, psychologists working in many different areas have found hypnosis to be 
a reliable, effective tool. It has been applied to a number of areas, including the 
following: 


e Controlling pain. Patients suffering from chronic pain may be given the sugges- 
tion, while hypnotized, that their pain is gone or reduced. They also may be 
taught to hypnotize themselves to relieve pain or gain a sense of control over 
their symptoms. Hypnosis has proved to be particularly useful during child- 
birth and dental procedures (Accardi & Milling, 2009; Hammond, 2007; 
Mehl-Madrona, 2004). 

Reducing smoking. Although it hasn’t been successful in stopping drug and 

alcohol abuse, hypnosis sometimes helps people stop smoking through 

hypnotic suggestions that the taste and smell of cigarettes are unpleasant 

(Elkins et al., 2006; Fuller, 2006; Green, Lynn, & Montgomery, 2008). 

e Treating psychological disorders. Hypnosis sometimes is used during 
treatment for psychological disorders. For example, it may be employed 
to heighten relaxation, reduce anxiety, increase expectations of success, or 
modify self-defeating thoughts (Golden, 2006; Iglesias, 2005; Zarren 
& Eimer, 2002). 

e Assisting in law enforcement. Witnesses and victims are sometimes better able to 
recall the details of a crime when hypnotized. In one often-cited case, a 
witness to the kidnapping of a group of California schoolchildren was placed 
under hypnosis and was able to recall all but one digit of the license number 
on the kidnapper’s vehicle. However, hypnotic recollections may also be 
inaccurate, just as other recollections are often inaccurate. Consequently, the 
legal status of hypnosis is unresolved (Kazar, 2006; Knight & Meyer, 2007; 
Whitehouse et al., 2005). 

e Improving athletic performance. Athletes sometimes turn to hypnosis to 
improve their performance. For example, some baseball players have used 
hypnotism to increase their concentration when batting, with considerable 
success (Barker & Jones, 2008; Grindstaff & Fisher, 2006; Lindsay, Maynard, 
& Thomas, 2005). 


Meditation: Regulating Our Own 
State of Consciousness 


When traditional practitioners of the ancient Eastern religion of Zen Buddhism 
want to achieve greater spiritual insight, they turn to a technique that has been 
used for centuries to alter their state of consciousness. This technique is called 
meditation. 





meditation A learned technique for 
refocusing attention that brings about 


an altered state of consciousness. ; : : f 
Meditation is a learned technique for refocusing attention that brings 


about an altered state of consciousness. Meditation typically consists of 

ae ~~ the repetition of a mantra—a sound, word, or syllable—over and over. In 
a (ammooog) some forms of meditation, the focus is on a picture, flame, or specific part 
A (i Q) of the body. Regardless of the nature of the particular initial stimulus, the 

M key to the procedure is concentrating on it so thoroughly that the medita- 

È yt 





tor becomes unaware of any outside stimulation and reaches a different 
state of consciousness. 

After meditation, people report feeling thoroughly relaxed. They some- 
times relate that they have gained new insights into themselves and the prob- 
lems they are facing. The long-term practice of meditation may even improve 
health because of the biological changes it produces. For example, during 





© Mischa Richter/The New Yorker Collection/ 


www.cartoonbank.com. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 15 Hypnosis and Meditation 157 





meditation, oxygen usage decreases, heart rate and blood pressure decline, and brain- 
wave patterns change (Barnes et al., 2004; Lee, Kleinman, & Kleinman, 2007; Travis 
et al., 2009). 

Anyone can meditate by following a few simple procedures. The fundamentals 
include sitting in a quiet room with the eyes closed, breathing deeply and rhythmi- 
cally, and repeating a word or sound—such as the word one—over and over. Prac- 
ticed twice a day for 20 minutes, the technique is effective in bringing about relaxation 
(Aftanas & Golosheykin, 2005; Benson et al., 1994). 

Meditation is a means of altering consciousness that is practiced in many differ- 
ent cultures, though it can take different forms and serve different purposes across 
cultures. In fact, one impetus for the study of consciousness is the realization that 
people in many different cultures routinely seek ways to alter their states of con- 
sciousness (Walsh & Shapiro, 2006). 


A group of Native-American Sioux men sit naked in a steam- Ex p | O ri nN g sees 


ing sweat lodge as a medicine man throws water on sizzling 


k ill f scaldi t i the air. 
Tocka to Send billowe OF scale stean mtoi air Cross-Cultural Routes to Altered States 


Aztec priests smear themselves with a mixture of crushed of Consciousness 
poisonous herbs, hairy black worms, scorpions, and lizards. 





Sometimes they drink the potion. 


During the 16th century, a devout Hasidic Jew lies across the tombstone of a celebrated 
scholar. As he murmurs the name of God repeatedly, he seeks to be possessed by the soul of 
the dead wise man’s spirit. If successful, he will attain a mystical state, and the deceased's 
words will flow out of his mouth. 


Each of these rituals has a common goal: suspension from the bonds of everyday 
awareness and access to an altered state of consciousness. Although they may seem 
exotic from the vantage point of many Western cultures, these rituals represent an 
apparently universal effort to alter consciousness (Bartocci, 2004; Irwin, 2006). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


158 Chapter 5 States of Consciousness 


yAlert Some scholars suggest that the quest to alter consciousness represents a basic 
human desire (Siegel, 1989). Whether or not we accept such an extreme view, varia- 
tions in states of consciousness clearly share some basic characteristics across a vari- 
ety of cultures. One is an alteration in thinking, which may become shallow, illogical, 
or otherwise different from normal. In addition, people’s sense of time can become 
disturbed, and their perceptions of the physical world and of themselves may change. 
They may lose self-control, doing things that they would never otherwise do. Finally, 
they may feel a sense of ineffability—the inability to understand an experience ratio- 
nally or describe it in words (Finkler, 2004; Martindale, 1981; Travis, 2006). 

Of course, realizing that efforts to produce altered states of consciousness are 
widespread throughout the world’s societies does not answer a fundamental question: 
Is the experience of unaltered states of consciousness similar across different cultures? 

Because humans share basic biological commonalties in the ways their brains and 
bodies are wired, we might assume that the fundamental experience of consciousness 
is similar across cultures. As a result, we could suppose that consciousness shows some 
basic similarities across cultures. However, the ways in which certain aspects of con- 
sciousness are interpreted and viewed show substantial differences from culture to cul- 
ture. For example, people in disparate cultures view the experience of the passage of 
time in varying ways. For instance, Arabs appear to perceive the passage of time more 
slowly than North Americans (Alon & Brett, 2007; Haynes, Nixon, & West, 2007). 


Remember that although 
there are alternate tech- 
niques used in meditation, 
they are all designed to 
bring about an altered state 
of consciousness in which attention 
is refocused. 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 





RECAP from the Golden Gate Bridge!” Could such a thing have 
: ; 3 A 2 happened? Why or why not? 
What is hypnosis, and are hypnotized people in a different 3. isa leamed technique for reiocusing aiieniion 
> S 
Bae E oe F ee to bring about an altered state of consciousness. 
e Hypnosis produces a state of heightened susceptibility A, Tag o coum, lanes 


to the suggestions of the hypnotist. Under hypnosis, 
significant behavioral changes occur, including in- 
creased concentration and suggestibility, heightened 
ability to recall and construct images, lack of initiative, RETHINK 
and acceptance of suggestions that clearly contradict 
reality. (p. 154) 


when she engages in meditation. 


1. Why do you think people in almost every culture seek 
ways of altering their states of consciousness? 


What are the effects of meditation? 2. From the perspective of a human resources specialist: Would 
e Meditation is a learned technique for refocusing atten- you allow (or even encourage) employees to engage in 
tion that brings about an altered state of consciousness. meditation during the workday? Why or why not? 
(p. 156) 
e Different cultures have developed their own unique Answers to Evaluate Questions 
ways to alter states of consciousness. (p. 157) enuen ‘p UoYeppIN ‘E ‘spe eayONAsep-jjas wozIəd 


0} əƏpeu aq youues pezyouddy are oym əjdoəd ‘ou ‘z ‘srsoudÁH ‘T 


EVALUATE 


1, _______isa state of heightened susceptibility to the 
suggestions of others. 

2. A friend tells you, “I once heard of a person who was 
murdered by being hypnotized and then told to jump 


KEY TERMS 


hypnosis p. 154 
meditation p. 156 


~~ www.urdukutabkhanapk. blogspot.com 
\ 


`Y 


N 
N 


John Brodhead’s bio reads like a script for an episode of VH1’s Behind the Music. 

A young rebel from the New Jersey suburbs falls in with a fast crowd, gets hooked on 
parties and booze and, with intensive counseling and a bit of tough love, manages to 
get his life back together. What makes his story different? Just one thing: his age. John 
is 13. (Rogers, 2002) 


John Brodhead was lucky. Now in recovery, John had begun to drink when he was 
in the sixth grade. He is not alone: The number of kids who start drinking by the 
eighth grade has increased by almost a third since the 1970s, even though alcohol 
consumption overall has stayed fairly steady among the general population. 

Drugs of one sort or another are a part of almost everyone’s life. From infancy 
on, most people take vitamins, aspirin, cold-relief medicine, and the like, and surveys 
find that 80% of adults in the United States have taken an over-the-counter pain 
reliever in the last six months. However, these drugs rarely produce an altered state 
of consciousness (Dortch, 1996). 

In contrast, some substances, known as psychoactive drugs, lead to an altered 
state of consciousness. Psychoactive drugs influence a person’s emotions, percep- 
tions, and behavior. Yet even this category of drugs is common in most of our lives. 
If you have ever had a cup of coffee or sipped a beer, you have taken a psychoactive 
drug. A large number of individuals have used more potent—and more dangerous— 
psychoactive drugs than coffee and beer (see Figure 1 on page 160); for instance, 
surveys find that 41% of high school seniors have used an illegal drug in the last 
year. In addition, 30% report having been drunk on alcohol. The figures for the adult 
population are even higher (Johnston et al., 2009). 

Of course, drugs vary widely in the effects they have on users, in part because 
they affect the nervous system in very different ways. Some drugs alter the limbic 
system, and others affect the operation of specific neurotransmitters across the syn- 
apses of neurons. For example, some drugs block or enhance the release of neu- 
rotransmitters, others block the receipt or the removal of a neurotransmitter, and still 
others mimic the effects of a particular neurotransmitter (see Figure 2 on page 160). 

Addictive drugs produce a physiological or psychological dependence (or both) 
in the user, and withdrawal from them leads to a craving for the drug that, in some 
cases, may be nearly irresistible. In physiological dependence, the body becomes so 
accustomed to functioning in the presence of a drug that it cannot function without 
it. In psychological dependence, people believe that they need the drug to respond to 
the stresses of daily living. Although we generally associate addiction with drugs 
such as heroin, everyday sorts of drugs, such as caffeine (found in coffee) and nic- 
otine (found in cigarettes), have addictive aspects as well (Li, Volkow, & Balu, 2007). 

We know surprisingly little about the underlying causes of addiction. One of the 
problems in identifying those causes is that different drugs (such as alcohol and 
cocaine) affect the brain in very different ways—yet they may be equally addicting. 
Furthermore, it takes longer to become addicted to some drugs than to others, even 
though the ultimate consequences of addiction may be equally grave (Crombag & 
Robinson, 2004; Nestler & Malenka, 2004; Smart, 2007). 

Why do people take drugs in the first place? There are many reasons, ranging 
from the perceived pleasure of the experience itself, to the escape that a drug-induced 


What are the major classifica- 
tions of drugs, and what are 
their effects? 





John Brodhead began to drink heavily 
when he was in the sixth grade. 


5 Drugs that 
influence a person’s emotions, 
perceptions, and behavior. 


Drugs that produce 
a biological or psychological depen- 
dence in the user so that withdrawal 
from them leads to a craving for the 
drug that, in some cases, may be 
nearly irresistible. 


159 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


160 Chapter 5 States of Consciousness 


FIGURE 1 How many teenagers use 
drugs? The results of the most recent 
comprehensive survey of 14,000 high 
school seniors across the United States 
show the percentage of respondents 
who have used various substances for 
nonmedical purposes at least once. Can 
you think of any reasons why teenagers— 
as opposed to older people—might be 
particularly likely to use drugs? (Source: 
Johnston et al., 2009.) 


FIGURE 2 Different drugs affect 
different parts of the nervous system 
and brain and each drug functions in 
one of these specific ways. 


lert 


Use Figure 2 to learn the dif- 

ferent ways that drugs pro- 
duce their effects on a 
neurological level. 






Alcohol 


Marijuana 


Cigarettes 


Amphetamines 






Tranquilizers 
Hallucinogens 
Cocaine 


Ecstacy i 
0 


Steroids 








5 10 15 20 25 30 35 40 45 50 


Percent 


high affords from the everyday pressures of life, to an attempt to achieve a religious 
or spiritual state. However, other factors having little to do with the nature of the 
experience itself, also lead people to try drugs (McDowell & Spitz, 1999). 

For instance, the highly publicized drug use of role models such as movie stars 
and professional athletes, the easy availability of some illegal drugs, and peer pres- 
sure all play a role in the decision to use drugs. In some cases, the motive is simply 
the thrill of trying something new. Finally, genetic factors may predispose some 
people to be more susceptible to drugs and to become addicted to them. Regardless 
of the forces that lead a person to begin using drugs, drug addiction is among the 





Blocks release of 
neurotransmitter Blocks receptor for 
neurotransmitter 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 16 Drug Use: The Highs and Lows of Consciousness 161 


most difficult of all behaviors to modify, even with extensive treatment (Lemonick, 
2000; Mosher & Akins, 2007; Ray & Hutchison, 2007). 

Because of the difficulty in treating drug problems, there is little disagreement 
that the best hope for dealing with the overall societal problem of substance abuse 
is to prevent people from becoming involved with drugs in the first place. However, 
there is little accord on how to accomplish this goal. 

Even drug reduction programs widely publicized for their effectiveness—such 
as DARE (Drug Abuse Resistance Education)—are of questionable effectiveness. 
Used in more than 80% of school districts in the United States, DARE consists of a 
series of 17 lessons on the dangers of drugs, alcohol, and gangs taught to fifth- and 
sixth-graders by a police officer. The program is highly popular with school officials, 
parents, and politicians. The problem? Repeated careful evaluations have been unable 
to demonstrate that the DARE program is effective in reducing drug use over the 
long term. In fact, one study even showed that DARE graduates were more likely to 
use marijuana than was a comparison group of nongraduates (Des Jarlais et al., 2006; 
Kalb, 2001b; Lucas, 2008; West & O’Neal, 2004). 


Stimulants: Drug Highs 


It’s 1:00 a.m., and you still haven’t finished reading the last chapter of the text on which 
you will be tested later in the morning. Feeling exhausted, you turn to the one thing 
that may help you stay awake for the next two hours: a cup of strong black coffee. 

If you have ever found yourself in such a position, you have resorted to a major 
stimulant, caffeine, to stay awake. Caffeine is one of a number of stimulants, drugs 
whose effect on the central nervous system causes a rise in heart rate, blood pressure, 
and muscular tension. Caffeine is present not only in coffee; it is an important ingre- 
dient in tea, soft drinks, and chocolate as well (see Figure 3). 

Caffeine produces several reactions. The major behavioral effects are an increase 
in attentiveness and a decrease in reaction time. Caffeine can also bring about an 
improvement in mood, most likely by mimicking the effects of a natural brain chem- 
ical, adenosine. Too much caffeine, however, can result in nervousness and insom- 
nia. People can build up a biological dependence on the drug. Regular users who 





Decaffeinated coffee I 
Percolated coffee 
Drip-brewed coffee 
Instant coffee 
Brewed tea 

Instant tea 


Cocoa B 


Many soft drinks 





Red Bull (8.2 oz) 


Weight-loss drugs, diuretics 
and stimulants 


Pain relievers 


Cold/allergy remedies 








0 25 50 75 100 125 150 175 200 225 
Milligrams 


stimulants Drugs that have an arousal 
effect on the central nervous system, 
causing a rise in heart rate, blood 
pressure, and muscular tension. 


FIGURE 3 How much caffeine do 
you consume? This chart shows the 
range of caffeine found in common 
foods and drinks. The average coffee 
drinker in the United States consumes 
about 200 milligrams of caffeine each 
day, or around three cups of coffee. 
(Blakeslee, 1991.) 


162 Chapter 5 States of Consciousness 
Drugs Street Name 
Stimulants 
Amphetamines 

Benzedrine Speed 
Dexedrine Speed 
Cocaine Coke, blow, snow, 
lady, crack 
Depressants 
Alcohol Booze 
Barbiturates 
Nembutal Yellowjackets, 
Seconal yellows 
Phenobarbital Reds 
Rohypnol Roofies, rope, 
“date-rape drug” 
Narcotics 
Heroin H, hombre, junk, 
smack, dope, crap, 
horse 
Morphine Drugstore dope, 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


cube, first line, mud 


Effects 


Increased confidence, 
mood elevation, sense of 
energy and alertness, 
decreased appetite, 
anxiety, irritability, 
insomnia, transient 
drowsiness, delayed 
orgasm 


Anxiety reduction, 
impulsiveness, 
dramatic mood 
swings, bizarre 
thoughts, suicidal 
behavior, slurred 
speech, disorientation, 
slowed mental and 
physical functioning, 
limited attention 
span 

Muscle relaxation, 
amnesia, sleep 


Anxiety and pain 
reduction, apathy, 
difficulty in concentration, 
slowed speech, decreased 
physical activity, drooling, 
itching, euphoria, nausea 


Withdrawal Symptoms 


Apathy, general fatigue, 
prolonged sleep, 
depression, disorientation, 
suicidal thoughts, agitated 
motor activity, irritability, 
bizarre dreams 


Weakness, restlessness, 
nausea and vomiting, 
headaches, nightmares, 
irritability, depression, 
acute anxiety, 
hallucinations, seizures, 
possible death 


Seizures 


Anxiety, vomiting, 
sneezing, diarrhea, lower 
back pain, watery eyes, 
runny nose, yawning, 
irritability, tremors, panic, 
chills and sweating, 
cramps 


Adverse/Overdose 
Reactions 


Elevated blood pressure, 
increase in body 
temperature, face picking, 
suspiciousness, bizarre 
and repetitious behavior, 
vivid hallucinations, 
convulsions, possible 
death 


Confusion, decreased 
response to pain, shallow 
respiration, dilated pupils, 
weak and rapid pulse, 
coma, possible death 


Seizures, coma, 
incapacitation, inability to 
resist sexual assault 


Depressed levels of 
consciousness, low blood 
pressure, rapid heart rate, 
shallow breathing, 
convulsions, coma, 
possible death 


FIGURE 4 Drugs and their effects. A comprehensive breakdown of effects of the most commonly used drugs. 


idyAlert 


Figure 4, which summarizes 
the different categories of 
drugs (stimulants, 
depressants, narcotics, 
and hallucinogens), will 
help you learn the effects of 
particular drugs. 


suddenly stop drinking coffee may experience headaches or depression. Many peo- 
ple who drink large amounts of coffee on weekdays have headaches on weekends 
because of the sudden drop in the amount of caffeine they are consuming (Clayton 
& Lundberg-Love, 2009; Hammond & Gold, 2008; Kendler, Myers, & Gardner, 2006). 

Nicotine, found in cigarettes, is another common stimulant. The soothing effects 
of nicotine help explain why cigarette smoking is addictive. Smokers develop a 
dependence on nicotine, and those who suddenly stop smoking develop a strong 
craving for the drug. This is not surprising: Nicotine activates neural mechanisms 
similar to those activated by cocaine, which, as we see next, is also highly addictive 
(Haberstick et al., 2007; Ray et al., 2008). 


AMPHETAMINES 


Amphetamines such as dexedrine and benzedrine, popularly known as speed, are 
strong stimulants. In small quantities, amphetamines—which stimulate the central 
nervous system—bring about a sense of energy and alertness, talkativeness, height- 
ened confidence, and a mood “high.” They increase concentration and reduce fatigue. 
Amphetamines also cause a loss of appetite, increased anxiety, and irritability. When 


Drugs 
Oxycodone 


Hallucinogens 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Street Name 
Oxy, OC, Percs 


Module 16 Drug Use: The Highs and Lows of Consciousness 163 


Effects 


Pain reduction, shallow 
breathing, slow heartbeat, 
seizure (convulsions); 
cold, clammy skin; 
confusion 


Withdrawal Symptoms 


Sweating, chills, abdominal 
cramps, insomnia, 
vomiting, diarrhea 


Cannabis Bhang, kif, ganja, Euphoria, relaxed Hyperactivity, insomnia, 
Marijuana dope, grass, pot, inhibitions, increased decreased appetite, anxiety 
Hashish hemp, joint, weed, appetite, disoriented 
Hash oil bone, Mary Jane, behavior 

reefer 
MDMA Ecstasy Heightened sense of Depression, anxiety, 
oneself and insight, sleeplessness 
feelings of peace, 
empathy, energy 
LSD Acid, quasey, Heightened aesthetic Not reported 
microdot, white responses; vision and 
lightning depth distortion; 
heightened sensitivity to 
faces and gestures; 
magnified feelings; 
paranoia, panic, euphoria 
Steroids 


Rhoids, juice 


Aggression, depression, 
acne, mood swings, 
masculine traits in women 
and feminine traits in men 


Symptoms can mimic other 
medical problems and 
include weakness, fatigue, 
decreased appetite, weight 
loss; women may note 
menstrual changes 


taken over long periods of time, amphetamines can cause feelings of being perse- 
cuted by others, as well as a general sense of suspiciousness. People taking amphet- 
amines may lose interest in sex. If taken in too large a quantity, amphetamines 
overstimulate the central nervous system to such an extent that convulsions and 
death can occur (Carhart-Harris, 2007). 

Methamphetamine is a white, crystalline drug that U.S. police now say is the most 
dangerous street drug. “Meth” is highly addictive and relatively cheap, and it pro- 
duces a strong, lingering high. It has made addicts of people across the social spec- 
trum, ranging from soccer moms to urban professionals to poverty-stricken inner-city 
residents. After becoming addicted, users take it more and more frequently and in 
increasing doses. Long-term use of the drug can lead to brain damage (Halkitis, 2009; 
Kish et al., 2009; Sharma, Sjoquist, & Ali, 2007). 

More than 1.5 million people in the United States are regular methamphetamine 
users. Because it can be made from nonprescription cold pills, retailers such as Wal- 
Mart and Target have removed these medications from their shelves. Illicit labs 
devoted to the manufacture of methamphetamine have sprung up in many locations 
around the United States (Jefferson, 2005). Figure 4 provides a summary of the effects 
of amphetamines and other illegal drugs. 


Adverse/Overdose 
Reactions 


Extreme drowsiness, muscle 
weakness, confusion, cold 
and clammy skin, pinpoint 
pupils, shallow breathing, 
slow heart rate, fainting, 

or coma 


Severe reactions rare but 
include panic, paranoia, 
fatigue, bizarre and 
dangerous behavior, 
decreased testosterone 
over long-term; immune- 
system effects 

Increase in body 
temperature, memory 
difficulties 


Nausea and chills; 
increased pulse, 
temperature, and blood 
pressure; slow, deep 
breathing; loss of appetite; 
insomnia; bizarre, 
dangerous behavior 


Long-term, high-dose 
effects of steroid use are 
largely unknown, but can 
lead to swelling and 
weight gain 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


164 Chapter 5 States of Consciousness 


depressants Drugs that slow down 
the nervous system. 


COCAINE 


Although its use has declined over the last decade, the stimulant cocaine and its 
derivative, crack, still represent a serious concern. Cocaine is inhaled or “snorted” 
through the nose, smoked, or injected directly into the bloodstream. It is rapidly 
absorbed into the body and takes effect almost immediately. 

When used in relatively small quantities, cocaine produces feelings of profound 
psychological well-being, increased confidence, and alertness. Cocaine produces this 
“high” through the neurotransmitter dopamine. Dopamine is one of the chemicals 
that transmit between neurons messages that are related to ordinary feelings of 
pleasure. Normally when dopamine is released, excess amounts of the neurotrans- 
mitter are reabsorbed by the releasing neuron. However, when cocaine enters the 
brain, it blocks reabsorption of leftover dopamine. As a result, the brain is flooded 
with dopamine-produced pleasurable sensations (Jarlais, Arasteh, & Perlis, 2007; 
Redish, 2004). 

However, there is a steep price to be paid for the pleasurable effects of cocaine. The 
brain may become permanently rewired, triggering a psychological and physical addic- 
tion in which users grow obsessed with obtaining the drug. Over time, users deteriorate 
mentally and physically. In extreme cases, cocaine can cause hallucinations—a common 
one is of insects crawling over one’s body. Ultimately, an overdose of cocaine can lead 
to death (George & Moselhy, 2005; Little et al., 2009; Paulozzi, 2006). 

Almost 2.5 million people in the United States are occasional cocaine users, and 
as many as 1.8 million people use the drug regularly. Given the strength of cocaine, 
withdrawal from the drug is difficult. Although the use of cocaine among high school 
students has declined in recent years, the drug still represents a major problem (John- 
ston et al., 2009). 





Depressants: Drug Lows 


In contrast to the initial effect of stimulants, which is an increase in arousal of the 
central nervous system, the effect of depressants is to impede the nervous system 
by causing neurons to fire more slowly. Small doses result in at least temporary feel- 
ings of intoxication—drunkenness—along with a sense of euphoria and joy. When 
large amounts are taken, however, speech becomes slurred and muscle control 
becomes disjointed, making motion difficult. Ultimately, heavy users may lose con- 
sciousness entirely. 


ALCOHOL 


The most common depressant is alcohol, which is used by more people than is any 
other drug. Based on liquor sales, the average person over the age of 14 drinks 2% 
gallons of pure alcohol over the course of a year. This works out to more than 200 
drinks per person. Although alcohol consumption has declined steadily over the 
last decade, surveys show that more than three-fourths of college students indicate 
that they have had a drink within the last 30 days (Jung, 2002; Midanik, Tam, & 
Weisner, 2007). 

One of the more disturbing trends is the high frequency of binge drinking among 
college students. For men, binge drinking is defined as having five or more drinks in 
one sitting; for women, who generally weigh less than men and whose bodies absorb 
alcohol less efficiently, binge drinking is defined as having four or more drinks at 
one sitting (Mokdad, Brewer, & Naimi, 2007). 

Around 50% of male college students and 40% of female college students say they 
engaged in binge drinking at least once within the previous two weeks (see Figure 5). 
Some 17% of female students and 31% of male students admitted drinking on 10 or 
more occasions during the previous 30 days. Furthermore, even light drinkers were 
affected by the high rate of alcohol use: Two-thirds of lighter drinkers said that they 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 16 Drug Use: The Highs and Lows of Consciousness 165 


Men Women 





had had their studying or sleep disturbed by drunk students, and a quarter of the 
women said they had been the target of an unwanted sexual advance by a drunk class- 
mate (Grucza, Norberg, & Beirut, 2009; Read et al., 2008; Wechsler et al., 2000, 2002). 

Women are typically somewhat lighter drinkers than men—although the gap 
between the sexes is narrowing for older women and has closed completely for 
teenagers. Women are more susceptible to the effects of alcohol, and alcohol abuse 
may harm the brains of women more than men (Mancinelli, Binetti, & Ceccanti, 2007; 
Mann et al., 2005; Wuethrich, 2001). 

There are also cultural and ethnic differences in alcohol consumption. For exam- 
ple, teenagers in Europe drink more than teenagers in the United States do. Further- 
more, people of East Asian backgrounds who live in the United States tend to drink 
significantly less than do Caucasians and African Americans, and their incidence of 
alcohol-related problems is lower. It may be that physical reactions to drinking, 
which may include sweating, a quickened heartbeat, and flushing, are more unpleas- 
ant for East Asians than for other groups (Garcia-Andrade, Wall, & Ehlers, 1997; 
Garlow, Purselle, & Heninger, 2007; Kantrowitz & Underwood, 2007). 

Although alcohol is a depressant, most people claim that it increases their sense 
of sociability and well-being. The discrepancy between the actual and the perceived 
effects of alcohol lies in the initial effects it produces in the majority of individuals 
who use it: release of tension and stress, feelings of happiness, and loss of inhibitions 
(Sayette, 1993; Steele & Josephs, 1990). 





Although most alcohol consumers are casual users, there are more than 14 million alcoholics 
in the United States. The effects of alcohol vary significantly, depending on who is drinking it 
and the setting in which people drink. If alcohol were a newly discovered drug, do you think 
its sale would be legal? 


FIGURE 5 Drinking habits of college 
students (Wechsler et al., 2003). For men, 
binge drinking was defined as consuming 
five or more drinks in one sitting; for 
women, the total was four or more. 


166 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 5 States of Consciousness 


Number of drinks Alcohol Typical effects 
consumed in two hours in blood 
(percentage) 


0.05 Judgment, thought, and restraint 
weakened; tension released, giving 
carefree sensation 


Tensions and inhibitions of everyday life 
lessened; cheerfulness 


Voluntary motor action affected, making 
hand and arm movements, walk, and 
speech clumsy 


Severe impairment—staggering, loud, 
incoherent, emotionally unstable, 100 
times greater traffic risk; exuberance and 
aggressive inclinations magnified 


Deeper areas of brain affected, with 
stimulus-response and understanding 
confused; stuporous; blurred vision 


Incapable of voluntary action; sleepy, 
difficult to arouse; equivalent of surgical 
anesthesia 
Se Sees 

iS 


OOTO 


Comatose; centers controlling breathing 
and heartbeat anesthetized; death 

S increasingly probable 
caaan a ae | 25 


Hele 
Goose 





Note: A drink refers to a typical |2-ounce bottle of beer, 
a 1.5-ounce shot of hard liquor, or a 5-ounce glass of wine. 


FIGURE 6 The effects of alcohol. The quantities represent only rough benchmarks; the effects 
vary significantly depending on an individual's weight, height, recent food intake, genetic 
factors, and even psychological state. 


As the dose of alcohol increases, however, the depressive effects become more 
pronounced (see Figure 6). People may feel emotionally and physically unstable. 
They also show poor judgment and may act aggressively. Moreover, memory is 
impaired, brain processing of spatial information is diminished, and speech becomes 
slurred and incoherent. Eventually they may fall into a stupor and pass out. If they 
drink enough alcohol in a short time, they may die of alcohol poisoning (Murphy et 
al., 1998; Thatcher & Clark, 2006; Zeigler et al., 2005). 

Although most people fall into the category of casual users, 14 million people in 
the United States—1 in every 13 adults—have a drinking problem. Alcoholics, people 
with alcohol-abuse problems, come to rely on alcohol and continue to drink even 
though it causes serious difficulties. In addition, they become increasingly immune 
to the effects of alcohol. Consequently, alcoholics must drink progressively more to 
experience the initial positive feelings that alcohol produces. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 16 Drug Use: The Highs and Lows of Consciousness 167 


In some cases of alcoholism, people must drink constantly in order to feel well 
enough to function in their daily lives. In other cases, though, people drink inconsis- 
tently but occasionally go on binges in which they consume large quantities of alcohol. 

It is not clear why certain people become alcoholics and develop a tolerance for 
alcohol, whereas others do not. There may be a genetic cause, although the question 
of whether there is a specific inherited gene that produces alcoholism is controversial. 
What is clear is that the chances of becoming an alcoholic are considerably higher if 
alcoholics are present in earlier generations of a person’s family. However, not all 
alcoholics have close relatives who are alcoholics. In these cases, environmental 
stressors are suspected of playing a larger role (Nurnberger & Bierut, 2007; Whitfield 
et al., 2004; Zimmermann, Blomeyer, & Laucht, 2007). 


BARBITURATES 


Barbiturates, which include drugs such as Nembutal, Seconal, and phenobarbital, are 
another form of depressant. Frequently prescribed by physicians to induce sleep or 
reduce stress, barbiturates produce a sense of relaxation. Yet they, too, are psycho- 
logically and physically addictive and, when combined with alcohol, can be deadly, 
since such a combination relaxes the muscles of the diaphragm to such an extent that 
the user stops breathing. 


ROHYPNOL 


Rohypnol is sometimes called the “date rape drug,” because, when it is mixed with 
alcohol, it can prevent victims from resisting sexual assault. Sometimes people who 
are unknowingly given the drug are so incapacitated that they have no memory of 
the assault. 


Narcotics: Relieving Pain 
and Anxiety 


Narcotics are drugs that increase relaxation and relieve pain and anxiety. Two of the 
most powerful narcotics, morphine and heroin, are derived from the poppy seed pod. 
Although morphine is used medically to control severe pain, heroin is illegal in the 
United States. This status has not prevented its widespread use. 

Heroin users usually inject the drug directly into their veins with a hypodermic 
needle. The immediate effect has been described as a “rush” of positive feeling, similar 
in some respects to a sexual orgasm—and just as difficult to describe. After the rush, 
a heroin user experiences a sense of well-being and peacefulness that lasts three to five 
hours. When the effects of the drug wear off, however, the user feels extreme anxiety 
and a desperate desire to repeat the experience. Moreover, larger amounts of heroin 
are needed each time to produce the same pleasurable effect. These last two properties 
are all the ingredients necessary for physiological and psychological dependence: The 
user is constantly either shooting up or attempting to obtain ever-increasing amounts 
of the drug. Eventually, the life of the addict revolves around heroin. 

Because of the powerful positive feelings the drug produces, heroin addiction 
is particularly difficult to cure. One treatment that has shown some success is the 
use of methadone. Methadone is a synthetic chemical that satisfies a heroin user’s 
physiological cravings for the drug without providing the “high” that accompanies 
heroin. When heroin users are placed on regular doses of methadone, they may be 
able to function relatively normally. The use of methadone has one substantial draw- 
back, however: Although it removes the psychological dependence on heroin, it 
replaces the physiological dependence on heroin with a physiological dependence 
on methadone. Researchers are attempting to identify nonaddictive chemical 








Even legal drugs, when used improperly, 
lead to addiction. 


narcotics Drugs that increase relax- 
ation and relieve pain and anxiety. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


168 Chapter 5 States of Consciousness 

90 substitutes for heroin as well as substitutes for other addictive drugs that do 
80 | not replace one addiction with another (Amato et al., 2005; Joe, Flynn, & 
70 Broome, 2007; Oviedo-Joekes et al., 2009; Verdejo, Toribio, & Orozco, 2005). 
60 Oxycodone (sold as the prescription drug OxyContin) is a type of pain 
50 reliever that has led to a significant amount of abuse. Many well-known 
40 people (including Courtney Love and Rush Limbaugh) have become depen- 
30 dent on it. 

20 


10 








HALLUCINOGENS: PSYCHEDELIC DRUGS 





0 
1995 2000 2002 2004 2006 


Year 





= 8th grade == 10th grade == |2th grade 











FIGURE 7 Although the level of 
marijuana use has declined slightly in 
recent years, overall the absolute 
number of teenagers who have used the 
drug in the last year remains relatively 
high. (Source: Johnston et al., 2009.) 


hallucinogen A drug that is capable of 


producing hallucinations, or changes 
in the perceptual process. 


\ 





This drawing, made by someone taking 
LSD, suggests the effects of hallucinogens 
on thinking. 


2008 
What do mushrooms, jimsonweed, and morning glories have in common? 


Besides being fairly common plants, each can be a source of a powerful hal- 
lucinogen, a drug that is capable of producing hallucinations, or changes in 
the perceptual process. 

The most common hallucinogen in widespread use today is marijuana, whose 
active ingredient—tetrahydrocannabinol (THC)—is found in a common weed, can- 
nabis. Marijuana is typically smoked in cigarettes or pipes, although it can be cooked 
and eaten. Just over 32% of high school seniors and 11% of eighth-graders report 
having used marijuana in the last year (Johnston et al., 2009) (see Figure 7). 

The effects of marijuana vary from person to person, but they typically consist of 
feelings of euphoria and general well-being. Sensory experiences seem more vivid and 
intense, and a person’s sense of self-importance seems to grow. Memory may be impaired, 
causing users to feel pleasantly “spaced out.” However, the effects are not universally 
positive. Individuals who use marijuana when they feel depressed can end up even more 
depressed, because the drug tends to magnify both good and bad feelings. 

There are clear risks associated with long-term, heavy marijuana use. Although 
marijuana does not seem to produce addiction by itself, some evidence suggests that 
there are similarities in the way marijuana and drugs such as cocaine and heroin 
affect the brain. Furthermore, there is some evidence that heavy use at least tempo- 
rarily decreases the production of the male sex hormone testosterone, potentially 
affecting sexual activity and sperm count (Iverson, 2000; Lane, Cherek, & Tcheremis- 
sine, 2007; Rossato, Pagano, & Vettor, 2008). 

In addition, marijuana smoked during pregnancy may have lasting effects on 
children who are exposed prenatally, although the results are inconsistent. Heavy use 
also affects the ability of the immune system to fight off germs and increases stress 
on the heart, although it is unclear how strong these effects are. There is one unques- 
tionably negative consequence of smoking marijuana: The smoke damages the lungs 
much the way cigarette smoke does, producing an increased likelihood of developing 
cancer and other lung diseases (Cornelius et al., 1995; Julien, 2001). 

Despite the possible dangers of marijuana use, there is little scientific evidence 
for the popular belief that users “graduate” from marijuana to more dangerous 
drugs. Furthermore, the use of marijuana is routine in certain cultures. For instance, 
some people in Jamaica habitually drink a marijuana-based tea related to religious 
practices. In addition, marijuana has several medical uses; it can be used to prevent 
nausea from chemotherapy, treat some AIDS symptoms, and relieve muscle spasms 
for people with spinal cord injuries. In a controversial move, several states have made 
the use of the drug legal if it is prescribed by a physician—although it remains ille- 
gal under U.S. federal law (Chapkis & Webb, 2008; Cohen, 2009; Iverson, 2000; 
Seamon et al., 2007). 


MDMA (ECSTASY) AND LSD 


MDMA (“Ecstasy”) and lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD, or “acid”) fall into the category 
of hallucinogens. Both drugs affect the operation of the neurotransmitter serotonin 
in the brain, causing an alteration in brain-cell activity and perception (Buchert et 
al., 2004; Cloud, 2000). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 16 Drug Use: The Highs and Lows of Consciousness 


Ecstasy users report a sense of peacefulness and calm. People on the drug report 
experiencing increased empathy and connection with others, as well as feeling more 
relaxed, yet energetic. Although the data are not conclusive, some researchers have 
found declines in memory and performance on intellectual tasks, and such findings 
suggest that there may be long-term changes in serotonin receptors in the brain (El- 
Mallakh & Abraham, 2007; Jones et al., 2008; Montgomery et al., 2005). 

LSD, which is structurally similar to serotonin, produces vivid hallucinations. 
Perceptions of colors, sounds, and shapes are altered so much that even the most 
mundane experience—such as looking at the knots in a wooden table—can seem 
moving and exciting. Time perception is distorted, and objects and people may be 
viewed in a new way, with some users reporting that LSD increases their under- 
standing of the world. For others, however, the experience brought on by LSD can 
be terrifying, particularly if users have had emotional difficulties in the past. Fur- 
thermore, people occasionally experience flashbacks, in which they hallucinate long 
after they initially used the drug (Baruss, 2003; Wu, Schlenger, & Galvin, 2006). 


In a society bombarded with commercials for drugs that are BECOMING AN 


guaranteed to do everything from curing restless leg syndrome INFORMED CONSUMER 


to erectile dysfunction, it is no wonder that drug-related 


problems are a major social issue. Yet many people with drug of Psyc h O | og y 


Identifying Drug and Alcohol Problems 


and alcohol problems deny that they have them, and even close 
friends and family members may fail to realize when occasional 
social use of drugs or alcohol has turned into abuse. 

Certain signs, however, indicate when use becomes abuse 
(National Institute on Drug Abuse, 2000). Among them are the following: 


e Always getting high to have a good time. 

e Being high more often than not. 

e Getting high to get oneself going. 

e Going to work or class while high. 

e Missing or being unprepared for class or work because you were high. 
e Feeling badly later about something you said or did while high. 
e Driving a car while high. 

e Coming in conflict with the law because of drugs. 

e Doing something while high that you wouldn’t do otherwise. 

e Being high in nonsocial, solitary situations. 

e Being unable to stop getting high. 

e Feeling a need for a drink or a drug to get through the day. 

e Becoming physically unhealthy. 

e Failing at school or on the job. 

e Thinking about liquor or drugs all the time. 

e Avoiding family or friends while using liquor or drugs. 


Any combination of these symptoms should be sufficient to alert you to the potential 
of a serious drug problem. Because drug and alcohol dependence are almost impossible 
to cure on one’s own, people who suspect that they have a problem should seek 
immediate attention from a psychologist, physician, or counselor. 

You can also get help from national hotlines. For alcohol difficulties, call the National 
Council on Alcoholism at (800) 622-2255. For drug problems, call the National Institute 
on Drug Abuse at (800) 662-4357. You can also check your telephone book for a local 
listing of Alcoholics Anonymous or Narcotics Anonymous. Finally, check out the 
websites of the National Institute on Alcohol Abuse and Alcoholism (www.niaaa.nih 
.gov) and the National Institute on Drug Abuse (www.nida.nih.gov). 


169 


170 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 5 States of Consciousness 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 


What are the major classifications of drugs, and what are their 
effects? 


Drugs can produce an altered state of consciousness. 
However, they vary in how dangerous they are and in 
whether they are addictive. (p. 159) 

Stimulants cause arousal in the central nervous 

system. Two common stimulants are caffeine and 
nicotine. More dangerous are cocaine and amphet- 
amines, which in large quantities can lead to convul- 
sions and death. (p. 161) 

Depressants decrease arousal in the central nervous sys- 
tem. They can cause intoxication along with feelings of 
euphoria. The most common depressants are alcohol and 
barbiturates. (p. 164) 

Alcohol is the most frequently used depressant. Its initial 
effects of released tension and positive feelings yield to 
depressive effects as the dose of alcohol increases. Both 
heredity and environmental stressors can lead to alcohol- 
ism. (p. 164) 

Morphine and heroin are narcotics, drugs that produce 
relaxation and relieve pain and anxiety. Because of their 
addictive qualities, morphine and heroin are particularly 
dangerous. (p. 167) 

Hallucinogens are drugs that produce hallucinations or 
other changes in perception. The most frequently used 
hallucinogen is marijuana, which has several long-term 
risks. Two other hallucinogens are LSD and Ecstasy. 

(p. 168) 

A number of signals indicate when drug use becomes 
drug abuse. A person who suspects that he or she has a 
drug problem should get professional help. People are 
almost never capable of solving drug problems on their 
own. (p. 169) 


KEY TERMS 


psychoactive drugs p. 159 stimulants p. 161 narcotics p. 167 
addictive drugs p. 159 depressants p. 164 hallucinogen p. 168 


EVALUATE 


1. Drugs that affect a person’s consciousness are referred to 
as i 

2. Match the type of drug to an example of that type. 

. narcotic—a pain reliever 

. amphetamine—a strong stimulant 

. hallucinogen—capable of producing hallucinations 

LSD 

. heroin 

. dexedrine, or speed 

3. Classify each drug listed as a stimulant (S), depressant 
(D), hallucinogen (H), or narcotic (N). 
1. nicotine 

. cocaine 

. alcohol 

. morphine 

. marijuana 

4. The effects of LSD can recur long after the drug has been 
taken. True or false? 

5, _______is a drug that has been used to treat people 
with heroin addiction. 


ea oPp WN 


ar WN 


RETHINK 


1. Why have drug education campaigns largely been ineffec- 
tive in stemming the use of illegal drugs? Should the use 
of certain now-illegal drugs be made legal? Would it be 
more effective to stress reduction of drug use rather than a 
complete prohibition of drug use? 

2. From the perspective of a substance abuse counselor: How 
would you explain why people start using drugs to the 
family members of someone who was addicted? What 
types of drug prevention programs would you advocate? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


auopeyjey *¢ 
fen P ‘H-S ‘N-P ‘A-E ‘S-Z ‘S-L"€ ‘e-e 9-7 ‘G-1 °% f@aOeOYDASd ‘T 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Psychology on the Web 


1. Find a resource on the web that interprets dreams and another that reports the 
results of scientific dream research. Compare the nature and content of the two sites 
in terms of the topics covered, the reliability of information provided, and the 
promises made about the use of the site and its information. Write a summary of 
what you found. 

2. There is considerable debate about the effectiveness of DARE, the Drug Abuse 
Resistance Education program. Find a discussion of both sides of the issue on the 
web, and summarize the arguments on each side. State your own preliminary 
conclusions about the DARE program. 


E D | log U = Our examination of states of consciousness has ranged 


widely. It focuses both on natural factors such as sleep, 
dreaming, and daydreaming and on more intentional modes of altering consciousness, 
including hypnosis, meditation, and drugs. As we consider why people seek to alter their 
consciousness, we need to reflect on the uses and abuses of the various consciousness- 
altering strategies in which people engage. 

Return briefly to the case of alcoholic Annie Fuller. Consider the following questions 
in light of your understanding of addictive drugs: 





1. How might you explain Annie Fuller's drinking problem? 

2. What would be some of the effects on Annie Fuller as she becomes increasingly 
intoxicated during the course of a day? 

3. What might be some of the factors associated with Annie Fuller developing 
alcoholism? 

4. What would be some of the indicators if Annie Fuller were to overdose on alcohol? 


171 








CHAPTER 6 


Learning 














www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Key Concepts for Chapter 6 


MODULE 17 








What is learning? @ How do we learn to form Classical Conditioning 


kw x 5 The Basi f Classical Conditioni 
associations between stimuli and responses? a N 
Applying Conditioning Principles 
to Human Behavior 


Extinction 
Generalization and Discrimination 


Beyond Traditional Classical 
Conditioning: Challenging 
Basic Assumptions 


MODULE 18 






What is the role of reward and punishment in Operant Conditioning 


i ; Thorndike's Law of Effect 
learning? @ What are some practical methods for 





The Basics of Operant Conditioning 


bringing about behavior change, both in Applying Psychology in the 21st Century: 
i Dollars for Scholars: Should Parents 
ourselves and in others? and Children Be Paid for Doing 


the Right Thing? 
PsychWork: Seeing Eye Dog Trainer 


Becoming an Informed Consumer of 
Psychology: Using Behavior Analysis 
and Behavior Modification 


MODULE 19 





What is the role of recognition and thought Cognitive Approaches to Learning 


in training? Latent Learning 
Observational Learning: Learning 
Through Imitation 


Neuroscience in Your Life: Learning 
Through Observation 





Exploring Diversity: Does Culture 
Influence How We Learn? 


173 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Prologue / Was a Blackberry Addict 





Senator Thad Cochran of Mississippi, like almost every politician 
on the Hill, got a BlackBerry after 9/11 for security purposes. But 
he gave it back. “I was always distracted,” Cochran said. “I couldn't 
concentrate. Every time the light came on or it beeped, | felt this 
compulsion to stop everything | was doing.’ Though he doesn't 


begrudge his colleagues for their BlackBerry addiction, Cochran 
says the result is that during meetings on the Hill almost every- 
one is “always checking messages” or typing, he says. “It just 
beeps or buzzes, all the time, and people get up and leave the 
room.’ (Begley, 2009, p. 37) 





Are you like Senator Cochran, reaching for your cellphone or 
BlackBerry the moment it beeps or vibrates? Do you know 
people who stop in mid-sentence when their cellphone rings to 
answer it? 

You might have heard jokes about people being addicted to 
their “crackberries,’ but actually, their behavior isn’t an addiction 
at all: It’s just a very-well learned response. This learning came 
about because of the same processes that allow us to learn to 
read a book, drive a car, play poker, study for a test, or perform 
any of the numerous activities that make up our daily routine. 
Each of us must acquire and then refine our skills and abilities 
through learning. 

Learning is a fundamental topic for psychologists and plays a 
central role in almost every specialty area of psychology. For 
example, a psychologist studying perception might ask, “How do 
we learn that people who look small from a distance are far away 
and not simply tiny?” A developmental psychologist might 
inquire, “How do babies learn to distinguish their mothers from 


174 


other people?” A clinical psychologist might wonder, “Why do 
some people learn to be afraid when they see a spider?” A social 
psychologist might ask, “How do we learn to believe that we've 
fallen in love?” 

Each of these questions, although drawn from very different 
branches of psychology, can be answered only through an 
understanding of basic learning processes. In each case, a skill or 
a behavior is acquired, altered, or refined through experience. 

Psychologists have approached the study of learning from 
several angles. Among the most fundamental are studies of the 
type of learning that is illustrated in responses ranging from a 
dog salivating when it hears its owner opening a can of dog food 
to the emotions we feel when our national anthem is played. 
Other theories consider how learning is a consequence of 
rewarding circumstances. Finally, several other approaches focus 
on the cognitive aspects of learning, or the thought processes 
that underlie learning. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 17 


Does the mere sight of the golden arches in front of McDonald’s make you feel pangs 
of hunger and think about hamburgers? If it does, you are displaying an elementary 
form of learning called classical conditioning. Classical conditioning helps explain such 
diverse phenomena as crying at the sight of a bride walking down the aisle, fearing 
the dark, and falling in love. 

Classical conditioning is one of a number of different types of learning that 
psychologists have identified, but a general definition encompasses them all: Learning 
is a relatively permanent change in behavior that is brought about by experience. 

How do we know when a behavior has been influenced by learning—or even is 
a result of learning? Part of the answer relates to the nature-nurture question, one of 
the fundamental issues underlying the field of psychology. In the acquisition of 
behaviors, experience—which is essential to the definition of learning—is the “nur- 
ture” part of the nature-nurture question. 

However, it’s not always easy to identify whether a change in behavior is due 
to nature or nurture, because some changes in behavior or performance come about 
through maturation alone and don’t involve experience. For instance, children 
become better tennis players as they grow older partly because their strength increases 
with their size—a maturational phenomenon. To understand when learning has 
occurred, we must differentiate maturational changes from improvements resulting 
from practice, which indicate that learning actually has occurred. 

Similarly, short-term changes in behavior that are due to factors other than learn- 
ing, such as declines in performance resulting from fatigue or lack of effort, are dif- 
ferent from performance changes that are due to actual learning. If Serena Williams 
has a bad day on the tennis court because of tension or fatigue, this does not mean 
that she has not learned to play correctly or has “unlearned” how to play well. 
Because there is not always a one-to-one correspondence between learning and per- 
formance, understanding when true learning has occurred is difficult. 

It is clear that we are primed for learning from the beginning of life. Infants 
exhibit a simple type of learning called habituation. Habituation is the decrease in 
response to a stimulus that occurs after repeated presentations of the same stimulus. 
For example, young infants may initially show interest in a novel stimulus, such as 
a brightly colored toy, but they will soon lose interest if they see the same toy over 
and over. (Adults exhibit habituation, too: Newlyweds soon stop noticing that they 
are wearing a wedding ring.) Habituation permits us to ignore things that have 
stopped providing new information. 

Most learning is considerably more complex than habituation, and the study of 
learning has been at the core of the field of psychology. Although philosophers since 
the time of Aristotle have speculated on the foundations of learning, the first system- 
atic research on learning was done at the beginning of the 20th century, when Ivan 
Pavlov (does the name ring a bell?) developed the framework for learning called 
classical conditioning. 


What is learning? 


How do we learn to form 


associations between stimuli 


and responses? 


A relatively permanent 


change in behavior brought about by 


experience. 


175 


176 Chapter 6 Learning 





Ivan Pavlov (center) developed the principles of classical 
conditioning. 


classical conditioning A type of 
learning in which a neutral stimulus 
comes to bring about a response after 
it is paired with a stimulus that 
naturally brings about that response. 


neutral stimulus A stimulus that, 
before conditioning, does not naturally 
bring about the response of interest. 


unconditioned stimulus (UCS) A 
stimulus that naturally brings about a 
particular response without having 
been learned. 


unconditioned response (UCR) A 
response that is natural and needs no 
training (e.g., salivation at the smell of 
food). 


conditioned stimulus (CS) A once- 
neutral stimulus that has been paired 
with an unconditioned stimulus to 
bring about a response formerly 
caused only by the unconditioned 
stimulus. 


conditioned response (CR) A 
response that, after conditioning, 
follows a previously neutral stimulus 
(e.g., salivation at the ringing of a bell). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


The Basics of Classical 
Conditioning 

Ivan Pavlov, a Russian physiologist, never intended to do 
psychological research. In 1904 he won the Nobel Prize for 
his work on digestion, testimony to his contribution to that 
field. Yet Pavlov is remembered not for his physiological 
research but for his experiments on basic learning processes— 
work that he began quite accidentally (Marks, 2004; Samoilov 
& Zayas, 2007). 

Pavlov had been studying the secretion of stomach acids 
and salivation in dogs in response to the ingestion of varying 
amounts and kinds of food. While doing that, he observed a 
curious phenomenon: Sometimes stomach secretions and salivation would begin in 
the dogs when they had not yet eaten any food. The mere sight of the experimenter 
who normally brought the food, or even the sound of the experimenter’s footsteps, 
was enough to produce salivation in the dogs. Pavlov’s genius lay in his ability to 
recognize the implications of this discovery. He saw that the dogs were responding 
not only on the basis of a biological need (hunger) but also as a result of learning— 
or, as it came to be called, classical conditioning. Classical conditioning is a type of 
learning in which a neutral stimulus (such as the experimenter’s footsteps) comes to 
elicit a response after being paired with a stimulus (such as food) that naturally 
brings about that response. 

To demonstrate classical conditioning, Pavlov (1927) attached a tube to the sali- 
vary gland of a dog, allowing him to measure precisely the dog’s salivation. He then 
rang a bell and, just a few seconds later, presented the dog with meat. This pairing 
occurred repeatedly and was carefully planned so that, each time, exactly the same 
amount of time elapsed between the presentation of the bell and the meat. At first 
the dog would salivate only when the meat was presented, but soon it began to 
salivate at the sound of the bell. In fact, even when Pavlov stopped presenting the 
meat, the dog still salivated after hearing the sound. The dog had been classically 
conditioned to salivate to the bell. 

As you can see in Figure 1, the basic processes of classical conditioning that 
underlie Pavlov’s discovery are straightforward, although the terminology he chose 
is not simple. Consider first the diagram in Figure 1a. Before conditioning, there are 
two unrelated stimuli: the ringing of a bell and meat. We know that normally the 
ringing of a bell does not lead to salivation but to some irrelevant response, such as 
pricking up the ears or perhaps a startle reaction. The bell is therefore called the 
neutral stimulus, because it is a stimulus that, before conditioning, does not naturally 
bring about the response in which we are interested. We also have meat, which 
naturally causes a dog to salivate—the response we are interested in conditioning. 
The meat is considered an unconditioned stimulus (UCS) because food placed in a 
dog’s mouth automatically causes salivation to occur. The response that the meat 
elicits (salivation) is called an unconditioned response (UCR)—a natural, innate, 
reflexive response that is not associated with previous learning. Unconditioned 
responses are always brought about by the presence of unconditioned stimuli. 

Figure 1b illustrates what happens during conditioning. The bell is rung just 
before each presentation of the meat. The goal of conditioning is for the dog to asso- 
ciate the bell with the unconditioned stimulus (meat) and therefore to bring about 
the same sort of response as the unconditioned stimulus. After a number of pairings 
of the bell and meat, the bell alone causes the dog to salivate. 

When conditioning is complete, the bell has evolved from a neutral stimulus to 
a conditioned stimulus (CS). At this time, salivation that occurs as a response to the 
conditioned stimulus (bell) is considered a conditioned response (CR). This situation 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 17 Classical Conditioning 


(a) Before conditioning 


Neutral stimulus Response unrelated to meat 








~ Sound of bell 
|e 


Pricking of ears 


KA 
\ 
Unconditioned stimulus (UCS) 


Unconditioned response (UCR) 


LBS n Salivation 


Meat 


(b) During conditioning 





Neutral stimulus Unconditioned response (UCR) 


=~ Sound of bell 


)) 






AA 


Salivation 


Unconditioned stimulus (UCS) 





Meat 


(c) After conditioning 





Conditioned stimulus (CS) Conditioned response (CR) 


j =~ Sound of bell 
(i 






Salivation 


CA ——s- >, 


is depicted in Figure 1c. After conditioning, then, the conditioned stimulus evokes 
the conditioned response. 

The sequence and timing of the presentation of the unconditioned stimulus and 
the conditioned stimulus are particularly important. Like a malfunctioning warning 
light at a railroad crossing that goes on after the train has passed by, a neutral stim- 
ulus that follows an unconditioned stimulus has little chance of becoming a condi- 
tioned stimulus. However, just as a warning light works best if it goes on right before 
a train passes, a neutral stimulus that is presented just before the unconditioned stim- 
ulus is most apt to result in successful conditioning. Research has shown that con- 
ditioning is most effective if the neutral stimulus (which will become a conditioned 
stimulus) precedes the unconditioned stimulus by between a half second and several 
seconds, depending on what kind of response is being conditioned (Bitterman, 2006; 
Wasserman & Miller, 1997). 


177 


FIGURE 1 The basic process of classical 
conditioning. (a) Before conditioning, 
the ringing of a bell does not bring 
about salivation—making the bell a 
neutral stimulus. In contrast, meat 
naturally brings about salivation, making 
the meat an unconditioned stimulus 
and salivation an unconditioned 
response. (b) During conditioning, the 
bell is rung just before the presentation 
of the meat. (c) Eventually, the ringing 
of the bell alone brings about salivation. 
We now can say that conditioning has 
been accomplished: The previously 
neutral stimulus of the bell is now 
considered a conditioned stimulus that 
brings about the conditioned response 
of salivation. 


yAlert 


Figure 1 can help you to learn 
and understand the process 
(and terminology) of clas- 
sical conditioning, which 
can be confusing. 


178 Chapter 6 Learning 





Because of a previous unpleasant 
experience, a person may expect a 

similar occurrence when faced with a 
comparable situation in the future, a 
process known as stimulus generalization. 
Can you think of ways that this process 
occurs in everyday life? 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Although the terminology Pavlov used to describe classical conditioning may 
seem confusing, the following summary can help make the relationships between 
stimuli and responses easier to understand and remember: 


e Conditioned = learned. 

e Unconditioned = not learned. 

e An unconditioned stimulus leads to an unconditioned response. 

e Unconditioned stimulus—unconditioned response pairings are not learned and 
not trained: They are naturally occurring. 

e During conditioning, a previously neutral stimulus is transformed into the 
conditioned stimulus. 

e A conditioned stimulus leads to a conditioned response, and a conditioned 
stimulus—conditioned response pairing is a consequence of learning and 
training. 

e An unconditioned response and a conditioned response are similar (such as 
salivation in Pavlov’s experiment), but the unconditioned response occurs 
naturally, whereas the conditioned response is learned. 


Applying Conditioning 
Principles to Human Behavior 


Although the initial conditioning experiments were carried out with animals, classical 
conditioning principles were soon found to explain many aspects of everyday human 
behavior. Recall, for instance, the earlier illustration of how people may experience 
hunger pangs at the sight of McDonald’s golden arches. The cause of this reaction is 
classical conditioning: The previously neutral arches have become associated with the 
food inside the restaurant (the unconditioned stimulus), causing the arches to become 
a conditioned stimulus that brings about the conditioned response of hunger. 

Emotional responses are especially likely to be learned through classical condi- 
tioning processes. For instance, how do some of us develop fears of mice, spiders, 
and other creatures that are typically harmless? In a now infamous case study, psy- 
chologist John B. Watson and colleague Rosalie Rayner (1920) showed that classical 
conditioning was at the root of such fears by conditioning an 11-month-old infant 
named Albert to be afraid of rats. “Little Albert,” like most infants, initially was 
frightened by loud noises but had no fear of rats. 

In the study, the experimenters sounded a loud noise whenever Little Albert 
touched a white, furry rat. The noise (the unconditioned stimulus) evoked fear (the 
unconditioned response). After just a few pairings of noise and rat, Albert began to 
show fear of the rat by itself, bursting into tears when he saw it. The rat, then, had 
become a CS that brought about the CR, fear. Furthermore, the effects of the condi- 
tioning lingered: five days later, Albert reacted with some degree of fear not only 
when shown a rat, but when shown objects that looked similar to the white, furry 
rat, including a white rabbit, a white seal-skin coat, and even a white Santa Claus 
mask. (By the way, although we don’t know for certain what happened to the unfor- 
tunate Little Albert, it appears he was a sickly child who died at the age of 5. In any 
case, Watson, the experimenter, has been condemned for using ethically questionable 
procedures that could never be conducted today; Beck, Levinson, & Irons, 2009.) 

Learning by means of classical conditioning also occurs during adulthood. For 
example, you may not go to a dentist as often as you should because of previous 
associations of dentists with pain. In more extreme cases, classical conditioning can 
lead to the development of phobias, which are intense, irrational fears that we will 
consider later in the book. For example, an insect phobia might develop in someone 
who is stung by a bee. The insect phobia might be so severe that the person refrains 
from leaving home. Posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD), suffered by some war veterans 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 17 Classical Conditioning 179 


and others who have had traumatic experiences, can also be produced by classical 
conditioning. Even years after their battlefield experiences, veterans may feel a rush 
of fear and anxiety at a stimulus such as a loud noise (Kaštelan et al., 2007; Kozaric- 
Kovacic, & Borovecki, 2005; Roberts, Moore, & Beckham, 2007). 

However, classical conditioning also accounts for pleasant experiences. For 
instance, you may have a particular fondness for the smell of a certain perfume or 
aftershave lotion because thoughts of an early love come rushing back whenever you 
encounter it. Or hearing a certain song can bring back happy or bittersweet emotions 
due to associations that you have developed in the past. Classical conditioning, then, 
explains many of the reactions we have to stimuli in the world around us. 


Extinction 


What do you think would happen if a dog that had become classically conditioned 
to salivate at the ringing of a bell never again received food when the bell was rung? 
The answer lies in one of the basic phenomena of learning: extinction. Extinction 
occurs when a previously conditioned response decreases in frequency and eventu- 
ally disappears. 

To produce extinction, one needs to end the association between conditioned 
stimuli and unconditioned stimuli. For instance, if we had trained a dog to salivate 
(the conditioned response) at the ringing of a bell (the conditioned stimulus), we 
could produce extinction by repeatedly ringing the bell but not providing meat. At 
first the dog would continue to salivate when it heard the bell, but after a few such 
instances, the amount of salivation would probably decline, and the dog would even- 
tually stop responding to the bell altogether. At that point, we could say that the 
response had been extinguished. In sum, extinction occurs when the conditioned 
stimulus is presented repeatedly without the unconditioned stimulus (see Figure 2). 

We should keep in mind that extinction can be a helpful phenomenon. Consider, 
for instance, what it would be like if the fear you experienced while watching the 
shower murder scene in the classic movie Psycho never was extinguished. You might 
well tremble with fright every time you took a shower. 

Once a conditioned response has been extinguished, has it vanished forever? Not 
necessarily. Pavlov discovered this phenomenon when he returned to his dog a few 
days after the conditioned behavior had seemingly been extinguished. If he rang a 
bell, the dog once again salivated—an effect known as spontaneous recovery, or the 





Acquisition (conditioned 


stimulus and unconditioned 
stimulus presented together) 





Extinction (conditioned 


Strong stimulus by itself) 


Spontaneous recovery of 
conditioned response 


Extinction follows 
(conditioned 
stimulus alone) 





Strength of 
conditioned response (CR) 





Weak | (a) Training (b) CS alone (c) Pause (d) Spontaneous recovery 





Time —————> 


extinction A basic phenomenon of 
learning that occurs when a previously 
conditioned response decreases in 
frequency and eventually disappears. 


spontaneous recovery The reemer- 
gence of an extinguished conditioned 
response after a period of rest and with 
no further conditioning. 


FIGURE 2 Acquisition, extinction, and 
spontaneous recovery of a classically 
conditioned response. (a) A conditioned 
response (CR) gradually increases in 
strength during training. (b) However, 
if the conditioned stimulus (CS) is 
presented by itself enough times, the 
conditioned response gradually fades, 
and extinction occurs. (c) After a pause 
(d) in which the conditioned stimulus is 
not presented, spontaneous recovery 
can occur. However, extinction typically 
reoccurs soon after. 


180 Chapter 6 Learning 


yAlert 


Remember that stimulus 
generalization relates to 
stimuli that are similar to 
one another, while 
stimulus discrimination 
relates to stimuli that are different 
from one another. 


stimulus generalization A process in 
which, after a stimulus has been 
conditioned to produce a particular 
response, stimuli that are similar to the 
original stimulus produce the same 
response 


stimulus discrimination The process 
that occurs if two stimuli are suffi- 
ciently distinct from one another that 
one evokes a conditioned response but 
the other does not; the ability to 
differentiate between stimuli. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


reemergence of an extinguished conditioned response after a period of time and with 
no further conditioning. 

Spontaneous recovery helps explain why it is so hard to overcome drug addic- 
tions. For example, cocaine addicts who are thought to be “cured” can experience an 
irresistible impulse to use the drug again if they are subsequently confronted by a 
stimulus with strong connections to the drug, such as a white powder (DiCano & 
Everitt, 2002; Plowright, Simonds, & Butler, 2006; Rodd et al., 2004). 


Generalization 
and Discrimination 


Despite differences in color and shape, to most of us a rose is a rose is a rose. The 
pleasure we experience at the beauty, smell, and grace of the flower is similar for 
different types of roses. Pavlov noticed a similar phenomenon. His dogs often sali- 
vated not only at the ringing of the bell that was used during their original condi- 
tioning but at the sound of a buzzer as well. 

Such behavior is the result of stimulus generalization. Stimulus generalization 
is a process in which, after a stimulus has been conditioned to produce a particular 
response, stimuli that are similar to the original stimulus produce the same response. 
The greater the similarity between two stimuli, the greater the likelihood of stimulus 
generalization. Little Albert, who, as we mentioned earlier, was conditioned to be 
fearful of white rats, grew afraid of other furry white things as well. However, 
according to the principle of stimulus generalization, it is unlikely that he would 
have been afraid of a black dog, because its color would have differentiated it suf- 
ficiently from the original fear-evoking stimulus. 

The conditioned response elicited by the new stimulus is usually not as intense 
as the original conditioned response, although the more similar the new stimulus is 
to the old one, the more similar the new response will be. It is unlikely, then, that 
Little Albert’s fear of the Santa Claus mask was as great as his learned fear of a rat. 
Still, stimulus generalization permits us to know, for example, that we ought to brake 
at all red lights, even if there are minor variations in size, shape, and shade. 

Stimulus discrimination, in contrast, occurs if two stimuli are sufficiently dis- 
tinct from each other that one evokes a conditioned response but the other does not. 
Stimulus discrimination provides the ability to differentiate between stimuli. For 
example, my dog Cleo comes running into the kitchen when she hears the sound of 
the electric can opener, which she has learned is used to open her dog food when 
her dinner is about to be served. She does not bound into the kitchen at the sound 
of the food processor, although it sounds similar. In other words, she discriminates 
between the stimuli of can opener and food processor. Similarly, our ability to dis- 
criminate between the behavior of a growling dog and that of one whose tail is 
wagging can lead to adaptive behavior—avoiding the growling dog and petting the 
friendly one. 





Beyond Traditional Classical 
Conditioning: Challenging 
Basic Assumptions 


Although Pavlov hypothesized that all learning is nothing more than long strings of 
conditioned responses, this notion has not been supported by subsequent research. 
It turns out that classical conditioning provides us with only a partial explanation of 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 17 Classical Conditioning 181 


how people and animals learn; indeed, Pavlov was wrong in some of his basic 
assumptions (Hollis, 1997). 

For example, according to Pavlov, the process of linking stimuli and responses 
occurs in a mechanistic, unthinking way. In contrast to this perspective, learning 
theorists influenced by cognitive psychology have argued that learners actively 
develop an understanding and expectancy about which particular unconditioned 
stimuli are matched with specific conditioned stimuli. A ringing bell, for instance, 
gives a dog something to think about: the impending arrival of food (Kirsch et al., 
2004; Rescorla, 198). 

Traditional explanations of how classical conditioning operates have also been 
challenged by John Garcia, a learning psychologist. He found that some organisms— 
including humans—were biologically prepared to quickly learn to avoid foods that 
smelled or tasted like something that made them sick. For instance, a dog quickly 
learns to avoid rotting food that in the past made it sick. Similarly, if every time you 
ate peanuts you had an upset stomach several hours later, eventually you would 
learn to avoid peanuts. In fact, you might develop a learned taste aversion, so that 
peanuts no longer even taste good to you (Garcia, 1990, 2003). 

The surprising part of Garcia’s discovery was his demonstration that condition- 
ing could occur even when the interval between exposure to the conditioned stimu- 
lus of tainted food and the response of sickness was as long as eight hours. 
Furthermore, the conditioning persisted over very long periods and sometimes 
occurred after just one exposure. 

These findings have had important practical implications. For example, to keep 
crows from stealing eggs, dairy farmers may lace an egg with a chemical and leave 
it in a place where crows will find it. The drug temporarily makes the crows ill, but 
it does not harm them permanently. After exposure to a chemical-laden egg, crows 
no longer finds them appetizing (Baker, Johnson, & Slater, 2007; Cox et al., 2004; 
Green, Henderson, & Collinge, 2003). 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP e Learning is not always permanent. Extinction occurs 
when a previously learned response decreases in fre- 
quency and eventually disappears. (p. 179) 

e Stimulus generalization is the tendency for a conditioned 
response to follow a stimulus that is similar to, but not 


What is learning? 
e Learning is a relatively permanent change in behavior 
resulting from experience. (p. 175) 


How do we learn to form associations between stimuli and the same as, the original conditioned stimulus. The con- 
responses? verse phenomenon, stimulus discrimination, occurs 
e One major form of learning is classical conditioning, when an organism learns to distinguish between stimuli. 
which occurs when a neutral stimulus—one that normally (p. 180) 


brings about no relevant response—is repeatedly paired 
with a stimulus (called an unconditioned stimulus) that 
brings about a natural, untrained response. (p. 176) EVALUATE 

e Conditioning occurs when the neutral stimulus is repeat- 
edly presented just before the unconditioned stimulus. 
After repeated pairings, the neutral stimulus elicits the 
same response that the unconditioned stimulus brings 
about. When this occurs, the neutral stimulus has be- 
come a conditioned stimulus, and the response a condi- 
tioned response. (p. 176) 


1, _____ involves changes brought about by experi- 
ence, whereas maturation describes changes resulting 
from biological development. 

2, _______ is the name of the scientist responsible for dis- 
covering the learning phenomenon known as 
conditioning, whereby an organism learns a response to a 
stimulus to which it normally would not respond. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


182 Chapter 6 Learning 


Refer to the passage below to answer questions 3 through 5: RETHINK 


The last three times little Theresa visited Dr. Lopez for 1. How likely is it that Little Albert, Watson’s experimental 


checkups, he administered a painful preventive immuni- 
zation shot that left her in tears. Today, when her mother 
takes her for another checkup, Theresa begins to sob as 
soon as she comes face to face with Dr. Lopez, even before 
he has had a chance to say hello. 

. The painful shot that Theresa received during each visit 





subject, went through life afraid of Santa Claus? Describe 
what could have happened to prevent his continual dread 
of Santa. 


. From the perspective of an advertising executive: How might 


knowledge of classical conditioning be useful in creating 
an advertising campaign? What, if any, ethical issues arise 
from this use? 


was a(n) that elicited the 

, her tears. 

4. Dr. Lopez is upset because his presence has become a 

for Theresa’s crying. 

5. Fortunately, Dr. Lopez gave Theresa no more shots for 
quite some time. Over that period she gradually stopped 
crying and even came to like him. _____ had. 
occurred. 


KEYTERMS 





Answers to Evaluate Questions 
uomugxy “¢ ‘snmwys pseuoyipuoo ‘p ‘asuodsar pauoytpuooun 
‘snmwys pouoyIpuooun ‘g ‘[eoIsseyD ‘AoTARY ‘Z ‘SuTUTeIT I 





learning p.175 unconditioned response conditioned response stimulus 

classical (UCR) p. 176 (CR) p. 176 generalization p. 180 
conditioning p. 176 conditioned stimulus extinction p. 179 stimulus 

neutral stimulus p. 176 (CS) p. 176 spontaneous discrimination p. 180 


unconditioned stimulus 
(UCS) p. 176 


recovery p. 179 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 18 


Very good .. . What a clever idea . . . Fantastic... I agree .. . Thank you. . . Excellent 
... Super... Right on . . . This is the best paper you've ever written; you get an A... 
You are really getting the hang of it . . . I’m impressed . . . You're getting a raise . . . 
Have a cookie . . . You look great... I love you... 


Few of us mind being the recipient of any of these comments. But what is especially 
noteworthy about them is that each of these simple statements can be used, through 
a process known as operant conditioning, to bring about powerful changes in behav- 
ior and to teach the most complex tasks. Operant conditioning is the basis for many 
of the most important kinds of human, and animal, learning. 

Operant conditioning is learning in which a voluntary response is strengthened 
or weakened, depending on its favorable or unfavorable consequences. When we say 
that a response has been strengthened or weakened, we mean that it has been made 
more or less likely to recur regularly. 

Unlike classical conditioning, in which the original behaviors are the natural, 
biological responses to the presence of a stimulus such as food, water, or pain, oper- 
ant conditioning applies to voluntary responses, which an organism performs delib- 
erately to produce a desirable outcome. The term operant emphasizes this point: The 
organism operates on its environment to produce a desirable result. Operant condi- 
tioning is at work when we learn that toiling industriously can bring about a raise 
or that studying hard results in good grades. 

As with classical conditioning, the basis for understanding operant conditioning 
was laid by work with animals. We turn now to some of that early research, which 
began with a simple inquiry into the behavior of cats. 


Thorndike’s Law of Effect 


If you placed a hungry cat in a cage and then put a small piece of food outside the 
cage, just beyond the cat’s reach, chances are that the cat would eagerly search for a 
way out of the cage. The cat might first claw at the sides or push against an opening. 
Suppose, though, you had rigged things so that the cat could escape by stepping on 
a small paddle that released the latch to the door of the cage (see Figure 1). Eventu- 
ally, as it moved around the cage, the cat would happen to step on the paddle, the 
door would open, and the cat would eat the food. 

What would happen if you then returned the cat to the box? The next time, it 
would probably take a little less time for the cat to step on the paddle and escape. 
After a few trials, the cat would deliberately step on the paddle as soon as it was 
placed in the cage. What would have occurred, according to Edward L. Thorndike 
(1932), who studied this situation extensively, was that the cat would have learned 
that pressing the paddle was associated with the desirable consequence of getting 
food. Thorndike summarized that relationship by formulating the law of effect: 
Responses that lead to satisfying consequences are more likely to be repeated. 

Thorndike believed that the law of effect operates as automatically as leaves fall 
off a tree in autumn. It was not necessary for an organism to understand that there 





What is the role of reward and 


punishment in learning? 


What are some practical 


methods for bringing about 


behavior change, both in 
ourselves and in others? 


Learning in 
which a voluntary response is 


strengthened or weakened, depending 


on its favorable or unfavorable 
consequences. 


183 


184 Chapter 6 Learning 


FIGURE 1 Edward L. Thorndike devised 
this puzzle box to study the process by 
which a cat learns to press a paddle to 
escape from the box and receive food. 
Do you think Thorndike’s work has 
relevance to the question of why 
humans voluntarily solve puzzles, such 
as crossword puzzles and jigsaw 
puzzles? Do they receive any rewards? 


reinforcement The process by which a 
stimulus increases the probability that 
a preceding behavior will be repeated. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





was a link between a response and a reward. Instead, Thorndike believed, over time 
and through experience the organism would make a direct connection between the 
stimulus and the response without any awareness that the connection existed. 


The Basics of Operant 
Conditioning 

Thorndike’s early research served as the foundation for the work of one of the 20th 
century's most influential psychologists, B. F. Skinner (1904-1990).You may have 
heard of the Skinner box (shown in Figure 2), a chamber with a highly controlled 
environment that was used to study operant conditioning processes with laboratory 
animals. Whereas Thorndike’s goal was to get his cats to learn to obtain food by 
leaving the box, animals in a Skinner box learn to obtain food by operating on their 
environment within the box. Skinner became interested in specifying how behavior 
varies as a result of alterations in the environment. 

Skinner, whose work went far beyond perfecting Thorndike’s earlier apparatus, 
is considered the inspiration for a whole generation of psychologists studying oper- 
ant conditioning. To illustrate Skinner’s contribution, let’s consider what happens to 
a rat in the typical Skinner box (Keehn, 1996; Pascual & Rodriguez, 2006). 

Suppose you want to teach a hungry rat to press a lever that is in its box. At first 
the rat will wander around the box, exploring the environment in a relatively random 
fashion. At some point, however, it will probably press the lever by chance, and when 
it does, it will receive a food pellet. The first time this happens, the rat will not learn 
the connection between pressing a lever and receiving food and will continue to 
explore the box. Sooner or later the rat will press the lever again and receive a pellet, 
and in time the frequency of the pressing response will increase. Eventually, the rat 
will press the lever continually until it satisfies its hunger, thereby demonstrating that 
it has learned that the receipt of food is contingent on pressing the lever. 





REINFORCEMENT: THE CENTRAL CONCEPT 
OF OPERANT CONDITIONING 


Skinner called the process that leads the rat to continue pressing the key “reinforce- 
ment.” Reinforcement is the process by which a stimulus increases the probability 
that a preceding behavior will be repeated. In other words, pressing the lever is more 
likely to occur again because of the stimulus of food. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 18 Operant Conditioning 185 


















































In a situation such as this one, the food is called a reinforcer. A reinforcer is any 
stimulus that increases the probability that a preceding behavior will occur again. 
Hence, food is a reinforcer, because it increases the probability that the behavior of 
pressing (formally referred to as the response of pressing) will take place. 

What kind of stimuli can act as reinforcers? Bonuses, toys, and good grades can 
serve as reinforcers—if they strengthen the probability of the response that occurred 
before their introduction. What makes something a reinforcer depends on individual 
preferences. Although a Hershey bar can act as a reinforcer for one person, an indi- 
vidual who dislikes chocolate may find one dollar more desirable. The only way we 
can know if a stimulus is a reinforcer for a particular organism is to observe whether 
the frequency of a previously occurring behavior increases after the presentation of 
the stimulus. 

Of course, we are not born knowing that one dollar can buy us a candy bar. 
Rather, through experience we learn that money is a valuable commodity because of 
its association with stimuli, such as food and drink, that are naturally reinforcing. 
This fact suggests a distinction between primary reinforcers and secondary reinforc- 
ers. A primary reinforcer satisfies some biological need and works naturally, regardless 
of a person’s previous experience. Food for a hungry person, warmth for a cold 
person, and relief for a person in pain all would be classified as primary reinforcers. 
A secondary reinforcer, in contrast, is a stimulus that becomes reinforcing because of 
its association with a primary reinforcer. For instance, we know that money is valu- 
able, because we have learned that it allows us to obtain other desirable objects, 
including primary reinforcers such as food and shelter. Money thus becomes a sec- 
ondary reinforcer (Moher et al., 2008). 


POSITIVE REINFORCERS, NEGATIVE REINFORCERS, 
AND PUNISHMENT 


In many respects, reinforcers can be thought of in terms of rewards; both a reinforcer 
and a reward increase the probability that a preceding response will occur again. But 
the term reward is limited to positive occurrences, and this is where it differs from a 
reinforcer—for it turns out that reinforcers can be positive or negative. 





FIGURE 2 B. F. Skinner with a Skinner 
box used to study operant conditioning. 
Laboratory rats learn to press the lever 
in order to obtain food, which is 
delivered in the tray. 


reinforcer Any stimulus that in- 
creases the probability that a preceding 
behavior will occur again. 


yAlert 


Remember that primary 
reinforcers satisfy a 
biological need; secondary 
reinforcers are effective 
due to previous association 
with a primary reinforcer. 


186 Chapter 6 Learning 


positive reinforcer A stimulus added 
to the environment that brings about 
an increase in a preceding response. 


negative reinforcer An unpleasant 
stimulus whose removal leads to an 
increase in the probability that a 
preceding response will be repeated in 
the future. 


punishment A stimulus that 
decreases the probability that a 
previous behavior will occur again. 





yAlert 


The differences between 

positive reinforcement, 
negative reinforcement, 
positive punishment, and 

negative punishment are 

tricky, so pay special attention to 

Figure 3 and the definitions in 

the text. 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


A positive reinforcer is a stimulus added to the environment that brings about 
an increase in a preceding response. If food, water, money, or praise is provided after 
a response, it is more likely that that response will occur again in the future. The 
paychecks that workers get at the end of the week, for example, increase the likeli- 
hood that they will return to their jobs the following week. 

In contrast, a negative reinforcer refers to an unpleasant stimulus whose removal 
leads to an increase in the probability that a preceding response will be repeated in 
the future. For example, if you have an itchy rash (an unpleasant stimulus) that is 
relieved when you apply a certain brand of ointment, you are more likely to use that 
ointment the next time you have an itchy rash. Using the ointment, then, is negatively 
reinforcing, because it removes the unpleasant itch. Similarly, if your iPod volume is 
so loud that it hurts your ears when you first turn it on, you are likely to reduce the 
volume level. Lowering the volume is negatively reinforcing, and you are more apt 
to repeat the action in the future when you first turn it on. Negative reinforcement, 
then, teaches the individual that taking an action removes a negative condition that 
exists in the environment. Like positive reinforcers, negative reinforcers increase the 
likelihood that preceding behaviors will be repeated (Magoon & Critchfield, 2008). 

Note that negative reinforcement is not the same as punishment. Punishment 
refers to a stimulus that decreases the probability that a prior behavior will occur 
again. Unlike negative reinforcement, which produces an increase in behavior, punish- 
ment reduces the likelihood of a prior response. If we receive a shock that is meant 
to decrease a certain behavior, then we are receiving punishment, but if we are 
already receiving a shock and do something to stop that shock, the behavior that 
stops the shock is considered to be negatively reinforced. In the first case, the specific 
behavior is apt to decrease because of the punishment; in the second, it is likely to 
increase because of the negative reinforcement. 

There are two types of punishment: positive punishment and negative punish- 
ment, just as there are positive reinforcement and negative reinforcement. (In both 
cases, “positive” means adding something, and “negative” means removing some- 
thing.) Positive punishment weakens a response through the application of an unpleas- 
ant stimulus. For instance, spanking a child for misbehaving or spending ten years 
in jail for committing a crime is positive punishment. In contrast, negative punishment 
consists of the removal of something pleasant. For instance, when a teenager is told 
she is “grounded” and will no longer be able to use the family car because of her 
poor grades, or when an employee is informed that he has been demoted with a cut 
in pay because of a poor job evaluation, negative punishment is being administered. 
Both positive and negative punishment result in a decrease in the likelihood that a 
prior behavior will be repeated. 

The following rules (and the summary in Figure 3) can help you distinguish 
these concepts from one another: 


e Reinforcement increases the frequency of the behavior preceding it; punishment 
decreases the frequency of the behavior preceding it. 

e The application of a positive stimulus brings about an increase in the frequency 
of behavior and is referred to as positive reinforcement; the application of a 
negative stimulus decreases or reduces the frequency of behavior and is called 
punishment. 

e The removal of a negative stimulus that results in an increase in the frequency 
of behavior is negative reinforcement; the removal of a positive stimulus that 
decreases the frequency of behavior is negative punishment. 


THE PROS AND CONS OF PUNISHMENT: 
WHY REINFORCEMENT BEATS PUNISHMENT 


Is punishment an effective way to modify behavior? Punishment often presents the 
quickest route to changing behavior that, if allowed to continue, might be dangerous 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 18 Operant Conditioning 


Intended Result When stimulus is added, the result is ... When stimulus is removed or terminated, the result is ... 
Increase in Positive reinforcement Negative reinforcement 
behavior 


Example: Giving a raise 


(reinforcement) for good performance 


Result: Increase in 


the ointment 
response of good 





performance Result: Increase in response of 
using ointment 
Decrease in Positive punishment Negative punishment 
behavior Example: Yelling at a Example: Restricting teenager's 
(punishment) 


teenager when she 
steals a bracelet 


Result: Decrease in 
frequency of response 
of stealing 


breaking curfew 





FIGURE 3 Types of reinforcement and punishment. 


to an individual. For instance, a parent may not have a second chance to warn a 
child not to run into a busy street, and so punishing the first incidence of this behav- 
ior may prove to be wise. Moreover, the use of punishment to suppress behavior, 
even temporarily, provides an opportunity to reinforce a person for subsequently 
behaving in a more desirable way. 

There are some rare instances in which punishment can be the most humane 
approach to treating certain severe disorders. For example, some children suffer from 
autism, a psychological disorder that can lead them to abuse themselves by tearing 
at their skin or banging their heads against the wall, injuring themselves severely in 
the process. In such cases—and when all other treatments have failed—punishment 
in the form of a quick but intense electric shock has been used to prevent self- 
injurious behavior. Such punishment, however, is used only to keep the child safe 
and to buy time until positive reinforcement procedures can be initiated (Ducharme, 
Sanjuan, & Drain, 2007; Matson & LoVullo, 2008; Toole et al., 2004). 

Punishment has several disadvantages that make its routine questionable. For 
one thing, punishment is frequently ineffective, particularly if it is not delivered 
shortly after the undesired behavior or if the individual is able to leave the setting 
in which the punishment is being given. An employee who is reprimanded by the 
boss may quit; a teenager who loses the use of the family car may borrow a friend’s 
car instead. In such instances, the initial behavior that is being punished may be 
replaced by one that is even less desirable. 

Even worse, physical punishment can convey to the recipient the idea that phys- 
ical aggression is permissible and perhaps even desirable. A father who yells at and 
hits his son for misbehaving teaches the son that aggression is an appropriate, adult 
response. The son soon may copy his father’s behavior by acting aggressively toward 
others. In addition, physical punishment is often administered by people who are 


Example: Applying ointment to 
relieve an itchy rash leads to a 
higher future likelihood of applying 


access to car due to breaking curfew 


Result: Decrease in response of 


187 








www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





in 


Dollars for Scholars: Should 
Parents and Children Be Paid 
for Doing the Right Thing? 


It’s one of the basic rules of parenting: Good 
fathers and mothers are supposed to be in- 
volved in their children’s lives and watch 
out for their best interests. Yet some parents 
don’t do this as much as they should. Per- 
haps these behaviors were never modeled 
by their own parents, or perhaps they are 
overwhelmed by the stressors in their lives. 
But for whatever reason, they don’t do such 
things for their children as taking them to 
the dentist, getting them a library card, or 
attending parent-teacher conferences. 

To deal with the problem, a privately 
funded program in New York city called 
Opportunity NYC is testing a highly contro- 
versial solution for getting low-income par- 
ents more involved in their children’s health 
and education: It pays them to do it. For in- 
stance, the program pays parents $200 for 
each child who receives an annual medical 
checkup. It pays $50 every two months for 
each child who attends school regularly 
during that period. It even makes some pay- 
ments to the children themselves if they are 
in high school, and, for instance, they take 
precollege standardized tests or accumulate 
sufficient credits toward graduation. 

Opportunity NYC has the goal of less- 
ening economic hardship in low-income 
families through these direct payments 
and encouraging families to take the initia- 
tive to invest in their children and to im- 
prove their futures. This seems noble 


188 


the 21st Century 


OPPORTUNITY A) 


enough—families get money they very 
much need, but only if they engage in cer- 
tain behaviors to advance the health and 
education of their children. In operant con- 
ditioning terms, the payment is a positive 
reinforcement. In principle, the beneficial 
behaviors should become well learned 
(OpportunityNYC.org, 2009). 

So what objections could people have to 
such a program? For one thing, it has been 
criticized as insulting and patronizing to 
low-income parents. But also, whether 
such a program would work to change be- 
havior for the better over the long term is 
an open question. People might be moti- 
vated by the rewards to learn to engage in 
the appropriate behaviors, but it’s unclear 
that those behaviors would persist without 
the rewards. Furthermore, some critics be- 





lieve it may undermine students’ natural 
enjoyment of learning; instead, they will 
focus on the financial rewards (Kelley, 
2007; Sebire, Standage, & Vansteenkiste, 
2009; Vansteenkiste et al., 2005). 

Opportunity NYC intentionally takes 
advantage of operant conditioning princi- 
ples in an attempt to change people’s be- 
havior for the greater social good. If it 
works as intended, it will help relieve the 
financial strains of poverty-stricken fami- 
lies, and it will have coaxed those parents 
into giving their children better futures 
than they might otherwise have had. Still, 
the idea of paying people to be socially re- 
sponsible and meet their parental obliga- 
tions strikes many people as the wrong 
approach to take, and it remains to be seen 
whether the program is effective. 








e Do you believe that rewarding students financially for doing well in school reduces 
their motivation to learn for learning’s sake? Why or why not? 
e How might you determine if Opportunity NYC is an effective program? 








themselves angry or enraged. It is unlikely that individuals in such an emotional 
state will be able to think through what they are doing or control carefully the degree 
of punishment they are inflicting. Ultimately, those who resort to physical punish- 
ment run the risk that they will grow to be feared. Punishment can also reduce the 
self-esteem of recipients unless they can understand the reasons for it (Leary et al., 
2008; Miller-Perrin, Perrin, & Kocur, 2009; Zolotor et al., 2008). 

Finally, punishment does not convey any information about what an alternative, 
more appropriate behavior might be. To be useful in bringing about more desirable 
behavior in the future, punishment must be accompanied by specific information 
about the behavior that is being punished, along with specific suggestions concerning 
a more desirable behavior. Punishing a child for staring out the window in school 
could merely lead her to stare at the floor instead. Unless we teach her appropriate 
ways to respond, we have merely managed to substitute one undesirable behavior 
for another. If punishment is not followed up with reinforcement for subsequent 
behavior that is more appropriate, little will be accomplished. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 18 Operant Conditioning 189 


In short, reinforcing desired behavior is a more appropriate technique for mod- 
ifying behavior than using punishment. Both in and out of the scientific arena, then, 
reinforcement usually beats punishment (Hiby, Rooney, & Bradshaw, 2004; Pogarsky 
& Piquero, 2003; Sidman, 2006). (Also see Applying Psychology in the 21st Century.) 


SCHEDULES OF REINFORCEMENT: TIMING LIFE’S REWARDS 


The world would be a different place if poker players never played cards again after 

the first losing hand, fishermen returned to shore as soon as they missed a catch, or 

telemarketers never made another phone call after their first hang-up. The fact that 

such unreinforced behaviors continue, often with great frequency and persistence, schedules of reinforcement Different 
illustrates that reinforcement need not be received continually for behavior to be patterns of frequency and timing of 
learned and maintained. In fact, behavior that is reinforced only occasionally can reinforcement following desired 
ultimately be learned better than can behavior that is always reinforced. behavior. 

When we refer to the frequency and timing of reinforcement that follows desired 
behavior, we are talking about schedules of reinforcement. Behavior that is rein- 
forced every time it occurs is said to be on a continuous reinforcement schedule; 
if it is reinforced some but not all of the time, it is on a partial (or intermittent) 
reinforcement schedule. Although learning occurs more rapidly under a continuous partial (or intermittent) reinforcement 
reinforcement schedule, behavior lasts longer after reinforcement stops when it is schedule Reinforcing of a behavior 
learned under a partial reinforcement schedule (Casey, Cooper-Brown, & Wacher, some but not all of the time. 

2006; Gottlieb, 2004; Reed, 2007; Staddon & Cerutti, 2003). 

Why should intermittent reinforcement result in stronger, longer-lasting learning 
than continuous reinforcement? We can answer the question by examining how we 
might behave when using a candy vending machine compared with a Las Vegas slot 
machine. When we use a vending machine, previous experience has taught us that 
every time we put in the appropriate amount of money, the reinforcement, a candy 
bar, ought to be delivered. In other words, the schedule of reinforcement is continu- 
ous. In comparison, a slot machine offers intermittent reinforcement. We have learned 
that after putting in our cash, most of the time we will not receive anything in return. 

At the same time, though, we know that we will occasionally win something. 

Now suppose that, unknown to us, both the candy vending machine and the slot 
machine are broken, and so neither one is able to dispense anything. It would not be 
very long before we stopped depositing coins into the broken candy machine. Probably 
at most we would try only two or three times before leaving the machine in disgust. 

But the story would be quite different with the broken slot machine. Here, we would fixed-ratio schedule A schedule by 
drop in money for a considerably longer time, even though there would be no payoff. which reinforcement is given only after 
In formal terms, we can see the difference between the two reinforcement sched- a specific number of responses are 

ules: Partial reinforcement schedules (such as those provided by slot machines) main- made. 

tain performance longer than do continuous reinforcement 
schedules (such as those established in candy vending 
machines) before extinction—the disappearance of the condi- 
tioned response—occurs. 

Certain kinds of partial reinforcement schedules produce 
stronger and lengthier responding before extinction than do 
others. Although many different partial reinforcement sched- 
ules have been examined, they can most readily be put into two 
categories: schedules that consider the number of responses made 
before reinforcement is given, called fixed-ratio and variable- 
ratio schedules, and those that consider the amount of time that 
elapses before reinforcement is provided, called fixed-interval 
and variable-interval schedules (Gottlieb, 2006; Pellegrini et al., 
2004; Reed & Morgan, 2008; Svartdal, 2003). 


continuous reinforcement 
schedule Reinforcing of a behavior 
every time it occurs. 


NEXT 
STIMULUS 


20 MILES 


Fixed- and Variable-Ratio Schedules In a fixed-ratio schedule, 
reinforcement is given only after a specific number of responses. 





© Christopher Weyant/The New Yorker Collection/ 
www.cartoonbank.com. All Rights Reserved. 


190 Chapter 6 Learning 


FIGURE 4 Typical outcomes of different 
reinforcement schedules. (a) In a fixed- 
ratio schedule, reinforcement is provided 
after a specific number of responses are 
made. Because the more responses, the 
more reinforcement, fixed-ratio schedules 
produce a high rate of responding. (b) In 
a variable-ratio schedule, responding also 
occurs at a high rate. (c) A fixed-interval 
schedule produces lower rates of 
responding, especially just after 
reinforcement has been presented, 
because the organism learns that a 
specified time period must elapse 
between reinforcements. (d) A variable- 
interval schedule produces a fairly 
steady stream of responses. 


variable-ratio schedule A schedule by 
which reinforcement occurs after a 
varying number of responses rather 
than after a fixed number. 


yAlert 


Remember that the different 
schedules of reinforcement 
affect the rapidity with 
which a response is 
learned and how long it 
lasts after reinforcement is no longer 
provided. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


(a) Fixed-ratio schedule (b) Variable-ratio schedule 


Responding occurs 


at a high, steady rate. 





There are 
relatively short 
pauses in 


responding after 
reinforcement is 
provided. 


Cumulative frequency of responses 
Cumulative frequency of responses 











Time Time 


(c) Fixed-interval schedule (d) Variable-interval schedule 


There are typically long 


pauses in responding after 
reinforcement is provided. 





Responding occurs 
at a steady rate. 


Cumulative frequency of responses 
Cumulative frequency of responses 

















Time Time 


For instance, a rat might receive a food pellet every tenth time it pressed a lever; here, 
the ratio would be 1:10. Similarly, garment workers are generally paid on fixed-ratio 
schedules: They receive a specific number of dollars for every blouse they sew. Because 
a greater rate of production means more reinforcement, people on fixed-ratio sched- 
ules are apt to work as quickly as possible (see Figure 4). 

In a variable-ratio schedule, reinforcement occurs after a varying number of 
responses rather than after a fixed number. Although the specific number of responses 
necessary to receive reinforcement varies, the number of responses usually hovers 
around a specific average. A good example of a variable-ratio schedule is a telephone 
salesperson’s job. He might make a sale during the 3rd, 8th, 9th, and 20th calls with- 
out being successful during any call in between. Although the number of responses 
he must make before making a sale varies, it averages out to a 20% success rate. 
Under these circumstances, you might expect that the salesperson would try to make 
as many calls as possible in as short a time as possible. This is the case with all 
variable-ratio schedules, which lead to a high rate of response and resistance to 
extinction. 


Fixed- and Variable-Interval Schedules: The Passage of Time In contrast to fixed- 
and variable-ratio schedules, in which the crucial factor is the number of responses, 
fixed-interval and variable-interval schedules focus on the amount of time that has 
elapsed since a person or animal was rewarded. One example of a fixed-interval 
schedule is a weekly paycheck. For people who receive regular, weekly paychecks, 
it typically makes relatively little difference exactly how much they produce in a 
given week. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 18 Operant Conditioning 191 


Because a fixed-interval schedule provides reinforcement for a response only if 
a fixed time period has elapsed, overall rates of response are relatively low. This is 
especially true in the period just after reinforcement, when the time before another 
reinforcement is relatively great. Students’ study habits often exemplify this reality. 
If the periods between exams are relatively long (meaning that the opportunity for 
reinforcement for good performance is given fairly infrequently), students often 
study minimally or not at all until the day of the exam draws near. Just before the 
exam, however, students begin to cram for it, signaling a rapid increase in the rate 
of their studying response. As you might expect, immediately after the exam there 
is a rapid decline in the rate of responding, with few people opening a book the day 
after a test. Fixed-interval schedules produce the kind of “scalloping effect” shown 
in Figure 4 (Saville, 2009). 

One way to decrease the delay in responding that occurs just after reinforcement, 
and to maintain the desired behavior more consistently throughout an interval, is to 
use a variable-interval schedule. In a variable-interval schedule, the time between 
reinforcements varies around some average rather than being fixed. For example, a 
professor who gives surprise quizzes that vary from one every three days to one 
every three weeks, averaging one every two weeks, is using a variable-interval sched- 
ule. Compared to the study habits we observed with a fixed-interval schedule, stu- 
dents’ study habits under such a variable-interval schedule would most likely be 
very different. Students would be apt to study more regularly because they would 
never know when the next surprise quiz was coming. Variable-interval schedules, in 
general, are more likely to produce relatively steady rates of responding than are 
fixed-interval schedules, with responses that take longer to extinguish after reinforce- 
ment ends. 


DISCRIMINATION AND GENERALIZATION 
IN OPERANT CONDITIONING 


It does not take a child long to learn that a red light at an intersection means stop 
and a green light indicates that it is permissible to continue, in the same way that a 
pigeon can learn to peck a key when a green light goes on but not when a red light 
appears. Just as in classical conditioning, then, operant learning involves the phe- 
nomena of discrimination and generalization. 

The process by which people learn to discriminate stimuli is known as stimu- 
lus control training. In stimulus control training, a behavior is reinforced in the pres- 
ence of a specific stimulus, but not in its absence. For example, one of the most 
difficult discriminations many people face is determining when someone’s friend- 
liness is not mere friendliness, but a signal of romantic interest. People learn to 
make the discrimination by observing the presence of certain nonverbal cues—such 
as increased eye contact and touching—that indicate romantic interest. When such 
cues are absent, people learn that no romantic interest is indicated. In this case, the 
nonverbal cue acts as a discriminative stimulus, one to which an organism learns 
to respond during stimulus control training. A discriminative stimulus signals the 
likelihood that reinforcement will follow a response. For example, if you wait until 
your roommate is in a good mood before you ask to borrow her favorite CD, your 
behavior can be said to be under stimulus control because you can discriminate 
between her moods. 

Just as in classical conditioning, the phenomenon of stimulus generalization, in 
which an organism learns a response to one stimulus and then exhibits the same 
response to slightly different stimuli, occurs in operant conditioning. If you have 
learned that being polite helps you to get your way in a certain situation (reinforcing 
your politeness), you are likely to generalize your response to other situations. Some- 
times, though, generalization can have unfortunate consequences, as when people 
behave negatively toward all members of a racial group because they have had an 
unpleasant experience with one member of that group. 


fixed-interval schedule A schedule 
that provides reinforcement for a 
response only if a fixed time period 
has elapsed, making overall rates of 
response relatively low. 


variable-interval schedule A schedule 
by which the time between reinforce- 
ments varies around some average 
rather than being fixed. 


192 Chapter 6 Learning 


shaping The process of teaching a 
complex behavior by rewarding closer 
and closer approximations of the 
desired behavior. 





Name: Lea Johnson 


Hanover, NH 


Position: Seeing Eye Guide Dog Trainer 
Education: BS, Geography, Dartmouth College, 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


SHAPING: REINFORCING WHAT DOESN’T COME NATURALLY 


Consider the difficulty of using operant conditioning to teach people to repair an 
automobile transmission. If you had to wait until they chanced to fix a transmission 
perfectly before you provided them with reinforcement, the Model T Ford might be 
back in style long before they mastered the repair process. 

There are many complex behaviors, ranging from auto repair to zoo manage- 
ment, that we would not expect to occur naturally as part of anyone’s spontaneous 
behavior. For such behaviors, for which there might otherwise be no opportunity to 
provide reinforcement (because the behavior would never occur in the first place), a 
procedure known as shaping is used. Shaping is the process of teaching a complex 
behavior by rewarding closer and closer approximations of the desired behavior. In 
shaping, you start by reinforcing any behavior that is at all similar to the behavior 
you want the person to learn. Later, you reinforce only responses that are closer to 
the behavior you ultimately want to teach. Finally, you reinforce only the desired 
response. Each step in shaping, then, moves only slightly beyond the previously 
learned behavior, permitting the person to link the new step to the behavior learned 
earlier (Krueger & Dayan, 2009). 

Shaping allows even lower animals to learn complex responses that would never 
occur naturally, ranging from lions jumping through hoops, dolphins rescuing divers 
lost at sea, or rodents finding hidden land mines. Shaping also underlies the learning 
of many complex human skills. For instance, the organization of most textbooks is 
based on the principles of shaping. Typically, information is presented so that new 
material builds on previously learned concepts or skills. Thus, the concept of shaping 
could not be presented until we had discussed the more basic principles of operant 
learning (Meyer & Ladewig, 2008). (Also see PsychWork.) 


7 P hW k For decades, guide dogs have provided a set of eyes to the 
È SYC O r visually impaired, expanding the opportunities open to them 


SEEING EYE GUIDE DOG TRAINER and increasing their independence. But it takes a great deal of 


training to make a dog an effective seeing eye guide dog, 
according to Lea Johnson, who works with The Seeing Eye 
agency in Morristown, New Jersey. Johnson teaches apprentice 
instructors to carry out the demanding, but rewarding, process 
of training dogs. 

“We hire college graduates, and while we don’t require a 
specific major, a background in psychology or animal science allows employees to 
more easily connect with different aspects of the job,” she said. 

An apprentice instructor needs to have self-motivation in order to complete all 
aspects of the dog’s training. In addition, they need to be able to work in a team 
setting, according to Johnson. But that’s only part of it. 

“The process of training the dogs is complex,” says Johnson. “For example, the 
dog must be obedient and respond to their visually-impaired owner. But they also 
get praised for sometimes refusing their owner’s commands, if it would put their 
owner in danger.” 

Once a dog learns the skills it needs, the trainer must then teach a visually- 
impaired person how to work with the dog. 

“After training dogs for four months, the trainers must be able to teach blind 
people the skills to care for and travel with their Seeing Eye dog safely,” Johnson 
said. Not only must trainers relate well to dogs, but they also must interact well with 
blind people. She adds, “The training of people is intense and emotionally challeng- 
ing in a very different way from the dog training portion. Without a good heart to 
start with, trainers would never be successful.” 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 18 Operant Conditioning 193 


BIOLOGICAL CONSTRAINTS ON LEARNING: YOU 
CAN’T TEACH AN OLD DOG JUST ANY TRICK 


Not all behaviors can be trained in all species equally well. 
Instead, there are biological constraints, built-in limitations in the 
ability of animals to learn particular behaviors. In some cases, 
an organism has a special predisposition that will aid in its learn- 
ing a behavior (such as pecking behaviors in pigeons). In other 
cases, biological constraints act to prevent or inhibit an organism 
from learning a behavior. For example, it’s impossible to train 
pigs to pick up a disk, because they are biologically programmed 
to push objects like it along the ground. Similarly, although a 
raccoon can be conditioned to drop a single coin into a piggy 
bank, it will do so only after rubbing the coin against the outside 
of the bank. The reason? After catching a fish, raccoons instinc- 
tually rub them against the ground to remove their outer cover- 
ing (Breland & Breland, 1966; Stevens & Pashler, 2002). 

The existence of biological constraints is consistent with evo- 
lutionary explanations of behavior. Clearly, there are adaptive 
benefits that promote survival for organisms that quickly learn— 
or avoid—certain behaviors. For example, our ability to rapidly 
learn to avoid touching hot surfaces increases our chances of 
survival. Additional support for the evolutionary interpreta- 
tion of biological constraints lies in the fact the associations that 
animals learn most readily involve stimuli that are most relevant to the specific envi- 
ronment in which they live (Cosmides & Tooby, 2004; Davis, 2007; Terry, 2003). 

Furthermore, psychologists taking an evolutionary perspective have suggested 
that we may be genetically predisposed to be fearful of certain stimuli, such as snakes 
or even threatening faces. For example, people in experiments learn associations 
relatively quickly between photos of faces with threatening expressions and neutral 
stimuli (such as an umbrella). In contrast, they are slower to learn associations 
between faces that have pleasant expressions and neutral stimuli. Stimuli that pose 
potential threats, like snakes or people with hostile facial expressions, posed a poten- 
tial danger to early humans, and there may be an evolved “fear module” in the brain 
that is sensitized to such threats (DeLoache & LoBue, 2009; Endres & Fendt, 2007; 
Georgiou et al., 2005; Gerdes, Uhl, & Alpers, 2009; Oehman & Mineka, 2003). 





Biological constraints make it nearly impossible for animals to learn 
certain behaviors. Here, psychologist Marian Breland attempts to 
overcome the natural limitations that inhibit the success of 
conditioning this rooster. 


COMPARING CLASSICAL AND OPERANT CONDITIONING 


We've considered classical conditioning and operant conditioning as two completely 
different processes. And, as summarized in Figure 5, there are a number of key dis- 
tinctions between the two forms of learning. For example, the key concept in classi- 
cal conditioning is the association between stimuli, whereas in operant conditioning 
it is reinforcement. Furthermore, classical conditioning involves an involuntary, nat- 
ural, innate behavior, but operant conditioning is based on voluntary responses made 
by an organism. 

Some researchers are asking if, in fact, the two types of learning are so different 
after all. Some learning psychologists have suggested that classical and operant con- 
ditioning might share some underlying processes. Arguing from an evolutionary 
viewpoint, they contend that it is unlikely that two completely separate basic pro- 
cesses would evolve. Instead, one process—albeit with considerable complexity in 
the way it operates—might better explain behavior. Although it’s too early to know 
if this point of view will be supported, it is clear that there are a number of processes 
that operate both in classical and operant conditioning, including extinction, stimulus 
generalization, and stimulus discrimination (Donahoe, 2003; Donahoe & Vergas, 2004; 
Silva, Goncalves, & Garcia-Mijares, 2007). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


194 Chapter 6 Learning 


Concept Classical Conditioning Operant Conditioning 
Basic principle Building associations between a conditioned stimulus Reinforcement increases the frequency of the behavior 
and conditioned response. preceding it; punishment decreases the frequency of the 


behavior preceding it. 


Nature Based on involuntary, natural, innate behavior. Behavior Organism voluntarily operates on its environment to produce 

of behavior is elicited by the unconditioned or conditioned stimulus. a desirable result. After behavior occurs, the likelihood of the 
behavior occurring again is increased or decreased by the 
behaviors consequences. 


Order of events Before conditioning, an unconditioned stimulus leads to Reinforcement leads to an increase in behavior; punishment 
an unconditioned response. After conditioning, a leads to a decrease in behavior 
conditioned stimulus leads to a conditioned response. 


Example After a physician gives a child a series of painful injections A student who, after studying hard for a test, earns an A (the 
(an unconditioned stimulus) that produce an emotional positive reinforcer), is more likely to study hard in the future. 
reaction (an unconditioned response), the child develops A student who, after going out drinking the night before a 
an emotional reaction (a conditioned response) whenever test, fails the test (punishment) is less likely to go out drinking 
he sees the physician (the conditioned stimulus). the night before the next test. 


r 





FIGURE 5 Comparing key concepts in classical conditioning and operant conditioning. 


BECOMING AN A couple who had been living together for three years began to fight 
INFORMED CONSUMER frequently. The issues of disagreement ranged from who was going to 
do the dishes to the quality of their love life. 


of Ps YC h O | O g y Disturbed, the couple went to a behavior analyst, a psychologist 


Using Behavior Analysis and Behavior who specialized in behavior-modification techniques. He asked them 


> to keep a detailed written record of their inti ti th t 
Modification p iled w eir interactions over the nex 


two weeks. 
When they returned with the data, he carefully reviewed the 
records with them. In doing so, he noticed a pattern: Each of their 
arguments had occurred just after one or the other had left a household chore undone, such 
as leaving dirty dishes in the sink or draping clothes on the only chair in the bedroom. 
Using the data the couple had collected, the behavior analyst asked them to list all the 
chores that could possibly arise and assign each one a point value depending on how long it 
took to complete. Then he had them divide the chores equally and agree in a written 
contract to fulfill the ones assigned to them. If either failed to carry out one of the assigned 
chores, he or she would have to place $1 per point in a fund for the other to spend. They also 
agreed to a program of verbal praise, promising to reward each other verbally for complet- 
ing a chore. 
The couple agreed to try it for a month and to keep careful records of the number of 


ts they had during that period. To thei ise, thi ber declined rapidly. 
behaviormodtoation A formalized arguments they had during that period. To their surprise, the number declined rapidly. 


technique for promoting the frequency This case provides an illustration of behavior modification, a formalized technique 
of desirable behaviors and decreasing for promoting the frequency of desirable behaviors and decreasing the incidence of 
the incidence of unwanted ones. unwanted ones. Using the basic principles of learning theory, behavior-modification 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 18 Operant Conditioning 195 


techniques have proved to be helpful in a variety of situations. People with severe 
mental retardation have, for the first time in their lives, started dressing and feeding 
themselves. Behavior modification has also helped people lose weight, give up smok- 
ing, and behave more safely (Delinsky, Latner, & Wilson, 2006; Ntinas, 2007; Wadden, 
Crerand, & Brock, 2005). 

The techniques used by behavior analysts are as varied as the list of processes 
that modify behavior. They include reinforcement scheduling, shaping, generaliza- 
tion training, discrimination training, and extinction. Participants in a behavior- 
change program do, however, typically follow a series of similar basic steps that 
include the following: 


e Identifying goals and target behaviors. The first step is to define desired behavior. 
Is it an increase in time spent studying? A decrease in weight? An increase in 
the use of language? A reduction in the amount of aggression displayed by a 
child? The goals must be stated in observable terms and must lead to specific 
targets. For instance, a goal might be “to increase study time,” whereas the 
target behavior would be “to study at least two hours per day on weekdays 
and an hour on Saturdays.” 

e Designing a data-recording system and recording preliminary data. To determine 
whether behavior has changed, it is necessary to collect data before any 
changes are made in the situation. This information provides a baseline against 
which future changes can be measured. 

e Selecting a behavior-change strategy. The crucial step is to select an appropriate 
strategy. Because all the principles of learning can be employed to bring 
about behavior change, a “package” of treatments is normally used. This 
might include the systematic use of positive reinforcement for desired 
behavior (verbal praise or something more tangible, such as food), as well as 
a program of extinction for undesirable behavior (ignoring a child who 
throws a tantrum). Selecting the right reinforcers is critical, and it may be 
necessary to experiment a bit to find out what is important to a particular 
individual. 

e Implementing the program. Probably the most important aspect of program 
implementation is consistency. It is also important to reinforce the intended 
behavior. For example, suppose a mother wants her son to spend more time 
on his homework, but as soon as he sits down to study, he asks for a snack. 
If the mother gets a snack for him, she is likely to be reinforcing her son’s 
delaying tactic, not his studying. 

e Keeping careful records after the program is implemented. Another crucial task is 
record keeping. If the target behaviors are not monitored, there is no way of 
knowing whether the program has actually been successful. 

e Evaluating and altering the ongoing program. Finally, the results of the program 
should be compared with baseline, pre-implementation data to determine its 
effectiveness. If the program has been successful, the procedures employed can 
be phased out gradually. For instance, if the program called for reinforcing 
every instance of picking up one’s clothes from the bedroom floor, the rein- 
forcement schedule could be modified to a fixed-ratio schedule in which every 
third instance was reinforced. However, if the program has not been successful 
in bringing about the desired behavior change, consideration of other approaches 
might be advisable. 


Behavior-change techniques based on these general principles have enjoyed wide 
success and have proved to be one of the most powerful means of modifying behav- 
ior. Clearly, it is possible to employ the basic notions of learning theory to improve 
our lives. 


196 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 6 Learning 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 


What is the role of reward and punishment in learning? 


Operant conditioning is a form of learning in which a 
voluntary behavior is strengthened or weakened. 
According to B. F. Skinner, the major mechanism 
underlying learning is reinforcement, the process by 
which a stimulus increases the probability that a 
preceding behavior will be repeated. (p. 183) 

Primary reinforcers are rewards that are naturally 
effective without previous experience, because they 
satisfy a biological need. Secondary reinforcers begin to 
act as if they were primary reinforcers through 
association with a primary reinforcer. (p. 184) 

Positive reinforcers are stimuli that are added to the 
environment and lead to an increase in a preceding 
response. Negative reinforcers are stimuli that 

remove something unpleasant from the environment, 
also leading to an increase in the preceding response. 
(p. 185) 

Punishment decreases the probability that a prior behav- 
ior will occur. Positive punishment weakens a response 
through the application of an unpleasant stimulus, 
whereas negative punishment weakens a response by 
the removal of something positive. In contrast to 
reinforcement, in which the goal is to increase the 
incidence of behavior, punishment is meant to decrease 
or suppress behavior. (p. 186) 

Schedules and patterns of reinforcement affect the 
strength and duration of learning. Generally, partial 
reinforcement schedules—in which reinforcers are not 
delivered on every trial—produce stronger and longer- 
lasting learning than do continuous reinforcement 
schedules. (p. 189) 

Among the major categories of reinforcement schedules 
are fixed- and variable-ratio schedules, which are based 
on the number of responses made, and fixed- and 
variable-interval schedules, which are based on the time 
interval that elapses before reinforcement is provided. 
(p. 190) 

Stimulus control training (similar to stimulus discrimina- 
tion in classical conditioning) is reinforcement of a be- 
havior in the presence of a specific stimulus but not in its 
absence. In stimulus generalization, an organism learns a 
response to one stimulus and then exhibits the same re- 
sponse to slightly different stimuli. (p. 191) 

Shaping is a process for teaching complex behaviors by 
rewarding closer and closer approximations of the de- 
sired final behavior. (p. 192) 

There are biological constraints, or built-in limitations, 
on the ability of an organism to learn: Certain behaviors 


will be relatively easy for individuals of a species to 
learn, whereas other behaviors will be either difficult or 
impossible for them to learn. (p. 193) 


What are some practical methods for bringing about behavior 
change, both in ourselves and in others? 

e Behavior modification is a method for formally using 
the principles of learning theory to promote the fre- 
quency of desired behaviors and to decrease or eliminate 
unwanted ones. (p. 194) 


EVALUATE 


1, _______ conditioning describes learning that occurs as 
a result of reinforcement. 

2. Match the type of operant learning with its definition: 

1. An unpleasant stimulus a. positive reinforcement 
is presented to decrease b. negative reinforcement 
behavior. c. positive punishment 

2. An unpleasant stimulus d. negative punishment 
is removed to increase 
behavior. 

3. A pleasant stimulus 
is presented to increase 
behavior. 

4. A pleasant stimulus 
is removed to decrease 
behavior. 

3. Sandy had had a rough day, and his son’s noisemaking 
was not helping him relax. Not wanting to resort to scold- 
ing, Sandy told his son in a serious manner that he was 
very tired and would like the boy to play quietly for an 
hour. This approach worked. For Sandy, the change in his 
son’s behavior was 
a. positively reinforcing. 

b. negatively reinforcing. 

4. Ina______ reinforcement schedule, behavior is re- 
inforced some of the time, whereas in a 
reinforcement schedule, behavior is reinforced all the time. 

5. Match the type of reinforcement schedule with its definition. 
1. Reinforcement occurs a. fixed-ratio 

after a set time period. b. variable-interval 

2. Reinforcement occurs c. fixed-interval 
after a set number d. variable-ratio 
of responses. 

3. Reinforcement occurs 
after a varying time period. 

4. Reinforcement occurs 
after a varying number 
of responses. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 18 Operant Conditioning 197 


RETHINK to set up a program to increase the likelihood that children 


f ee eee : will complete their homework more frequently? 
1. Using the scientific literature as a guide, what would you P q 7 


tell parents who wish to know if the routine use of physi- ; 
j f 5 Answers to Evaluate Questions 
cal punishment is a necessary and acceptable form of child p-F G-¢ e-z 2-1 ‘g ‘snonunuo. 
rearing? “(qua}qTu193uT 10) tensed ‘p q'e p-p L-g /q-Z -1'7 “quested “TL 
2. From the perspective of an educator: How would you use 
your knowledge of operant conditioning in the classroom 


KEYTERMS 


operant conditioning schedules of fixed-ratio schedule p. 189 variable-interval 

p. 183 reinforcement p. 189 variable-ratio schedule p.191 
reinforcement p. 184 continuous reinforcement schedule p.190 shaping p. 192 
reinforcer p. 185 schedule p. 189 fixed-interval behavior 
positive reinforcer p. 186 partial (or intermittent) schedule p. 191 modification p. 194 
negative reinforcer p. 186 reinforcement 
punishment p. 186 schedule p. 189 


> 


Join Us on Fb WhatsApp & Telegram 


Chor Only Books otea in pdf 
Fo.com/groups/4581844109658 70 


TELEGRAM.ME/CSSASPIRANTSFORUM 


WAASNA NONDE 
TLZ- EALO =A OE 


SEPARA TE GROUP FOR FEMALES ASPIRANTS 


What is the role of cognition 
and thought in learning? 


An 
approach to the study of learning that 
focuses on the thought processes that 
underlie learning. 


> StudyAlert 


Remember that the cognitive 
learning approach focuses 
on the internal thoughts 
and expectations of 
learners, whereas classical 
and operant conditioning approaches 
focus on external stimuli, responses, 
and reinforcement. 


Learning in which a 
new behavior is acquired but is not 
demonstrated until some incentive is 
provided for displaying it. 


198 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Consider what happens when people learn to drive a car. They don’t just get behind 
the wheel and stumble around until they randomly put the key into the ignition and, 
later, after many false starts, accidentally manage to get the car to move forward, 
thereby receiving positive reinforcement. Rather, they already know the basic ele- 
ments of driving from previous experience as passengers, when they more than likely 
noticed how the key was inserted into the ignition, the car was put in drive, and the 
gas pedal was pressed to make the car go forward. 

Clearly, not all learning is due to operant and classical conditioning. In fact, such 
activities as learning to drive a car imply that some kinds of learning must involve 
higher-order processes in which people’s thoughts and memories and the way they 
process information account for their responses. Such situations argue against regard- 
ing learning as the unthinking, mechanical, and automatic acquisition of associations 
between stimuli and responses, as in classical conditioning, or the presentation of 
reinforcement, as in operant conditioning. 

Some psychologists view learning in terms of the thought processes, or cogni- 
tions, that underlie it—an approach known as cognitive learning theory. Although 
psychologists working from the cognitive learning perspective do not deny the 
importance of classical and operant conditioning, they have developed approaches 
that focus on the unseen mental processes that occur during learning, rather than 
concentrating solely on external stimuli, responses, and reinforcements. 

In its most basic formulation, cognitive learning theory suggests that it is not 
enough to say that people make responses because there is an assumed link between 
a stimulus and a response—a link that is the result of a past history of reinforcement 
for a response. Instead, according to this point of view, people, and even lower ani- 
mals, develop an expectation that they will receive a reinforcer after making a response. 
Two types of learning in which no obvious prior reinforcement is present are latent 
learning and observational learning. 





Latent Learning 


Evidence for the importance of cognitive processes comes from a series of animal 
experiments that revealed a type of cognitive learning called latent learning. In latent 
learning, a new behavior is learned but not demonstrated until some incentive is 
provided for displaying it (Tolman & Honzik, 1930). In short, latent learning occurs 
without reinforcement. 

In the studies demonstrating latent learning, psychologists examined the behav- 
ior of rats in a maze such as the one shown in Figure 1a. In one experiment, a group 
of rats was allowed to wander around the maze once a day for 17 days without ever 
receiving a reward. Understandably, those rats made many errors and spent a rela- 
tively long time reaching the end of the maze. A second group, however, was always 
given food at the end of the maze. Not surprisingly, those rats learned to run quickly 
and directly to the food box, making few errors. 

A third group of rats started out in the same situation as the unrewarded rats, 
but only for the first 10 days. On the 11th day, a critical experimental manipulation 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 19 Cognitive Approaches to Learning 


Finish 10 
(with food) 





Unrewarded control 


OKNA, AN 
NG 

BEBRDANAS 
k 


Experimental group 


LA One-way door 0 | | | 


A Curtain Days 
(a) (b) 


FIGURE 1 (a) In an attempt to demonstrate latent learning, rats were allowed to roam 
through a maze of this sort once a day for 17 days. (b) The rats that were never rewarded 
(the unrewarded control condition) consistently made the most errors, whereas those that 
received food at the finish every day (the rewarded control condition) consistently made far 
fewer errors. But the results also showed latent learning: Rats that were initially unrewarded 
but began to be rewarded only after the 10th day (the experimental group) showed an 
immediate reduction in errors and soon became similar in error rate to the rats that had been 
rewarded consistently. According to cognitive learning theorists, the reduction in errors 
indicates that the rats had developed a cognitive map—a mental representation—of the 
maze. Can you think of other examples of latent learning? 








Average number of errors 


























was introduced: From that point on, the rats in this group were given food for com- 
pleting the maze. The results of this manipulation were dramatic, as you can see 
from the graph in Figure 1b. The previously unrewarded rats, which had earlier 
seemed to wander about aimlessly, showed such reductions in run- 
ning time and declines in error rates that their performance almost 
immediately matched that of the group that had received rewards 
from the start. 

To cognitive theorists, it seemed clear that the unrewarded rats 
had learned the layout of the maze early in their explorations; they 
just never displayed their latent learning until the reinforcement was 
offered. Instead, those rats seemed to develop a cognitive map of the 
maze—a mental representation of spatial locations and directions. 

People, too, develop cognitive maps of their surroundings. For 
example, latent learning may permit you to know the location of a 
kitchenware store at a local mall you’ve frequently visited, even 
though you’ve never entered the store and don’t even like to cook. 

The possibility that we develop our cognitive maps through 
latent learning presents something of a problem for strict operant 








© Gahan Wilson/The New Yorker Collection/www. 


cartoonbank.com. 


200 Chapter 6 Learning 





Albert Bandura examined the principles 
of observational learning. 


observational learning Learning by 
observing the behavior of another 
person, or model. 


yAlert 


A key point of observational 
learning approaches is that 
the behavior of models who 
are rewarded for a given 
behavior is more likely to be 
imitated than behavior in which the 
model is punished for the behavior. 





This boy is displaying observational 
learning based on previous observation 
of his father. How does observational 
learning contribute to learning gender 
roles? 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


conditioning theorists. If we consider the results of the maze-learning experiment, 
for instance, it is unclear what reinforcement permitted the rats that initially received 
no reward to learn the layout of the maze, because there was no obvious reinforcer 
present. Instead, the results support a cognitive view of learning, in which changes 
occurred in unobservable mental processes (Frensch & Rtinger, 2003; Iaria et al., 2009; 
Stouffer & White, 2006). 


Observational Learning: 
Learning Through Imitation 


Let’s return for a moment to the case of a person learning to drive. How can we 
account for instances in which an individual with no direct experience in carrying 
out a particular behavior learns the behavior and then performs it? To answer this 
question, psychologists have focused on another aspect of cognitive learning: obser- 
vational learning. 

According to psychologist Albert Bandura and colleagues, a major part of human 
learning consists of observational learning, which is learning by watching the behav- 
ior of another person, or model. Because of its reliance on observation of others—a 
social phenomenon—the perspective taken by Bandura is often referred to as a social 
cognitive approach to learning (Bandura, 1999, 2004). 

Bandura dramatically demonstrated the ability of models to stimulate learning 
in a classic experiment. In the study, young children saw a film of an adult wildly 
hitting a 5-feet-tall inflatable punching toy called a Bobo doll (Bandura, Ross, & Ross, 
1963a, 1963b). Later the children were given the opportunity to play with the Bobo 
doll themselves, and, sure enough, most displayed the same kind of behavior, in 
some cases mimicking the aggressive behavior almost identically. 

Not only negative behaviors are acquired through observational learning. In one 
experiment, for example, children who were afraid of dogs were exposed to a 
model—dubbed the Fearless Peer—playing with a dog (Bandura, Grusec, & Men- 
love, 1967). After exposure, observers were considerably more likely to approach a 
strange dog than were children who had not viewed the Fearless Peer. 

Observational learning is particularly important in acquiring skills in which the 
operant conditioning technique of shaping is inappropriate. Piloting an airplane and 
performing brain surgery, for example, are behaviors that could hardly be learned 
by using trial-and-error methods without grave cost—literally—to those involved in 
the learning process. 

Observational learning may have a genetic basis. For example, we find observa- 
tional learning at work with mother animals teaching their young such activities as 
hunting. In addition, the discovery of mirror neurons that fire when we observe 
another person carrying out a behavior (discussed in the chapter on neuroscience) 
suggests that the capacity to imitate others may be innate (Lepage & Theoret, 2007; 
Thornton & McAuliffe, 2006; Schulte-Ruther et al., 2007) (see Figure 2). 

Not all behavior that we witness is learned or carried out, of course. One crucial 
factor that determines whether we later imitate a model is whether the model is 
rewarded for his or her behavior. If we observe a friend being rewarded for putting 
more time into his studies by receiving higher grades, we are more likely to imitate 
his behavior than we would if his behavior resulted only in being stressed and tired. 
Models who are rewarded for behaving in a particular way are more apt to be mim- 
icked than are models who receive punishment. Observing the punishment of a 
model, however, does not necessarily stop observers from learning the behavior. 
Observers can still describe the model’s behavior—they are just less apt to perform 
it (Bandura, 1977, 1986, 1994). 

Observational learning is central to a number of important issues relating to 
the extent to which people learn simply by watching the behavior of others. For 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 19 Cognitive Approaches to Learning 201 


Learning Through 


Observation 





FIGURE 2 Mirror neurons are neurons in the brain that respond both during the observation 
and execution of an action, suggesting a mechanism that may be involved with learning 
through imitation. In a study investigating the mirror neuron system in humans, participants 
were scanned while observing actions relating to communication (a) and objects (b). The brain 
scans (c and d) illustrate the areas of the brain that form the human mirror neuron system that 
respond during the observation and execution of these actions. (Source: Montgomery et al., 2007.) 





Ok 


(a) Communication-related movement (c) 








Turn Key 
(b) Object-related movement (d) 


instance, the degree to which observation of media aggression produces subsequent 
aggression on the part of viewers is a crucial—and controversial—question, as we 
discuss next. 


VIOLENCE IN TELEVISION AND VIDEO GAMES: 
DOES THE MEDIA’S MESSAGE MATTER? 


In an episode of The Sopranos television series, fictional mobster Tony Soprano murdered 
one of his associates. To make identification of the victim’s body difficult, Soprano and 
one of his henchmen dismembered the body and dumped the body parts. 

A few months later, two real-life half brothers in Riverside, California, strangled 
their mother and then cut her head and hands from her body. Victor Bautista, 20, 
and Matthew Montejo, 15, were caught by police after a security guard noticed that 
the bundle they were attempting to throw in a dumpster had a foot sticking out of 
it. They told police that the plan to dismember their mother was inspired by the 
Sopranos episode (Martelle, Hanley, & Yoshino, 2003). 

Like other “media copycat” killings, the brothers’ cold-blooded brutality raises 
a critical issue: Does observing violent and antisocial acts in the media lead viewers 
to behave in similar ways? Because research on modeling shows that people fre- 
quently learn and imitate the aggression that they observe, this question is among 
the most important issues being addressed by psychologists. 

Certainly, the amount of violence in the mass media is enormous. By the time 
of elementary school graduation, the average child in the United States will have 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


202 Chapter 6 Learning 


viewed more than 8,000 murders and more than 800,000 violent acts on 
network television (Huston et al., 1992; Mifflin, 1998). 

Most experts agree that watching high levels of media violence makes 
viewers more susceptible to acting aggressively, and recent research sup- 
ports this claim (Boxer et al., 2009; Carnagey, Anderson, & Bartholow, 
2007; Savage & Yancey, 2008). For example, one survey of serious and 
violent young male offenders incarcerated in Florida showed that one- 
fourth of them had attempted to commit a media-inspired copycat crime 
(Surette, 2002). A significant proportion of those teenage offenders noted 
that they paid close attention to the media. 

Violent video games have also been linked with actual aggression. In 
one of a series of studies by psychologist Craig Anderson and his col- 
leagues, for example, college students who frequently played violent 
video games, such as Postal or Doom, were more likely to have been 
involved in delinquent behavior and aggression. Frequent players also 
had lower academic achievement (Anderson et al., 2004; Anderson & Car- 
nagey, 2009; Swing & Anderson, 2007). 

Several aspects of media violence may contribute to real-life aggressive 
behavior (Bushman & Anderson, 2001; Johnson et al., 2002). For one thing, 
experiencing violent media content seems to lower inhibitions against car- 
rying out aggression—watching television portrayals of violence or using 
violence to win a video game makes aggression seem a legitimate response 
to particular situations. Exposure to media violence also may distort our understand- 
ing of the meaning of others’ behavior, predisposing us to view even nonaggressive 
acts by others as aggressive. Finally, a continuous diet of aggression may leave us 
desensitized to violence, and what previously would have repelled us now produces 
little emotional response. Our sense of the pain and suffering brought about by aggres- 
sion may be diminished (Bartholow, Bushman, & Sestir, 2006; Carnagey, Anderson, 
& Bushman, 2007; Weber, Ritterfeld, & Kostygina, 2006). 

What about real-life exposure to actual violence? Does it also lead to increases in 
aggression? The answer is yes. Exposure to actual firearm violence (being shot or 
being shot at) doubles the probability that an adolescent will commit serious violence 
over the next two years. Whether the violence is real or fictionalized, then, observing 
violent behavior leads to increases in aggressive behavior (Allwood, 2007; Bingen- 
heimer, Brennan, & Earls, 2005). 





Illustrating observational learning, this infant observes 
an adult on the television and then is able to imitate 
his behavior. Learning has obviously occurred through 
the mere observation of the television model. 


Ş È Ex p | O r | nN g When a member of the Chilcotin Indian tribe teaches her daughter to 
fg DIVERSITY prepare salmon, at first she allows the daughter only to observe the 


eae Culture Influence: How We Learn? entire process. A little later, she permits her child to try out some basic 
i parts of the task. Her response to questions is noteworthy. For example, 





e 3 g 73 
a ana: 


when the daughter asks about how to do “the backbone part,” the 

mother’s response is to repeat the entire process with another salmon. 
The reason? The mother feels that one cannot learn the individual parts of the task apart from 
the context of preparing the whole fish. (Tharp, 1989) 


It should not be surprising that children raised in the Chilcotin tradition, which stresses 
instruction that starts by communicating the entire task, may have difficulty with 
traditional Western schooling. In the approach to teaching most characteristic of 
Western culture, tasks are broken down into their component parts. Only after each 
small step is learned is it thought possible to master the complete task. 

Do the differences in teaching approaches between cultures affect how people 
learn? Some psychologists, taking a cognitive perspective on learning, suggest that 
people develop particular learning styles, characteristic ways of approaching material, 
based on their cultural background and unique pattern of abilities (Anderson & 
Adams, 1992; Barmeyer, 2004; Wilkinson & Olliver-Gray, 2006). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 19 Cognitive Approaches to Learning 


Relational Style 


|. Perceive information as part of total 
picture 

2. Exhibit improvisational and intuitive 
thinking 

3. More easily learn materials that have 
a human, social content and are 
characterized by experimental/cultural 
relevance 


4. Have a good memory for verbally 
presented ideas and information, 
especially if relevant 


5. Are more task-oriented concerning 
nonacademic areas 


6. Are influenced by authority figures’ 
expression of confidence or doubt in 
students’ ability 


7. Prefer to withdraw from unstimulating 
task performance 





8. Style conflicts with the traditional school 
environment 





FIGURE 3 A comparison of analytical versus relational approaches to learning offers one 
example of how learning styles differ along several dimensions. 


Learning styles differ along several dimensions. For example, one central dimen- 
sion is relational versus analytical approaches to learning. As illustrated in Figure 3, 
people with a relational learning style master material best through exposure to a full 
unit or phenomenon. Parts of the unit are comprehended only when their relation- 
ship to the whole is understood. 

In contrast, those with an analytical learning style do best when they can carry 
out an initial analysis of the principles and components underlying a phenomenon 
or situation. By developing an understanding of the fundamental principles and 
components, they are best able to understand the full picture. 

According to James Anderson and Maurianne Adams, particular minority groups 
in Western societies display characteristic learning styles. For instance, they argue 
that Caucasian females and African-American, Native-American, and Hispanic- 
American males and females are more apt to use a relational style of learning than 
are Caucasian and Asian-American males, who are more likely to employ an ana- 
lytical style (Adams et al., 2000; Anderson & Adams, 1992). 

The conclusion that members of particular ethnic and gender groups have similar 
learning styles is controversial. Because there is so much diversity within each particu- 
lar racial and ethnic group, critics argue that generalizations about learning styles can- 
not be used to predict the style of any single individual, regardless of group membership. 

Still, it is clear that values about learning, which are communicated through a per- 
son’s family and cultural background, have an impact on how successful students are 
in school. One theory suggests that members of minority groups who were voluntary 
immigrants are more apt to be successful in school than those who were brought into 
a majority culture against their will. For example, Korean children in the United States— 
the sons and daughters of voluntary immigrants—perform quite well, as a group, in 
school. In contrast, Korean children in Japan, who were often the sons and daughters 
of people who were forced to immigrate during World War II, essentially as forced 
laborers, do less well in school. The theory suggests that the motivation to succeed is 
lower for children in forced immigration groups (Foster, 2005; Ogbu, 1992, 2003). 


Analytical Style 


Able to dis-embed information from 
total picture (focus on detail) 


. Exhibit sequential and structured thinking 


. More easily learn materials that are 


inanimate and impersonal 


. Have a good memory for abstract ideas 


and irrelevant information 


. Are more task-oriented concerning 


academics 


. Are not greatly affected by the opinions 


of others 


. Show ability to persist at unstimulating 


tasks 


. Style matches most school environments 


203 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


204 Chapter 6 Learning 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 3. In_________ learning, a new behavior is learned but is 
tsh til iate reinf ti ted. 
What is the role of cognition and thought in learning? Se ane Ti eine aes Oa det tea eng 
Son A e 4. Bandura’s _____ theory of learning states that 
ognitive approaches to learning consider learning in T ae ala) (another 


terms of thought processes, or cognition. Phenomena 
such as latent learning—in which a new behavior is 
learned but not performed until some incentive is 
provided for its performance—and the apparent 
development of cognitive maps support cognitive 
approaches. (p. 198) 

Learning also occurs from observing the behavior of 
others. The major factor that determines whether an 
observed behavior will actually be performed is the 
nature of the reinforcement or punishment a model 
receives. (p. 200) 

Observation of violence is linked to a greater likelihood 
of subsequently acting aggressively. (p. 201) 


person displaying the behavior of interest). 


RETHINK 


1. The relational style of learning sometimes conflicts with 
the traditional school environment. Could a school be 
created that takes advantage of the characteristics of the 
relational style? How? Are there types of learning for 
which the analytical style is clearly superior? 

2. From the perspective of a social worker: What advice would 
you give to families about children’s exposure to violent 
media and video games? 


e Learning styles are characteristic ways of approaching 


learning, based on a person’s cultural background and Answers to Evaluate Questions 
unique pattern of abilities. Whether an individual has an Jepow ‘TeuoHeasasgo “fF 4U g {UOTeyDadxa ‘z ‘sassad01d 
analytical or a relational style of learning, for example, [eyUOUT tBIM pauaouos ATeUTTAd are systrooty SupUTea] IAHPUZOD ‘asTey `T 


may reflect family background or culture. (p. 202) 


EVALUATE 


1. Cognitive learning theorists are concerned only with overt 
behavior, not with its internal causes. True or false? 

2. In cognitive learning theory, it is assumed that people 
develop a(n) ______ about receiving a reinforcer 
when they behave a certain way. 


KEY TERMS 


cognitive learning latent learning observational 
theory p. 198 p. 198 learning p. 200 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Psychology on the Web 


1. B. F. Skinner had an impact on society and on thought that is only hinted at in our 
discussion of learning. Find additional information on the web about Skinner's life 
and influence. See what you can find out about his ideas for an ideal, utopian society 
based on the principles of conditioning and behaviorism. Write a summary of your 
findings. 

2. Select a topic discussed in this set of modules that is of interest to you—for example, 
reinforcement versus punishment, teaching complex behaviors by shaping, violence 
in video games, relational versus analytical learning styles, behavior modification, and 
so on. Find at least two sources of information on the web about your topic and 
summarize the results of your quest. It may be most helpful to find two different 
approaches to your topic and compare them. 


E © | log U e Here we have discussed several kinds of learning, 


ranging from classical conditioning, which depends on 
the existence of natural stimulus-response pairings, to operant conditioning, in which 
reinforcement is used to increase desired behavior. These approaches to learning focus 
on outward, behavioral learning processes. Cognitive approaches to learning focus on 
mental processes that enable learning. 

We have also noted that learning is affected by culture and individual differences, 
with individual learning styles potentially affecting the ways in which people learn most 
effectively. And we saw some ways in which our learning about learning can be put to 
practical use, through such means as behavior-modification programs designed to 
decrease negative behaviors and increase positive ones. 

Return to the prologue of this set of modules and consider the following questions 
about the very-well-learned response to cellphones and other handheld devices: 





1. When people check their messages as soon as their device beeps, is that an example 
of classical conditioning, operant conditioning, or cognitive learning? Why? 

2. In the example of people checking their messages, what is the stimulus and what is 
the response? 

3. What is the reinforcement that keeps the message-checking behavior strong? Is it 
positive or negative reinforcement? Is the behavior ever punished? How so? 

4. If the message-checking behavior is so frequent that it becomes a problem, as 
Senator Cochran described, what strategies could be used to stop or control the 
behavior? 





205 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Key Concepts for Chapter 7 


MODULE 20 










What is memory? @ Are there different kinds The Foundations of Memory 


. . S M 

of memory? @ What are the biological bases E as 
Short-Term Memory 
of memory? Working Memory 
Long-Term Memory 


Neuroscience in Your Life: Experience, 
Memory, and the Brain 


Applying Psychology in the 21st Century: 
In Pursuit of a Memory Eraser 


MODULE 21 





What causes difficulties and failures in Recalling Long-Term Memories 


. Retrieval Cues 

remembering? , 
Levels of Processing 

Explicit and Implicit Memory 

Flashbulb Memories 


Constructive Processes in Memory: 
Rebuilding the Past 





Exploring Diversity: Are There Cross- 
Cultural Differences in Memory? 





Why do we forget information? @ What are Forgetting: When Memory Fails 


3 s ; Why We Forget 

the major memory impairments? 7 9 

Proactive and Retroactive Interference: 
The Before and After of Forgetting 
Memory Dysfunctions: Afflictions 

of Forgetting 

Neuroscience in Your Life: Alzheimer's 
Disease and Brain Deterioration 
Becoming an Informed Consumer of 
Psychology: Improving Your Memory 


207 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Prologue Remembering It All 





Meet Jill Price at one of her favorite Los Angeles restaurants, and 
she'll scoot into a booth and start to reminisce. “On Wednesday, 
December 11, 1996, my friend and | came here hunting for the 
perfect crab cake,” she says. She also remembers Saturday, 
November 4, 1 995 (she-saw on TV that Israeli Prime Minister 
Yitzhak Rabin was killed) and Friday, September 20, 1985 (her first 
visit to the eatery; she wore a large hat). “It’s all flashing through 
my head,’ she says matter-of-factly. “m totally in the moment. 

| just have a split screen of my past, too” \ 


What Price, 42, has is the first-ever diagnosed case of highly 
superior autobiographical memory, which forces her to recall 
every day of her life from the age of 14. Given a date, she can tell 
you on what day of the week it fell, what she did and any 
historical event she heard of that day. “It’s like | walk around with 
a video camera,’ says Price. “My memories are nonstop and 
involuntary.’ (Perry, 2008, p. 143) 





While we might think that having perfect recall for all the events 
in our lives would be a blessing, Jill Price’s experience suggests 
otherwise. Her extremely rare condition (called hyperthymestic 
syndrome) affects the part of her memory that stores experiences 
related to life events. She has perfect, vivid memories of virtually 
every day of her life, memories both good and bad. And while it's 
pleasant for her to relive her fond memories, she can't escape the 
pain of the bad ones (Parker, Cahill, & McGaugh, 2006; Price, 
2008). 

Price's condition illustrates the complexity and the mystery of 
the phenomenon we call memory. Memory allows us to retrieve 
a vast amount of information. We are able to remember the name 


208 


of a friend we haven't talked with for years and recall the details 
of a picture that hung in our bedroom as a child. At the same 
time, though, memory failures are common. We forget where we 
left the keys to the car and fail to answer an exam question about 
material we studied only a few hours earlier. Why? 

We turn now to the nature of memory, considering the ways 
in which information is stored and retrieved. We examine the 
problems of retrieving information from memory, the accuracy 
of memories, and the reasons information is sometimes forgotten. 
We also consider the biological foundations of memory and 
discuss some practical means of increasing memory capacity. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 20 


You are playing a game of Trivial Pursuit, and winning the game comes down to 
one question: On what body of water is Mumbai located? As you rack your brain 
for the answer, several fundamental processes relating to memory come into play. 
You may never, for instance, have been exposed to information regarding Mumbai's 
location. Or if you have been exposed to it, it may simply not have registered in a 
meaningful way. In other words, the information might not have been recorded prop- 
erly in your memory. The initial process of recording information in a form usable 
to memory, a process called encoding, is the first stage in remembering something. 

Even if you had been exposed to the information and originally knew the name 
of the body of water, you may still be unable to recall it during the game because of 
a failure to retain it. Memory specialists speak of storage, the maintenance of material 
saved in memory. If the material is not stored adequately, it cannot be recalled later. 

Memory also depends on one last process—retrieval: Material in memory storage has 
to be located and brought into awareness to be useful. Your failure to recall Mumbai's 
location, then, may rest on your inability to retrieve information that you learned earlier. 

In sum, psychologists consider memory to be the process by which we encode, 
store, and retrieve information (see Figure 1). Each of the three parts of this definition— 
encoding, storage, and retrieval—represents a different process. You can think of these 
processes as being analogous to a computer’s keyboard (encoding), hard drive (storage), 
and software that accesses the information for display on the screen (retrieval). Only if 
all three processes have operated will you experience success and be able to recall the 
body of water on which Mumbai is located: the Arabian Sea. 

Recognizing that memory involves encoding, storage, and retrieval gives us a start 
in understanding the concept. But how does memory actually function? How do we 
explain what information is initially encoded, what gets stored, and how it is retrieved? 

According to the three-system approach to memory that dominated memory research 
for several decades, there are different memory storage systems or stages through 
which information must travel if it is to be remembered (Atkinson & Shiffrin, 1968, 
1971). Historically, the approach has been extremely influential in the development 
of our understanding of memory, and—although new theories have augmented it— 
it still provides a useful framework for understanding how information is recalled. 


Retrieval 
(Recovery of 
stored information) 


Encoding 
(Initial recording 
of information) 


Storage 
(Information saved 
for future use) 


loading from/ j 
drive a ji 





What is memory? 


Are there different kinds of 
memory? 


What are the biological bases 
of memory? 


The process by which we 
encode, store, and retrieve information. 


‘IGURE 1 Memory is built on three 
basic processes—encoding, storage, and 
retrieval—that are analogous to a 
computer's keyboard, hard drive, and 
software to access the information for 
display on the screen. The analogy is 
not perfect, however, because human 
memory is less precise than a computer. 
How might you modify the analogy to 
make it more accurate? 


209 


210 Chapter 7 Memory 





4 


Informati 









www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Elaborative rehearsal 
(moves information 
into long-term memory) 


Repetitive 
rehearsal (retains 
information in 
short-term memory) 











Forgetting Forgetting 
typically within within 15 to 
| second 25 seconds 


sensory memory The initial, momen- 
tary storage of information, lasting 
only an instant. 


short-term memory Memory that 
holds information for 15 to 25 seconds. 


long-term memory Memory that 
stores information on a relatively 
permanent basis, although it may be 
difficult to retrieve. 


y Alert 


Although the three types 
of memory are discussed 
as separate memory 
stores, these are not 
mini-warehouses located in 
specific areas of the brain. Instead, 
they represent three different types 
of memory systems with different 
characteristics. 






FIGURE 2 In this three-stage model of memory, information initially recorded by the person's 
sensory system enters sensory memory, which momentarily holds the information. The infor- 
mation then moves to short-term memory, which stores it for 15 to 25 seconds. Finally, the 
information can move into long-term memory, which is relatively permanent. Whether the 
information moves from short-term to long-term memory depends on the kind and amount of 
rehearsal of the material that is carried out. (Source: Atkinson & Shifrin, 1968.) 


The three-system memory theory proposes the existence of the three separate 
memory stores shown in Figure 2. Sensory memory refers to the initial, momentary 
storage of information that lasts only an instant. Here an exact replica of the stimu- 
lus recorded by a person’s sensory system is stored very briefly. In a second stage, 
short-term memory holds information for 15 to 25 seconds and stores it according 
to its meaning rather than as mere sensory stimulation. The third type of storage 
system is long-term memory. Information is stored in long-term memory on a rela- 
tively permanent basis, although it may be difficult to retrieve. 


Sensory Memory 


A momentary flash of lightning, the sound of a twig snapping, and the sting of a pin- 
prick all represent stimulation of exceedingly brief duration, but they may nonetheless 
provide important information that can require a response. Such stimuli are initially— 
and fleetingly—stored in sensory memory, the first repository of the information the 
world presents to us. Actually, there are several types of sensory memories, each related 
to a different source of sensory information. For instance, iconic memory reflects infor- 
mation from the visual system. Echoic memory stores auditory information coming from 
the ears. In addition, there are corresponding memories for each of the other senses. 

Sensory memory can store information for only a very short time. If information 
does not pass into short-term memory, it is lost for good. For instance, iconic mem- 
ory seems to last less than a second, and echoic memory typically fades within two 
or three seconds. However, despite the brief duration of sensory memory, its preci- 
sion is high: Sensory memory can store an almost exact replica of each stimulus to 
which it is exposed (Darwin, Turvey, & Crowder, 1972; Long & Beaton, 1982; Sams 
et al., 1993; Deouell, Parnes, & Pickard, 2006). 

Psychologist George Sperling (1960) demonstrated the existence of sensory mem- 
ory in a series of clever and now-classic studies. He briefly exposed people to a series 
of 12 letters arranged in the following pattern: 


F T Y C 
K D N È 
Y W B M 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 20 The Foundations of Memory 211 


When exposed to this pattern of letters for just one twentieth of a second, most 
people could recall only four or five of the letters accurately. Although they knew 
that they had seen more, the memory of those letters had faded by the time they 
reported the first few letters. It was possible, then, that the information had initially 
been accurately stored in sensory memory. But during the time it took to verbalize 
the first four or five letters, the memory of the other letters faded. 

To test that possibility, Sperling conducted an experiment in which a high, 
medium, or low tone sounded just after a person had been exposed to the full pat- 
tern of letters. People were told to report the letters in the highest line if a high tone 
was sounded, the middle line if the medium tone occurred, or the lowest line at the 
sound of the low tone. Because the tone occurred after the exposure, people had to A momentary flash of lightning leaves a 
rely on their memories to report the correct row. sensory visual memory, a fleeting but 

The results of the study clearly showed that people had been storing the com- @%act replica of the stimulus that fades 
plete pattern in memory. They accurately recalled the letters in the line that had been away. 
indicated by the tone regardless of whether it was the top, middle, or bottom line. 

Obviously, all the lines they had seen had been stored in sensory memory. Despite 
its rapid loss, then, the information in sensory memory was an accurate representa- 
tion of what people had seen. 

By gradually lengthening the time between the presentation of the visual pattern 
and the tone, Sperling was able to determine with some accuracy the length of time 
that information was stored in sensory memory. The ability to recall a particular row 
of the pattern when a tone was sounded declined progressively as the period between 
the visual exposure and the tone increased. This decline continued until the period 
reached about one second in duration, at which point the row could not be recalled 
accurately at all. Sperling concluded that the entire visual image was stored in sensory 
memory for less than a second. 

In sum, sensory memory operates as a kind of snapshot that stores information— 
which may be of a visual, auditory, or other sensory nature—for a brief moment in 
time. But it is as if each snapshot, immediately after being taken, is destroyed and 
replaced with a new one. Unless the information in the snapshot is transferred to 
some other type of memory, it is lost. 








Short-Term Memory 


Because the information that is stored briefly in sensory memory consists of repre- 
sentations of raw sensory stimuli, it is not meaningful to us. If we are to make sense 
of it and possibly retain it, the information must be transferred to the next stage of 
memory: short-term memory. Short-term memory is the memory store in which 
information first has meaning, although the maximum length of retention there is 
relatively short (Hamilton & Martin, 2007). 
The specific process by which sensory memories are transformed into short-term 
memories is not clear. Some theorists suggest that the information is first translated 
into graphical representations or images, and others hypothesize that the transfer 
occurs when the sensory stimuli are changed to words (Baddeley & Wilson, 1985). 
What is clear, however, is that unlike sensory memory, which holds a relatively full 
and detailed—if short-lived—representation of the world, short-term memory has 
incomplete representational capabilities. 
In fact, the specific amount of information that can be held in short-term memory 
has been identified as seven items, or “chunks,” of information, with variations up 
to plus or minus two chunks. A chunk is a meaningful grouping of stimuli that can chunk A meaningful grouping of 
be stored as a unit in short-term memory. According to George Miller (1956),a chunk stimuli that can be stored as a unit in 
can be individual letters or numbers, permitting us to hold a seven-digit phone short-term memory. 
number (such as 226-4610) in short-term memory. 
But a chunk also may consist of larger categories, such as words or other mean- 
ingful units. For example, consider the following list of 21 letters: 


PBSFOXCNNABCCBSMTVNBC 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


212 Chapter 7 Memory 





FIGURE 3 Examine the chessboard on the left for about five seconds. Then cover up the 
board and draw the position of the pieces on the blank chessboard. (You could also use a 
chessboard of your own and place the pieces in the same positions.) Unless you are an 
experienced chess player, you are likely to have great difficulty carrying out such a task. Yet 
chess masters—those who win tournaments—do this quite well (deGroot, 1966). They are 
able to reproduce correctly 90% of the pieces on the board. In comparison, inexperienced 
chess players are typically able to reproduce only 40% of the board properly. The chess 
masters do not have superior memories in other respects; they generally test normally on 
other measures of memory. What they can do better than others is see the board in terms 
of chunks or meaningful units and reproduce the position of the chess pieces by using 
those units. 


Because the list exceeds seven chunks, it is difficult to recall the letters after one 
exposure. But suppose they were presented as follows: 


PBS FOX CNN ABC CBS MTV NBC 


In this case, even though there are still 21 letters, you’d be able to store them in 
short-term memory since they represent only seven chunks. 

Chunks can vary in size from single letters or numbers to categories that are far 
more complicated. The specific nature of what constitutes a chunk varies according 
to one’s past experience. You can see this for yourself by trying an experiment that 
was first carried out as a comparison between expert and inexperienced chess play- 
ers and is illustrated in Figure 3 (deGroot, 1978; Oberauer, 2007; Gilchrist, Cowan, & 
Naveh-Benjamin, 2009). 

Although it is possible to remember seven or so relatively complicated sets of 
information entering short-term memory, the information cannot be held there very 
long. Just how brief is short-term memory? If you've ever looked up a telephone 
number in a phone directory, repeated the number to yourself, put away the direc- 
tory, and then forgotten the number after you’ve tapped the first three numbers into 
your phone, you know that information does not remain in short-term memory very 
long. Most psychologists believe that information in short-term memory is lost after 
15 to 25 seconds—unless it is transferred to long-term memory. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 20 The Foundations of Memory 213 


REHEARSAL 


The transfer of material from short- to long-term memory 
proceeds largely on the basis of rehearsal, the repetition of 


information that has entered short-term memory. Rehearsal a 
accomplishes two things. First, as long as the information | EPN | 
is repeated, it is maintained in short-term memory. More Eaa Ean 


important, however, rehearsal allows us to transfer the 
information into long-term memory (Kvavilashvili & 
Fisher, 2007). 

Whether the transfer is made from short- to long-term 
memory seems to depend largely on the kind of rehearsal that 
is carried out. If the information is simply repeated over and 
over again—as we might do with a telephone number while 
we rush from the phone book to the phone—it is kept current | 
in short-term memory, but it will not necessarily be placed in ; 
long-term memory. Instead, as soon as we stop punching in 
the phone numbers, the number is likely to be replaced by 
other information and will be completely forgotten. 

In contrast, if the information in short-term memory is 
rehearsed using a process called elaborative rehearsal, it is 
much more likely to be transferred into long-term memory. 
Elaborative rehearsal occurs when the information is consid- 
ered and organized in some fashion. The organization might = 
include expanding the information to make it fit into a logical 
framework, linking it to another memory, turning it into an image, or transforming it 
in some other way. For example, a list of vegetables to be purchased at a store could 
be woven together in memory as items being used to prepare an elaborate salad, could 
be linked to the items bought on an earlier shopping trip, or could be thought of in 
terms of the image of a farm with rows of each item. 

By using organizational strategies such as these—called mnemonics—we can vastly 
improve our retention of information. Mnemonics (pronounced “neh MON ix”) are 
formal techniques for organizing information in a way that makes it more likely to 
be remembered. For instance, when a beginning musician learns that the spaces on 
the music staff spell the word FACE, or when we learn the rhyme “Thirty days hath 
September, April, June, and November . . . ,” we are using mnemonics (Bellezza, 
2000; Carney & Levin, 2003; Sprenger, 2007). 


Working Memory 


Rather than seeing short-term memory as an independent way station into which 
memories arrive, either to fade or to be passed on to long-term memory, many con- 
temporary memory theorists conceive of short-term memory as far more active. In this 
view, short-term memory is like an information-processing system that manages both 
new material gathered from sensory memory and older material that has been pulled 
from long-term storage. In this increasingly influential view, short-term memory is 
referred to as working memory and defined as a set of temporary memory stores that 
actively manipulate and rehearse information (Bayliss et al., 2005a, 2005b; Unsworth 
& Engle, 2005). 

Working memory is thought to contain a central executive processor that is 
involved in reasoning and decision making. The central executive coordinates three 
distinct storage-and-rehearsal systems: the visual store, the verbal store, and the epi- 
sodic buffer. The visual store specializes in visual and spatial information, whereas 
the verbal store holds and manipulates material relating to speech, words, and 
numbers. The episodic buffer contains information that represents episodes or 














[How NOT To REMEMBER NAMES 














0.K He has a bothen nose That 
rhymes with Sutten Willie Sutton Willie 
Sutton robbed banks. Banks" rhymes 
with Hanks. Tom Hanks. His name 
is Tam: No. His name is Hank. 
Ne. Tom Hank. Wait. “Hank ” 
Sounds weird. T've get t FRANK/ 
Ne Frank .. 











not 





© Roz Chast/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


rehearsal The repetition of information 
that has entered short-term memory. 


working memory A set of active, 
temporary memory stores that actively 
manipulate and rehearse information. 


214 


Chapter 7 Memory 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





FIGURE 4 Working memory is an active “workspace” in which information is retrieved and 
manipulated and in which information is held through rehearsal (Gathercole & Baddeley, 
1993). It consists of a “central executive” that coordinates the visual store (which concentrates 
on visual and spatial information), the verbal store (which concentrates on speech, words, and 
numbers), and the episodic buffer (which represents episodes or occurrences that are 
encountered). (Source: Adapted from Baddeley, Chincotta, & Adlam, 2001.) 


events (Baddeley, 2001; Bröder & Schiffer, 2006; Rudner & Rönnberg, 2008; see 
Figure 4). 

Working memory permits us to keep information in an active state briefly so that 
we can do something with the information. For instance, we use working memory 
when we're doing a multistep arithmetic problem in our heads, storing the result of 
one calculation while getting ready to move to the next stage. (I make use of my 
working memory when I figure a 20% tip in a restaurant by first calculating 10% of 
the total bill and then doubling it.) 

Although working memory aids in the recall of information, it uses a significant 
amount of cognitive resources during its operation. In turn, this can make us less 
aware of our surroundings—something that has implications for the debate about 
the use of cell telephones in automobiles. If a phone conversation requires thinking, 
it will burden working memory and leave drivers less aware of their surroundings, 
an obviously dangerous state of affairs (Sifrit, 2006; Strayer & Drews, 2007). 

Furthermore, stress can reduce the effectiveness of working memory by reducing 
its capacity. In fact, one study found that students with the highest working memory 
capacity and greatest math ability were the ones who were most vulnerable to pres- 
sure to perform well. Those who should have performed best, then, were the ones 
most apt to choke on the test because their working memory capacities were reduced 
by the stress (Beilock & Carr, 2005; Carey, 2004). 


Long-Term Memory 


Material that makes its way from short-term memory to long-term memory enters a 
storehouse of almost unlimited capacity. Like a new file we save on a hard drive, the 
information in long-term memory is filed and coded so that we can retrieve it when 
we need it. 

Evidence of the existence of long-term memory, as distinct from short-term mem- 
ory, comes from a number of sources. For example, people with certain kinds of brain 
damage have no lasting recall of new information received after the damage occurred, 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 20 The Foundations of Memory 215 


although people and events stored in memory before the injury remain intact (Milner, 
1966). Because information that was encoded and stored before the injury can be 
recalled and because short-term memory after the injury appears to be operational— 
new material can be recalled for a very brief period—we can infer that there are two 
distinct types of memory: one for short-term and one for long-term storage. 

Results from laboratory experiments are also consistent with the notion of sepa- 
rate short-term and long-term memory. For example, in one set of studies, people 
were asked to recall a relatively small amount of information (such as a set of three 
letters). Then, to prevent practice of the initial information, participants were required 
to recite some extraneous material aloud, such as counting backward by threes 
(Brown, 1958; Peterson & Peterson, 1959). By varying the amount of time between 
the presentation of the initial material and the need for its recall, investigators found 
that recall was quite good when the interval was very short but declined rapidly 
thereafter. After 15 seconds had gone by, recall hovered at around 10% of the mate- 
rial initially presented. 

Apparently, the distraction of counting backward prevented almost all the initial 
material from reaching long-term memory. Initial recall was good because it was 
coming from short-term memory, but those memories were lost at a rapid rate. Even- 
tually, all that could be recalled was the small amount of material that had made its 
way into long-term storage despite the distraction of counting backward. 

The distinction between short- and long-term memory is also supported by the 
serial position effect, in which the ability to recall information in a list depends on 
where in the list an item appears. For instance, often a primacy effect occurs, in which 
items presented early in a list are remembered better. There is also a recency effect, in 
which items presented late in a list are remembered best (Bonanni et al., 2007; Tan 
& Ward, 2008; Tydgat & Grainger, 2009). 


LONG-TERM MEMORY MODULES 


Just as short-term memory is often conceptualized in terms of working memory, 
many contemporary researchers now regard long-term memory as having several 
different components, or memory modules. Each of these modules represents a sepa- 
rate memory system in the brain. 

One major distinction within long-term memory is that between declarative mem- 
ory and procedural memory. Declarative memory is memory for factual information: 
names, faces, dates, and facts, such as “a bike has two wheels.” In contrast, procedural 
memory (or nondeclarative memory) refers to memory for skills and habits, such as how 
to ride a bike or hit a baseball. Information about things is stored in declarative mem- 
ory; information about how to do things is stored in procedural memory (Feldhusen, 
2006; Brown & Robertson, 2007; Bauer, 2008). 

Declarative memory can be subdivided into semantic memory and episodic 
memory. Semantic memory is memory for general knowledge and facts about the 
world, as well as memory for the rules of logic that are used to deduce other facts. 
Because of semantic memory, we remember that the ZIP code for Beverly Hills is 
90210, that Mumbai is on the Arabian Sea, and that memoree is the incorrect spelling 
of memory. Thus, semantic memory is somewhat like a mental almanac of facts 
(Nyberg & Tulving, 1996; Tulving, 2002). 

In contrast, episodic memory is memory for events that occur in a particular 
time, place, or context. For example, recall of learning to ride a bike, our first kiss, 
or arranging a surprise 21st birthday party for our brother is based on episodic 
memories. Episodic memories relate to particular contexts. For example, remember- 
ing when and how we learned that 2 xX 2 = 4 would be an episodic memory; the fact 
itself (that 2 X 2 = 4) is a semantic memory. (Also see Figure 5.) 

Episodic memories can be surprisingly detailed. Consider, for instance, how you’d 
respond if you were asked to identify what you were doing on a specific day two years 
ago. Impossible? You may think otherwise as you read the following exchange between 


declarative memory Memory for 
factual information: names, faces, 
dates, and the like. 


procedural memory Memory for skills 
and habits, such as riding a bike or 
hitting a baseball; sometimes referred 
to as nondeclarative memory. 


semantic memory Memory for general 
knowledge and facts about the world, 
as well as memory for the rules of logic 
that are used to deduce other facts. 


episodic memory Memory for events 
that occur in a particular time, place, 
or context. 


216 Chapter 7 Memory 


FIGURE 5 Long-term memory can be 
subdivided into several different types. 
What type of long-term memory is 
involved in your recollection of the 
moment you first arrived on your 
campus at the start of college? What 
type of long-term memory is involved 
in remembering the lyrics to a song, 
compared with the tune of a song? 


y Alert 


Use Figure 5 to help clarify 

the distinctions between 
the different types of long- 
term memory. 







semantic networks Mental represen- 
tations of clusters of interconnected 
information. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 








Semantic memory 
(general memory) 
Example: George Washington 
wore a wig 


Episodic memory 
(personal knowledge) 
Example: Remembering your 
visit to Washington’s home, 
Mount Vernon 


a researcher and a participant in a study who was asked, in a memory experiment, what 
he was doing “on Monday afternoon in the third week of September two years ago.” 


PARTICIPANT: Come on. How should I know? 
EXPERIMENTER: Just try it anyhow. 


PARTICIPANT: OK. Let’s see: Two years ago . . . I would be in high school in Pitts- 
burgh .... That would be my senior year. Third week in September—that’s just after 
summer—that would be the fall term . . . . Let me see. I think I had chemistry lab on 
Mondays. I don’t know. I was probably in chemistry lab. Wait a minute—that would be 
the second week of school. I remember he started off with the atomic table—a big fancy 
chart. I thought he was crazy trying to make us memorize that thing. You know, I think 
I can remember sitting . . . . (Lindsay & Norman, 1977). 


Episodic memory, then, can provide information about events that happened 
long in the past (Reynolds & Takooshian, 1988). But semantic memory is no less 
impressive, permitting us to dredge up tens of thousands of facts ranging from the 
date of our birthday to the knowledge that $1 is less than $5. 


SEMANTIC NETWORKS 


Try to recall, for a moment, as many things as you can think of that are the color 
red. Now pull from your memory the names of as many fruits as you can recall. 

Did the same item appear when you did both tasks? For many people, an apple 
comes to mind in both cases since it fits equally well in each category. And the fact 
that you might have thought of an apple when doing the first task makes it even 
more likely that you'll think of it when doing the second task. 

It’s actually quite amazing that we're able to retrieve specific material from the 
vast store of information in our long-term memories. According to some memory 
researchers, one key organizational tool that allows us to recall detailed information 
from long-term memory is the associations that we build between different pieces of 
information. In this view, knowledge is stored in semantic networks, mental repre- 
sentations of clusters of interconnected information (Collins & Quillian, 1969; Collins 
& Loftus, 1975; Cummings, Ceponiene, & Koyama, 2006). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 20 The Foundations of Memory 217 








FIGURE 6 Semantic networks in memory consist of relationships between pieces of information, 
such as those relating to the concept of a fire engine. The lines suggest the connections that 
indicate how the information is organized within memory. The closer together two concepts are, 
the greater the strength of the association. (Source: Collins & Loftus, 1975.) 


Consider, for example, Figure 6, which shows some of the relationships in 
memory relating to fire engines, the color red, and a variety of other semantic 
concepts. Thinking about a particular concept leads to recall of related concepts. 
For example, seeing a fire engine may activate our recollections of other kinds of 
emergency vehicles, such as an ambulance, which in turn may activate recall of the 
related concept of a vehicle. And thinking of a vehicle may lead us to think about 
a bus that we’ve seen in the past. Activating one memory triggers the activation of 
related memories in a process known as spreading activation (Foster et al., 2008; 
Kreher et al, 2008). 


THE NEUROSCIENCE OF MEMORY 


Can we pinpoint a location in the brain where long-term memories reside? Is there 
a single site that corresponds to a particular memory, or is memory distributed in 
different regions across the brain? Do memories leave an actual physical trace that 
scientists can view? 

The search for the engram, the term for the physical memory trace that corresponds 
to a memory, has proved to be a major puzzle to psychologists and other neuroscien- 
tists interested in memory. Using advanced brain scanning procedures in their efforts 
to determine the neuroscientific basis of memory formation, investigators have learned 
that certain areas and structures of the brain specialize in different types of memory- 
related activities. The hippocampus, a part of the brain’s limbic system (see Figure 7), 
plays a central role in the consolidation of memories. Located within the brain’s medial 
temporal lobes just behind the eyes, the hippocampus aids in the initial encoding of 
information, acting as a kind of neurological e-mail system. That information is 


218 Chapter 7 Memory 


FIGURE 7 The hippocampus and 
amygdala, parts of the brain's limbic 
system, play a central role in the 
consolidation of memories. (Source: Van 
De Graff, 2000.) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





subsequently passed along to the cerebral cortex of the brain, where it is actually 
stored (Govindarajan, Kelleher, & Tonegawa, 2006; J. Peters et al., 2007; Lavenex & 
Lavenex, 2009). 

The significance of the hippocampus is exemplified by studies of individuals 
who have particularly good, yet specialized, types of memories. For instance, taxi 
drivers in London, England, must have accurate, complete recall of the location of 
the maze of streets and alleys within a six-mile radius of the center of the city. It 
takes years of study to memorize the material. MRI brain scans of taxi drivers show 
that, relative to non-taxi drivers with fewer navigational skills, the back of the hip- 
pocampus is larger while the front is smaller. The findings are consistent with the 
idea that particular areas of the hippocampus are involved in the consolidation of 





“The matters about which I’m being questioned, Your Honor, are all 
things I should have included in my long-term memory but which 
I mistakenly inserted in my short-term memory.” 


© Ed Fisher/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 20 The Foundations of Memory 219 


Experience, 


Memory, and the Brain 





FIGURE 8 Illustrating how experience can shape how our brain processes memories, these 
fMRI scans show how the number of years spent driving a taxi relates to the size of particular 
areas of the hippocampus as participants become spatial and navigational memory experts. 
In (a) we see areas of the posterior (back) hippocampus that increase in activity (in yellow) 
with the number of years spent driving a taxi. We also see the relationship illustrated 
graphically in the graph to the right of the figure. In contrast, in (b) we see areas of the 
anterior (front) hippocampus that show reduced activity (also in yellow) after many years of 
driving a taxi and the relationship portrayed graphically. The changes in the areas activated 
during memory tasks show how practice can shape the brain and how this allows us to 
develop specialized skills such as memory for spatial locations. (Source: Maguire et al., 2006.) 





w 
oO 


N 
oO 


S 











Years experience taxi driving 


Al 0.0 l 


Posterior HC grey matter volume 


w 
Oo 

















bo 

= 

= 

x 

me] 

x 

g 7° ‘ 

v 

z aNu 

„2 =. u g Lal 

5 10 = 

a m 

x 

© 

T > 

D 

Pol 0.0 l 
Anterior hippocampus (HC) Anterior HC grey matter volume 


spatial memories (see Figure 8; Maguire, Woollett, & Spiers, 2006; Spiers & Maguire, 
2007; Woollett & Maguire, 2009). 

The amygdala, another part of the limbic system, also plays an important role 
in memory. The amygdala is especially involved with memories involving emo- 
tion. For example, if you are frightened by a large Doberman, you're likely to 
remember the event vividly—an outcome related to the functioning of the amyg- 
dala. Encountering the Doberman or any large dog in the future is likely to reac- 
tivate the amygdala and bring back the unpleasant memory (Hamann, 2001; 
Buchanan & Adolphs, 2004; Talmi et al., 2008). 


Memory at the Level of Neurons. Although it is clear that the hippocampus and 
amygdala play a central role in memory formation, how is the transformation of 
information into a memory reflected at the level of neurons? 

One answer is long-term potentiation, which shows that certain neural pathways 
become easily excited while a new response is being learned. At the same time, the 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





In Pursuit of aMemory Eraser 


Most of us would welcome a way to im- 
prove our memories. Wouldn’t it be help- 
ful to retain more information in textbooks, 
or to remember the names of people we 
meet at a party, or just to recall where we 
left our car keys? 

But would it also be important to devise 
a method to be able to forget information in 
memory? 

Some researchers believe so, and they are 
trying to identify ways to allow people to for- 
get traumatic events, persistent fears, or even 
annoying habits. Researchers recently took 
an important step in the direction of being 
able to erase unwanted memories by discov- 
ering an intriguing substance that seems to 
be a key player in the formation of memories. 
The substance, called PKM-zeta, seems to be 
present whenever a connection between 
neurons is strengthened. So researchers put 
PKM-zeta to a test in lab rats. The rats were 
trained to associate a certain tone with re- 
ceiving a mild electric shock, such that they 
became fearful of the tone—a learning expe- 
rience that was stored in their memories. 
However, when the rats’ brains were injected 
later with a drug that stops production of 
PKM-zeta, a strange thing happened: The 
tone no longer produced a fear response. The 
rats forgot what they had originally learned 
(Pastalkova et al., 2006; Yao et al., 2008). 

The study supports the hypothesis that 
memory, far from being a rigid and durable 
phenomenon, is actually fluid and malleable. 


220 


A treatment that can stop the encoding of 
memories has the potential to help people 
who are suffering from intrusive traumatic 
memories. For example, the blood-pressure 
drug propranolol affects areas of the brain 
that are responsible for memory storage. In 
one study, researchers gave a small group of 
hospital patients who had just suffered an 
accident or a rape a course of treatment with 
either propranolol or a placebo. Three 
months later, when the patients were ex- 
posed to reminders of their traumatic expe- 
riences, those who had received the 
propranolol had a less stressful reaction than 
those who had received the placebo (Jha, 
2006; Brunet et al., 2007; Brunet et al., 2008). 

Ultimately, the goal of researchers is to 
use substances such as PKM-zeta and pro- 
pranolol to interfere with the reconsolida- 
tion of traumatic memories that have been 
reawakened. The idea is not to block the 
memory entirely but to reduce its inten- 
sity upon reconsolidation so that the vic- 
tim will remember what happened but 
not be tortured by the memory. If that 
happens, victims may be treated more 
successfully (Pitman & Delahanty, 2005; 
Carey, 2009a). 





New treatments erase intrusive traumatic 
memories. 


The inhibition of PKM-zeta did not induce 
the rats to forget what they learned as much 
as it stopped the memory from being re- 
encoded in the brain after it was activated. 





e Why might researchers want only to reduce the intensity of a traumatic memory 
rather than erase it altogether? 

e What might be some of the practical or ethical issues involved with erasing un- 
wanted memories? 





number of synapses between neurons increase as the dendrites branch out to receive 
messages. These changes reflect a process called consolidation, in which memories 
become fixed and stable in long-term memory. Long-term memories take some time 
to stabilize; this explains why events and other stimuli are not suddenly fixed in 
memory. Instead, consolidation may continue for days and even years (McGaugh, 
2003; Meeter & Murre, 2004; Kawashima, Izaki, & Grace, 2006). 

Because a stimulus may contain different sensory aspects, visual, auditory, and 
other areas of the brain may be simultaneously processing information about that stim- 
ulus. Information storage appears to be linked to the sites where this processing occurs, 
and it is therefore located in the particular areas that initially processed the information 
in terms of its visual, auditory, and other sensory stimuli. For this reason, memory traces 
are distributed throughout the brain. For example, when you recall a beautiful beach 
sunset, your recollection draws on memory stores located in visual areas of the brain 
(the view of the sunset), auditory areas (the sounds of the ocean), and tactile areas (the 
feel of the wind) (Desimone, 1992; Brewer et al., 1998; Squire, Clark, & Bayley, 2004). 

In short, the physical stuff of memory—the engram—is produced by a complex of 
biochemical and neural processes. Scientists are just beginning to understand how the 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 20 The Foundations of Memory 221 


brain compiles the individual neural components into a single, coherent memory. It 
may be that the same neurons that fire when we are initially exposed to material are 
reactivated during efforts to recall that information. Still, although memory researchers 
have made considerable strides in understanding the neuroscience behind memory, 
more remains to be learned—and remembered (Gelbard-Sagiv et al., 2008). (For more 
on the biological basis of memory, see Applying Psychology in the 21st Century.) 


RECAP 


What is memory? 
e Memory is the process by which we encode, store, and 
retrieve information. (p. 209) 


Are there different kinds of memory? 

e Sensory memory, corresponding to each of the sensory 
systems, is the first place where information is saved. 
Sensory memories are very brief, but they are precise, 
storing a nearly exact replica of a stimulus. (p. 210) 

e Roughly seven (plus or minus two) chunks of informa- 
tion can be transferred and held in short-term memory. 
Information in short-term memory is held from 15 to 
25 seconds and, if not transferred to long-term memory, 
is lost. (p. 211) 


e Memories are transferred into long-term storage through 


rehearsal. If memories are transferred into long-term 
memory, they become relatively permanent. (p. 213) 

e Some theorists view short-term memory as a working 
memory in which information is retrieved and manipu- 


lated and held through rehearsal. In this view, it is a cen- 


tral executive processor involved in reasoning and 
decision making; it coordinates a visual store, a verbal 
store, and an episodic buffer. (p. 213) 

e Long-term memory can be viewed in terms of memory 
modules, each of which is related to separate memory 
systems in the brain. For instance, we can distinguish 
between declarative memory and procedural memory. 
Declarative memory is further divided into episodic 
memory and semantic memory. (p. 214) 

e Semantic networks suggest that knowledge is stored in 
long-term memory as mental representations of clusters 
of interconnected information. (p. 217) 


What are the biological bases of memory? 
e The hippocampus and amygdala are especially impor- 
tant in the establishment of memory. (p. 218) 


KEY TERMS 


memory p. 209 long-term memory p. 210 
sensory memory p. 209 chunk p. 211 
short-term memory p. 210 rehearsal p. 213 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


e Memories are distributed across the brain, relating to 
the different sensory information-processing systems in- 
volved during the initial exposure to a stimulus. (p. 218) 


EVALUATE 


1. Match the type of memory with its definition: 
1. long-term memory a. holds information 15 to 
2. short-term memory 25 seconds 
3. sensory memory b. stores information on a 
relatively permanent basis 
c. direct representation of a 
stimulus 

2. A(n) is a meaningful group of stimuli that can 

be stored together in short-term memory. 

3. There appear to be two types of declarative memory: 
memory for knowledge and facts and 
memory for personal experiences. 

4. Some memory researchers believe that long-term memory 

is stored as associations between pieces of information in 
networks. 


RETHINK 


1. It is a truism that “you never forget how to ride a bicycle.” 
Why might this be so? In what type of memory is infor- 
mation about bicycle riding stored? 

2. From a marketing specialist’s perspective: How might adver- 
tisers and others use ways of enhancing memory to pro- 
mote their products? What ethical principles are involved? 
Can you think of a way to protect yourself from unethical 
advertising? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 
oyueulas ‘p ‘orpostda yuewas ‘g Nyo ‘z ‘2-2-7 ‘G-[ ‘TL 


working memory p. 213 semantic memory p. 216 
declarative memory p. 215 episodic memory p. 216 
procedural memory p. 216 semantic networks p. 217 


MODULE 21 


What causes difficulties and 
failures in remembering? 


The 
inability to recall information that one 
realizes one knows—a result of the 
difficulty of retrieving information 
from long-term memory. 


Memory task in which specific 
information must be retrieved. 


Memory task in which 
individuals are presented with a 
stimulus and asked whether they have 
been exposed to it in the past or to 
identify it from a list of alternatives. 





FIG! Try to recall the names of these characters. Because this 
is a recall task, it is relatively difficult. 


222 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


An hour after his job interview, Ricardo was sitting in a coffee shop, telling his friend 
Laura how well it had gone, when the woman who had interviewed him walked in. 
“Well, hello, Ricardo. How are you doing?” Trying to make a good impression, Ricardo 
began to make introductions, but suddenly realized he could not remember the 
interviewer’s name. Stammering, he desperately searched his memory but to no avail. 
“I know her name,” he thought to himself, “but here I am, looking like a fool. I can kiss 
this job good-bye.” 


Have you ever tried to remember someone’s name, convinced that you knew it but 
unable to recall it no matter how hard you tried? This common occurrence—known 
as the tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon—exemplifies how difficult it can be to retrieve 
information stored in long-term memory (Schwartz, 2001, 2002, 2008; Cleary, 2006; 
Brennen, Vikan, & Dybdahl, 2007). 


Retrieval Clues 


Perhaps recall of names and other memories is not perfect because there is so much 
information stored in long-term memory. Because the material that makes its way to 
long-term memory is relatively permanent, the capacity of long-term memory is vast. 
For instance, if you are like the average college student, your vocabulary includes 
some 50,000 words, you know hundreds of mathematical “facts,” and you are able to 
conjure up images—such as the way your childhood home looked—with no trouble 
at all. In fact, simply cataloging all your memories would probably take years of work. 

How do we sort through this vast array of material and retrieve specific informa- 
tion at the appropriate time? One way is through retrieval cues. A retrieval cue is a 
stimulus that allows us to recall more easily information that 
is in long-term memory. It may be a word, an emotion, or a 
sound; whatever the specific cue, a memory will suddenly 
come to mind when the retrieval cue is present. For example, 
the smell of roasting turkey may evoke memories of Thanks- 
giving or family gatherings. 

Retrieval cues guide people through the information stored 
in long-term memory in much the same way that a search 
engine such as Google guides people through the Internet. 
They are particularly important when we are making an effort 
to recall information, as opposed to being asked to recognize 
material stored in memory. In recall, a specific piece of infor- 
mation must be retrieved—such as that needed to answer a 
fill-in-the-blank question or to write an essay on a test. In con- 
trast, recognition occurs when people are presented with a 
stimulus and asked whether they have been exposed to it pre- 
viously or are asked to identify it from a list of alternatives. 

As you might guess, recognition is generally a much easier 
task than recall (see Figures 1 and 2). Recall is more difficult 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 21 Recalling Long-Term Memories 


Answer this recognition question: 
Which of the following are the names of the seven dwarfs in the Disney movie 
Snow White and the Seven Dwarfs? 


Goofy Bashful 
Sleepy Meanie 
Smarty Doc 
Scaredy Happy 
Dopey Angry 
Grumpy Sneezy 
Wheezy Crazy 


(Azaaus pue ‘Adaays ‘Addey ‘dwn ‘Aadog 0q ‘|njyseg aie sJaMsUe 7291102 BU) 


because it consists of a series of processes: a search through memory, retrieval of poten- 
tially relevant information, and then a decision regarding whether the information you 
have found is accurate. If the information appears to be correct, the search is over, but 
if it is not, the search must continue. In contrast, recognition is simpler because it 
involves fewer steps (Miserando, 1991; Leigh, Zinkhan, & Swaminathan, 2006). 





Levels of Processing 


One determinant of how well memories are recalled is the way in which material is 
first perceived, processed, and understood. The levels-of-processing theory empha- 
sizes the degree to which new material is mentally analyzed. It suggests that the 
amount of information processing that occurs when material is initially encountered 
is central in determining how much of the information is ultimately remembered. 
According to this approach, the depth of information processing during exposure to 
material—meaning the degree to which it is analyzed and considered—is critical; the 
greater the intensity of its initial processing, the more likely we are to remember it 
(Craik, 1990; Troyer, Hafliger, & Cadieux, 2006; Craik & Lockhart, 2008). 

Because we do not pay close attention to much of the information to which we 
are exposed, very little mental processing typically takes place, and we forget new 
material almost immediately. However, information to which we pay greater atten- 
tion is processed more thoroughly. Therefore, it enters memory at a deeper level— 
and is less apt to be forgotten than is information processed at shallower levels. 

The theory goes on to suggest that there are considerable differences in the ways 
in which information is processed at various levels of memory. At shallow levels, 
information is processed merely in terms of its physical and sensory aspects. For 
example, we may pay attention only to the shapes that make up the letters in the 
word dog. At an intermediate level of processing, the shapes are translated into mean- 
ingful units—in this case, letters of the alphabet. Those letters are considered in the 
context of words, and specific phonetic sounds may be attached to the letters. 

At the deepest level of processing, information is analyzed in terms of its mean- 
ing. We may see it in a wider context and draw associations between the meaning 
of the information and broader networks of knowledge. For instance, we may think 
of dogs not merely as animals with four legs and a tail, but also in terms of their 
relationship to cats and other mammals. We may form an image of our own dog, 
thereby relating the concept to our own lives. According to the levels-of-processing 
approach, the deeper the initial level of processing of specific information, the longer 
the information will be retained. 

There are considerable practical implications to the notion that recall depends 
on the degree to which information is initially processed. For example, the depth of 
information processing is critical when learning and studying course material. Rote 


223 


FIGURE 2 Naming the characters in 
Figure 1 (a recall task) is more difficult 
than solving the recognition problem 
posed in this list. 


y Alert 


Remember the distinction 
between recall (in which 
specific information 
must be retrieved) and 
recognition (in which 
information is presented and must 
be identified or distinguished from 
other material). 


levels-of-processing theory The 
theory of memory that emphasizes the 
degree to which new material is 
mentally analyzed. 


224 Chapter 7 Memory 


explicit memory Intentional or 
conscious recollection of information. 


implicit memory Memories of which 
people are not consciously aware but 
that can affect subsequent performance 
and behavior. 


priming A phenomenon in which 
exposure to a word or concept (called a 
prime) later makes it easier to recall 
related information, even when there 
is no conscious memory of the word or 
concept. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


memorization of a list of key terms for a test is unlikely to produce long-term recol- 
lection of information because processing occurs at a shallow level. In contrast, think- 
ing about the meaning of the terms and reflecting on how they relate to information 
that one currently knows results in far more effective long-term retention (Conway, 
2002; Wenzel, Zetocha, & Ferraro, 2007). 





Explicit and Implicit Memory 


If you’ve ever had surgery, you probably hoped that the surgeons were focused 
completely on the surgery and gave you their undivided attention while slicing into 
your body. The reality in most operating rooms is quite different, though. Surgeons 
may be chatting with nurses about a new restaurant as soon as they sew you up. 

If you are like most patients, you are left with no recollection of the conversation 
that occurred while you were under anesthesia. However, it is very possible that 
although you had no conscious memories of the discussions on the merits of the 
restaurant, on some level you probably did recall at least some information. In fact, 
careful studies have found that people who are anesthetized during surgery can 
sometimes recall snippets of conversations they heard during surgery—even though 
they have no conscious recollection of the information (Kihlstrom et al., 1990; Sebel, 
Bonke, & Winogard, 1993). 

The discovery that people have memories about which they are unaware has 
been an important one. It has led to speculation that two forms of memory, explicit 
and implicit, may exist side by side. Explicit memory refers to intentional or con- 
scious recollection of information. When we try to remember a name or date we have 
encountered or learned about previously, we are searching our explicit memory. 

In contrast, implicit memory refers to memories of which people are not con- 
sciously aware but that can affect subsequent performance and behavior. Skills that 
operate automatically and without thinking, such as jumping out of the path of an 
automobile coming toward us as we walk down the side of a road, are stored in 
implicit memory. Similarly, a feeling of vague dislike for an acquaintance, without 
knowing why we have that feeling, may be a reflection of implicit memories. Perhaps 
the person reminds us of someone else in our past that we didn’t like, even though 
we are not aware of the memory of that other individual (Tulving, 2000; Uttl, Graf, 
& Consentino, 2003; Coates, Butler, & Berry, 2006; Voss & Paller, 2008). 

Implicit memory is closely related to the prejudice and discrimination people 
exhibit toward members of minority groups. As we first discussed in the module on 
conducting psychological research, even though people may say and even believe they 
harbor no prejudice, assessment of their implicit memories may reveal that they have 
negative associations about members of minority groups. Such associations can influ- 
ence people’s behavior without their being aware of their underlying beliefs (Green- 
wald, Nosek, & Banaji, 2003; Greenwald, Nosek, & Sriram, 2006; Hofmann et al., 2008). 

One way that memory specialists study implicit memory is through experiments 
that use priming. Priming is a phenomenon in which exposure to a word or concept 
(called a prime) later makes it easier to recall related information. Priming effects occur 
even when people have no conscious memory of the original word or concept (Schacter 
& Badgaiyan, 2001; Toth & Daniels, 2002; Schacter, Dobbins, & Schnyer, 2004). 

The typical experiment designed to illustrate priming helps clarify the phenom- 
enon. In priming experiments, participants are rapidly exposed to a stimulus such 
as a word, an object, or perhaps a drawing of a face. The second phase of the exper- 
iment is done after an interval ranging from several seconds to several months. At 
that point, participants are exposed to incomplete perceptual information that is 
related to the first stimulus, and they are asked whether they recognize it. For exam- 
ple, the new material may consist of the first letter of a word that had been presented 
earlier or a part of a face that had been shown earlier. If participants are able to 
identify the stimulus more readily than they identify stimuli that have not been 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 21 


presented earlier, priming has taken place. Clearly, the earlier stimulus has been 
remembered—although the material resides in implicit memory, not explicit memory. 

The same thing happens to us in our everyday lives. Suppose several months ago 
you watched a documentary on the planets, and the narrator described the moons of 
Mars, focusing on its moon named Phobos. You promptly forget the name of the moon, 
at least consciously. Then, several months later, you’re completing a crossword puzzle 
that you have partially filled in, and it includes the letters obos. As soon as you look 
at the set of letters, you think of Phobos, and suddenly you recall for the first time 
since your initial exposure to the information that it is one of the moons of Mars. The 
sudden recollection occurred because your memory was primed by the letters obos. 

In short, when information that we are unable to consciously recall affects our 
behavior, implicit memory is at work. Our behavior may be influenced by experi- 
ences of which we are unaware—an example of what has been called “retention 
without remembering” (Horton et al., 2005). 


Flashbulb Memories 


Where were you on February 1, 2003? You will most likely draw a blank until this 
piece of information is added: February 1, 2003, was the date the Space Shuttle 
Columbia broke up in space and fell to Earth. 

You probably have little trouble recalling your exact location and a variety of 
other trivial details that occurred when you heard about the shuttle disaster, even 
though the incident happened a few years ago. Your ability to remember details 
about this fatal event illustrates a phenomenon known as flashbulb memory. Flash- 
bulb memories are memories related to a specific, important, or surprising event 
that are so vivid they represent a virtual snapshot of the event. 

Several types of flashbulb memories are common among college students. For 
example, involvement in a car accident, meeting one’s roommate for the first time, 
and the night of high school graduation are all typical flashbulb memories (Romeu, 
2006; Bohn & Berntsen, 2007; Talarico, 2009; see Figure 3). 

Of course, flashbulb memories do not contain every detail of an original scene. 
I remember vividly that more than four decades ago I was sitting in Mr. Sharp’s 
10th-grade geometry class when I heard that President John Kennedy had been shot. 
However, although I recall where I was sitting and how my classmates reacted to 
the news, I do not recollect what I was wearing or what I had for lunch that day. 

Furthermore, the details recalled in flashbulb memories are often inaccurate. For 
example, think back to the tragic day when the World Trade Center in New York 
was attacked by suicidal terrorists. Do you remember watching television that morn- 
ing and seeing images of the first plane, and then the second plane, striking the 
towers? 

If you do, you are among the 73% of Americans who recall viewing the initial 
television images of both planes on September 11, 2001. However, that recollection 
is wrong: In fact, television broadcasts showed images only of the second plane on 
September 11. No video of the first plane was available until early the following 
morning, September 12, when it was shown on television (Begley, 2002). 

Flashbulb memories illustrate a more general phenomenon about memory: Mem- 
ories that are exceptional are more easily retrieved (although not necessarily accu- 
rately) than are those relating to events that are commonplace. The more distinctive 
a stimulus is, and the more personal relevance the event has, the more likely we are 
to recall it later (Berntsen & Thomsen, 2005; Shapiro, 2006; Talarico & Rubin, 2007). 

Even with a distinctive stimulus, however, we may not remember where the 
information came from. Source amnesia occurs when an individual has a memory for 
some material but cannot recall where he or she encountered it. For example, source 
amnesia can explain situations in which you meet someone you know but can’t 
remember where you’d met that person initially. 





Recalling Long-Term Memories 225 


flashbulb memories Memories 
centered on a specific, important, or 
surprising event that are so vivid it is 
as if they represented a snapshot of the 
event. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


226 Chapter 7 Memory 


Being in or witnessing 
a car accident 


Met a roommate 
for the first time 


Night of high school 
graduation 


Night of your senior 
prom 


An early romantic 
experience 


Public speaking 


Receipt of college 
admissions letter 


First date-when you 
met him or her 


First airplane flight 


Moment you opened 
your SAT scores 


constructive processes Processes in 
which memories are influenced by 
the meaning we give to events. 


schemas Organized bodies of 
information stored in memory that 
bias the way new information is 
interpreted, stored, and recalled. 



























10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 


Percentage of sample reporting that event resulted 
in “flashbulb memories” 
FIGURE 3 These are the most common flashbulb memory events, based on a survey of 
college students. What are some of your flashbulb memories? (Source: From David C. Rubin, 
“The Subtle Deceiver: Recalling Our Past,” Psychology Today, September 1985, pp. 39-46. Reprinted 
with permission from Psychology Today magazine. (Copyright © 1985 Sussex Publishers, LLC.)) 


Similarly, our motivation to remember material when we are exposed to it ini- 
tially affects how well we can later recall it. If we know we are going to need to 
recall material later, we are going to be more attentive to it. In contrast, if we don’t 
expect to need to recall material later, then we are less likely to remember it (Naveh- 
Benjamin et al., 2000; Kassam et al., 2009). 


Constructive Processes in 
Memory: Rebuilding the Past 


As we have seen, although it is clear that we can have detailed recollections of sig- 
nificant and distinctive events, it is difficult to gauge the accuracy of such memories. 
In fact, it is apparent that our memories reflect, at least in part, constructive processes, 
processes in which memories are influenced by the meaning we give to events. When 
we retrieve information, then, the memory that is produced is affected not just by 
the direct prior experience we have had with the stimulus, but also by our guesses 
and inferences about its meaning. 

The notion that memory is based on constructive processes was first put forward 
by Frederic Bartlett, a British psychologist. He suggested that people tend to remem- 
ber information in terms of schemas, organized bodies of information stored in 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 21 Recalling Long-Term Memories 227 


memory that bias the way new information is interpreted, stored, and recalled 
(Bartlett, 1932). Our reliance on schemas means that memories often consist of a 
general reconstruction of previous experience. Bartlett argued that schemas are based 
not only on the specific material to which people are exposed, but also on their 
understanding of the situation, their expectations about the situation, and their 
awareness of the motivations underlying the behavior of others. 

One of the earliest demonstrations of schemas came from a classic study that 
involved a procedure similar to the children’s game of “telephone,” in which infor- 
mation from memory is passed sequentially from one person to another. In the 
study, a participant viewed a drawing in which there were a variety of people of 
differing racial and ethnic backgrounds on a subway car, one of whom—a white 
person—was shown with a razor in his hand (Allport & Postman, 1958). The first 
participant was asked to describe the drawing to someone else without looking 
back at it. Then that person was asked to describe it to another person (without 
looking at the drawing), and then the process was repeated with still one more 
participant. 

The report of the last person differed in significant, yet systematic, ways from 
the initial drawing. Specifically, many people described the drawing as depicting an 
African American with a knife—an incorrect recollection, given that the drawing 
showed a razor in the hand of a Caucasian person. The transformation of the Cau- 
casian’s razor into an African American’s knife clearly indicates that the participants 
held a schema that included the unwarranted prejudice that African Americans are 
more violent than Caucasians and thus more apt to be holding a knife. In short, our 
expectations and knowledge—and prejudices—affect the reliability of our memories 
(McDonald & Hirt, 1997; Newby-Clark & Ross, 2003). 


MEMORY IN THE COURTROOM: 
THE EYEWITNESS OF TRIAL 


For Calvin Willis, the inadequate memories of two people cost him more than two 
decades of his life. Willis was the victim of mistaken identity when a young rape 
victim picked out his photo as the perpetrator of the rape. On that basis, he was tried, 
convicted, and sentenced to life in prison. Twenty-one years later, DNA testing showed 
that Willis was innocent, and the victim’s identification wrong (Corsello, 2005). 

Unfortunately, Willis is not the only victim to whom apologies have had to be 
made; many cases of mistaken identity have led to unjustified legal actions. Research 
on eyewitness identification of suspects, as well as on memory for other details of 
crimes, has shown that eyewitnesses are apt to make significant errors when they try 
to recall details of criminal activity—even if they are highly confident about their 
recollections (Thompson, 2000; Wells, Olson, & Charman, 2002; Zaragoza, Belli, & 
Payment, 2007). 

One reason is the impact of the weapons used in crimes. When a criminal per- 
petrator displays a gun or knife, it acts like a perceptual magnet, attracting the eyes 
of the witnesses. As a consequence, witnesses pay less attention to other details of 
the crime and are less able to recall what actually occurred (Steblay et al., 2003; 
Zaitsu, 2007; Pickel, 2009). 

One reason eyewitnesses are prone to memory-related errors is that the specific 
wording of questions posed to them by police officers or attorneys can affect the 
way they recall information, as a number of experiments illustrate. For example, 
in one experiment the participants were shown a film of two cars crashing into 
each other. Some were then asked the question, “About how fast were the cars 
going when they smashed into each other?” On average, they estimated the speed 
to be 40.8 miles per hour. In contrast, when another group of participants was 
asked, “About how fast were the cars going when they contacted each other?” the 
average estimated speed was only 31.8 miles per hour (Loftus & Palmer, 1974; see 
Figure 4). 


y Alert 


A key fact about memory is 
that it is a constructive pro- 
cess in which memories 
are influenced by the 
meaning given to what is 
being recalled. 


228 Chapter 7 Memory 


FIGURE 4 After viewing an accident 
involving two cars, the participants in a 
study were asked to estimate the speed 
of the two cars involved in the collision. 
Estimates varied substantially, depending 
on the way the question was worded. 
(Source: Loftus & Palmer, 1974.) 





Six years after being convicted of 
murder based on a so-called repressed 
memory of his daughter, George 
Franklin Sr’s conviction was overturned. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


About how fast were the cars going when they. each other? 





“Smashed into” 


“Collided with” 


“Bumped into” 


“Hit” 


“Contacted” 








0 10 20 30 40 50 


Estimated miles per hour 


Children’s Reliability. The problem of memory reliability becomes even more 
acute when children are witnesses because increasing evidence suggests that chil- 
dren’s memories are highly vulnerable to the influence of others (Loftus, 1993; 
Douglas, Goldstein, & Bjorklund, 2000). For instance, in one experiment, 5- to 
7-year-old girls who had just had a routine physical examination were shown an 
anatomically explicit doll. The girls were shown the doll’s genital area and asked, 
“Did the doctor touch you here?” Three of the girls who did not have a vaginal or 
anal exam said that the doctor had in fact touched them in the genital area, and 
one of those three made up the detail “The doctor did it with a stick” (Saywitz & 
Goodman, 1990). 

Children’s memories are especially susceptible to influence when the situation 
is highly emotional or stressful. For example, in trials in which there is significant 
pretrial publicity or in which alleged victims are questioned repeatedly, often by 
untrained interviewers, the memories of the alleged victims may be influenced by 
the types of questions they are asked (Scullin, Kanaya, & Ceci, 2002; Lamb & Gar- 
retson, 2003; Quas, Malloy, & Melinder, 2007; Goodman & Quas, 2008). 


Repressed and False Memories: Separating Truth from Fiction. Consider the case 
of George Franklin Sr., a man charged with murdering his daughter’s playmate. The 
entire case was based on memories of Franklin’s daughter, who claimed that she had 
repressed them until she began to have flashbacks of the event two decades later. 
Gradually, the memories became clearer until she recalled her father lifting a rock 
over his head and then seeing her friend covered with blood. On the basis of her 
memories, her father was convicted—but later was cleared of the crime after an 
appeal of the conviction. 

There is good reason to question the validity of repressed memories, recollections 
of events that are initially so shocking that the mind responds by pushing them into 
the unconscious. Supporters of the notion of repressed memory (based on Freud’s 
psychoanalytic theory) suggest that such memories may remain hidden, possibly 
throughout a person’s lifetime, unless they are triggered by some current circum- 
stance, such as the probing that occurs during psychological therapy. 

However, memory researcher Elizabeth Loftus maintains that so-called repressed 
memories may well be inaccurate or even wholly false—representing false memory. For 
example, false memories develop when people are unable to recall the source of a 
memory of a particular event about which they have only vague recollections. When 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 21 


the source of the memory becomes unclear or ambiguous, people may become con- 
fused about whether they actually experienced the event or whether it was imagined. 
Ultimately, people come to believe that the event actually occurred (Loftus, 2004; 
Wade, Sharman, & Garry, 2007; Bernstein & Loftus, 2009a). 

There is great controversy regarding the legitimacy of repressed memories. Many 
therapists give great weight to authenticity of repressed memories, and their views 
are supported by research showing that there are specific regions of the brain that 
help keep unwanted memories out of awareness. On the other side of the issue are 
researchers who maintain that there is insufficient scientific support for the existence 
of such memories. There is also a middle ground: memory researchers who suggest 
that false memories are a result of normal information processing. The challenge for 
those on all sides of the issue is to distinguish truth from fiction (Brown & Pope, 
1996; Strange, Clifasefi, & Garry, 2007; Bernstein & Loftus, 2009b). 


AUTOBIOGRAPHICAL MEMORY: WHERE PAST MEETS PRESENT 


Your memory of experiences in your own past may well be a fiction—or at least a 
distortion of what actually occurred. The same constructive processes that make us 
inaccurately recall the behavior of others also reduce the accuracy of autobiographi- 
cal memories. Autobiographical memories are our recollections of circumstances and 
episodes from our own lives. Autobiographical memories encompass the episodic 
memories we hold about ourselves (Rubin, 1999; Sutin & Robins, 2007). 

For example, we tend to forget information about our past that is incompatible 
with the way in which we currently see ourselves. One study found that adults who 
were well adjusted but who had been treated for emotional problems during the 
early years of their lives tended to forget important but troubling childhood events, 
such as being in foster care. College students misremember their bad grades—but 
remember their good ones (see Figure 5; Walker, Skowronski, & Thompson, 2003; 
Kemps & Tiggemann, 2007). 

Similarly, when a group of 48-year-olds were asked to recall how they had 
responded on a questionnaire they had completed when they were high school fresh- 
man, their accuracy was no better than chance. For example, although 61% of the 
questionnaire respondents said that playing sports and other physical activities was 
their favorite pastime, only 23% of the adults recalled it accurately (Offer et al., 2000). 

It is not just certain kinds of events that are distorted; particular periods of life 
are remembered more easily than others. For example, when people reach late adult- 
hood, they remember periods of life in which they experienced major transitions, 
such as attending college and working at their first job, better than they remember 
their middle-age years. Similarly, although most adults’ earliest memories of their 
own lives are of events that occurred when they were toddlers, toddlers show evi- 
dence of recall of events that occurred when they were as young as 6 months old 
(Simcock & Hayne, 2002; Wang, 2003; Cordnoldi, De Beni, & Helstrup, 2007). 


Travelers who have visited areas of the world in which there is 
no written language often have returned with tales of people 
with phenomenal memories. For instance, storytellers in some 
preliterate cultures can recount long chronicles that recall the 
names and activities of people over many generations. Those 
feats led experts to argue initially that people in preliterate 
societies develop a different, and perhaps better, type of memory 
than do those in cultures that employ a written language. They suggested that in a 
society that lacks writing, people are motivated to recall information with accuracy, 


in Memory? 


especially information relating to tribal histories and traditions that would be lost if 


they were not passed down orally from one generation to another (Daftary & Meri, 
2002; Berntsen & Rubin, 2004). 


Exploring eens a 


Are There Cross-Cultural Differences 


Recalling Long-Term Memories 229 


100 
90 
80 
70 
60 
50 


Percentage of grades recalled accurately 








A B C D 
Original grade assigned 


FIGURE 5 We tend to distort memories 
of unpleasant events. For example, 
college students are much more likely to 
accurately recall their good grades while 
inaccurately recalling their poor ones 
(Bahrick et al., 1996). Now that you 
know this, how well do you think you 
can recall your high school grades? 


autobiographical memories Our 
recollections of circumstances and 
episodes from our own lives. 





230 Chapter 7 Memory 


Storytellers in many cultures can 
recount hundreds of years of history in 
vivid detail. Research has found that this 
amazing ability is due less to basic 
memory processes than to the ways in 
which they acquire and retain 
information. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Today, memory researchers dismiss that view. For one thing, preliterate peoples 
don’t have an exclusive claim to amazing memory feats. Some Hebrew scholars 
memorize thousands of pages of text and can recall the locations of particular words 
on the page. Similarly, poetry singers in the Balkans can recall thousands of lines of 
poetry. Even in cultures in which written language exists, then, astounding feats of 
memory are possible (Strathern & Stewart, 2003; Rubin et al., 2007). 

Memory researchers now suggest that there are both similarities and differences in 
memory across cultures. Basic memory processes such as short-term memory capacity 
and the structure of long-term memory—the “hardware” of memory—are universal 
and operate similarly in people in all cultures. In contrast, cultural differences can be 
seen in the way information is acquired and rehearsed—the “software” of memory. 
Culture determines how people frame information initially, how much they practice 
learning and recalling it, and the strategies they use to try to recall it (Mack, 2003; Wang 
& Conway, 2006; Rubin et al., 2007). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 21 Recalling Long-Term Memories 231 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 


What causes difficulties and failures in remembering? 


The tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon is the temporary 
inability to remember information that one is certain one 
knows. Retrieval cues are a major strategy for recalling 
information successfully. (p. 222) 

The levels-of-processing approach to memory suggests 
that the way in which information is initially perceived 
and analyzed determines the success with which it is 
recalled. The deeper the initial processing, the greater the 
recall. (p. 223) 

Explicit memory refers to intentional or conscious 
recollection of information. In contrast, implicit memory 
refers to memories of which people are not consciously 
aware but that can affect subsequent performance and 
behavior. (p. 224) 

Flashbulb memories are memories centered on a specific, 
important event. The more distinctive a memory is, the 
more easily it can be retrieved. (p. 225) 

Memory is a constructive process: We relate memories to 
the meaning, guesses, and expectations we give to 
events. Specific information is recalled in terms of 
schemas, organized bodies of information stored in 
memory that bias the way new information is 
interpreted, stored, and recalled. (p. 226) 

Eyewitnesses are apt to make substantial errors when 
they try to recall the details of crimes. The problem of 
memory reliability becomes even more acute when the 
witnesses are children. (p. 227) 

Autobiographical memory is influenced by constructive 
processes. (p. 229) 


KEY TERMS 


tip-of-the-tongue 
phenomenon p. 222 

recall p. 222 

recognition p. 222 


levels-of-processing 
theory p. 223 

explicit memory p. 224 

implicit memory p. 224 


EVALUATE 


1. 


While with a group of friends at a dance, Eva bumps into 
a man she dated last month. But when she tries to intro- 
duce him to her friends, she cannot remember his name. 
What is the term for this occurrence? 

is the process of retrieving a specific item from 
memory. 


. A friend tells you, “I know exactly where I was and what I 


was doing when I heard that Michael Jackson died.” What 
is this type of memory phenomenon called? 

theory states that the 
more a person analyzes a statement, the more likely he or 
she is to remember it later. 





RETHINK 


1. 


Research shows that an eyewitness’s memory for details 
of crimes can contain significant errors. How might a 
lawyer use this information when evaluating an 
eyewitness’s testimony? Should eyewitness accounts 

be permissible in a court of law? 


. From a social worker's perspective: Should a child victim of 


sexual abuse be allowed to testify in court, based on what 
you've learned about children’s memories under stress? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


priming p. 224 
flashbulb memories p. 225 
constructive 


Sutssa001d-Jo-s[aagy p 
{Arowa qmqysep 'g e17 ‘uousutousyd an8uo0}j-sy}-jo-dy *T 


schemas p. 226 
autobiographical 

memories p. 229 
processes p. 226 


Why do we forget information? 


What are the major memory 
impairments? 


FIGURE 1 In his classic work, Ebbinghaus 
found that the most rapid forgetting 
occurs in the first nine hours after 
exposure to new material. However, the 
rate of forgetting then slows down and 
declines very little even after many days 
have passed (Ebbinghaus, 1885, 1913). 
Check your own memory: What were you 
doing exactly two hours ago? What were 
you doing last Tuesday at 5 p.m.? Which 
information is easier to retrieve? 


232 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Known in the scientific literature by the alias of H.M., he could remember, quite 
literally, nothing—nothing, that is, that had happened since the loss of his brain’s 
temporal lobes and hippocampus during experimental surgery to reduce epileptic 
seizures. Until that time, H.M.’s memory had been quite normal. But after the operation 
he was unable to recall anything for more than a few minutes, and then the memory 
was seemingly lost forever. He did not remember his address, or the name of the 
person to whom he was talking. H.M. would read the same magazine over and over 
again. According to his own description, his life was like waking from a dream and 
being unable to know where he was or how he got there. (Milner, 1966, 2005) 


As the case of H.M. illustrates, a person without a normal memory faces severe dif- 
ficulties. All of us who have experienced even routine instances of forgetting—such 
as not remembering an acquaintance’s name or a fact on a test—understand the very 
real consequences of memory failure. 

Of course, memory failure is also essential to remembering important informa- 
tion. The ability to forget inconsequential details about experiences, people, and 
objects helps us avoid being burdened and distracted by trivial stores of meaningless 
data. Forgetting permits us to form general impressions and recollections. For exam- 
ple, the reason our friends consistently look familiar to us is because we're able to 
forget their clothing, facial blemishes, and other transient features that change from 
one occasion to the next. Instead, our memories are based on a summary of various 
critical features—a far more economical use of our memory capabilities. 

The first attempts to study forgetting were made by German psychologist 
Hermann Ebbinghaus about a hundred years ago. Using himself as the only 
participant in his study, Ebbinghaus memorized lists of three-letter nonsense 
syllables—meaningless sets of two consonants with a vowel in between, such as 
FIW and BOZ. By measuring how easy it was to relearn a given list of words after 
varying periods of time had passed since the initial learning, he found that forget- 
ting occurred systematically, as shown in Figure 1. As the figure indicates, the most 








109 Immediate recall 
6 80 
5 
[= e 
S w 
ey 
60 HE 20 minutes ] ] 
o | hour g 
= 
g 40 9 hours | 

20 

0 2 4 6 8 10 15 20 25 31 


Elapsed time (days) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 22 Forgetting: When Memory Fails 233 


rapid forgetting occurs in the first nine hours, particularly in the first hour. After 
nine hours, the rate of forgetting slows and declines little, even after the passage 
of many days. 

Despite his primitive methods, Ebbinghaus’s study had an important influence 
on subsequent research, and his basic conclusions have been upheld. There is almost 
always a strong initial decline in memory, followed by a more gradual drop over 
time. Furthermore, relearning of previously mastered material is almost always faster 
than starting from scratch, whether the material is academic information or a motor 
skill such as serving a tennis ball (Wixted & Carpenter, 2007). 


Why We Forget 


Why do we forget? One reason is that we may not have paid attention to the mate- 
rial in the first place—a failure of encoding. For example, if you live in the United 
States, you probably have been exposed to thousands of pennies during your life. 
Despite this experience, you probably don’t have a clear sense of the details of the 
coin. (See this for yourself by looking at Figure 2.) Consequently, the reason for your 
memory failure is that you probably never encoded the information into long-term 
memory initially. Obviously, if information was not placed in memory to start with, 
there is no way the information can be recalled. 

But what about material that has been encoded into memory and that can’t later 
be remembered? Several processes account for memory failures, including decay, 
interference, and cue-dependent forgetting. 

Decay is the loss of information through nonuse. This explanation for forgetting 
assumes that memory traces, the physical changes that take place in the brain when 
new material is learned, simply fade away over time (Grann, 2007). 

Although there is evidence that decay does occur, this does not seem to be the 
complete explanation for forgetting. Often there is no relationship between how long 
ago a person was exposed to information and how well that information is recalled. 
If decay explained all forgetting, we would expect that the more time that has 
elapsed between the initial learning of information and our attempt to recall it, the 
harder it would be to remember it because there would be more time for the mem- 
ory trace to decay. Yet people who take several consecutive tests on the same 








decay The loss of information in 
memory through its nonuse. 


FIGURE 2 One of these pennies is the 
real thing. Can you find it? Why is this 
task harder than it seems at first? 
(Source: Nickerson & Adams, 1979.) 
Vy S! JƏMSUE 7291102 BY} 
‘Apuey Auuad e əney 3,U0p Nod y| 


234 Chapter 7 Memory 


© Frank Cotham/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


All Rights Reserved. 


interference The phenomenon by 
which information in memory disrupts 
the recall of other information. 


cue-dependent forgetting Forgetting 
that occurs when there are insufficient 
retrieval cues to rekindle information 
that is in memory. 


y Alert 


Memory loss through decay 
comes from nonuse of the 
memory; memory loss 
through interference is 
due to the presence of 
other information in memory. 


proactive interference Interference in 
which information learned earlier 
disrupts the recall of newer material. 


retroactive interference Interference 
in which there is difficulty in the recall 
of information learned earlier because 
of later exposure to different material. 





“He must have forgotten something.” 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


material often recall more of the initial information 
when taking later tests than they did on earlier tests. 
If decay were operating, we would expect the opposite 
to occur (Payne, 1986). 

Because decay does not fully account for forget- 
ting, memory specialists have proposed an additional 
mechanism: interference. In interference, information 
in memory disrupts the recall of other information 
(Naveh-Benjamin, Guez, & Sorek, 2007; Pilotti, 
Chodorow, & Shono, 2009). 

To distinguish between decay and interference, 
think of the two processes in terms of a row of books 
on a library shelf. In decay, the old books are constantly 
crumbling and rotting away, leaving room for new 
arrivals. Interference processes suggest that new books 
knock the old ones off the shelf, where they become 
inaccessible. 

Finally, forgetting may occur because of cue- 
dependent forgetting, forgetting that occurs when 
there are insufficient retrieval cues to rekindle infor- 
mation that is in memory (Tulving & Thompson, 1983). For example, you may not 
be able to remember where you lost a set of keys until you mentally walk through 
your day, thinking of each place you visited. When you think of the place where 
you lost the keys—say, the library—the retrieval cue of the library may be sufficient 
to help you recall that you left them on the desk in the library. Without that retrieval 
cue, you may be unable to recall the location of the keys. 

Most research suggests that interference and cue-dependent forgetting are key 
processes in forgetting (Mel’nikov, 1993; Bower, Thompson, & Tulving, 1994). We 
forget things mainly because new memories interfere with the retrieval of old ones 
or because appropriate retrieval cues are unavailable, not because the memory trace 
has decayed. 


Proactive and Retroactive 
Interference: The Before 
and After of Forgetting 


There are actually two sorts of interference that influence forgetting: proactive and 
retroactive. In proactive interference, information learned earlier disrupts the recall 
of newer material. Suppose, as a student of foreign languages, you first learned 
French in the 10th grade, and then in the 11th grade you took Spanish. When in the 
12th grade you take a college achievement test in Spanish, you may find you have 
difficulty recalling the Spanish translation of a word because all you can think of is 
its French equivalent (Bunting, 2006). 

In contrast, retroactive interference refers to difficulty in the recall of informa- 
tion because of later exposure to different material. If, for example, you have diffi- 
culty on a French achievement test because of your more recent exposure to Spanish, 
retroactive interference is the culprit (see Figure 3). One way to remember the differ- 
ence between proactive and retroactive interference is to keep in mind that proactive 
interference progresses in time—the past interferes with the present—whereas retro- 
active interference retrogresses in time, working backward as the present interferes 
with the past (Jacoby et al., 2007). 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 22 Forgetting: When Memory Fails 235 


Proactive interference: 
Spanish test performance 
impaired by study of French 


= 





Time 


= 





Retroactive interference: 
French test performance 
impaired by study of Spanish 


= 


Time 


Although the concepts of proactive and retroactive interference illustrate how 
material may be forgotten, they still do not explain whether forgetting is caused by 
the actual loss or modification of information or by problems in the retrieval of 
information. Most research suggests that material that has apparently been lost 
because of interference can eventually be recalled if appropriate stimuli are presented 
(Tulving & Psotka, 1971; Anderson, 1981), but the question has not been fully 
answered. 


Memory Dysfunctions: 
Afflictions of Forgetting 





First you notice that you’re always misplacing things or that common nouns are 
evading you as stubbornly as the names of new acquaintances. Pretty soon you're 
forgetting appointments and getting flustered when you drive in traffic. On bad days 
you find you can’t hold numbers in your mind long enough to dial the phone. You try 
valiantly to conceal your lapses, but they become ever more glaring. You crash your car. 
You spend whole mornings struggling to dress yourself properly. And even as you lose 
the ability to read or play the piano, you’re painfully aware of what's happening to 
you. (Cowley, 2000, p. 46) 


These memory problems are symptomatic of Alzheimer’s disease, an illness 
characterized in part by severe memory problems. Alzheimer’s is the fourth leading 
cause of death among adults in the United States, affecting an estimated 5 million 
people. 

In the beginning, Alzheimer’s symptoms appear as simple forgetfulness of things 
such as appointments and birthdays. As the disease progresses, memory loss becomes 
more profound, and even the simplest tasks—such as using a telephone—are forgot- 
ten. Ultimately, victims may lose their ability to speak or comprehend language, and 
physical deterioration sets in, leading to death. 

The causes of Alzheimer’s disease are not fully understood. Increasing evidence 
suggests that Alzheimer’s results from an inherited susceptibility to a defect in the 
production of the protein beta amyloid, which is necessary for the maintenance of 
nerve cell connections. When the synthesis of beta amyloid goes awry, large clumps 
of cells form, triggering inflammation and the deterioration of nerve cells in the brain 
(Selkoe, 2002; Detoledo-Morrell, Stoub, & Wang, 2007; Horinek, Varjassyova, & Hort, 
2007; Selkoe, 2008; see Figure 4). 


FIGURE 3 Proactive interference occurs 
when material learned earlier interferes 
with the recall of newer material. In this 
example, studying French before studying 
Spanish interferes with performance on 
a Spanish test. In contrast, retroactive 
interference exists when material learned 
after initial exposure to other material 
interferes with the recall of the first 
material. In this case, retroactive inter- 
ference occurs when recall of French is 
impaired because of later exposure to 
Spanish. 


Alzheimer’s disease An illness 
characterized in part by severe 
memory problems. 


236 Chapter 7 Memory 


(a) 


Year | 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Alzheimer’s Disease 


and Brain Deterioration 





FIGURE 4 Alzheimer’s disease, which is the fourth leading cause of death among adults 
in the United States, is characterized by significant memory declines. Here a man with 
Alzheimer’s was followed for three years to learn how his brain processed autobiographical 
memories as the disease progressed. (a) While in the first year, the areas of the brain used 
for long-term memory, such as the hippocampus and surrounding temporal lobe (shown 
in yellow, orange, and red), are activated similarly to healthy adults. (b) In the second 
year, as his abilities began to decline, other areas of the brain began to compensate by 
increasing the levels of activity in surrounding areas. Finally, in the third year when his 
abilities decreased substantially, other areas of the brain were no longer able to compen- 
sate and activity decreased overall. On the other hand, some areas of the brain—such as 
those involved in visual processing—did not change over the three-year period, illustrat- 
ing that not all areas of the brain are affected by the disease. (Source: Maguire et al., 2010.) 


(b) (c) 





Areas involved in visual processing that do not change over time 


amnesia Memory loss that occurs 
without other mental difficulties. 


retrograde amnesia Amnesia in which 
memory is lost for occurrences prior to 
a certain event. 


anterograde amnesia Amnesia in 
which memory is lost for events that 
follow an injury. 


Korsakoff’s syndrome A disease that 
afflicts long-term alcoholics, leaving 
some abilities intact but including 
hallucinations and a tendency to 
repeat the same story. 


Alzheimer’s disease is one of a number of memory dysfunctions. Another is 
amnesia, memory loss that occurs without other mental difficulties. The type of 
amnesia immortalized in countless Hollywood films involves a victim who receives 
a blow to the head and is unable to remember anything from his or her past. In 
reality, amnesia of this type, known as retrograde amnesia, is quite rare. In retrograde 
amnesia, memory is lost for occurrences prior to a certain event. Usually, lost mem- 
ories gradually reappear, although full restoration may take as long as several years. 
In certain cases, some memories are lost forever. But even in cases of severe memory 
loss, the loss is generally selective. For example, although people suffering from 
retrograde amnesia may be unable to recall friends and family members, they still 
may be able to play complicated card games or knit a sweater quite well (Verfaellie 
& Keane, 2002; Bright, Buckman, & Fradera, 2006). 

A second type of amnesia is exemplified by people who remember nothing of 
their current activities. In anterograde amnesia loss of memory occurs for events that 
follow an injury. Information cannot be transferred from short-term to long-term 
memory, resulting in the inability to remember anything other than what was in 
long-term storage before the accident (Gilboa, Winocur, & Rosenbaum, 2006). 

Amnesia is also a result of Korsakoff’s syndrome, a disease that afflicts long- 
term alcoholics. Although many of their intellectual abilities may be intact, Korsa- 
koff’s sufferers display a strange array of symptoms, including hallucinations and a 
tendency to repeat the same story over and over (van Oort & Kessels, 2009). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 22 Forgetting: When Memory Fails 


Fortunately, most of us have intact memory, and the occasional failures we suffer y Alert 
may actually be preferable to having a perfect memory. Consider, for instance, the 
case of a man who had total recall. After reading passages of Dante’s The Divine 
Comedy in Italian—a language he did not speak—he was able to repeat them from 
memory some 15 years later. He could memorize lists of 50 unrelated words and 
recall them at will more than a decade later. He could even repeat the same list of 
words backward, if asked (Luria, 1968). 

Such a skill at first may seem to be enviable, but it actually presented quite a 
problem. The man’s memory became a jumble of lists of words, numbers, and names; 
when he tried to relax, his mind was filled with images. Even reading was difficult 
since every word evoked a flood of thoughts from the past that interfered with his 
ability to understand the meaning of what he was reading. Partially as a consequence 
of the man’s unusual memory, psychologist A. R. Luria, who studied his case, found 
him to be a “disorganized and rather dull-witted person” (Luria, 1968, p. 65). We 
might be grateful, then, that forgetfulness plays a role in our lives. 


relatively rare. 


Apart from the advantages of forgetting, say, a bad date, most BECOMING AN 
of us would like to find ways to improve our memories. Among INFORMED CONSUMER 
the effective strategies for studying and remembering course 


Kea of Psychology 


e Use the keyword technique. If you are studying a foreign Improving Your Memory 
language, try the keyword technique of pairing a foreign 
word with a common English word that has a similar 
sound. This English word is known as the keyword. For example, to learn the 
Spanish word for duck (pato, pronounced pot-o), you might choose the keyword 
pot; for the Spanish word for horse (caballo, pronounced cob-eye-yo), the keyword 
might be eye. Once you have thought of a keyword, imagine the Spanish word 
“interacting” with the English keyword. You might envision a duck taking a bath 
in a pot to remember the word pato or a horse with a large, bulging eye in the 
center of its head to recall caballo (Carney & Levin, 1998; Wyra, Lawson, & Hungi, 
2007). 

e Rely on organization cues. Recall material you read in textbooks by organizing the 
material in memory the first time you read it. Organize your reading on the basis 
of any advance information you have about the content and about its arrange- 
ment. You will then be able to make connections and see relationships among the 
various facts and process the material at a deeper level, which in turn will later 
aid recall. 

e Take effective notes. “Less is more” is perhaps the best advice for taking lecture 
notes that facilitate recall. Rather than trying to jot down every detail of a lecture, 
it is better to listen and think about the material, and take down the main points. 
In effective note taking, thinking about the material when you first hear it is more 
important than writing it down. This is one reason that borrowing someone else’s 
notes is a bad idea; you will have no framework in memory that you can use to 
understand them (Feldman, 2010). 

e Practice and rehearse. Although practice does not necessarily make perfect, it 
helps. By studying and rehearsing material past initial mastery—a process called 
overlearning—people are able to show better long-term recall than they show if 
they stop practicing after their initial learning of the material. 

e Don't believe claims about drugs that improve memory. Advertisements for One-a- 
Day vitamins with ginkgo biloba or Quanterra Mental Sharpness Product would 
have you believe that taking a drug or supplement can improve your memory. 
Not so, according to the results of numerous studies. No research has shown that 
commercial memory enhancers are effective (Gold, Cahill, & Wenk, 2002; McDan- 
iel, Maier, & Einstein, 2002; Burns, Bryan, & Nettelbeck, 2006). 


237 


Except for Alzheimer’s dis- 
ease, memory disorders are 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


238 Chapter 7 Memory 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP retrieving material as a result of the interference of 
previously learned material. 
4. Match the following memory disorders with the correct 
information: 
1. Affects alcoholics; may 
result in hallucinations. 
What are the major memory impairments? 2. Memory loss occurring 
e Among the memory dysfunctions are Alzheimer’s without other mental 
disease, which leads to a progressive loss of memory, problems. 
and amnesia, a memory loss that occurs without other 3. Beta amyloid defect; 
mental difficulties and can take the forms of retrograde progressive forgetting 
amnesia and anterograde amnesia. Korsakoff’s syn- and physical deterioration. 
drome is a disease that afflicts long-term alcoholics, 
resulting in memory impairment. (p. 235) 
e Among the techniques for improving memory are the 
keyword technique to memorize foreign language vo- 1. 


Why do we forget information? 
e Several processes account for memory failure, including 
decay, interference (both proactive and retroactive), and 


. Alzheimer’s di 
cue-dependent forgetting. (p. 233) cele ed eee 


b. Korsakoff’s syndrome 
c. Amnesia 


RETHINK 


What are the implications of proactive and retroactive 


cabulary; using the encoding specificity phenomenon; 
organizing text material and lecture notes; and practice 
and rehearsal, leading to overlearning. (p. 237) 


interference for learning multiple foreign languages? 
Would earlier language training in a different language 
help or hinder learning a new language? 


2. From a healthcare provider’s perspective: Alzheimer’s disease 
and amnesia are two of the most pervasive memory 
dysfunctions that threaten many individuals. What sorts 
of activities might health care providers offer their 
patients to help them combat their memory loss? 


EVALUATE 


1. If, after learning the history of the Middle East for a class 
two years ago, you now find yourself unable to recall 
what you learned, you are experiencing memory 

, caused by nonuse. 

2. Difficulty in accessing a memory because of the presence 
of other information is known as : 

3, _____ interference occurs when material is difficult 
to retrieve because of subsequent exposure to other 
material; interference refers to difficulty in 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 
e-€ 0-7 ‘G-T p faaovord APLI tE faouaresIoyut Z (ÁLP T 


KEY TERMS 


decay p. 233 proactive Alzheimer’s disease p. 235 anterograde amnesia p. 236 
interference p. 234 interference p. 234 amnesia p. 236 Korsakoff’s 
cue-dependent retroactive retrograde amnesia p. 236 syndrome p. 236 


forgetting p. 234 interference p. 234 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Psychology on the Web 


1. The study of repressed memories can lead down unusual pathways—even more 
unusual than the criminal investigation pathway. Two other areas in which repressed 
memories play a large part are alien abduction and reincarnation. Find two sources 
on the web that deal with one of these issues—one supportive and one skeptical. 
Read what they say and relate it to your knowledge of memory. Summarize your 
findings and indicate which side of the controversy your study of memory leads you 
to favor. 

2. Memory is a topic of serious interest to psychologists, but it is also a source of amuse- 
ment. Find a website that focuses on the amusing side of memory such as memory 
games, tests of recall, or lists of mnemonics. Hint: There’s even a mnemonics genera- 
tor out there! Write down the addresses of any interesting sites that you encounter 
and summarize what you found. 


E O | log U Q Our examination of memory has highlighted the 


processes of encoding, storage, and retrieval, and 
theories about how these processes occur. We also encountered several phenomena 
relating to memory, including the tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon and flashbulb memo- 
ries. Above all, we observed that memory is a constructive process by which interpreta- 
tions, expectations, and guesses contribute to the nature of our memories. 

Before moving on to the next chapter, return to the prologue on Jill Price's perfect 
memory for events in her life. Consider the following questions in light of what you now 
know about memory. 





1. How might you explain why Price can remember trivial life events from years ago but 
keeps forgetting to return a library book? 

2. How might Price’s unusual memory be useful to her? How might it be detrimental to 
her? 

3. What might Price's skills suggest about the nature of memory and forgetting? 

4. From a researcher's point of view, what would you do to ascertain that Price's memo- 
ries are truly accurate? 





239 








CHAPTER 8 


Cognition and Language 











www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Key Concepts for Chapter 8 


MODULE 23 








What is thinking? @ What processes underlie Thinking and Reasoning 


: ror x Mental Images: Examining the Mind's Eye 
reasoning and decision making? 
Concepts: Categorizing the World 


Neuroscience in Your Life: How We Learn 
to Categorize Our World Using Concepts 


Reasoning: Making Up Your Mind 


Computers and Problem Solving: 
Searching for Artificial Intelligence 





MODULE 24 






How do people approach and solve problems? Problem Solving 
Preparation: Understanding 


and Diagnosing Problems 


solving? @ What is creativity? Production: Generating Solutions 
Judgment: Evaluating Solutions 


@ What are the major obstacles to problem 


Applying Psychology in the 21st Century: 
The Thoughts Behind Avoiding Danger: 
When Hunches Save Lives 


Impediments to Solutions: Why Is 
Problem Solving Such a Problem? 


Creativity and Problem Solving 


Becoming an Informed Consumer 
of Psychology: Thinking 
Critically and Creatively 


MODULE 25 





How do people use language? @ How does Language 


Grammar: Language’s Language 


language develop? 


Language Development: 
Developing a Way with Words 


The Influence of Language 
on Thinking: Do Eskimos Have More 
Words for Snow than Texans Do? 


Do Animals Use Language? 
Exploring Diversity: Teaching with 
Linguistic Variety: Bilingual Education 
Neuroscience in Your Life: 
Bilingualism and the Brain 


241 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Prologue Eureka! 





Clifford Matson’s “Eureka!” moment arrived the day the silverfish 
invaded his bathroom. 

Dr. Matson, tall, white-haired and retired after 50 years of 
practicing dentistry in his hometown of Junction City, Oregon, 
was fretting about the insects skittering around the bathroom. 
Then several seemingly unrelated thoughts collided in his mind. 

One was about the pesky silverfish. One concerned a book he 
was reading about neem trees, tropical trees grown in India and 
Burma that have seeds with their own natural pesticides. The 
third thought arrived when Dr. Matson noticed the small cork 
squares separating the double-pane windows in the bathroom. 


A couple of drops of neem oil on one of those little cork 
squares ought to be just the ticket to get rid of silverfish, 
Dr. Matson thought. 

He tried it, and the silverfish died. Then he tried the neem 
oil-soaked cork squares on cockroaches, and they bit the dust as 
well. 

Two years ago the U.S. Patent and Trademark Office granted 
Dr. Matson Patent No. 6,093,413 for Cork-EZ, an adhesive-backed 
piece of cork the size of a Scrabble square that delivers a natural 
pesticide derived from cedar bark. (Richardson, 2002, p. R7). 





Clifford Matson has big plans for his invention. But whether or 
not Cork-EZ revolutionizes the extermination field, it is clear that 
Matson has that elusive quality that marks successful inventors: 
creativity. 

Where does Matson’s creativity come from? More generally, 
how do people use information to devise innovative solutions to 
problems? And how do people think about, understand, and, 
through language, describe the world? 

Answers to these questions come from cognitive psychology, 
the branch of psychology that focuses on the study of higher 
mental processes, including thinking, language, memory, 
problem solving, knowing, reasoning, judging, and decision 
making. Clearly, the realm of cognitive psychology is broad. 


242 


Cognitive psychology centers on three major topics: thinking 
and reasoning, problem solving and creativity, and language. The 
first topic we consider in this chapter is thinking and reasoning. 
Then we examine different strategies for approaching problems, 
means of generating solutions, and ways of making judgments 
about the usefulness and accuracy of solutions. Finally, we discuss 
how language is developed and acquired, its basic characteristics, 
and the relationship between language and thought. 


cognitive psychology The branch of psychology that focuses on the 
study of higher mental processes, including thinking, language, 
memory, problem solving, knowing, reasoning, judging, and decision 
making. 


Thinking and Reasoning 





What are you thinking about at this moment? Key Concepts 

The mere ability to pose such a question underscores the distinctive nature of What is thinking? 
the human ability to think. No other species contemplates, analyzes, recollects, or 
plans the way humans do. Understanding what thinking is, however, goes beyond What processes underlie 
knowing that we think. Philosophers, for example, have argued for generations about reasoning and decision 
the meaning of thinking, with some placing it at the core of human beings’ under- mns 
standing of their own existence. making? 

Psychologists define thinking as the manipulation of mental representations of 
information. A representation may take the form of a word, a visual image, a sound, thinking The manipulation of mental 
or data in any other sensory modality stores in memory. Thinking transforms a par- representations of information. 
ticular representation of information into new and different forms, allowing us to 
answer questions, solve problems, or reach goals. 

Although a clear sense of what specifically occurs when we think remains elu- 
sive, our understanding of the nature of the fundamental elements involved in think- 
ing is growing. We begin by considering our use of mental images and concepts, the 
building blocks of thought. 





“What do you think I think about what you think 
I think you’ve been thinking about?” 


© Michael Maslin/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


243 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


244 Chapter 8 Cognition and Language 


Mental Images: 
Examining the Mind's Eye 


Think of your best friend. 

Chances are that you “see” some kind of visual image when asked to think of 
her or him, or any other person or object, for that matter. To some cognitive psy- 
chologists, such mental images constitute a major part of thinking. 

mental images Representations in the Mental images are representations in the mind of an object or event. They are 
mind of an object or event. not just visual representations; our ability to “hear” a tune in our heads also relies 
on a mental image. In fact, every sensory modality may produce corresponding men- 
tal images (Kosslyn, 2005; De Beni, Pazzaglia, & Gardini, 2007; Gardini et al., 2009). 

Research has found that our mental images have many of the properties of the 
actual stimuli they represent. For example, it takes the mind longer to scan mental 
images of large objects than small ones, just as the eye takes longer to scan an actual 
large object than an actual small one. Similarly, we are able to manipulate and rotate 
mental images of objects, just as we are able to manipulate and rotate them in the real 
world (Mast & Kosslyn, 2002; Iachini & Giusberti, 2004; Zacks, 2008; see Figure 1). 

Some experts see the production of mental images as a way to improve various 
skills. For instance, many athletes use mental imagery in their training. Basketball 









TA 
as 









FIGURE 1 Try to mentally rotate one of each pair of patterns to see if it is the same as the 
other member of that pair. It’s likely that the farther you have to mentally rotate a pattern, 
the longer it will take to decide if the patterns match one another. Does this mean that it will 
take you longer to visualize a map of the world than a map of the United States? Why or why 


not? (Source: From Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three-dimensional objects. 
Science, 171, no 3972, 701-703 (Figure 1, p. 702). Reprinted with permission from AAAS.) 





(a) 
(b) 
(c) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 23 Thinking and Reasoning 245 


players may try to produce vivid and detailed images of the court, the basket, the 
ball, and the noisy crowd. They may visualize themselves taking a foul shot, watch- 
ing the ball, and hearing the swish as it goes through the net. And it works: The use 
of mental imagery can lead to improved performance in sports (Mamassis & Doganis, 
2004; Fournier, Deremaux, & Bernier, 2008; Moran, 2009). 

Mental imagery may improve other types of skills as well. For example, piano 
players who simply mentally rehearse an exercise show brain activity that is virtually 
identical to that of the people who actually practice the exercise manually. Appar- 
ently, carrying out the task involved the same network of brain cells as the network 
used in mentally rehearsing it (Pascual-Leone et al., 1995; Kensinger & Schacter, 2006; 
Sanders et al., 2008). 





Concepts: Categorizing the World 


If someone asks you what is in your kitchen cabinet, you might answer with a 
detailed list of items (a jar of peanut butter, three boxes of macaroni and cheese, six 
unmatched dinner plates, and so forth). More likely, though, you would respond by 
naming some broader categories, such as “food” and “dishes.” 

Using such categories reflects the operation of concepts. Concepts are mental 
groupings of similar objects, events, or people. Concepts enable us to organize com- 
plex phenomena into simpler, and therefore more easily usable, cognitive categories 
(Goldstone & Kersten, 2003; Murphy, 2005; Connolly, 2007). 

Concepts help us classify newly encountered objects on the basis of our past 
experience. For example, we can surmise that someone tapping a handheld screen is 
probably using some kind of computer or PDA, even if we have never encountered 
that specific model before. Ultimately, concepts influence behavior. We would assume, 
for instance, that it might be appropriate to pet an animal after determining that it 
is a dog, whereas we would behave differently after classifying the animal as a wolf. 

When cognitive psychologists first studied concepts, they focused on those that 
were clearly defined by a unique set of properties or features. For example, an equilat- 
eral triangle is a closed shape that has three sides of equal length. If an object has these 
characteristics, it is an equilateral triangle; if it does not, it is not an equilateral triangle. 

Other concepts—often those with the most relevance to our everyday lives— 
are more ambiguous and difficult to define. For instance, broader concepts such as 
“table” and “bird” have a set of general, relatively loose characteristic features, rather 
than unique, clearly defined properties that distinguish an example of the concept 
from a nonexample. When we consider these more ambiguous concepts, we usually 
think in terms of examples called prototypes. Prototypes are typical, highly repre- 
sentative examples of a concept that correspond to our mental image or best example 
of the concept. For instance, although a robin and an ostrich are both examples of 





How do you view these structures? Whether you categorize them as two houses of worship 
(left and middle), as two similar examples of architecture (middle and right) or simply as three 
buildings, you are using concepts. 





Many athletes use mental imagery to 
focus on a task, a process they call 
“getting in the zone.’ What are some 
other occupations that require the use 
of strong mental imagery? 


concepts A mental grouping of similar 
objects, events, or people. 


prototypes Typical, highly representa- 
tive examples of a concept. 





246 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 8 Cognition and Language 


How We Learn 


to Categorize Our World Using Concepts 





FIGURE 2 To illustrate how the brain learns to categorize objects using concepts, participants 
in a study were trained to identify specific models of a car, such as Toyotas versus Hondas 
(labeled “subordinate” categories in the figure). Others were trained to identify them as general 
categories, such as SUVs versus sedans (labeled as “basic” categories). Others simply viewed 
photos of cars (labeled “exposure”). Measures of electrical activity in the brain differed according 
to the level of categorization on which participants had been trained. (Source: Scott et al., 2008) 


64 






Post-Test 


oO — N 


Subordinate 


Basic 


Luli 





Exposure 


=7 
—100 O 100 200 300 400 500 600 700 800 


N250 





Basic Exposure Subordinate 


—2.2 uv > 2.2 pv 
O D y 


birds, the robin is an example that comes to most people’s minds far more readily. 
Consequently, robin is a prototype of the concept “bird.” Similarly, when we think 
of the concept of a table, we're likely to think of a coffee table before we think of a 
drafting table, making a coffee table closer to our prototype of a table. See Figure 2. 

Relatively high agreement exists among people in a particular culture about which 
examples of a concept are prototypes as well as which examples are not. For instance, 
most people in Western cultures consider cars and trucks good examples of vehicles, 
whereas elevators and wheelbarrows are not considered very good examples. Conse- 
quently, cars and trucks are prototypes of the concept of a vehicle (see Figure 3). 

Concepts enable us to think about and understand more readily the complex 
world in which we live. For example, the suppositions we make about the reasons 
for other people’s behavior are based on the ways in which we classify behavior. 
Hence, our conclusion about a person who washes her hands 20 times a day could 
vary, depending on whether we place her behavior within the conceptual framework 
of a health-care worker or a mental patient. Similarly, physicians make diagnoses by 
drawing on concepts and prototypes of symptoms that they learned about in medi- 
cal school. Finally, concepts and prototypes facilitate our efforts to draw suitable 
conclusions through the cognitive process we turn to next: reasoning. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 23 Thinking and Reasoning 247 






4 


FIGURE 3 Prototypes are typical, highly representative examples of a concept. For instance, 
a highly typical prototype of the concept “furniture” is a chair, whereas a stove is not a good 
prototype. High agreement exists within a culture about which examples of a concept are 
prototypes. (Source: Adapted from Rosch & Mervis, 1975.) 


Reasoning: Making Up Your Mind 





Professors deciding when students’ assignments are due. 
An employer determining who to hire out of a pool of job applicants. 
The president concluding that it is necessary to send troops to a foreign nation. 


What do these three situations have in common? Each requires reasoning, the process 
by which information is used to draw conclusions and make decisions. 

Although philosophers and logicians have considered the foundations of reason- 
ing for centuries, it is only relatively recently that cognitive psychologists have begun 
to investigate how people reason and make decisions. Their efforts have contributed 
to our understanding of formal reasoning processes as well as the cognitive shortcuts 
we routinely use—shortcuts that sometimes may lead our reasoning capabilities 
astray (Johnson-Laird, 2006). 


SYLLOGISTIC REASONING: THE FORMAL RULES OF LOGIC 


If you've ever played a card game like poker and tried to figure out what cards your 
opponent was holding, you probably used syllogistic reasoning, a kind of formal 
reasoning in which a person draws a conclusion from a set of assumptions. In using 
syllogistic reasoning, we begin with a general assumption that we believe is true and 
then derive specific implications from that assumption. If the assumption is true, the 
conclusions must also be true (Fisk & Sharp, 2002; Marrero & Gamez, 2004; Shynkaruk 
& Thompson, 2006). 






y Alert 


Figure 3 will help you 
remember that prototypes 
represent “best” or most 
common examples of a 
particular concept. For 

example, a Prius might be a 
prototype of the concept of 
“hybrid car.” 


syllogistic reasoning Formal reason- 
ing in which people draw a conclusion 
from a set of assumptions. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


248 Chapter 8 Cognition and Language 


A major technique for studying syllogistic reasoning involves asking people to 
evaluate a series of statements that present two assumptions, or premises, that are 
used to derive a conclusion. For example, consider the following syllogism: 


Premise 1 All professors are mortal. 
Premise 2 Dr. Rivera is a professor. 
Conclusion Therefore, Dr. Rivera is mortal. 


Because both premises are true, by applying logic appropriately we come to an 
accurate conclusion. More abstractly, we can state the syllogism as the following: 


Premise 1 All A’s are B. 
Premise 2 Cis an A. 
Conclusion Therefore, C is a B. 


However, even if the premises are correct, people may apply logic incorrectly. 
For example, consider the following syllogism: 


Premise 1 All A’s are B. 
Premise 2 C isan A. 
Conclusion Therefore, all A’s are C. 


Although it may not be immediately apparent, the conclusion is illogical— 
something we will see more readily if we make the syllogism more concrete: 


Premise 1 All professors are mortal. 
Premise 2 Professor Rivera is a professor. 
Conclusion Therefore, all professors are Dr. Rivera. 


In short, syllogistic reasoning is only as accurate as the premises and the validity 
of the logic applied to the premises. 


ALGORITHMS AND HEURISTICS 


When faced with making a decision, we often turn to various kinds of cognitive 


algorithm A rule that, if applied shortcuts, known as algorithms and heuristics, to help us. An algorithm is a rule 
appropriately, guarantees a solution to that, if applied appropriately, guarantees a solution to a problem. We can use an 
a problem. algorithm even if we cannot understand why it works. For example, you may know 


that you can find the length of the third side of a right triangle by using the formula 
a + b? = œ, although you may not have the foggiest notion of the mathematical 
principles behind the formula. 
For many problems and decisions, however, no algorithm is available. In those 
heuristic A thinking strategy that may instances, we may be able to use heuristics to help us. A heuristic is a thinking strategy 


lead us to a solution to a problem or that may lead us to a solution to a problem or decision, but—unlike algorithms—may 
decision, but—unlike algorithms— sometimes lead to errors. Heuristics increase the likelihood of success in coming to a 
may sometimes lead to errors. solution, but, unlike algorithms, they cannot ensure it. For example, when I play 


tic-tac-toe, I follow the heuristic of placing an X in the center square when I start the 
game. This tactic doesn’t guarantee that I will win, but experience has taught me that 
it will increase my chances of success. Similarly, some students follow the heuristic of 
preparing for a test by ignoring the assigned textbook reading and only studying their 
lecture notes—a strategy that may or may not pay off. 
ly Alert Although heuristics often help people solve problems and make decisions, cer- 
tain kinds of heuristics may lead to inaccurate conclusions. For example, we some- 
times use the representativeness heuristic, a rule we apply when we judge people by 
the degree to which they represent a certain category or group of people. Suppose, 
for instance, you are the owner of a fast-food store that has been robbed many times 
by teenagers. The representativeness heuristic would lead you to raise your guard 
each time someone of this age group enters your store (even though, statistically, it 
is unlikely that any given teenager will rob the store) (Fisk, Bury, & Holden, 2006; 
Nilsson, Juslin, & Olsson, 2008). 


Remember that algorithms 
are rules that always provide 
a solution, while heuristics 
are shortcuts that may 
provide a solution. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 23 Thinking and Reasoning 249 


The availability heuristic involves judging the probability of an event on the basis 
of how easily the event can be recalled from memory. According to this heuristic, we 
assume that events we remember easily are likely to have occurred more frequently 
in the past—and are more likely to occur in the future—than events that are harder 
to remember. 

For instance, the availability heuristic makes us more afraid of dying in a plane 
crash than in an auto accident, despite statistics clearly showing that airplane travel 
is much safer than auto travel. Similarly, although 10 times as many people die from 
falling out of bed than from lightning strikes, we’re more afraid of being hit by 
lightning. The reason is that plane crashes and lightning strikes receive far more 
publicity, and they are therefore more easily remembered (Oppenheimer, 2004; Fox, 
2006; Kluger, 2006; Caruso, 2008). 

Are algorithms and heuristics confined to human thinking, or can we program 
computers to mimic human thinking and problem solving? As we discuss next, sci- 
entists are certainly trying. 


Computers and Problem 
Solving: Searching for 
Artificial Intelligence 


To the music experts, there was no mistaking who had written the piano piece: 
Johann Sebastian Bach, the prolific German composer who was born in the 15th 
century. 

But the experts were wrong. The piece they all thought was a Bach composition 
was actually created by a computer named “EMI” by David Cope of the University 
of California. After a variety of actual Bach pieces had been scanned into its memory, 
EMI was able to produce music that was so similar to Bach’s actual music that it 
fooled knowledgeable listeners (Johnson, 1997; Cope, 2001). 

Such computer mimicry is possible because composers have a particular “signa- 
ture” that reflects patterns, sequences, and combinations of notes. By employing 
those “signatures,” computers can create compositions that have the full scope and 
emotional appeal of actual works—and show just as much creativity as those written 
by the actual composer (Cope, 2001, 2003). 

Computers are making significant inroads in terms of the ability to solve prob- 
lems and carry out some forms of intellectual activities. According to experts who 
study artificial intelligence, the field that examines how to use technology to imitate 
the outcome of human thinking, problem solving, and creative activities, computers 
can show rudiments of humanlike thinking because of their knowledge of where to 
look—and where not to look—for an answer to a problem. They suggest that the 
capacity of computer programs (such as those that play chess) to evaluate potential 
moves and to ignore unimportant possibilities gives them thinking ability (Sabater 
& Sierra, 2005; Prasad, 2006; Copeland & Proudfoot, 2007). 

Many of the questions surrounding the ability of computers to think and behave 
creatively have not been answered. Still, it is clear that computers are becoming 
increasingly sophisticated, ever more closely approximating human thought 
processes. 








A computer using artificial intelligence 
software was able to mimic compositions 
by Johann Sebastian Bach so successfully 
it fooled expert musicologists. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


250 Chapter 8 Cognition and Language 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP EVALUATE 


What is thinking? 1. are representations in the mind of 





Cognitive psychology encompasses the higher mental 
processes, including the way people know and under- 
stand the world, process information, make decisions 
and judgments, and describe their knowledge and un- 
derstanding to others. (p. 242) 

Thinking is the manipulation of mental representations 
of information. Thinking transforms such representa- 
tions into novel and different forms, permitting people 
to answer questions, solve problems, and reach goals. 
(p. 243) 

Mental images are representations in the mind of an ob- 
ject or event. (p. 244) 

Concepts are categorizations of objects, events, or people 
that share common properties. (p. 245) 

Prototypes are representative examples of concepts. 

(p. 245) 


What processes underlie reasoning and decision making? 


In syllogistic reasoning, people derive implications from 
a set of assumptions that they know to be true. (p. 247) 
Decisions sometimes (but not always) may be improved 
through the use of algorithms and heuristics. An algo- 


rithm is a rule that, if applied appropriately, guarantees a 


solution; a heuristic is a cognitive shortcut that may lead 
to a solution but is not guaranteed to do so. (p. 248) 


KEY TERMS 


cognitive psychology p. 242 
thinking p. 243 


mental images p. 244 
concepts p. 245 


prototypes p. 245 
syllogistic reasoning p. 247 


an object or event. 
are categorizations of objects that share com- 
mon properties. 


. When you think of the concept “chair,” you immediately 


think of a comfortable easy chair. A chair of this type 
could be thought of as a of the category 
“chair.” 


. When you ask your friend how best to study for your psy- 


chology final, he tells you, “I’ve always found it best to 
skim over the notes once, then read the book, then go over 
the notes again.” What decision-making tool might this be 
an example of? 


RETHINK 


1. How might the availability heuristic contribute to preju- 


dices based on race, age, and gender? Can awareness of 
this heuristic prevent this from happening? 


2. From the perspective of a human resources specialist: How 


might you use the research on mental imagery to improve 
employees’ performance? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


oysunəy ‘p ‘adAjojor1d *¢ ‘sydaou09 ‘g ‘saseurt jezuow T 


algorithm p. 248 
heuristic p. 248 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 24 


According to an old legend, a group of Vietnamese monks guard three towers on 
which sit 64 golden rings. The monks believe that if they succeed in moving the rings 
from the first tower to the third according to a series of rigid rules, the world as we 
know it will come to an end. (Should you prefer that the world remain in its present 
state, there’s no need for immediate concern: The puzzle is so complex that it will 
take the monks about a trillion years to solve it.) 

In the Tower of Hanoi puzzle, a simpler version of the task facing the monks, 
three disks are placed on three posts in the order shown in Figure 1. The goal of the 
puzzle is to move all three disks to the third post, arranged in the same order, by 
using as few moves as possible. There are two restrictions: Only one disk can be 
moved at a time, and no disk can ever cover a smaller one during a move. 

Why are cognitive psychologists interested in the Tower of Hanoi problem? 
Because the way people go about solving such puzzles helps illuminate how people 
solve complex, real-life problems. Psychologists have found that problem solving 
typically involves the three steps illustrated in Figure 2: preparing to create solutions, 
producing solutions, and evaluating the solutions that have been generated. 


How do people approach and 
solve problems? 


What are the major obstacles 
to problem solving? 


What is creativity? 














2 3 
Start Finish i 
E 1 The goal of the Tower of Hanoi puzzle is to move all three disks from the first post y Alert 

to the third and still preserve the original order of the disks, using the fewest number of Use the three steps of prob- 
moves possible while following the rules that only one disk at a time can be moved and no lem solving to organize your 
disk can cover a smaller one during a move. Try it yourself before you look at the solution, studying: Preparation, 
which is listed according to the sequence of moves. Production, and Judgment 

(‘€ 0} D pue ‘£ 0} g ‘L O} D ‘E O} Y 'Z O} D ‘Z O} g 'E 0} D AOW :UONIN|OS) (PPJ). 

Preparation Production Judgment 
i — r 
Pinel Snel igi Generating solutions Evaluating solutions 


diagnosing problems 


FIGURE 2 Steps in problem solving. 


251 


252 


© Michael Maslin/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 8 Cognition and Language 


Preparation: Understanding 
and Diagnosing Problems 


When approaching a problem like the Tower of Hanoi, most people begin by trying 
to understand the problem thoroughly. If the problem is a novel one, they probably 
will pay particular attention to any restrictions placed on coming up with a solution— 
such as the rule for moving only one disk at a time in the Tower of Hanoi problem. 
If, by contrast, the problem is a familiar one, they are apt to spend considerably less 
time in this preparation stage. 





Problems vary from well defined to ill defined. In a well-defined problem—such as 


a mathematical equation or the solution to a jigsaw puzzle—both the nature of the 
problem itself and the information needed to solve it are available and clear. Thus, 
we can make straightforward judgments about whether a potential solution is appro- 
priate. With an ill-defined problem, such as how to increase morale on an assembly 
line or to bring peace to the Middle East, not only may the specific nature of the 
problem be unclear, the information required to solve the problem may be even less 
obvious (Evans, 2004; Vartanian, 2009). 


KINDS OF PROBLEMS 


Typically, a problem falls into one of the three categories shown in Figure 3: arrange- 
ment, inducing structure, and transformation. Solving each type requires somewhat 
different kinds of psychological skills and knowledge (Spitz, 1987; Chronicle, Mac- 
Gregor, & Ormerod, 2004). 


Arrangement problems require the problem solver to rearrange or recombine ele- 


ments in a way that will satisfy a certain criterion. Usually, several different arrange- 
ments can be made, but only one or a few of the arrangements will produce a 
solution. Anagram problems and jigsaw puzzles are examples of arrangement prob- 
lems (Coventry et al., 2003). 


In problems of inducing structure, a person must identify the existing relation- 


ships among the elements presented and then construct a new relationship among 
them. In such a problem, the problem solver must determine not only the rela- 





“I don’t know about hair care, Rapunzel, 
but I’m thinking a good cream rinse plus 
protein conditioner might just solve 
both our problems.” 


tionships among the elements but also the structure and size of the 
elements involved. In the example shown in Figure 3, a person must 
first determine that the solution requires the numbers to be consid- 
ered in pairs (14-24-34-44-54-64). Only after identifying that part of 
the problem can a person determine the solution rule (the first num- 
ber of each pair increases by one, while the second number remains 
the same). 

The Tower of Hanoi puzzle represents the third kind of problem— 
transformation problems—that consist of an initial state, a goal state, and 
a method for changing the initial state into the goal state. In the Tower 
of Hanoi problem, the initial state is the original configuration, the goal 
state is to have the three disks on the third peg, and the method is the 
rules for moving the disks (Mataix-Cols & Bartres-Faz, 2002; Emick & 
Welsh, 2005; Majeres, 2007). 

Whether the problem is one of arrangement, inducing structure, or 
transformation, the preparation stage of understanding and diagnosing 
is critical in problem solving because it allows us to develop our own 
cognitive representation of the problem and to place it within a per- 
sonal framework. We may divide the problem into subparts or ignore 
some information as we try to simplify the task. Winnowing out nones- 
sential information is often a critical step in the preparation stage of 
problem solving. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


a. Arrangement problems 
|. Anagrams: Rearrange the letters in each set to make an English word: 


SCT Ia] Sobur] skorin] SAE Nv] SLV aN] 


2. Two strings hang from a ceiling but are too far apart to allow a person to hold one and walk 
to the other. On the floor are a book of matches, a screwdriver, and a few pieces of cotton. 
How could the strings be tied together? 











b. Problems of inducing structure 
|. What number comes next in the series? 
142434445464 


2. Complete these analogies: 
baseball is to bat as tennis is to 
merchant is to sell as customer is to 


c. Transformation problems 
|. Water jars:A person has three jars with the following capacities: 


Jar B: Jar C: 
7 ounces 5 ounces 


How can the person measure exactly || ounces of water? 


Jar A: 
28 ounces 





2. Ten coins are arranged in the following way. By moving only two of the coins, 
make two rows that each contains six coins. 





Module 24 Problem Solving 253 


FIGURE 3 The three major categories of 
problems: (a) arrangement, (b) inducing 
structure, and (c) transformation. 
Solutions appear in Figure 4 on p. 254. 
(Source: Bourne & Dominowski, Cognitive 
Processes, p. 233. Copyright © 1986 Prentice- 
Hall, Inc. Reproduced by permission of 
Pearson Education, Inc.) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


254 Chapter 8 Cognition and Language 


FIGURE 4 Solutions to the problems in 
Figure 3 on page 253. (Source: Bourne & 
Dominowski, Cognitive Processes, p. 268. 
Copyright © 1986 Prentice-Hall, Inc. 
Reproduced by permission of Pearson 
Education, Inc.) 


Top 


Saturday 


Altitude on mountain 








Bottom 





Sunrise Sunset 
Time of day 


FIGURE 5 You can solve the mountain- 
climbing problem by using a graph. Keep 
in mind that the goal is not to determine 
the time but just to indicate whether an 
exact time exists. Consequently, the 
speed at which the traveler is moving is 
unimportant. Can you think of other 
approaches that might lead to a solution? 
(Source: Anderson, 1980.) 


a. Arrangement problems 
|. FACET, DOUBT, THICK, NAIVE, ANVIL 


2. The screwdriver is tied to one of the 
strings. This makes a pendulum that can 
be swung to reach the other string. 


b. Problems of inducing structure 
1. 7 
2. racket; buy 


c. Transformation problems 


|. Fill jar A; empty into jar B once and into jar C twice. 
What remains in jar A is | | ounces 





REPRESENTING AND ORGANIZING THE PROBLEM 


A crucial aspect of the initial encounter with a problem is the way in which we represent 
it to ourselves and organize the information presented to us (Brown & Walter, 1993; 
Davidson, Deuser, & Sternberg, 1994). Consider the following problem: 


A man climbs a mountain on Saturday, leaving at daybreak and arriving at the top near 
sundown. He spends the night at the top. The next day, Sunday, he leaves at daybreak 
and heads down the mountain, following the same path that he climbed the day before. 
The question is this: Will there be any time during the second day when he will be at 
exactly the same point on the mountain as he was at exactly that time on the first day? 


If you try to solve this problem by using algebraic or verbal representations, you 
will have a good deal of trouble. However, if you represent the problem with the 
kind of simple diagram shown in Figure 5, the solution will become apparent. 

Our ability to represent a problem—and the solution we eventually come to— 
depends on the way a problem is phrased, or framed. Imagine that you were a cancer 
patient having to choose between surgery and radiation and were given the two sets 
of treatment options shown in Figure 6 (Iversky & Kahneman, 1987; Chandran & 
Menon, 2004). When the options are framed in terms of the likelihood of survival, 
only 18% of participants in a study chose radiation over surgery. However, when the 
choice was framed in terms of the likelihood of dying, 44% chose radiation over 
surgery—even though the outcomes are identical in both sets of framing conditions. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 24 Problem Solving 255 





FIGURE 6 A decision often is affected by the way a problem is framed. In this case, most 
would choose radiation over surgery, despite similar results. 


Production: 
Generating Solutions 


After preparation, the next stage in problem solving is the production of possible 
solutions. If a problem is relatively simple, we may already have a direct solution 
stored in long-term memory, and all we need to do is retrieve the appropriate infor- 
mation. If we cannot retrieve or do not know the solution, we must generate pos- 
sible solutions and compare them with information in long- and short-term memory. 
At the most basic level, we can solve problems through trial and error. Thomas 
Edison invented the lightbulb only because he tried thousands of different kinds of 
materials for a filament before he found one that worked (carbon). The difficulty with 
trial and error, of course, is that some problems are so complicated that it would take 
a lifetime to try out every possibility. For example, according to some estimates, there 
are some 10'”” possible sequences of chess moves (Fine & Fine, 2003). 
In place of trial and error, complex problem solving often involves the use of 
heuristics, cognitive shortcuts that can generate solutions. Probably the most fre- 
quently applied heuristic in problem solving is a means-ends analysis, which means-ends analysis Involves 
involves repeated tests for differences between the desired outcome and what repeated tests for differences between 
currently exists. Consider this simple example (Newell & Simon, 1972; Huber, Beck- the desired outcome and what 
mann, & Herrmann, 2004; Chrysikou, 2006): currently exists. 





I want to take my son to preschool. What’s the difference between what I have and 
what I want? One of distance. What changes distance? My automobile. My automobile 
won't work. What is needed to make it work? A new battery. What has new batteries? 
An auto repair shop. ... 


In a means-end analysis, each step brings the problem solver closer to a resolution. 
Although this approach is often effective, if the problem requires indirect steps that 
temporarily increase the discrepancy between a current state and the solution, means-ends 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


256 Chapter 8 Cognition and Language 


analysis can be counterproductive. For example, sometimes the fastest route to the 
summit of a mountain requires a mountain climber to backtrack temporarily; a means- 
end approach—that implies that the mountain climber should always forge ahead and 
upward—will be ineffective in such instances. 

For other problems, the best approach is to work backward by focusing on the 
goal, rather than the starting point, of the problem. Consider, for example, the water 
lily problem: 


Water lilies are growing on Blue Lake. The water lilies grow rapidly, so that the amount 
of water surface covered by lilies doubles every 24 hours. On the first day of summer, 
there was just one water lily. On the 90th day of the summer, the lake was entirely 
covered. On what day was the lake half covered? (Reisberg, 1997) 


If you start searching for a solution to the problem by thinking about the 
initial state on day 1 (one water lily) and move forward from there, you’re facing 
a daunting task of trial-and-error estimation. But try taking a different approach: 
Start with day 90, when the entire lake was covered with lilies. Given that the 
lilies double their coverage daily, on the prior day only half the lake was covered. 
The answer, then, is day 89, a solution found by working backward (Bourne et al., 
1986; Hunt, 1994). 


FORMING SUBGOALS: DIVIDING PROBLEMS INTO THEIR PARTS 


Another heuristic commonly used to generate solutions is to divide a problem into 
intermediate steps, or subgoals, and solve each of those steps. For instance, in our 
modified Tower of Hanoi problem, we could choose several obvious subgoals, such 
as moving the largest disk to the third post. 

If solving a subgoal is a step toward the ultimate solution 
to a problem, identifying subgoals is an appropriate strategy. 
THE FAR SIDE” By GARY LARSON In some cases, however, forming subgoals is not all that help- 
ful and may actually increase the time needed to find a solu- 
tion. For example, some problems cannot be subdivided. 
Others—like some complicated mathematical problems—are 
so complex that it takes longer to identify the appropriate 
subdivisions than to solve the problem by other means (Reed, 

1996; Kaller et al., 2004; Fishbach, Dhar, Zhang, 2006). 


INSIGHT: SUDDEN AWARENESS 


Some approaches to generating possible solutions focus less 
on step-by-step heuristics than on the sudden bursts of 
comprehension that one may experience during efforts to 
solve a problem. In a classic study the German psychologist 
Wolfgang Köhler examined learning and problem-solving 
processes in chimpanzees (Kohler, 1927). In his studies, 
Kohler exposed chimps to challenging situations in which 
the elements of the solution were all present; all the chimps 
needed to do was put them together. 

In one of Kéhler’s studies, chimps were kept in a 
cage in which boxes and sticks were strewn about, and a 
bunch of tantalizing bananas hung from the ceiling, out 
of reach. Initially, the chimps made trial-and-error 
attempts to get to the bananas: They would throw the 
sticks at the bananas, jump from one of the boxes, or leap 
wildly from the ground. Frequently, they would seem to 
give up in frustration, leaving the bananas dangling 


ee 


N 


sa 
ml 





The Far Side” by Gary Larson © 1981 FarWorks, Inc. All Rights Reserved. The Far Side” and 
the Larson” signature are registered trademarks of FarWorks, Inc. Used with permission. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





(b) 


FIGURE 7 (a) In an impressive display of insight, Sultan, one of the chimpanzees in Kohler’s 
experiments in problem solving, sees a bunch of bananas that is out of reach. (b) He then 
carries over several crates, stacks them, and (c) stands on them to reach the bananas. 


temptingly overhead. But then, in what seemed like a sudden revelation, they would 
stop whatever they were doing and stand on a box to reach the bananas with a stick 
(Figure 7). Köhler called the cognitive process underlying the chimps’ new behavior 
insight, a sudden awareness of the relationships among various elements that had 
previously appeared to be unrelated. 

Although Köhler emphasized the apparent suddenness of insightful solutions, 
subsequent research has shown that prior experience and trial-and-error practice in 
problem solving must precede “insight.” Consequently, the chimps’ behavior may 
simply represent the chaining together of previously learned responses, no different 
from the way a pigeon learns, by trial and error, to peck a key (Epstein, 1996; Wind- 
holz & Lamal, 2002). 


Judgment: Evaluating Solutions 


The final stage in problem solving is judging the adequacy of a solution. Often this 
is a simple matter: If the solution is clear—as in the Tower of Hanoi problem—we 
will know immediately whether we have been successful (Varma, 2007). 

If the solution is less concrete or if there is no single correct solution, evaluating 
solutions becomes more difficult. In such instances, we must decide which alternative 
solution is best. Unfortunately, we often quite inaccurately estimate the quality of 
our own ideas. For instance, a team of drug researchers working for a particular 
company may consider their remedy for an illness to be superior to all others, over- 
estimating the likelihood of their success and downplaying the approaches of com- 
peting drug companies (Eizenberg & Zaslavsky, 2004). 

Theoretically, if we rely on appropriate heuristics and valid information to make 
decisions, we can make accurate choices among alternative solutions. However, as 
we see next, several kinds of obstacles to and biases in problem solving affect the 
quality of the decisions and judgments we make. (Also see Applying Psychology in the 
21st Century.) 





Module 24 Problem Solving 257 





insight A sudden awareness of the 
relationships among various elements 
that had previously appeared to be 
independent of one another. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


in the 21st Century 





The Thoughts Behind 
Avoiding Danger: When 
Hunches Save Lives 


The sight was not that unusual, at least 
not for Mosul, Iraq: a car parked on the 
sidewalk, facing opposite traffic, its 
windows rolled up tight. Two young boys 
stared out the back window, their faces 
leaning together as if to share a whisper. 
The soldier patrolling closest to the car 
stopped. He had an urge to move back 
before he knew why: “My body suddenly 
got cooler, you know, that danger 
feeling.” (Carey, 2009b) 


That “danger feeling” saved the sol- 
dier’s life: Moments after he turned to re- 
treat, an improvised explosive device—a 
home-made bomb—detonated the car, 
throwing the soldier to the ground. 

United States troops patrolling Iraq 
have expensive military equipment to help 
them avoid the threat of hidden bombs, but 
it turns out that even the best equipment is 
not as effective as the soldiers’ own 
hunches. Researchers are studying sol- 
diers’ thought and decision-making pro- 
cesses to determine how exactly they sense 
imminent danger, and the initial answers 
are surprising. 


We might expect that training plays a 
part in this ability, and it does: Elite sol- 
diers are highly trained in what to look for. 
But they also seem to manage their anxiety, 
process visual imagery, and interpret their 
own emotional reactions in ways that make 
them more sensitive to danger (Rachman, 
1990; Li et al., 2007). 

Keeping their anxiety at bay helps sol- 
diers stay focused on the task of spotting 
bombs, and some ways they do this are by 
thinking of themselves as being on the of- 
fensive rather than on the defensive and by 
identifying strongly with their units. Stud- 
ies also show that well-trained troops tend 
to recover more quickly than others from 
the cortisol rush produced by a threat 
(Haslam et al., 2005). 

Heightened visual ability also plays a 
part. Highly-trained soldiers seem to be 
particularly sensitized to relevant visual 
images—they sometimes have an uncanny 
knack for picking up on subtle features or 
changes in the environment that may be 


ETHINK 


clues to a threatening situation, and they 
tend to process this information faster than 
other people do (Carey, 2009b). 

In fact, the visual signals can be so sub- 
tle and the soldiers can process them so 
quickly that they may sense danger with- 
out consciously knowing what clues are 
telling them it exists. Studies of people 
who respond to threats in this manner 
show that the brain regions involved in de- 
cision making and in interpreting bodily 
sensations to produce an emotion are par- 
ticularly active. This heightened ability to 
process emotions has also been shown in 
trained soldiers (Clark et al., 2008). 

Consequently, when faced with the task 
of detecting stimuli that are nearly unde- 
tectable, trained soldiers are able to make 
use of information that most of us would 
miss, and they do so quickly. The hunches 
that result often make the difference 
between life and death (Carey, 2009b; 
Gigerenzer, 2007). 








e How might you train soldiers to better make use of their cognitive processes to 
respond effectively to dangerous situations? 

e What relevance might the findings about soldiers’ hunches and their ability to 
avoid danger have to other areas of life? 








Impediments to Solutions: 


Why Is Problem Solving 
Such a Problem? 


Consider the following problem-solving test illustrated in Figure 8 (Duncker, 1945): 





You are given a set of tacks, candles, and matches, each in a small box, and told your 
goal is to place three candles at eye level on a nearby door so that wax will not drip on 
the floor as the candles burn. How would you approach this challenge? 


If you have difficulty solving the problem, you are not alone. Most people cannot 
solve it when it is presented in the manner illustrated in the figure, in which the 
objects are inside the boxes. However, if the objects were presented beside the boxes, 
just resting on the table, chances are that you would solve the problem much more 
readily—which, in case you are wondering, requires tacking the boxes to the door 
and then placing the candles inside them (see Figure 10 on page 260). 

The difficulty you probably encountered in solving this problem stems from its 
presentation, which misled you at the initial preparation stage. Actually, significant 


258 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





obstacles to problem solving can exist at each of the three major stages. Although 
cognitive approaches to problem solving suggest that thinking proceeds along fairly 
rational, logical lines as a person confronts a problem and considers various solu- 
tions, several factors can hinder the development of creative, appropriate, and accu- 
rate solutions. 


FUNCTIONAL FIXEDNESS AND MENTAL SET 


The difficulty most people experience with the candle problem is caused by functional 
fixedness, the tendency to think of an object only in terms of its typical use. For instance, 
functional fixedness probably leads you to think of this book as something to read 
instead of its potential use as a doorstop or as kindling for a fire. In the candle problem, 
because the objects are first presented inside the boxes, functional fixedness leads most 
people to see the boxes simply as containers for the objects they hold rather than as a 
potential part of the solution. They cannot envision another function for the boxes. 

Functional fixedness is an example of a broader phenomenon known as mental set, 
the tendency for old patterns of problem solving to persist. A classic experiment 
(Luchins, 1946) demonstrated this phenomenon. As you can see in Figure 9, the 
object of the task is to use the jars in each row to measure out the designated 
amount of liquid. (Try it yourself to get a sense of the power of mental set before 
moving on.) 

If you have tried to solve the problem, you know that the first five rows are all 
solved in the same way: First fill the largest jar (B) and then from it fill the middle-size 
jar (A) once and the smallest jar (C) two times. What is left in B is the designated 
amount. (Stated as a formula, the designated amount is B-A-2C.) The demonstration 
of mental set comes in the sixth row of the problem, a point at which you probably 
encountered some difficulty. If you are like most people, you tried the formula and were 
perplexed when it failed. Chances are, in fact, that you missed the simple (but different) 
solution to the problem, which involves merely subtracting C from A. Interestingly, 
people who were given the problem in row 6 first had no difficulty with it at all. 


Given jars with these capacities (in ounces): 





Th 21 127 3 100 
P 14 163 25 99 
8 18 43 10 5 
4 9 42 6 2I 
5 20 59 31 
6 28 76 3 25 


Module 24 Problem Solving 259 


FIGURE 8 The problem here is to place 
three candles at eye level on a nearby 
door so that the wax will not drip on 
the floor as the candles burn—using 
only material in the figure. For a solution 
turn to Figure 10 on p. 260. 


functional fixedness The tendency to 
think of an object only in terms of its 
typical use. 


mental set The tendency for old 
patterns of problem solving to persist. 


FIGURE 9 Try this classic demonstration, 
which illustrates the importance of 
mental set in problem solving. The 
object is to use the jars in each row to 
measure out the designated amount of 
liquid. After you figure out the solution 
for the first five rows, you'll probably 
have trouble with the sixth row—even 
though the solution is actually easier. 
In fact, if you had tried to solve the 
problem in the sixth row first, you 
probably would have had no difficulty 
at all. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


260 Chapter 8 Cognition and Language 


FIGURE 10 A solution to the problem 
in Figure 8 involves tacking the boxes to 
the door and placing the candles in the 
boxes. 


confirmation bias The tendency to 
seek out and weight more heavily 
information that supports one’s 
initial hypotheses and to ignore 
contradictory information that 
supports alternative hypotheses or 
solutions. 

















Mental set can affect perceptions as well as patterns of problem solving. It can 
prevent you from seeing beyond the apparent constraints of a problem. For example, 
try to draw four straight lines so that they pass through all nine dots in the grid 
below—without lifting your pencil from the page. 


If you had difficulty with the problem, it was probably because you felt compelled 
to keep your lines within the grid. If you had gone outside the boundaries, however, 
you would have succeeded by using the solution shown in Figure 11 on page 263. 
(The phrase “thinking outside the box”—a term commonly used in business today to 
encourage creativity—stems from research on overcoming the constraining effects of 
mental set.) 


INACCURATE EVALUATION OF SOLUTIONS 


When the United States invaded Iraq, it did so because governmental leaders believed 
that Saddam Hussein possessed weapons of mass destruction that posed a threat to 
the security of the Middle East. The belief proved false, and a bipartisan U.S. Senate 
committee later found that the belief was based on a flawed interpretation of intel- 
ligence information. According to the Senate committee, government leaders had 
made up their minds early on about the presence of weapons of mass destruction, 
and they ignored contradictory evidence and focused more on information that sup- 
ported their view (U.S. Senate Select Committee on Intelligence, 2004). 

The mistake made by governmental leaders exemplifies confirmation bias in 
which problem solvers favor initial hypotheses and ignore contradictory information 
that supports alternative hypotheses or solutions. Even when we find evidence that 
contradicts a solution we have chosen, we are apt to stick with our original hypothesis. 

Confirmation bias occurs for several reasons. For one thing, because rethinking 
a problem that appears to be solved already takes extra cognitive effort, we are apt 
to stick with our first solution. For another, we give greater weight to subsequent 
information that supports our initial position than to information that is not sup- 
portive of it (Evans & Feeney, 2004; Parmley, 2007; Rassin, 2008). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Creativity and Problem Solving 


Despite obstacles to problem solving, many people adeptly discover creative solu- 
tions to problems. One enduring question that cognitive psychologists have sought 
to answer is what factors underlie creativity, the ability to generate original ideas or 
solve problems in novel ways. 

Although identifying the stages of problem solving helps us understand how 
people approach and solve problems, it does little to explain why some people 
come up with better solutions than others do. For instance, even the possible solu- 
tions to a simple problem often show wide discrepancies. Consider, for example, 
how you might respond to the question “How many uses can you think of for a 
newspaper?” 

Now compare your solution with this one proposed by a 10-year-old boy: 


You can read it, write on it, lay it down and paint a picture on it . . . You could put 
it in your door for decoration, put it in the garbage can, put it on a chair if the chair 
is messy. If you have a puppy, you put newspaper in its box or put it in your 
backyard for the dog to play with. When you build something and you don’t want 
anyone to see it, put newspaper around it. Put newspaper on the floor if you have 
no mattress, use it to pick up something hot, use it to stop bleeding, or to catch the 
drips from drying clothes. You can use a newspaper for curtains, put it in your shoe 
to cover what is hurting your foot, make a kite out of it, shade a light that is too 
bright. You can wrap fish in it, wipe windows, or wrap money in it . . . You put 
washed shoes in newspaper, wipe eyeglasses with it, put it under a dripping sink, 
put a plant on it, make a paper bowl out of it, use it for a hat if it is raining, tie it on 
your feet for slippers. You can put it on the sand if you had no towel, use it for 
bases in baseball, make paper airplanes with it, use it as a dustpan when you sweep, 
ball it up for the cat to play with, wrap your hands in it if it is cold. (Ward, Kogan, 
& Pankove, 1972) 


This list shows extraordinary creativity. Unfortunately, it is much easier to iden- 
tify examples of creativity than to determine its causes. Similarly, it’s not clear that 
the kind of creativity shown by highly creative people in the arts, such as Pablo 








Module 24 Problem Solving 261 


creativity The ability to generate 
original ideas or solve problems in 
novel ways. 


“Pll be happy to give you innovative 
thinking. What are the guidelines?” 


© Leo Cullum/The New Yorker Collection/www. 


cartoonbank.com. 


Pablo Picasso is considered one of the 
greatest artists of the 20th century. Do 
you think he relied more on convergent 
or divergent thinking in his art? 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


262 Chapter 8 Cognition and Language 


Fallingwater, designed by legendary 
architect Frank Lloyd Wright, is known 
as a masterpiece of visionary 
architecture. What processes might 
account for Wright's creativity? 


divergent thinking The ability to 
generate unusual, yet nonetheless 
appropriate, responses to problems or 
questions. 


convergent thinking The ability to 
produce responses that are based 
primarily on knowledge and logic. 


y Alert 


Remember divergent 
thinking produces different 
and diverse kinds of 
responses, while 
convergent thinking 
produces more commonsense kinds 
of responses. 


BECOMING AN 


INFORMED CONSUMER 


of Psychology 


Thinking Critically and Creatively 





Picasso, is the same kind of creativity shown by highly creative people in the sci- 
ences, such as Albert Einstein (Simonton, 2009). 

However, we do know that several characteristics are associated with 
creativity. For one thing, highly creative individuals show divergent thinking, 
the ability to generate unusual, yet appropriate, responses to problems or ques- 
tions. This type of thinking contrasts with convergent thinking, which produces 
responses that are based primarily on knowledge and logic. For instance, some- 
one relying on convergent thinking would answer “You read it” to the query 
“What can you do with a newspaper?” In contrast, “You use it as a dustpan” is 
a more divergent—and creative—response (Runco, 2006; Cropley, 2006; Schepers, 
van den Berg, 2007). 

Another aspect of creativity is its cognitive complexity, or preference for elaborate, 
intricate, and complex stimuli and thinking patterns. For instance, creative people 
often have a wider range of interests and are more independent and more interested 
in philosophical or abstract problems than are less creative individuals (Barron, 1990; 
Richards, 2006). 

One factor that is not closely related to creativity is intelligence. Traditional intel- 
ligence tests, which ask focused questions that have only one acceptable answer, tap 
convergent thinking skills. Highly creative people may therefore find that such tests 
penalize their divergent thinking. This may explain why researchers consistently find 
that creativity is only slightly related to school grades and intelligence when intel- 
ligence is measured using traditional intelligence tests (Sternberg & O'Hara, 2000; 
Heilman, 2005). 


Can we learn to be better thinkers? 

Cognitive researchers have found that people can learn the 
abstract rules of logic and reasoning and that such knowledge can 
improve our reasoning about the underlying causes of everyday 
events in our lives. Research suggests that critical and creative 
thinkers are made, not born. Consider, for instance, the following 
suggestions for increasing critical thinking and creativity 
(Burbach, Matkin, & Fritz, 2004; Kaufman & Baer, 2006). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 24 Problem Solving 263 


Redefine problems. We can modify boundaries and assumptions by rephrasing a 
problem at either a more abstract or a more concrete level. 

Use subgoals. By developing subgoals, we can divide a problem into intermediate 
steps. This process, known as fractionation, allows us to examine each part for 
new possibilities and approaches, leading to a novel solution for the problem as a 
whole. 

Adopt a critical perspective. Rather than passively accepting assumptions or 
arguments, we can evaluate material critically, consider its implications, and 
think about possible exceptions and contradictions. 

Consider the opposite. By considering the opposite of a concept we’re seeking to 
understand, we can sometimes make progress. For example, to define “good 
mental health,” it may be useful to consider what “bad mental health” means. 
Use analogies. Analogies provide alternative frameworks for the interpretation of 
facts and help us uncover new understanding. One particularly effective means 
of coming up with analogies is to look for examples in the animal world. For 
instance, architects discovered how to construct the earliest skyscrapers by 
noting how lily pads on a pond could support the weight of a person (Getner & 
Holyoak, 1997; Bearman, Ball, & Omerod, 2007; Cho, Holyoak, & Cannon, 2007). 
Think divergently. Instead of the most logical or common use for an object, 
consider how you might use the object if you were forbidden to use it in the 
usual way. 

Use heuristics. Heuristics are cognitive shortcuts that can help bring about a 
solution to a problem. If the problem has a single correct answer and you can 
use or construct a heuristic, you can often find the solution more rapidly and 
effectively. 

Experiment with various solutions. Don’t be afraid to use different routes to find 
solutions for problems (verbal, mathematical, graphic, even dramatic). For 
instance, try to come up with every conceivable idea you can, no matter how 
wild or bizarre it may seem at first. After you’ve come up with a list of solutions, 
review each one and try to think of ways to make what at first appeared imprac- 
tical seem more feasible. 


FIGURE 11 A solution to the nine-dot 
problem requires the use of lines drawn 
beyond the boundaries of the figure— 
something that our mental set may 
prevent us from seeing easily. 


264 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 8 Cognition and Language 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 


How do people approach and solve problems? 


Problem solving typically involves three major stages: 
preparation, production of solutions, and evaluation of 
solutions that have been generated. (p. 251) 

Preparation involves placing the problem in one of three 
categories. In arrangement problems, a group of 
elements must be rearranged or recombined in a way 
that will satisfy a certain criterion. In problems of 
inducing structure, a person first must identify the 
existing relationships among the elements presented and 
then construct a new relationship among them. Finally, 
transformation problems consist of an initial state, a goal 
state, and a method for changing the initial state into the 
goal state. (p. 252) 

A crucial aspect of the preparation stage is the represen- 
tation and organization of the problem. (p. 254) 

In the production stage, people try to generate solutions. 
They may find solutions to some problems in long-term 
memory. Alternatively, they may solve some problems 
through simple trial and error and use algorithms and 
heuristics to solve more complex problems. (p. 255) 
Using the heuristic of a means-ends analysis, a person 
will repeatedly test for differences between the desired 
outcome and what currently exists, trying each time to 
come closer to the goal. (p. 255) 

Kohler’s research with chimpanzees illustrates insight, a 
sudden awareness of the relationships among elements 
that had previously seemed unrelated. (p. 256) 


What are the major obstacles to problem solving? 


Several factors hinder effective problem solving. Mental 
set, of which functional fixedness is an example, is the 
tendency for old patterns of problem solving to persist. 
Inappropriate use of algorithms and heuristics can also 
act as an obstacle to the production of solutions. 
Confirmation bias, in which initial hypotheses are 
favored, can hinder the accurate evaluation of solutions 
to problems. (p. 258) 


KEY TERMS 


means-ends analysis p.255 functional fixedness p. 259 
insight p. 257 mental set p. 259 


What is creativity? 

e Creativity is the ability to combine responses or ideas in 
novel ways. Creativity is related to divergent thinking 
(the ability to generate unusual, but still appropriate, 
responses to problems or questions) and cognitive 
complexity. (p. 261) 


EVALUATE 


1. Solving a problem by trying to reduce the difference 
between the current state and the goal state is known as a 


2, _______ is the term used to describe the sudden 
“flash” of revelation that often accompanies the solution 
to a problem. 

3. Thinking of an object only in terms of its typical use is 
known as . A broader, related 
tendency for old problem-solving patterns to persist is 
known as a . 

4. describes the phenomenon of 
favoring an initial hypothesis and ignoring subsequent 
competing hypotheses. 

5. Generating unusual but appropriate approaches to a ques- 
tion is known as 














RETHINK 


1. Is the reasoning in the following syllogism correct or 
incorrect? Why? 
Creative people often have trouble with traditional intelli- 
gence tests. 
I have trouble with traditional intelligence tests. Therefore, 
I am a creative person. 

2. From the perspective of a manufacturer: How might you 
encourage your employees to develop creative ways to 
improve the products that you produce? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


SUTYUTYY JUƏBIƏA1p 'G ‘serq UonewIyuo ‘h 
14əS Jez}uəw ‘ssəupəxy jeuompuny 'g 4UsIsut*z UOewIozsueLI ‘T 


confirmation bias p. 260 divergent thinking p. 262 
creativity p. 261 convergent thinking p. 262 





=~ _www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


‘Twas brillig, and the slithy toves 
Did gyre and gimble in the wabe: 
All mimsy were the borogoves, 
And the mome raths outgrabe. 


Although few of us have ever come face to face with a tove, we have little difficulty 
in discerning that in Lewis Carroll’s (1872) poem “Jabberwocky,” the expression slithy 
toves contains an adjective, slithy, and the noun it modifies, toves. 

Our ability to make sense out of nonsense, if the nonsense follows typical rules 
of language, illustrates the complexity of both human language and the cognitive 
processes that underlie its development and use. The use of language—the com- 
munication of information through symbols arranged according to systematic 
rules—is a central cognitive ability, one that is indispensable for us to communicate 
with one another. Not only is language central to communication, it is also closely 
tied to the very way in which we think about and understand the world. Without 
language, our ability to transmit information, acquire knowledge, and cooperate 
with others would be tremendously hindered. No wonder psychologists have 
devoted considerable attention to studying language (Stapel & Semin, 2007; Hoff, 
2008; Reisberg, 2009). 








Grammar: Language's Language 


To understand how language develops and relates to thought, we first need to review 
some of the formal elements of language. The basic structure of language rests on 
grammar, the system of rules that determine how our thoughts can be expressed. 

Grammar deals with three major components of language: phonology, syntax, 
and semantics. Phonology is the study of phonemes, the smallest basic units of 
speech that affect meaning, and of the way we use those sounds to form words and 
produce meaning. For instance, the a sound in fat and the a sound in fate represent 
two different phonemes in English (Hardison, 2006). 

Linguists have identified more than 800 different phonemes among all the 
world’s languages. Although English speakers use just 52 phonemes to produce 
words, other languages use as few as 15 to as many as 141. Differences in phonemes 
are one reason people have difficulty learning other languages. For example, to a 
Japanese speaker, whose native language does not have an r phoneme, pronouncing 
such English words as roar presents some difficulty (Gibbs, 2002; Iverson et al., 2003). 

Syntax refers to the rules that indicate how words and phrases can be combined 
to form sentences. Every language has intricate rules that guide the order in which 
words may be strung together to communicate meaning. English speakers have no 
difficulty recognizing that “TV down the turn” is not a meaningful sequence, 
whereas “Turn down the TV” is. To understand the effect of syntax in English, 
consider the changes in meaning caused by the different word orders in the follow- 
ing three utterances: “John kidnapped the boy,” “John, the kidnapped boy,” and 
“The boy kidnapped John” (Eberhard, Cutting, & Bock, 2005; Robert, 2006). 


How do people use language? 


How does language develop? 


The communication of 
information through symbols arranged 
according to systematic rules. 


The system of rules that 
determine how our thoughts can be 
expressed. 


The study of the smallest 
units of speech, called phonemes. 


The smallest units of speech. 


Ways in which words and 
phrases can be combined to form 
sentences. 


265 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


266 Chapter 8 Cognition and Language 


semantics The rules governing the The third major component of language is semantics, the meanings of words 

meaning of words and sentences. and sentences. Semantic rules allow us to use words to convey the subtle nuances 
in meaning. For instance, we are able to make the distinction between “The truck hit 
Laura” (which we might say if we had just seen a truck crashing into Laura) versus 
“Laura was hit by a truck” (which we might say to explain why Laura didn’t show 
up for a party) (Richgels, 2004; Pietarinen, 2006). 

Despite the complexities of language, most of us acquire the basics of grammar 
without even being aware that we have learned its rules. Moreover, even though we 
may have difficulty explicitly stating the rules of grammar, our linguistic abilities are 
so sophisticated that we can utter an infinite number of different statements. How 
do we acquire such abilities? 


Language Development: 
Developing a Way with Words 


To parents, the sounds of their infant babbling and cooing are music to their ears 
(except, perhaps, at three o’clock in the morning). These sounds also serve an 
important function. They mark the first step on the road to the development of 





language. 

BABBLING 
babble Meaningless speechlike Children babble—make speech-like but meaningless sounds—from around the age 
sounds made by children from around of 3 months through 1 year. While babbling, they may produce, at one time or 
the age of 3 months through 1 year. another, any of the sounds found in all languages, not just the one to which they are 


exposed. Even deaf children display their own form of babbling, for infants who are 
unable to hear yet who are exposed to sign language from birth “babble” with their 
hands (Pettito, 1993; Locke, 2006). 

An infant’s babbling increasingly reflects the specific language being spoken in 
the infant’s environment, initially in terms of pitch and tone and eventually in terms 
of specific sounds. Young infants can distinguish among all 869 phonemes that have 
been identified across the world’s languages. However, after the age of 6 to 8 months, 
that ability begins to decline. Infants begin to “specialize” in the language to which 
they are exposed as neurons in their brains reorganize to respond to the particular 
phonemes infants routinely hear. 

Some theorists argue that a critical period exists for language development early 
in life in which a child is particularly sensitive to language cues and most easily 
acquires language. In fact, if children are not exposed to language during this critical 
period, later they will have great difficulty overcoming this deficit (Bates, 2005; Shafer 
& Garrido-Nag, 2007). 

Cases in which abused children have been isolated from contact with others 
support the theory of such critical periods. In one case, for example, a girl named 
Genie was exposed to virtually no language from the age of 20 months until she was 
rescued at age 13. She was unable to speak at all. Despite intensive instruction, she 
learned only some words and was never able to master the complexities of language 
(Rymer, 1994; Veltman & Browne, 2001). 





PRODUCTION OF LANGUAGE 


A syllable in signed language, similar to 
the ones seen in the manual babbling of By the time children are approximately 1 year old, they stop producing sounds that 


deaf infants and in the spoken babbling are not in the language to which they have been exposed. It is then a short step to 
of hearing infants. The similarities in the production of actual words. In English, these are typically short words that start 
language structure suggest that with a consonant sound such as b, d, m, p, and t—this helps explain why mama and 


language has biological roots. dada are so often among babies’ first words. Of course, even before they produce 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


their first words, children can understand a fair amount of the language they hear. 
Language comprehension precedes language production. 

After the age of 1 year, children begin to learn more complicated forms of lan- 
guage. They produce two-word combinations, the building blocks of sentences, and 
sharply increase the number of different words they are able to use. By age 2, the 
average child has a vocabulary of more than 50 words. Just six months later, that 
vocabulary has grown to several hundred words. At that time, children can produce 
short sentences, although they use telegraphic speech—sentences that sound as if 
they were part of a telegram, in which words not critical to the message are left out. 
Rather than saying, “I showed you the book,” a child using telegraphic speech may 
say, “I show book,” and “I am drawing a dog” may become “Drawing dog.” As 
children get older, of course, they use less telegraphic speech and produce increas- 
ingly complex sentences (Volterra et al., 2003). 

By age 3, children learn to make plurals by adding s to nouns and to form the 
past tense by adding -ed to verbs. This skill also leads to errors, since children tend 
to apply rules inflexibly. In such overgeneralization, children employ rules even 
when doing so results in an error. Thus, although it is correct to say “he walked” for 
the past tense of walk, the -ed rule doesn’t work quite so well when children say “he 
runned” for the past tense of run (Howe, 2002; Rice et al., 2004; Gershkoff-Stowe, 
Connell, & Smith, 2006; Kidd & Lum, 2008). 

By age 5, children have acquired the basic rules of language. However, they do 
not attain a full vocabulary and the ability to comprehend and use subtle gram- 
matical rules until later. For example, a 5-year-old boy who sees a blindfolded doll 
and is asked, “Is the doll easy or hard to see?” would have great trouble answering 
the question. In fact, if he were asked to make the doll easier to see, he would prob- 
ably try to remove the doll’s blindfold. By the time they are 8 years old, however, 
children have little difficulty understanding this question because they realize that 
the doll’s blindfold has nothing to do with an observer’s ability to see the doll 
(Chomsky, 1968; Hoff, 2003). 


UNDERSTANDING LANGUAGE ACQUISITION: 
IDENTIFYING THE ROOTS OF LANGUAGE 


Anyone who spends even a little time with children will notice the enormous strides 
that they make in language development throughout childhood. However, the rea- 
sons for this rapid growth are far from obvious. Psychologists have offered two major 
explanations: one based on learning theory and the other based on innate processes. 


Learning Theory Approaches: Language as a Learned Skill. The learning-theory 
approach suggests that language acquisition follows the principles of reinforcement 
and conditioning discovered by psychologists who study learning. For example, a 
child who says “mama” receives hugs and praise from her mother, which reinforce 
the behavior of saying “mama” and make its repetition more likely. This view suggests 
that children first learn to speak by being rewarded for making sounds that approxi- 
mate speech. Ultimately, through a process of shaping, language becomes more and 
more like adult speech (Skinner, 1957; Ornat & Gallo, 2004). 

In support of the learning-theory approach to language acquisition, the more that 
parents speak to their young children, the more proficient the children become in 
language use. In addition, by the time they are 3 years old, children who hear higher 
levels of linguistic sophistication in their parents’ speech show a greater rate of 
vocabulary growth, vocabulary use, and even general intellectual achievement than 
do children whose parents’ speech is more simple (Hart & Risley, 1997). 

The learning-theory approach is less successful in explaining how children 
acquire language rules. Children are reinforced not only when they use language 
correctly, but also when they use it incorrectly. For example, parents answer a child’s 
query of “Why the dog won't eat?” as readily as they do the correctly phrased question, 


Module 25 Language 267 


telegraphic speech Sentences in 
which words not critical to the 
message are left out. 


overgeneralization The phenomenon 
by which children apply language 
rules even when the application results 
in an error. 


learning-theory approach (to 
language development) The theory 
that language acquisition follows the 
principles of reinforcement and 
conditioning. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


268 Chapter 8 Cognition and Language 


nativist approach (to language 
development) The theory that a 
genetically determined, innate mecha- 
nism directs language development. 


universal grammar Noam Chomsky’s 
theory that all the world’s languages 
share a common underlying structure. 


language-acquisition device A neural 
system of the brain hypothesized by 
Noam Chomsky to permit under- 
standing of language. 





Noam Chomsky argues that all languages share a universal grammar. 


“Why won't the dog eat?” Listeners understand both sentences equally well. Learn- 
ing theory, then, has difficulty fully explaining language acquisition. 


Nativist Approaches: Language as an Innate Skill. Pointing to such problems with 
learning-theory approaches to language acquisition, linguist Noam Chomsky (1968, 
1978, 1991) provided a groundbreaking alternative. Chomsky argued that humans 
are born with an innate linguistic capability that emerges primarily as a function of 
maturation. According to his nativist approach to language, all the world’s languages 
share a common underlying structure called a universal grammar. Chomsky sug- 
gested that the human brain has a neural system, the language-acquisition device 
that not only lets us understand the structure language provides, but also gives us 
strategies and techniques for learning the unique characteristics of our native lan- 
guage (McGilvray, 2004; Lidz & Gleitman, 2004; White, 2007). 

Chomsky used the concept of the language-acquisition device as a metaphor, 
and he did not identify a specific area of the brain in which it resides. However, 
evidence collected by neuroscientists suggests that the ability to use language, which 
was a significant evolutionary advance in human beings, is tied to specific neuro- 
logical developments (Sakai, 2005; Sahin, Pinker, & Halgren, 2006; Willems & 
Hagoort, 2007). 

For example, scientists have discovered a gene related to the development of 
language abilities that may have emerged as recently—in evolutionary terms—as 
100,000 years ago. Furthermore, it is clear that there are specific sites within the brain 
that are closely tied to language and that the shape of the human mouth and throat 
are tailored to the production of speech. And there is evidence that features of specific 
types of languages are tied to particular genes, such as in “tonal” languages in which 
pitch is used to convey meaning (Hauser, Chomsky, & Fitch, 2002; Chandra, 2007; 
Dediu & Ladd, 2007; Gontier, 2008; Grigorenko, 2009). 

Still, Chomsky’s view has its critics. For instance, learning theorists contend that 
the apparent ability of certain animals, such as chimpanzees, to learn the fundamen- 
tals of human language (as we discuss later in this module) contradicts the innate 
linguistic capability view. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Interactionist Approaches. To reconcile the differing views, many theorists take 
an interactionist approach to language development. The interactionist approach 
suggests that language development is produced through a combination of genet- 
ically determined predispositions and environmental circumstances that help 
teach language. 

Specifically, proponents of the interactionist approach suggest that the brain’s 
hardwired language-acquisition device that Chomsky and geneticists point to pro- 
vides the hardware for our acquisition of language, whereas the exposure to lan- 
guage in our environment that learning theorists observe allows us to develop the 
appropriate software. But the issue of how language is acquired remains hotly con- 
tested (Pinker & Jackendoff, 2005; Hoff, 2008; Waxman, 2009). 


The Influence of Language 
on Thinking: Do Eskimos 
Have More Words for Snow 
than Texans Do? 


Do Eskimos living in the frigid Arctic have a more expansive vocabulary for discuss- 
ing snow than people living in warmer climates? 

It makes sense, and arguments that the Eskimo language has many more words 
than English for snow have been made since the early 1900s. At that time, linguist 
Benjamin Lee Whorf contended that because snow is so relevant to Eskimos’ lives, 
their language provides a particularly rich vocabulary to describe it—considerably 
larger than what we find in other languages, such as English (Martin & Pullum, 1991; 
Pinker, 1994). 

The contention that the Eskimo language is especially abundant in snow-related 
terms led to the linguistic-relativity hypothesis, the notion that language shapes 
and, in fact, may determine the way people in a specific culture perceive and under- 
stand the world. According to this view, language provides us with categories that 
we use to construct our view of people and events in the world around us. Conse- 
quently, language shapes and produces thought (Whorf, 1956; Casasanto, 2008; Tan 
et al., 2008). 

Let’s consider another possibility, however. Suppose that instead of language 
being the cause of certain ways of thinking, thought produces language. The only 
reason to expect that Eskimo language might have more words for snow than English 
does is that snow is considerably more relevant to Eskimos than it is to people in 
other cultures. 

Which view is correct? Most recent research refutes the linguistic-relativity 
hypothesis and suggests, instead, that thinking produces language. In fact, new anal- 
yses of the Eskimo language suggest that Eskimos have no more words for snow 
than English speakers. If one examines the English language closely, one sees that it 
is hardly impoverished when it comes to describing snow (consider, for example, 
sleet, slush, blizzard, dusting, and avalanche). 

Still, the linguistic-relativity hypothesis has not been entirely discarded. A newer 
version of the hypothesis suggests that speech patterns may influence certain aspects 
of thinking. For example, in some languages, such as English, speakers distinguish 
between nouns that can be counted (such as “five chairs”) and nouns that require a 
measurement unit to be quantified (such as “a liter of water”). In some other lan- 
guages, such as the Mayan language called Yucatec, however, all nouns require a 
measurement unit. In such cultures, people appear to think more closely about what 
things are made of than do people in cultures in which languages such as English 





Module 25 Language 269 


interactionist approach (to language 
development) The view that language 
development is produced through a 
combination of genetically determined 
predispositions and environmental 
circumstances that help teach language. 


y Alert 


It’s important to be able to 
compare and contrast the 
major approaches to 
language development: 
learning theory, nativist, 
and interactionist approaches. 


linguistic-relativity hypothesis 

The notion that language shapes and 
may determine the way people in a 
particular culture perceive and 
understand the world. 


270 






www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 8 Cognition and Language 


y Alert 

The linguistic-relativity hypo- 

thesis suggests language 
leads to thought. 


© James Stevenson/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 





“He’s pretty good at rote categorization and single-object relational tasks 
but he’s not so hot at differentiating between representational and 
associational signs, and he’s very weak on syntax.” 


are spoken. In contrast, English speakers focus more on the shape of objects (Gentner, 
Goldin, & Goldin-Meadow, 2003; Tsukasaki & Ishii, 2004). 

In short, although research does not support the linguistic-relativity hypothesis that 
language causes thought, it is clear that language influences how we think. And, of 
course, it certainly is the case that thought influences language, suggesting that lan- 
guage and thinking interact in complex ways (Ross, 2004; Thorkildsen, 2006; Proud- 
foot, 2009). 


Do Animals Use Language? 


One question that has long puzzled psychologists is whether language is uniquely 
human or if other animals are able to acquire it as well. Many animals communicate 
with one another in rudimentary forms. For instance, fiddler crabs wave their claws 
to signal, bees dance to indicate the direction in which food will be found, and certain 
birds call “zick, zick” during courtship and “kia” when they are about to fly away. 
However, researchers have yet to demonstrate conclusively that these animals use true 
language, which is characterized in part by the ability to produce and communicate 
new and unique meanings by following a formal grammar. 

Psychologists, however, have been able to teach chimps to communicate at sur- 
prisingly high levels. For instance, after four years of training, a chimp named Washoe 
learned to make signs for 132 words and combine those signs into simple sentences. 
Even more impressively, Kanzi, a pygmy chimpanzee, has linguistic skills that some 
psychologists claim are close to those of a 2-year-old human being. Kanzi’s trainers 
suggest that he can create grammatically sophisticated sentences and can even invent 
new rules of syntax (Raffaele, 2006; Savage-Rumbaugh, Toth, & Schick, 2007). 

Despite the skills primates such as Kanzi display, critics contend that the lan- 
guage such animals use still lacks the grammar and the complex and novel construc- 
tions of human language. Instead, they maintain that the chimps are displaying a 
skill no different from that of a dog that learns to lie down on command to get a 
reward. Furthermore, we lack firm evidence that animals can recognize and respond 
to the mental states of others of their species, an important aspect of human 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 25 Language 271 





Sue Savage-Rumbaugh with a primate friend, Panbanisha. Does the use of sign language by 
primates indicate true mastery of language? 


communication. Consequently, the issue of whether other animals can use language 
in a way that humans do remains controversial (Aboitiz, Garcia, & Brunetti, 2006; 
Hillix, 2007; Liszkowski et al., 2009). 


enrolled in some form of bilingual or English as a Second 
Language instruction. And New York City is far from the only 
school district with a significant population of nonnative 
English speakers. From the biggest cities to the most rural areas, 
the face—and voice—of education in the United States is 
changing. More and more schoolchildren today have last names 
like Kim, Valdez, and Karachnicoff. In seven states, including Texas and Colorado, 
more than one-quarter of the students are not native English speakers. For some 
47 million Americans, English is their second language (Holloway, 2000; see Figure 1). 

How to appropriately and effectively teach the increasing number of children who 
do not speak English is not always clear. Many educators maintain that bilingual 
education is best. With a bilingual approach, students learn some subjects in their native 
language while simultaneously learning English. Proponents of bilingualism believe 
that students must develop a sound footing in basic subject areas and that, initially at 
least, teaching those subjects in their native language is the only way to provide them 
with that foundation. During the same period, they learn English, with the eventual 
goal of shifting all instruction into English. 

In contrast, other educators insist that all instruction ought to be in English from the 
moment students, including those who speak no English at all, enroll in school. In 
immersion programs, students are immediately plunged into English instruction in all 
subjects. The reasoning—endorsed by voters in California in a referendum designed to 
end bilingual education—is that teaching students in a language other than English 
simply hinders nonnative English speakers’ integration into society and ultimately 
does them a disservice. Proponents of English immersion programs point as evidence 
to improvements in standardized test scores that followed the end of bilingual 
education programs (Wildavsky, 2000). 


In New York City, 1 in 6 of the city’s 1.1 million students is Explori ng PEPPER g ; 


Teaching with Linguistic Variety: S 
Bilingual Education 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


272 Chapter 8 Cognition and Language 





WASHINGTON 





s ; | 1. RID ATER- am ta 


3 A l a NEW HAMPSHIRE 
a lat m MINNESOTA [SS Pe VERMONT MAINE 





























; F 

; DAAG E = seus fEHICHIGAN pew 

may y a Sel SS ee MASSACHUSETTS 

l B E, a se RHODE ISLAND 

w +— CONNECTICUT 
I NEW JERSEY 


DELAWARE 
MARYLAND 




























































































i ane J feat poe Number of 

Teas sete daresi non-English 

” mast? bi (Sasina ge i speakers (by county) 

CALIFORNIA E E "u n EuS UE AB 4 Aena Ho 

a 1 ma LAB EC EE I-99 

TAs SH [E 100-499 

LOUISIANA i — ]500-999 
HS | 000—4,999 
E 5,000-19,999 
E 20,000—49,999 
15 50,000-99,999 
E | 00,000-499,999 
1 500,000-999,999 
HSS |,000,000-3,500,000 
































FLORIDA 












kog 
HAWAII ® 














FIGURE 1 The language of diversity. Some 22% of the people in the United States speak 
a language other than English at home. Most of them speak Spanish; the rest speak an 
astounding variety of different languages. Where are the largest clusters of non-English 
speakers in the United States, and what do you think explains these concentrations? 
(Source: MLA Language Map, 2005, based on 2000 Census.) 


Although the controversial issue of bilingual education versus immersion has 
strong political undercurrents, evidence shows that the ability to speak two languages 
provides significant cognitive benefits over speaking only one language. For example, 
bilingual speakers show more cognitive flexibility and may understand concepts more 
easily than those who speak only one language. They have more linguistic tools for 
thinking because of their multiple-language abilities. In turn, this makes them more 
creative and flexible in solving problems (Heyman & Diesendruck, 2002; Bialystok & 
Martin, 2004; Kuo, 2007). 

Furthermore, speaking several languages changes the organization of the brain. 
For example, bilingual speakers who learn their second language as adults show 
different areas of brain activation compared with those who learn their second 
language in childhood. In addition, brain scans show that people who speak multiple 
languages have distinct patterns of brain activity according to the language that they 
are using (see Figure 2) (Kim et al., 1997; Kovelman, Baker, & Petitto, 2008; Kovacs & 
Mehler, 2009). 

Related to questions about bilingual education is the matter of biculturalism, that is, 
being a member of two cultures and its psychological impact. Some psychologists 
argue that society should promote an alternation model of bicultural competence. Such 
a model supports members of a culture in their efforts to maintain their original 
cultural identity as well as in their integration into the adopted culture. In this view, a 
person can belong to two cultures and have two cultural identities without having to 
choose between them. Whether society will adopt the alternation model remains to be 
seen (Carter, 2003; Benet-Martinez, Lee, & Leu, 2006; Tadmor, 2007). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 25 Language 273 


Bilingualism and the Brain 





FIGURE 2 Researchers studying bilinguals (individuals fluent in two languages) and 
monolinguals (individuals fluent in one language) found differences in the way in which 
language was processed between the two groups. For example, this fMRI scan shows that 
bilinguals process information differently according to whether they are speaking in only one 
language or are switching between languages. The results show that there may be distinct 
patterns of activity for each language used. (Source: Kovelman et al., 2008.) 





RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP The interactionist approach argues that language 
development is produced through a combination of 
genetically determined predispositions and environ- 
mental circumstances that help teach language. 

(p. 267) 

e The linguistic-relativity hypothesis suggests that 
language shapes and may determine the way people 
think about the world. Most evidence suggests that 
although language does not determine thought, it does 

How does language develop? affect the way people store information in memory and 

e Language production, which follows language compre- how well they can retrieve it. (p. 269) 
hension, develops out of babbling, which then leads to e The degree to which language is a uniquely human skill 
the production of actual words. After 1 year of age, remains an open question. Some psychologists contend 


How do people use language? 

e Language is the communication of information through 
symbols arranged according to systematic rules. All 
languages have a grammar—a system of rules that 
determines how thoughts can be expressed—that 
encompasses the three major components of language: 
phonology, syntax, and semantics. (p. 265) 


children use two-word combinations, increase their 
vocabulary, and use telegraphic speech, which drops 
words not critical to the message. By age 5, acquisition 
of language rules is relatively complete. (p. 266) 
Learning theorists suggest that language is acquired 
through reinforcement and conditioning. In contrast, 
the nativist approach suggests that an innate language- 


acquisition device guides the development of language. 


that even though certain primates communicate at a 
high level, those animals do not use language. Other 
psychologists suggest that those primates truly 
understand and produce language in much the same 
way as humans. (p. 270) 

People who speak more than one language may have a 
cognitive advantage over those who speak only one. 
(p. 271) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


274 Chapter 8 Cognition and Language 


EVALUATE 6. In his theory of language acquisition, Chomsky argues 
that language acquisition is an innate ability tied to the 


1. Match the component of grammar with its definition: ead Ine Dinin E E 


1. Syntax a. Rules showing how words can 


2. Phonology be combined into sentences. 
3. Semantics b. Rules governing the meaning of 
words and sentences. 
c. The study of the sound units 
that affect speech. 
. Language production and language comprehension 
develop in infants about the same time. True or false? 
refers to the phenomenon in 
which young children omit nonessential portions of 
sentences. 





RETHINK 


1. 


Do people who use two languages, one at home and one 
at school, automatically have two cultures? Why might 
people who speak two languages have cognitive advan- 
tages over those who speak only one? 


. From the perspective of a childcare provider: How would you 


encourage children’s language abilities at the different 
stages of development? 


4, Achild knows that adding -ed to certain words puts them 
in the past tense. As a result, instead of saying “He came,” 
the child says “He comed.” This is an example of 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 

an} °9 
i ‘Zuue g ‘uoyezyerouasIso ‘p ‘ydaeds oryder8əja; g tuoyonpord 
5, theory assumes that language acquisition is əZen3ZueĽg sapadaid uorsuəyərdwoo a8ensury ‘aspey 7 /q-¢ ‘0-7 ‘e-L “LE 
based on principles of operant conditioning and shaping. 


KEY TERMS 


language p. 265 
grammar p. 265 
phonology p. 265 
phonemes p. 265 
syntax p. 265 
semantics p. 266 


babble p. 266 
telegraphic speech p. 267 
overgeneralization p. 267 
learning-theory approach 
(to language 
development) p. 267 


nativist approach (to 
language development) 
p. 268 

universal grammar p. 268 

language-acquisition 
device p. 268 


interactionist approach 
(to language 
development) p. 269 

linguistic-relativity 
hypothesis p. 269 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Psychology on the Web 


1. In addition to mental images of sights and sounds, are there mental representations 
that correspond to the other senses? See whether you can answer this question by 
searching the web. Summarize your findings in writing. 

2. Do animals think? What evidence is there on either side of this question? Search the 
web for at least one example of research and/or argument on each side of this 
question. Summarize your findings and use your knowledge of cognitive psychology 
to state your own position on this question. 


E © | | OQ U eC The study of cognition occupies a central place in the 


field of psychology and encompasses a variety of 
areas—including thinking, problem solving, decision-making, creativity, language, 
memory, and intelligence. Specialists in cognitive psychology have made significant 
advances in the last decade that have influenced the entire field of psychology. 

Before proceeding, turn back to the prologue about Clifford Matson’s “Eureka!” 
moment. Answer the following questions in light of what you have learned about 
reasoning, problem solving, and creativity: 





1. What factors led up to Matson’s invention of Cork-EZ? 

2. How do the concepts of functional fixedness and mental set relate to Matson’s 
inventiveness? Are they related to the notion of prototypes? 

3. How do you think insight is involved in Matson’s inventiveness? 

4. In what ways do you think divergent and convergent thinking are involved in the pro- 
cesses of invention? Do they play different roles in the various stages of the act of 
invention, including identifying the need for an invention, devising possible solutions, 
and creating a practical invention? 





275 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Key Concepts for Chapter 9 


MODULE 26 






What are the different definitions and What Is Intelligence? 


Theories of Intelligence: Are There 


conceptions of intelligence? @ What are the Different Kinds of intelligence? 





major approaches to measuring intelligence, The Biological Basis of Intelligence 


Neuroscience in Your Life: 


and what do intelligence tests measure? Your Intelligent Brain 


Practical and Emotional Intelligence: 
Toward a More Intelligent 

View of Intelligence 

Applying Psychology 

in the 21st Century: 

Artificial Intelligence: 

Building a Smarter Robot 


Assessing Intelligence 


Contemporary IQ Tests: 
Gauging Intelligence 
Reliability and Validity: 
Taking the Measure of Tests 


Adaptive Testing: Using Computers 
to Assess Performance 


Becoming an Informed Consumer 
of Psychology: Scoring Better 
on Standardized Tests 





MODULE 27 








How can the extremes of intelligence be Variations in Intellectual Ability 
Mental Retardation 
(Intellectual Disabilities) 


their full potential? PsychWore Director 
of Special Education 


The Intellectually Gifted 


characterized? @ How can we help people reach 


MODULE 28 





Are traditional IQ tests culturally biased? Group Differences in Intelligence: 


Genetic and Environmental 


@ Are there racial differences in intelligence? Dēterminants 


Exploring Diversity: The Relative 
Influence of Genetics and Environment: 
Nature, Nurture, and IQ 


@ To what degree is intelligence influenced 
by the environment and to what degree by 
heredity? 


277 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Prologue Chris Burke and Sho Yano 










When Chris Burke was born, physicians suggested 
to his parents that he immediately be institu- 
tionalized. He had Down syndrome, a common 
genetic defect that produces mental retarda- 
tion, and the physicians predicted that the 
chances of his leading anything like a normal 
life were slim. Chris's parents ignored the 
advice and raised Chris like their other 
children. 
~~~ It was a decision that led to an extraordi- 
nary life in which Chris has made significant 
contributions to society. He speaks regularly on 
behalf of people with developmental.disabilities, has coau- 
thored book chapters, and has appeared regularly in a variety 
of television shows. (Hassold & Patterson,\1999; Burke & 
McDaniel, 2001) 


Writing at age 2. Reading at age 3. Playing classical music on 
the piano at age 4, and composing music at age 5. 

But that's just the start of Sho Yano’s resume. With an IQ 
measured at 200, he entered Loyola University at 
the age of 9 and graduated summa cum 
laude at the age of 12. Then it was off to 
a joint graduate program in molecular 
genetics and medicine. He received a 
PhD at the age of 18, and he’s now in 
medical school. Along the way, he 
received a black belt in tae kwon do 
and played piano on a national radio 
program. He's also prone to under- 
statement, saying, “It’s just fun learning.’ 
(People Weekly, 2003, p. 125; Breslin, 2004; 
University of Chicago, 2009) 











Two very different people with widely different intellectual capabi- 
lities and strengths—and yet, at their core, Chris Burke and Sho 
Yano share basic aspects of humanity and even, one could argue, 
intelligence that ultimately make them more similar than different. 

In the following modules, we consider intelligence in all its 
many varieties. Intelligence represents a focal point for psycholo- 
gists intent on understanding how people are able to adapt their 
behavior to the environment in which they live. It also represents 
a key aspect of how individuals differ from one another in the 
way in which they learn about and understand the world. 

We begin by considering the challenges involved in defining 
and measuring intelligence. If you are like most people, you 


278 


have probably wondered how smart you are compared with 
others. Psychologists, too, have pondered the nature of intelli- 
gence. We examine some of their conceptions of intelligence as 
well as efforts to develop and use standardized tests as a means 
of measuring intelligence. We also consider the two extremes of 
individual differences in intelligence: mental retardation and 
giftedness. Finally, we explore what are probably the two most 
controversial issues surrounding intelligence: the degree to 
which intelligence is influenced by heredity and by the environ- 
ment and whether traditional tests of intelligence are biased 
toward the dominant cultural groups in society—difficult issues 
that have both psychological and social significance. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 26 


Members of the Trukese tribe in the South Pacific often sail a hundred miles in open 
ocean waters. Although their destination may be just a small dot of land less than a mile 


What are the different defini- 
tions and conceptions of 
intelligence? 


wide, the Trukese are able to navigate precisely toward it without the aid of a compass, 
chronometer, sextant, or any of the other sailing tools that are used by Western naviga- 
tors. They are able to sail accurately even when the winds do not allow a direct approach 


to the island and they must take a zigzag course. (Gladwin, 1964; Mytinger, 2001) : 
What are the major approaches 


to measuring intelligence, and 
what do intelligence tests 
measure? 


How are the Trukese able to navigate so effectively? If you asked them, they could 
not explain it. They might tell you that they use a process that takes into account the 
rising and setting of the stars and the appearance, sound, and feel of the waves 
against the side of the boat. But at any given moment as they are sailing along, they 
could not identify their position or say why they are doing what they are doing. Nor 
could they explain the navigational theory underlying their sailing technique. 

Some people might say that the inability of the Trukese to explain in Western 
terms how their sailing technique works is a sign of primitive or even unintelligent 
behavior. In fact, if we gave Trukese sailors a Western standardized test of naviga- 
tional knowledge and theory or, for that matter, a traditional test of intelligence, they 
might do poorly on it. Yet, as a practical matter, it is not possible to accuse the Trukese 
of being unintelligent: Despite their inability to explain how they do it, they are able 
to navigate successfully through the open ocean waters. 

Trukese navigation points out the difficulty in coming to grips with what is 
meant by intelligence. To a Westerner, traveling in a straight line along the most 
direct and quickest route by using a sextant and other navigational tools is likely to 
represent the most “intelligent” kind of behavior; in contrast, a zigzag course, based 
on the “feel” of the waves, would not seem very reasonable. To the Trukese, who 
are used to their own system of navigation, however, the 
use of complicated navigational tools might seem so overly 
complex and unnecessary that they might think of Western 
navigators as lacking in intelligence. 

It is clear from this example that the term intelligence can 
take on many different meanings. If, for instance, you lived 
in a remote part of the Australian outback, the way you 
would differentiate between more intelligent and less intel- 
ligent people might have to do with successfully mastering 
hunting skills, whereas to someone living in the heart of 
urban Miami, intelligence might be exemplified by being 
“streetwise” or by achieving success in business. 

Each of these conceptions of intelligence is reasonable. 
Each represents an instance in which more intelligent people 
are better able to use the resources of their environment than 
are less intelligent people, a distinction that is presumably 
basic to any definition of intelligence. Yet it is also clear that 
these conceptions represent very different views of intelligence. 





That two such different sets of behavior can exemplify 
the same psychological concept has long posed a challenge 


What does the Trukese people's method of navigation—which is done 
without maps or instruments—tell us about the nature of intelligence? 


279 


280 Chapter 9 Intelligence 


intelligence The capacity to under- 
stand the world, think rationally, and 
use resources effectively when faced 
with challenges. 


g or g-factor The single, general factor 
for mental ability assumed to underlie 
intelligence in some early theories of 
intelligence. 


fluid intelligence Intelligence that 
reflects information-processing 
capabilities, reasoning, and memory. 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


to psychologists. For years they have grappled with the issue of devising a general 
definition of intelligence. Ironically, laypersons have fairly clear ideas of what intel- 
ligence is, although the nature of their ideas is related to their culture. Westerners 
view intelligence as the ability to establish categories and debate rationally. In con- 
trast, people in Eastern cultures and some African communities view intelligence 
more in terms of understanding and relating to one another (Nisbett, 2003; Brislin, 
Worthley, & MacNab, 2006; Sternberg, 2005, 2007). 

The definition of intelligence that psychologists employ contains some of the 
same elements found in the layperson’s conception. To psychologists, intelligence is 
the capacity to understand the world, think rationally, and use resources effectively 
when faced with challenges. 

This definition does not lay to rest a key question asked by psychologists: Is 
intelligence a unitary attribute, or are there different kinds of intelligence? We turn 
now to various theories of intelligence that address the issue. 


Theories of Intelligence: 
Are There Different Kinds 
of Intelligence? 


Perhaps you see yourself as a good writer but as someone who lacks ability in math. 
Or maybe you view yourself as a “science” person who easily masters physics but 
has few strengths in interpreting literature. Perhaps you view yourself as generally 
fairly smart with intelligence that permits you to excel across domains. 

The different ways in which people view their own talents mirror a question that 
psychologists have grappled with. Is intelligence a single, general ability, or is it 
multifaceted and related to specific abilities? Early psychologists interested in intel- 
ligence assumed that there was a single, general factor for mental ability, which they 
called g, or the g-factor. This assumption was based on the fact that different types 
of measures of intelligence, whether they focused on, say, mathematical expertise, 
verbal competency, or spatial visualization skills, all ranked test-takers in roughly 
the same order. People who were good on one test generally were good on others; 
those who did poorly on one test tended to do poorly on others. 

Given that there was a correlation between performance on the different types of 
tests, the assumption was that there was a general, global intellectual ability underly- 
ing performance on the various measures—the g-factor. This 
general intelligence factor was thought to underlie performance 
in every aspect of intelligence, and it was the g-factor that was 
presumably being measured on tests of intelligence (Spearman, 
1927; Colom, Jung, & Haier, 2006; Haier et al., 2009). 

More recent theories explain intelligence in a different 
light. Rather than viewing intelligence as a unitary entity, 
some psychologists consider it to be a multidimensional con- 
cept that includes different types of intelligence (Tenopyr, 
2002; Stankov, 2003; Sternberg & Pretz, 2005). 





FLUID AND CRYSTALLIZED INTELLIGENCE 


Some psychologists suggest that there are two different kinds 
of intelligence: fluid intelligence and crystallized intelligence. 
Fluid intelligence reflects information-processing capabili- 


Piloting a helicopter requires the use of both fluid intelligence and ties, reasoning, and memory. Fluid intelligence encompasses 
crystallized intelligence. Which of the two kinds of intelligence do the ability to reason abstractly. If we were asked to solve an 
you believe is more important for such a task? analogy, group a series of letters according to some criterion, 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 26 What Is Intelligence? 281 


or remember a set of numbers, we would be using fluid 
intelligence. We use fluid intelligence when we're trying 
to rapidly solve a puzzle (Kane & Engle, 2002; Saggino 
Perfetti, & Spitoni, 2006; Di Fabio & Palazzeschi, 2009). 


In contrast, crystallized intelligence is the accumula- PE G A 
tion of information, skills, and strategies that people have | 
learned through experience and that they can apply in go / 
problem-solving situations. It reflects our ability to call up ‘ f | 
information from long-term memory. We would be likely i i 
to rely on crystallized intelligence, for instance, if we were / eS 
asked to participate in a discussion about the solution to 2 es oo 
the causes of poverty, a task that allows us to draw on our d I< \ 
own past experiences and knowledge of the world. In NO Can 


contrast to fluid intelligence, which reflects a more general 
kind of intelligence, crystallized intelligence is more a 
reflection of the culture in which a person is raised. The 
differences between fluid intelligence and crystallized 
intelligence become especially evident in late adulthood, when people show declines 
in fluid, but not crystallized, intelligence (Aartsen, Martin, & Zimprich, 2002; Bueh- 
ner, Krumm, & Ziegler, 2006; Tranter & Koutstaal, 2008). 


GARDNER’S MULTIPLE INTELLIGENCES: 
THE MANY WAYS OF SHOWING INTELLIGENCE 


Psychologist Howard Gardner has taken an approach very different from traditional 
thinking about intelligence. Gardner argues that rather than asking “How smart are 
you?” we should be asking a different question: “How are you smart?” In answering 
the latter question, Gardner has developed a theory of multiple intelligences that 
has become quite influential (Gardner, 2000). 

Gardner argues that we have a minimum eight different forms of intelligence, 
each relatively independent of the others: musical, bodily kinesthetic, logical- 
mathematical, linguistic, spatial, interpersonal, intrapersonal, and naturalist. (Figure 1 
describes the eight types of intelligence, with some of Gardner’s examples of people 
who excel in each type.) In Gardner’s view, each of the multiple intelligences is linked 
to an independent system in the brain. Furthermore, he suggests that there may be 
even more types of intelligence, such as existential intelligence, which involves identify- 
ing and thinking about the fundamental questions of human existence. For example, 
the Dalai Lama might exemplify this type of intelligence (Gardner, 1999, 2000). 

Although Gardner illustrates his conception of the specific types of intelligence 
with descriptions of well-known people, each person has the same eight kinds of 
intelligence—in different degrees. Moreover, although the eight basic types of intel- 
ligence are presented individually, Gardner suggests that these separate intelligences 
do not operate in isolation. Normally, any activity encompasses several kinds of 
intelligence working together. 

The concept of multiple intelligences has led to the development of intelligence 
tests that include questions in which more than one answer can be correct; these 
provide an opportunity for test takers to demonstrate creative thinking. In addition, 
many educators, embracing the concept of multiple intelligences, have designed 
classroom curricula that are meant to draw on different aspects of intelligence (Kelly 
& Tangney, 2006; Douglas, Burton, & Reese-Durham, 2008; Tirri & Nokelainen, 2008). 


IS INFORMATION PROCESSING INTELLIGENCE? 


One of the newer contributions to understanding intelligence comes from the work 
of cognitive psychologists who take an information-processing approach. They assert 
that the way people store information in memory and use that information to solve 


n S) a g W Ste” 


J 
J 
=," 
4 
at 
— 
© WB. Park/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


“To be perfectly frank, I’m not nearly as smart 
as you seem to think I am.” 


crystallized intelligence The 
accumulation of information, skills, 
and strategies that are learned through 
experience and can be applied in 
problem-solving situations. 


theory of multiple intelligences 
Gardner’s intelligence theory that 
proposes that there are eight distinct 
spheres of intelligence. 


y Alert 


Remember, Gardner's 
theory suggests that each 
individual has every kind 
of intelligence but in 
different degrees. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


282 Chapter 9 Intelligence 





- Musical intelligence (skills in tasks involving music). 
Case example: 


When he was 3, Yehudi Menuhin was smuggled 
into San Francisco Orchestra concerts by his 
parents. By the time he was |0 years old, 
Menuhin was an international 

performer. 


2 . Bodily k meer Pelee (skills in using the mee iai or 


various portions of it in the solution of problems or in the 
construction of products or displays, exemplified by dancers, athletes, 
actors, and surgeons). Case example: 





Fifteen-year-old Babe Ruth played third 
base. During one game, his team’s 
pitcher was doing very poorly and Babe 
loudly criticized him from third base. 
Brother Matthias, the coach, called out, 
"Ruth, if you know so much about it, you 
pitch!” Ruth said later that at the very 
moment he took the pitcher's mound, 
he knew he was supposed to be a 
pitcher 





- Logical-mathematical intelligence 
(skills in problem solving and 
scientific thinking). Case example: 


Barbara McClintock, who won the 
Nobel Prize in medicine, describes 
one of her breakthroughs, which 
came after thinking about a problem 
for half an hour ...:"‘Suddenly | 
jumped and ran back to the (corn) 
field. At the top of the field (the 
others were still at the bottom) | 
shouted, ‘Eureka, | have it!” 












e Linguistic intelligence (skills 
involved in the production and use 
of language). Case example: 





At the age of 10,T. S. Eliot created a 
magazine called Fireside, to which 
he was the sole contributor. 





5. Spatial intelligence (skills involving - 


spatial configurations, such as those used 
by artists and architects). Case example: 


Natives of the Truk Islands navigate at sea 
without instruments. During the actual 
trip, the navigator must envision mentally a 
reference island as it passes under a 
particular star and from that he computes 
the number of segments completed, the 
proportion of the trip remaining, and any 
corrections in heading. 





6. Interpersonal we 
- 


(skills in interacting with others, 
such as sensitivity to the moods, 
temperaments, motivations, and 
intentions of others). Case example: 





When Anne Sullivan began instructing 
the deaf and blind Helen Keller, her task 
was one that had eluded others for 
years. Yet, just two weeks after 
beginning her work with Keller, Sullivan 
achieved great success. 





. Intrapersonal intelligence (knowledge of the internal aspects o 
oneself; access to one’s own feelings and emotions). Case example: 
In her essay “A Sketch of the Past,’ Virginia Woolf displays deep insight 
into her own inner life through these lines, 
describing her reaction to several specific 
memories from her childhood that still, in 
adulthood, shock her: “Though | still have 
the peculiarity that | receive these sudden 
shocks, they are now always welcome; 
after the first surprise, | always feel instantly 
that they are particularly valuable. And so | 
go on to suppose that the shock-receiving 
capacity is what makes me a writer” 


a 








patterns in nature). Case example: 


During prehistoric times, hunter/ 
gatherers would rely on naturalist 
intelligence to identify what flora and 
fauna were edible. People who are 
adept at distinguishing nuances 
between large numbers of similar 
objects may be expressing naturalist 
intelligence abilities. 





FIGURE 1 Howard Gardner believes that there are eight major kinds of intelligences, 
corresponding to abilities in different domains. In what area does your greatest intelligence 
reside, and why do you think you have particular strengths in that area? (Source: Adapted from 


Gardner, 2000.) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


intellectual tasks provides the most accurate measure of intelligence. Consequently, 
rather than focusing on the structure of intelligence or its underlying content or 
dimensions, information-processing approaches examine the processes involved in 
producing intelligent behavior (Hunt, 2005; Neubauer & Fink, 2005; Pressley & 
Harris, 2006). 

For example, research shows that people with high scores on tests of intelligence 
spend more time on the initial encoding stages of problems, identifying the parts of 
a problem and retrieving relevant information from long-term memory, than do 
people with lower scores. This initial emphasis on recalling relevant information pays 
off in the end; those who use this approach are more successful in finding solutions 
than are those who spend relatively less time on the initial stages (Sternberg, 1990; 
Deary & Der, 2005; Hunt, 2005). 

Other information-processing approaches examine the sheer speed of processing. 
For example, research shows that the speed with which people are able to receive 
information from memory is related to verbal intelligence. In general, people with 
high scores on measures of intelligence react more quickly on a variety of information- 
processing tasks ranging from reactions to flashing lights to distinguishing between 
letters. The speed of information processing, then, may underlie differences in intel- 
ligence (Jensen, 2005; Gontkovsky & Beatty, 2006; Helmbold, Troche, & Rammsayer, 
2007; Sheppard & Vernon, 2008). 


The Biological Basis 
of Intelligence 


Using brain-scanning methods, researchers have identified several areas of the brain 
that relate to intelligence. For example, the brains of people completing intelligence 
test questions in both verbal and spatial domains show activation in a similar loca- 
tion: the lateral prefrontal cortex. That area is above the outer edge of the eyebrow 
about where people rest their heads in the palms of their hands if they are thinking 
hard about a problem. This area of the brain is critical to juggling many pieces of 
information simultaneously and solving new problems. In addition, higher intelli- 
gence is related to the thickness of the cerebral cortex (Schaer & Eliez, 2007; Karama 
et al., 2009; Luders et al., 2009). 

Similarly, tasks requiring different types of intelligence involve different areas of 
the brain. For instance, as can be seen in Figure 2, when tasks require more general 
global intelligence, more areas of the brain are involved than tasks requiring more spe- 
cific sorts of intelligence (such as fluid and crystallized intelligence) (Colom et al., 2009). 

These findings suggest that there is a global “workspace” in the brain that orga- 
nizes and coordinates information, helping to transfer material to other parts of the 
brain. In this view, the activity in the workspace represents general intelligence (Gray, 
Chabris, & Braver, 2003). 

Research using nonhumans has also begun to help us better understand the 
biological underpinnings of intelligence. For example, rats raised in enriched envi- 
ronments (meaning an environment containing more toys, tunnels, and so on) 
develop more complex connections between neurons along with more rapid learning 
and better memory. Other studies show differences in metabolism (the rate at which 
food is converted to energy and expended by the body) that seem to be related to 
intelligence (Rampon et al., 2000; Haier, 2003; Yang et al., 2007). 

Ultimately, the search for the biological underpinnings of intelligence will not 
lead in a single direction. Rather, the most plausible scenario is that multiple areas 
of the brain, as well as multiple kinds of functioning, are related to intelligent 
behavior. 





Module 26 What Is Intelligence? 


283 


284 Chapter 9 Intelligence 


practical intelligence According to 
Sternberg, intelligence related to 
overall success in living. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Your Intelligent Brain 


FIGURE 2 Neuroscientists are finding that separate brain areas are involved in different kinds 
of intelligence. For example, the images in (a) show areas of brain activation associated with 
tasks that specifically tap general intelligence. In comparison, the images in (b) show areas of 
brain activation associated with tasks that tap more crystallized intelligence. You can see that 
some of the areas are similar. (Source: Colom et al., 2009, adapted by Kevin Head.) 


(a) 


(b) 





Practical and Emotional 
Intelligence: Toward a More 
Intelligent View of Intelligence 


Consider the following situation: 





An employee who reports to one of your subordinates has asked to talk with you about 
waste, poor management practices, and possible violations of both company policy and the 
law on the part of your subordinate. You have been in your present position only a year, 
but in that time you have had no indications of trouble about the subordinate in question. 
Neither you nor your company has an “open door” policy, so it is expected that employees 
should take their concerns to their immediate supervisors before bringing a matter to the 
attention of anyone else. The employee who wishes to meet with you has not discussed 
this matter with her supervisors because of its delicate nature. (Sternberg, 1998, p. 17) 


Your response to this situation has a lot to do with your future success in a business 
career, according to psychologist Robert Sternberg. The question is one of a series 
designed to help give an indication of your intelligence. However, it is not traditional 
intelligence that the question is designed to tap but rather intelligence of a specific kind: 
practical intelligence. Practical intelligence is intelligence related to overall success in 
living (Sternberg, 2000, 2002; Sternberg & Hedlund, 2002; Wagner, 2002; Muammar, 2007). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


You are given a map of an entertainment 
park. You walk from the lemonade stand 
to the computer games arcade. Your 
friend walks from the shooting gallery to 
the roller coaster. Which of these are 
you BOTH most likely to pass? (a) the 
merry-go-round, (b) the music hall, 

(c) the pizza stand, or (d) the dog show. 












eS 8 


Noting that traditional tests were designed to relate to academic success, Stern- 
berg points to evidence showing that most traditional measures of intelligence do 
not relate especially well to career success (McClelland, 1993). Specifically, although 
successful business executives usually score at least moderately well on intelligence 
tests, the rate at which they advance and their ultimate business achievements are 
only minimally associated with traditional measures of their intelligence. 

Sternberg argues that career success requires a very different type of intelligence 
from that required for academic success. Whereas academic success is based on 
knowledge of a specific information base obtained from reading and listening, practi- 
cal intelligence is learned mainly through observation of others’ behavior. People who 
are high in practical intelligence are able to learn general norms and principles and 
apply them appropriately. Consequently, practical intelligence tests, like the one 
shown in Figure 3, measure the ability to employ broad principles in solving everyday 
problems (Sternberg & Pretz, 2005; Stemler & Sternberg, 2006; Stemler et al., 2009). 

In addition to practical intelligence, Sternberg argues there are two other basic, inter- 
related types of intelligence related to life success: analytical and creative. Analytical 
intelligence focuses on abstract but traditional types of problems measured on IQ tests, 
while creative intelligence involves the generation of novel ideas and products (Benderly, 
2004; Sternberg, Kaufman, & Pretz, 2004; Sternberg, Grigorenko, & Kidd, 2005). 

Some psychologists broaden the concept of intelligence even further beyond the 
intellectual realm to include emotions. Emotional intelligence is the set of skills that 
underlie the accurate assessment, evaluation, expression, and regulation of emotions 
(Mayer, Salovey, & Caruso, 2004; Humphrey, Curran, & Morris, 2007; Mayer, Salovey, 
& Caruso, 2008). 

Emotional intelligence is the basis of empathy for others, self-awareness, and 
social skills. It encompasses the ability to get along well with others. It provides us 


Module 26 What Is Intelligence? 285 


FIGURE 3 Most standard tests of 
intelligence primarily measure analytical 
skills; more comprehensive tests 
measure creative and practical abilities 
as well. (Source: Sternberg, 2000, p. 389.) 


emotional intelligence The set of 
skills that underlie the accurate 
assessment, evaluation, expression, 
and regulation of emotions. 


n 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


the 21st Century 





Artificial Intelligence: 
Building a Smarter Robot 


A robot that can open doors and find 
electrical outlets to recharge itself. 
Computer viruses that no one can 
stop. Predator drones, which, though 
still controlled remotely by humans, 
come close to a machine that can kill 
on its own (Markoff, 2009). 


Are computers on the verge of outsmarting 
humans? That’s the concern of specialists 
in artificial intelligence, a field of science that 
studies whether computers and other ma- 
chines can learn to perceive their environ- 
ment and respond in a way that mimics 
human intelligence. 

As artificial intelligence technology rap- 
idly advances, increasingly sophisticated 
machines that can simulate a host of human 
functions are now available or are on the ho- 
rizon. The military uses intelligent guidance 
systems that help bombs find their targets 
and remote-operated drones that perform 
dangerous combat missions. Department 
stores even sell robotic vacuum cleaners 
that independently roam across floors and 
carpets to track down and sweep up dirt 
and then park themselves to recharge for 
the next day (Aguilar et al., 2009). 

Most of us see these advances as benefi- 
cial to society, allowing machines to per- 
form tasks that are tedious or even 
dangerous for people to handle. But some 
are concerned that the use of artificial intel- 
ligence systems may be going too far, to the 





y Alert 


Traditional intelligence 
relates to academic 
performance; practical 
intelligence relates to 
success in life; emotional 
intelligence relates to 
emotional skills. 


286 





The vacuum cleaner is just one of many kinds 
of robots that are becoming increasingly 
sophisticated. 


point where people are handing over im- 
portant decisions to cold, unfeeling ma- 
chines. The prospect of automated systems 
that make medical decisions or robots that 
hunt down and kill people may be a bit far- 


fetched, but scientists are nevertheless 
echoing laypeople’s concerns that we may 
be handing over too much control to tech- 
nology (Foster & Stiffman, 2009). 

Artificial intelligence and robotics re- 
searchers have pointed to a number of con- 
cerns. For example, some potential problems 
include possible criminal uses of artificial 
intelligence systems, such as the develop- 
ment of insidious computer viruses or the 
use of technology to steal sensitive personal 
data. Other less dramatic concerns include 
the possibility of automated systems dis- 
placing human jobs as machines become 
sophisticated enough to take over functions 
that currently only humans can perform, 
such as operating cars or taking telephone 
calls (Association for the Advancement of 
Artificial Intelligence, 2009; Mobasher & 
Tuzhilin, 2009). 

The overriding concern is that artificial 
intelligence technology is progressing 
faster than society is prepared to deal 
with it. We still have a number of social, 
ethical, and legal questions to grapple 
with, even as machines become increas- 
ingly suited to perform human functions. 
Scientists want to be sure that such ma- 
chines continue to benefit society into the 
future rather than replace old problems 
with new ones (Markoff, 2009). 


ETHINK 

e What kinds of human intelligence might machines be unable to mimic? 

e Do you believe society should place limitations on just how smart machines can 
become? 














with an understanding of what other people are feeling and experiencing, which 
permits us to respond appropriately to others’ needs. These abilities may help explain 
why people with only modest scores on traditional intelligence tests can be quite 
successful: the basis of their success may be a high emotional intelligence, which 
allows them to respond appropriately and quickly to others’ feelings. 

Although the notion of emotional intelligence makes sense, it has yet to be quan- 
tified in a rigorous manner. Furthermore, the view that emotional intelligence is so 
important that skills related to it should be taught in school has raised concerns 
among some educators. They suggest that the nurturance of emotional intelligence 
is best left to students’ families, especially because there is no well-specified set of 
criteria for what constitutes emotional intelligence (Sleek, 1997; Becker, 2003). 

Still, the notion of emotional intelligence reminds us that there are many ways 
to demonstrate intelligent behavior—just as there are multiple views of the nature of 
intelligence (Fox & Spector, 2000; Barrett & Salovey, 2002). Figure 4 presents a sum- 
mary of the different approaches used by psychologists, and Applying Psychology in 
the 21st Century discusses advances in artificial intelligence. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Major Approaches to Intelligence 





Approach Characteristics 
Fluid and crystallized Fluid intelligence relates to reasoning, memory, 
intelligence and information-processing capabilities; crystallized 


intelligence relates to information, skills, and 
strategies learned through experience 


Gardner's multiple intelligences Eight independent forms of intelligence 


Information-processing Intelligence is reflected in the ways people store 
approaches and use material to solve intellectual tasks 
Practical intelligence Intelligence in terms of nonacademic, career, and 


personal success 


Emotional intelligence Intelligence that provides an understanding of what 
other people are feeling and experiencing and permits 
us to respond appropriately to others’ needs 


Assessing Intelligence 


Given the variety of approaches to the components of intelligence, it is not surprising 
that measuring intelligence has proved challenging. Psychologists who study intel- 
ligence have focused much of their attention on the development of intelligence tests 
and have relied on such tests to quantify a person’s level of intelligence. These tests 
have proved to be of great benefit in identifying students in need of special attention 
in school, diagnosing cognitive difficulties, and helping people make optimal educa- 
tional and vocational choices. At the same time, their use has proved controversial, 
raising important social and educational issues. 

Historically, the first effort at intelligence testing was based on an uncomplicated 
but completely wrong assumption: that the size and shape of a person’s head could 
be used as an objective measure of intelligence. The idea was put forward by Sir 
Francis Galton (1822-1911), an eminent English scientist whose ideas in other domains 
proved to be considerably better than his notions about intelligence. 

Galton’s motivation to identify people of high intelligence stemmed from per- 
sonal prejudices. He sought to demonstrate the natural superiority of people of high 
social class (including himself) by showing that intelligence is inherited. He hypoth- 
esized that head configuration, which is genetically determined, is related to brain 
size and therefore is related to intelligence. 

Galton’s theories were proved wrong on virtually every count. Head size and 
shape are not related to intellectual performance, and subsequent research has found 
little relationship between brain size and intelligence. However, Galton’s work did 
have at least one desirable result: He was the first person to suggest that intelligence 
could be quantified and measured in an objective manner (Jensen, 2002). 





BINET AND THE DEVELOPMENT OF IQ TESTS 


The first real intelligence tests were developed by the French psychologist Alfred 
Binet (1857-1911). His tests followed from a simple premise: If performance on cer- 
tain tasks or test items improved with chronological, or physical, age, performance 
could be used to distinguish more intelligent people from less intelligent ones within 
a particular age group. On the basis of this principle, Binet devised the first formal 
intelligence test, which was designed to identify the “dullest” students in the Paris 
school system in order to provide them with remedial aid. 

Binet began by presenting tasks to same-age students who had been labeled 
“bright” or “dull” by their teachers. If a task could be completed by the bright students 
but not by the dull ones, he retained that task as a proper test item; otherwise it was 
discarded. In the end he came up with a test that distinguished between the bright and 


Module 26 What Is Intelligence? 287 


FIGURE 4 Just as there are many views 
of the nature of intelligence, there are 
also numerous ways to demonstrate 
intelligent behavior. This summary 
provides an overview of the various 
approaches used by psychologists. 


intelligence tests Tests devised 
to quantify a person’s level of 
intelligence. 





Alfred Binet 


288 Chapter 9 Intelligence 


Now in its fifth edition, the Stanford-Binet 
test consists of a series of items that vary 
in nature according to the age of the 
person being tested. What can we learn 
about a person from a test of this type? 


mental age The age for which a given 
level of performance is average or 


typical. 


intelligence quotient (IQ) A score 
that takes into account an individual’s 
mental and chronological ages. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





dull groups, and—with further work—one that distinguished among children in differ- 
ent age groups (Binet & Simon, 1916; Sternberg & Jarvin, 2003). 

On the basis of the Binet test, children were assigned a score relating to their 
mental age, the age for which a given level of performance is average or typical. For 
example, if the average 8-year-old answered, say, 45 items correctly on a test, anyone 
who answered 45 items correctly would be assigned a mental age of 8 years. Conse- 
quently, whether the person taking the test was 20 years old or 5 years old, he or 
she would have the same mental age of 8 years (Cornell, 2006). 

Assigning a mental age to students provided an indication of their general level of 
performance. However, it did not allow for adequate comparisons among people of 
different chronological ages. By using mental age alone, for instance, we might assume 
that an 18-year-old responding at a 20-year-old’s level would be demonstrating the same 
degree of intelligence as a 5-year-old answering at a 7-year-old’s level, when actually 
the 5-year-old would be displaying a much greater relative degree of intelligence. 

A solution to the problem came in the form of the intelligence quotient, (IQ), 
a score that takes into account an individual’s mental and chronological ages. His- 
torically, the first IQ scores employed the following formula in which MA stands for 
mental age and CA for chronological age: 


=- MA 
IQ score = CA x 100 
Using this formula, we can return to the earlier example of an 18-year-old per- 
forming at a mental age of 20 and calculate an IQ score of (20/18) x 100 = 111. In 
contrast, the 5-year-old performing at a mental age of 7 comes out with a consider- 
ably higher IQ score: (7/5) xX 100 = 140. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


The average IQ score is 
100, and 68% of people 
score between 85 and I 15. 


Number of scores —~> 





95% 









14% 34% 





34% | 14% 
115 





55 70 85 100 130 145 160 


Intelligence test score 


FIGURE 5 The average and most common IQ score is 100, and 68% of all people are within a 
30-point range centered on 100. Some 95% of the population have scores that are within 30 
points above or below 100, and 99.8% have scores that are between 55 and 145. 


As a bit of trial and error with the formula will show you, anyone who has a 
mental age equal to his or her chronological age will have an IQ equal to 100. More- 
over, people with a mental age that is lower than their chronological age will have 
IQs that exceed 100. 

Although the basic principles behind the calculation of an IQ score still hold, 
today IQ scores are determined in a different manner and are known as deviation IQ 
scores. First, the average test score for everyone of the same age who takes the test 
is determined, and that average score is assigned an IQ of 100. Then, with the aid of 
statistical techniques that calculate the differences (or “deviations”) between each 
score and the average, IQ scores are assigned. 

As you can see in Figure 5, when IQ scores from large numbers of people are 
plotted on a graph, they form a bell-shaped distribution (called “bell-shaped” because 
it looks like a bell when plotted). Approximately two-thirds of all individuals fall 
within 15 IQ points of the average score of 100. As scores increase or fall beyond that 
range, the percentage of people in a category falls considerably. 


CONTEMPORARY IQ TESTS: GAUGING INTELLIGENCE 


Remnants of Binet’s original intelligence test are still with us, although the test has 
been revised in significant ways. Now in its fifth edition and called the Stanford-Binet 
Intelligence Scale, the test consists of a series of items that vary according to the age 
of the person being tested (Roid, Nellis, & McClellan, 2003). For example, young 
children are asked to copy figures or answer questions about everyday activities. 
Older people are asked to solve analogies, explain proverbs, and describe simi- 
larities that underlie sets of words. 

The test is administered orally and includes both verbal and nonverbal assess- 
ments. An examiner begins by finding a mental age level at which a person is able 
to answer all the questions correctly and then moves on to successively more 
difficult problems. When a mental age level is reached at which no items can be 
answered, the test is over. By studying the pattern of correct and incorrect responses, 
the examiner is able to compute an IQ score for the person being tested. In addi- 
tion, the Stanford-Binet test yields separate subscores that provide clues to a test- 
taker’s particular strengths and weaknesses. 

The IQ tests most frequently used in the United States were devised by psy- 


Module 26 What Is Intelligence? 289 





y Alert 

It’s important to know the 

traditional formula for IQ 
scores in which IQ is the ra- 
tio of mental age divided 
by chronological age, mul- 


tiplied by 100. Remember, though, 
that today, the actual calculator of IQ 
scores is done in a more sophisti- 
cated manner. 














© John O'Brien/The New Yorker Collection/www. 


cartoonbank.com. 








chologist David Wechsler and are known as the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV, 


290 Chapter 9 Intelligence 


Name 
Information 


Comprehension 


Arithmetic 


Similarities 


Figure weights 


Matrix reasoning 


Block design item 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Types of Items on WAIS-IV 

















































































































Goal of Item Example 
Assess general information Who wrote Tom Sawyer? 
Assess understanding and evaluation Why is copper often used for electrical wires? 
of social norms and past experience 
Assess math reasoning through verbal Three women divided 18 golf balls equally among themselves. 
problems How many golf balls did each person receive? 
Test understanding of how objects or In what way are a circle and a triangle alike? 
concepts are alike, tapping abstract 
reasoning 
Test Perceptual Reasoning Problems require test-taker to determine which possibility 

balances the final scale. 

af RA 

x x o oa 
Qa Qe O 
l 2 3 4 5 

Test spatial reasoning Test-taker must decide which of the five possibilities replaces 

the question mark and completes the sequence. 

w | ? 
Wel | & Lye] * 
l 2 3 4 5 

Test understanding of relationship of Problems require test-takers to reproduce a design in fixed 


parts to whole 


amount of time. 





FIGURE 6 Typical kinds of items found on the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scales (WAIS-IV). 
(Simulated items similar to those in the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale, Fourth Edition (WAIS-IV). Copyright 
© 2003 NCS Pearson, Inc. Reproduced with permission. All rights reserved. “Wechsler Adult Intelligence 
Scale” “WAIS” and “Weschler” are trademarks, in the US and/or other countries, of Pearson Education, Inc. 
or its affiliates.) 


or, more commonly, the WAIS-IV (for adults) and a children’s version, the Wechsler 
Intelligence Scale for Children-IV, or WISC-IV. Both the WAIS-IV and the WISC-IV 
measure verbal comprehension, perceptual reasoning, working memory, and process- 
ing speed (see sample WAIS-IV items in Figure 6). 

Because the Stanford-Binet, WAIS-IV, and WISC-IV all require individualized, 
one-on-one administration, they are relatively difficult to administer and score on a 
large-scale basis. Consequently, there are now a number of IQ tests that allow group 
administration. Rather than having one examiner ask one person at a time to respond 
to individual items, group IQ tests are strictly paper-and-pencil tests. The primary 
advantage of group tests is their ease of administration (Anastasi & Urbina, 1997). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


However, sacrifices are made in group testing that in some cases may outweigh 
the benefits. For instance, group tests generally offer fewer kinds of questions than 
do tests administered individually. Furthermore, people may be more motivated to 
perform at their highest ability level when working on a one-to-one basis with a test 
administrator than they are in a group. Finally, in some cases, it is simply impossible 
to employ group tests, particularly with young children or people with unusually 
low IQs (Aiken, 1996). 


RELIABILITY AND VALIDITY: TAKING THE MEASURE OF TESTS 


When we use a ruler, we expect to find that it measures an inch in the same way it did 
the last time we used it. When we weigh ourselves on the bathroom scale, we hope 
that the variations we see on the scale are due to changes in our weight and not to 
errors on the part of the scale (unless the change in weight is in an unwanted direction!). 

In the same way, we hope that psychological tests have reliability—that they 
measure consistently what they are trying to measure. We need to be sure that each 
time we administer the test, a test-taker will achieve the same results—assuming that 
nothing about the person has changed relevant to what is being measured. 

Suppose, for instance, that when you first took the SAT exams, you scored 400 
on the verbal section of the test. Then, after taking the test again a few months later, 
you scored 700. Upon receiving your new score, you might well stop celebrating for 
a moment to question whether the test is reliable for it is unlikely that your abilities 
could have changed enough to raise your score by 300 points (T. R. Coyle, 2006). 

But suppose your score changed hardly at all, and both times you received a 
score of about 400. You couldn’t complain about a lack of reliability. However, if you 
knew your verbal skills were above average, you might be concerned that the test 
did not adequately measure what it was supposed to measure. In sum, the question 
has now become one of validity rather than reliability. A test has validity when it 
actually measures what it is supposed to measure. 

Knowing that a test is reliable is no guarantee that it is also valid. For instance, 
Sir Francis Galton assumed that skull size is related to intelligence, and he was able 
to measure skull size with great reliability. However, the measure of skull size was 
not valid—it had nothing to do with intelligence. In this case, then, we have reli- 
ability without validity. 

However, if a test is unreliable, it cannot be valid. Assuming that all other factors— 
motivation to score well, knowledge of the material, health, and so forth—are similar, 
if a person scores high the first time he or she takes a specific test and low the second 
time, the test cannot be measuring what it is supposed to measure. Therefore, the test 
is both unreliable and not valid. 

Test validity and reliability are prerequisites for accurate assessment of 
intelligence—as well as for any other measurement task carried out by psychologists. 
Consequently, the measures of personality carried out by personality psychologists, 
clinical psychologists’ assessments of psychological disorders, and social psycholo- 
gists’ measures of attitudes must meet the tests of validity and reliability for the 
results to be meaningful (Feldt, 2005; Phelps, 2005; Yao, Zhour, & Jiang, 2006). 

Assuming that a test is both valid and reliable, one further step is necessary in 
order to interpret the meaning of a particular test-taker’s score: the establishment of 
norms. Norms are standards of test performance that permit the comparison of one 
person’s score on a test to the scores of others who have taken the same test. For 
example, a norm permits test-takers to know that they have scored, say, in the top 
15% of those who have taken the test previously. Tests for which norms have been 
developed are known as standardized tests. 

Test designers develop norms by calculating the average score achieved by a 
specific group of people for whom the test has been designed. Then the test design- 
ers can determine the extent to which each person’s score differs from the scores of 


Module 26 What Is Intelligence? 291 


reliability The property by which 
tests measure consistently what they 
are trying to measure. 


validity The property by which tests 
actually measure what they are 
supposed to measure. 


norms Standards of test performance 
that permit the comparison of one 
person’s score on a test with the scores 
of other individuals who have taken 
the same test. 


292 


Chapter 9 Intelligence 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


the other individuals who have taken the test in the past and provide future test- 
takers with a qualitative sense of their performance. 

Obviously, the samples of test-takers who are employed in the establishment of 
norms are critical to the norming process. The people used to determine norms must 
be representative of the individuals to whom the test is directed. 


Adaptive Testing: Using 
Computers to Assess Performance 


Ensuring that tests are reliable, valid, and based on appropriate norms has become 
more critical with computer-administered testing. In computerized versions of tests 
such as the Graduate Record Exam, a test used to determine entrance to graduate 











===» Wrong answer; move 
to easier question 

> Right answer; move to 

more difficult question 














A>X>Z PE ‘pT E X-0p'qE ‘SI'LE OS AZ ‘069$ FZ ‘9'i ‘SdeMsuYy 


FIGURE 7 Adaptive testing. In the Graduate Record Examination, the computer randomly 
selects a first question of medium difficulty. If the test-taker answers the question correctly, 
the computer poses a more difficult question. Once the test-taker gives an incorrect answer, 
he or she is given a question at the next easiest level, as illustrated in this example. Test- 
takers are graded based on the level of difficulty of the questions they answer correctly, 
meaning that two test-takers who answer the same number of questions correctly can end 
up with very different scores. What do you think are the drawbacks of adaptive testing of this 
sort? Do you think such tests may discriminate against test-takers who are less familiar with 
computers compared with those who have easy access to them? (Source: New York Times Graphics.) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


school, not only are test questions viewed and answered on a computer, but the test 
itself is individualized. With adaptive testing, every test-taker does not receive identi- 
cal sets of test questions. Instead, the computer first presents a randomly selected 
question of moderate difficulty. If the test-taker answers it correctly, the computer 
then presents a randomly chosen item of slightly greater difficulty. If the test-taker 
answers it incorrectly, the computer presents a slightly easier item. Each question 
becomes slightly harder or easier than the question preceding it, depending on 
whether the previous response is correct. Ultimately, the greater the number of dif- 
ficult questions answered correctly, the higher the score (Chang & Ansley, 2003; 
Marszalek, 2007; Belov & Armstrong, 2009; see Figure 7). 

Because computerized adaptive testing pinpoints a test-taker’s level of profi- 
ciency fairly quickly, the total time spent taking the exam is shorter than it is with a 
traditional exam. Test-takers are not forced to spend a great deal of time answering 
questions that are either much easier or much harder than they can handle. 

Critics of adaptive testing suggest that it may discriminate against test-takers who 
have less access to computers and thus may have less practice with them or may be 
more intimidated by the testing medium. In other cases, high-ability test-takers who 
make an early mistake and find the items getting easier may begin to feel such anxi- 
ety that their performance suffers, leading to a downward spiral in their performance. 
Still, most research suggests that adaptive testing provides scores equivalent to those 
of traditional paper-and-pencil measures for most types of testing (Tonidandel, Qui- 
nones, & Adams, 2002; Passos, Berger, & Tan, 2007; Rulison & Loken, 2009). 


Even though psychologists disagree about the nature of BECOMING 


Module 26 What Is Intelligence? 


intelligence, intelligence tests—as well as many other kinds of AN INFORMED CONSUMER 


tests—are widely used in a variety of situations. For example, if 


you are thinking about becoming a physician, a lawyer, or of Psyc ho | ogy 


almost any other kind of professional in a field that requires 
advanced training, you will have to take a formal exam in order 
to be accepted for post-graduate training. 

If you do have to take such an exam, you can do several 
things to maximize your score, including the following (Lurie, Robinson, & Pecsenye, 
2005; Feldman, 2010): 


e Learn as much as you can about the test before you take it. Know what sections will be 
on the test and how much each section is worth. 

e Practice. Try as many practice tests as you can find. The more practice you have, 
the easier it will be when you actually take the test. 

e Ifthe test is administered on a computer, as it probably will be, take practice tests on a 
computer. The more familiar you are with computers, the more at ease you will 
feel when you sit down to take the test. 

e Time yourself carefully. Don’t spend too much time on early items at the expense 
of later ones. Your goal should be not perfection but maximization of the number 
of correct responses you get. 

e Be aware of the scoring policy. If you are not penalized for wrong answers, guess. If 
there are penalties, be more conservative about guessing. 

e [fit is a paper-and-pencil test, complete answer sheets accurately. Check and check 
again. If the test is on a computer, check your answer thoroughly before going on 
to the next question, because you won't be able to go back and change your 
answer once you've submitted it. 


Scoring Better on Standardized Tests 


293 


294 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 9 Intelligence 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 


What are the different definitions and conceptions of 
intelligence? 


Because intelligence can take many forms, defining it is 
challenging. One commonly accepted view is that intelli- 
gence is the capacity to understand the world, think 
rationally, and use resources effectively when faced with 
challenges. (p. 279) 

The earliest psychologists assumed that there is a general 
factor for mental ability called g. However, later psychol- 
ogists disputed the view that intelligence is uni- 
dimensional. (p. 280) 

Some researchers suggest that intelligence can be broken 
down into fluid intelligence and crystallized intelligence. 
Gardner’s theory of multiple intelligences proposes that 
there are eight spheres of intelligence. (p. 280) 
Information-processing approaches examine the 
processes underlying intelligent behavior rather than 
focusing on the structure of intelligence. (p. 281) 
Practical intelligence is intelligence related to overall 
success in living; emotional intelligence is the set of skills 
that underlie the accurate assessment, evaluation, 
expression, and regulation of emotions. (p. 284) 


What are the major approaches to measuring intelligence, and 
what do intelligence tests measure? 


Intelligence tests have traditionally compared a person’s 
mental age and chronological age to yield an IQ, or intel- 
ligence quotient, score. (p. 287) 

Specific tests of intelligence include the Stanford-Binet 
test, the Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV (WAIS-IV), 
and the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-IV 
(WISC-IV). (p. 289) 

Tests are expected to be both reliable and valid. Reliability 
refers to the consistency with which a test measures what 
it is trying to measure. A test has validity when it actually 
measures what it is supposed to measure. (p. 291) 


KEY TERMS 


intelligence p. 280 theory of multiple 

g or g-factor p. 280 intelligences p. 281 

fluid intelligence p. 280 practical intelligence p. 284 
crystallized emotional 


intelligence p. 281 intelligence p. 285 


e Norms are standards of test performance that permit the 
comparison of one person’s score on a test to the scores 
of others who have taken the same test. (p. 291) 


EVALUATE 


1. ______is a measure of intelligence that takes into 
account a person’s chronological and mental ages. 

2. Some psychologists make the distinction between 

intelligence, which reflects reasoning, 
memory, and information-processing capabilities, and 

intelligence, which is the information, skills, 
and strategies that people have learned through 
experience. 

3. Psychologists using an approach 
to intelligence focus on the way people store information 
in memory and use that information to solve intellectual 
tasks. 





RETHINK 


1. What is the role of emotional intelligence in the 
classroom? How might emotional intelligence be tested? 
Should emotional intelligence be a factor in determining 
academic promotion to the next grade? 

2. From the human resource specialist’s perspective: Job 
interviews are really a kind of test, but they rely on 
interviewers’ judgments and have no formal validity or 
reliability. Do you think job interviews can be made to 
have greater validity and reliability? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


Zurssovord-uoyewozur ‘g ‘pozeso “pmp ‘z OL 


Ta 


intelligence reliability p. 291 
tests p. 287 validity p. 291 
mental age p. 288 norms p. 291 
intelligence quotient 
(IQ) p. 288 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 27 


“Hey, hey, hey, Fact Track!” The 11-year-old speaker chose one of his favorite programs. . . . 
“What is your name?” appeared on the monitor. 


“Daniel Skandera,” he typed. A menu scrolled up listing the program’s possibilities. 
Daniel chose multiplication facts, Level 1... . 


Randomly generated multiplication facts flashed on the screen: “4 X 6,” “2 X 9,” 
“3 X 3,” "7 X 6.” Daniel responded, deftly punching in his answers on the computer’s 
numeric key-pad .... 


The computer tallied the results. “You completed 20 problems in 66 seconds. You beat 
your goal. Problems correct = 20. Congratulations Daniel!” And with that the 11-year- 
old retreated hastily to the TV room. The Lakers and 76ers were about to tip off for an 
NBA championship game. (Heward & Orlansky, 1988, p. 100) 


If you consider people with mental retardation as inept and dull, it is time to revise 
your view. As in the case of Daniel Skandera, individuals with deficits of intellectual 
abilities can lead full, rounded lives and in some cases perform well in certain kinds 
of academic endeavors. 

More than 7 million people in the United States, including around 11 per 1,000 
children, have been identified as far enough below average in intelligence that they 
can be regarded as having a serious deficit. Individuals with low IQs (people with 
mental retardation or intellectual disabilities) as well as those with unusually high 
IQs (the intellectually gifted) require special attention if they are to reach their full 
potential. 


Mental Retardation 
(Intellectual Disabilities) 


Although sometimes thought of as a rare phenomenon, mental retardation (or, as it 
is increasingly being called, intellectual disabilities) occurs in 1%-3% of the popula- 
tion. There is wide variation among those labeled mentally retarded, in large part 
because of the inclusiveness of the definition developed by the American Association 
on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities. The association suggests that mental 
retardation (or intellectual disability) is a disability characterized by significant 
limitations both in intellectual functioning and in conceptual, social, and practical 
adaptive skills (AAMR, 2002). (Although experts are increasingly using the term 
intellectual disability instead of mental retardation, our discussion will use the original 
term because it continues to be more widespread.) 

Although below-average intellectual functioning can be measured in a rela- 
tively straightforward manner—using standard IQ tests—it is more difficult to 
determine how to gauge limitations in adaptive behavior. Consequently, there is a 
lack of uniformity in how experts apply the labels mental retardation and intellectual 
disabilities. People labeled mentally retarded vary from those who can be taught to 





How are the extremes of 
intelligence characterized? 


How can we help people 
reach their full potential? 


A condition characterized 


by significant limitations both in 
intellectual functioning and in 
conceptual, social, and practical 
adaptive skills. 


295 


296 Chapter 9 Intelligence 


fetal alcohol syndrome The most 
common cause of mental retardation in 
newborns, occurring when the mother 
uses alcohol during pregnancy. 


familial retardation Mental retarda- 
tion in which no apparent biological 

defect exists but there is a history of 

retardation in the family. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


work and function with little special attention to those who cannot be trained and 
are institutionalized throughout their lives (Detterman, Gabriel, & Ruthsatz, 2000; 
Greenspan, 2006). 

Most people with mental retardation have relatively minor deficits and are clas- 
sified as having mild retardation. These individuals, who have IQ scores ranging from 
55 to 69, constitute some 90% of all people with mental retardation. Although their 
development is typically slower than that of their peers, they can function quite 
independently by adulthood and are able to hold jobs and have families of their own 
(Bates et al., 2001; Smith, 2006). 

With greater degrees of intellectual deficit—moderate retardation (IQs of 40 to 54), 
severe retardation (IQs of 25 to 39), and profound retardation (IQs below 25)—the dif- 
ficulties are more pronounced. For people with moderate retardation, deficits are 
obvious early, with language and motor skills lagging behind those of peers. Although 
these individuals can hold simple jobs, they need to have a moderate degree of 
supervision throughout their lives. Individuals with severe and profound mental 
retardation are generally unable to function independently and typically require care 
for their entire lives (Garwick, 2007). 


IDENTIFYING THE ROOTS OF MENTAL RETARDATION 


What produces mental retardation? In nearly one-third of the cases there is an iden- 
tifiable cause related to biological or environmental factors. The most common pre- 
ventable cause of retardation is fetal alcohol syndrome, produced by a mother’s use 
of alcohol while pregnant. Increasing evidence shows that even small amounts of 
alcohol intake can produce intellectual deficits. One in every 750 infants is born with 
fetal alcohol syndrome in the United States (West & Blake, 2005; Manning & Hoyme, 
2007; Murthy et al., 2009). 

Down syndrome, the type of mental retardation experienced by actor Chris 
Burke (discussed at the start of the chapter), represents another major cause of 
mental retardation. Down syndrome results when a person is born with 47 chromo- 
somes instead of the usual 46. In most cases, there is an extra copy of the 21st 
chromosome, which leads to problems in how the brain and body develop (Sher- 
man et al., 2007). 

In other cases of mental retardation, an abnormality occurs in the structure of 
particular chromosomes. Birth complications, such as a temporary lack of oxygen, 
may also cause retardation. In some cases, mental retardation occurs after birth fol- 
lowing a head injury, a stroke, or infections such as meningitis (Plomin, 2005; Bittles, 
Bower, & Hussain, 2007). 

However, the majority of cases of mental retardation are classified as familial 
retardation, in which no apparent biological defect exists but there is a history of 
retardation in the family. Whether the family background of retardation is caused by 
environmental factors, such as extreme continuous poverty leading to malnutrition, 
or by some underlying genetic factor is usually impossible to determine (Zigler et 
al., 2002). 


INTEGRATING INDIVIDUALS WITH MENTAL RETARDATION 


Important advances in the care and treatment of those with retardation have been 
made since the Education for All Handicapped Children Act (Public Law 94-142) 
was passed by Congress in the mid-1970s. In this federal law, Congress stipulated 
that people with retardation are entitled to a full education and that they must be 
educated and trained in the least restrictive environment. The law increased the 
educational opportunities for individuals with mental retardation, facilitating their 
integration into regular classrooms as much as possible—a process known as main- 
streaming (Katsiyannis, Zhang, & Woodruff, 2005; Aussilloux & Bagdadli, 2006; 
Gibb et al., 2007). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 27 Variations in Intellectual Ability 297 


The philosophy behind mainstreaming suggests that the interaction of students with 
and without mental retardation in regular classrooms will improve educational oppor- 
tunities for those with retardation, increase their social acceptance, and facilitate their 
integration into society as a whole. Of course, special education classes still exist; some 
individuals with retardation function at too low of a level to benefit from placement in 
regular classrooms. Moreover, children with mental retardation who are mainstreamed 
into regular classes typically attend special classes for at least part of the day (Hastings 
& Oakford, 2003; Williamson, McLeskey, & Hoppey, 2006; also see PsychWork). 


For Shannon Lynch, Director of Special Education at The Wil- 
low Creek Charter School in Prescott, Arizona, being a special Psyc h Wo r k 


DIRECTOR OF SPECIAL 


education teacher means providing ways for every student to 

achieve success. Each child presents different needs, and 

Lynch approaches her teaching accordingly. EDUCATION 
“For children with specific learning disabilities, I have 


found it is helpful to utilize repetition, hands-on activities, Bite Suaalein Lich 





music, movements with information, and rhymes to get the Position: Director of Special Education 

information to stick,” she explained. Education: BA, Liberal Studies, Northern Arizona 
“Children on the autism spectrum have challenges with University, Flagstaff, Arizona; MA, Special 

social skills. Great programs exist that provide models and Education, Northern Arizona University, Flagstaff, 

scripts of appropriate social interactions,” Lynch said. Emotional Arizona; State of Arizona Teaching Certificate in 

disabilities require a very different approach and involve provid- Elementary and Special Education 


ing counseling to cope with the difficult emotions students face. 

“The biggest factor for students with special needs is providing ways for them 
to achieve success. They have spent much of their academic career feeling incapable 
and inadequate, so just giving them small opportunities for success can make a huge 
difference in their confidence,” Lynch noted. 


Some educators argue that an alternative to mainstreaming, called full inclusion, 
might be more effective. Full inclusion is the integration of all students, even those 
with the most severe educational disabilities, into regular classes and an avoidance 
of segregated special education classes. Teacher aides are assigned to help the 
children with special needs progress. Schools with full inclusion have no separate 
special education classes. However, full inclusion is a controversial practice, and it 
is not widely applied (Praisner, 2003; Spence-Cochran & Pearl, 2006; Begeny & 
Martens, 2007). 


The Intellectually Gifted 


Another group of people—the intellectually gifted—differ from those with average 
intelligence as much as individuals with mental retardation although in a different 
manner. Accounting for 2%—4% of the population, the intellectually gifted have IQ 
scores greater than 130. Sho Yano, the individual described in the chapter prologue 
who graduated from college at the age of 12, exemplifies a case of someone who is 
particularly intellectually gifted. 

Although the stereotype associated with the gifted suggests that they are awk- 
ward, shy social misfits who are unable to get along well with peers, most research 
indicates that just the opposite is true. The intellectually gifted are most often outgo- 
ing, well-adjusted, healthy, popular people who are able to do most things better than 
the average person can (Lubinski et al., 2006; Guldemond et al., 2007; Mueller, 2009). 

For example, in a famous study by psychologist Lewis Terman that started in 
the early 1920s, 1,500 children who had IQ scores above 140 were followed for the 





y Alert 


Remember that in most cases 
of mental retardation, there 
is no apparent biological 

deficiency, but a history of 
mental retardation exists in 
the family. 


intellectually gifted The 2% 4% 
segment of the population who have 
IQ scores greater than 130. 


298 


Chapter 9 Intelligence 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


rest of their lives. From the start, the members of this group were more physically, 
academically, and socially capable than their nongifted peers. In addition to doing 
better in school, they also showed better social adjustment than average. All these 
advantages paid off in terms of career success: As a group, the gifted received more 
awards and distinctions, earned higher incomes, and made more contributions in art 
and literature than typical individuals. Perhaps most important, they reported greater 
satisfaction in life than the nongifted (Hegarty, 2007). 

Of course, not every member of the group Terman studied was successful. Fur- 
thermore, high intelligence is not a homogeneous quality; a person with a high over- 
all IQ is not necessarily gifted in every academic subject but may excel in just one 
or two. A high IQ is not a universal guarantee of success (Shurkin, 1992; Winner, 
2003; Clemons, 2006). 

Although special programs attempting to overcome the deficits of people with 
mental retardation abound, programs targeted at the intellectually gifted are more 
rare. One reason for this lack of attention is that although there are as many gifted 
individuals as there are those with mental retardation, the definition of gifted is 
vague, especially compared with definitions of mental retardation. Furthermore, 
there is a persistent view that the gifted ought to be able to “make it on their own”; 
if they can’t, they really weren't gifted in the first place (Robinson, 2003; Parke, 2003; 
Sparks, 2007). 

More enlightened approaches, however, have acknowledged that without 
some form of special attention, the gifted become bored and frustrated with the 
pace of their schooling and may never reach their potential. Consequently, pro- 
grams for the gifted are designed to provide enrichment that allows participants’ 
talents to flourish (Adams-Byers, Squilkr, & Moon, 2004; Delcourt, Cornell, & 
Goldberg, 2007). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


RECAP 


How are extremes of intelligence characterized? 

e The levels of mental retardation (or intellectual disability) 
include mild, moderate, severe, and profound retardation. 
(p. 295) 

e About one-third of the cases of retardation have a known 
biological cause; fetal alcohol syndrome is the most 
common. Most cases, however, are classified as familial 
retardation, for which there is no known biological 
cause. (p. 296) 

e The intellectually gifted are people with IQ scores greater 
than 130. Intellectually gifted people tend to be healthier 
and more successful than the nongifted. (p. 297) 


How can we help people reach their full potential? 

e Advances in the treatment of people with mental 
retardation include mainstreaming, the integration of 
individuals with mental retardation into regular education 


Module 27 Variations in Intellectual Ability 299 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


3. 


is the process by which students with mental 
retardation are placed in normal classrooms to facilitate 
learning and reduce isolation. 


. Most forms of retardation have no identifiable biological 


cause. True or false? 


. People with high intelligence are generally shy and so- 


cially withdrawn. True or false? 


RETHINK 


1. 


Why do you think negative stereotypes of gifted individu- 
als and people with intellectual disabilities persist, even in 
the face of contrary evidence? How can these stereotypes 
be changed? 


. From a school administrator’s perspective: What advantages 


and disadvantages do you think full inclusion programs 
would present for students with mental retardation? For 
students without mental retardation? 


classrooms as much as possible (along with some partici- 
pation in segregated special education classes); and full in- 
clusion, in which all students, even those with the most 
severe educational disabilities, are fully integrated into 
regular classes (and separate classes are avoided). (p. 296) 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 
OI AMO] & YM 
aso} uey} }dəpe Ájjenos ərow ATTeI9UasS are p33 ayy ‘esTey *G ONI H 
(3urueəysuru *¢ ‘auorpuds umoq ‘z Juowredu [eJUSUT zo s22139p 
SNOLIVA YIM əjdoəd zo ə3ULI apIM V aqIIDSap 0} Pasn ST WI} IY} ‘ISTLF `T 


EVALUATE 


1. The term mental retardation is applied specifically to people 
with an IQ below 60. True or false? 

2, ________is a disorder caused by an extra chromosome 
that is responsible for some cases of mental retardation. 


KEY TERMS 


mental retardation (or intellectual disability) p. 295 
fetal alcohol syndrome p. 296 


familial retardation p. 296 
intellectually gifted p. 297 


Are traditional IQ tests cultur- 
ally biased? 


Are there racial differences in 
intelligence? 


To what degree is intelligence 
influenced by the environ- 
ment and to what degree by 
heredity? 


300 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Kwang is often washed with a pleck tied to a: 

(a) rundel 

(b) flink 

(c) pove 

(d) quirj 
If you found this kind of item on an intelligence test, you would probably complain 
that the test was totally absurd and had nothing to do with your intelligence or 
anyone else’s—and rightly so. How could anyone be expected to respond to items 
presented in a language that was so unfamiliar? 

Yet to some people, even more reasonable questions may appear just as nonsen- 
sical. Consider the example of a child raised in a city who is asked about procedures 
for milking cows, or someone raised in a rural area who is asked about subway 
ticketing procedures. Obviously, the previous experience of the test-takers would 
affect their ability to answer correctly. And if such types of questions were included 
on an IQ test, a critic could rightly contend that the test had more to do with prior 
experience than with intelligence. 

Although IQ tests do not include questions that are so clearly dependent on prior 
knowledge as questions about cows and subways, the background and experiences 
of test-takers do have the potential to affect results. In fact, the issue of devising fair 
intelligence tests that measure knowledge unrelated to culture and family background 
and experience is central to explaining an important and persistent finding: Members 
of certain racial and cultural groups consistently score lower on traditional intelligence 
tests than do members of other groups. For example, as a group, blacks tend to aver- 
age 10 to 15 IQ points lower than whites. Does this variation reflect a true difference 
in intelligence, or are the questions biased with regard to the kinds of knowledge 
they test? Clearly, if whites perform better because of their greater familiarity with 
the kind of information that is being tested, their higher IQ scores are not an indica- 
tion that they are more intelligent than members of other groups (Templer & Arikawa, 
2006; Fagan & Holland, 2007; Morgan, Marsiske, & Whitfield, 2008). 

There is good reason to believe that some standardized IQ tests contain elements 
that discriminate against minority-group members whose experiences differ from 
those of the white majority. Consider the question “What should you do if another 
child grabbed your hat and ran off with it?” Most white middle-class children answer 
that they would tell an adult, and this response is scored as correct. However, a 
reasonable response might be to chase the person and fight to get the hat back, the 
answer that is chosen by many urban black children—but one that is scored as incor- 
rect (Miller-Jones, 1991; Aiken, 1997; Reynolds & Ramsay, 2003). 

Furthermore, tests may include even subtler forms of bias against minority 
groups. For example, assessments of cognitive ability developed in the United States 
may favor responses that implicitly reflect North American or European values, cus- 
toms, or traditions. At the same time, such tests are biased against African and other 
cultural value systems (Helms, 1992; Byrne & Watkins, 2003). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 28 Group Differences in Intelligence: Genetic and Environmental Determinants 301 


For example, the traditional Western value of “rugged individualism” means that 
correct answers to test items may require a test-taker to reason independently of a 
particular social context. In contrast, the African cultural value of communalism, in 
which one’s group is valued more than individuals, may leave test-takers from that 
tradition unable to answer a question that provides no information about the social 
context (Greenfield, 1997; Kwate, 2001). 


In an attempt to produce a culture-fair IQ test, one that does Ex p | O ri nN g estes 


not discriminate against the members of any minority group, 
psychologists have tried to devise test items that assess 
experiences common to all cultures or emphasize questions 
that do not require language usage. However, test makers 
have found this difficult to do because past experiences, 
attitudes, and values almost always have an impact on 
respondents’ answers (Fagan & Holland, 2009). 

For example, children raised in Western cultures group things on the basis of what culture-fair IO test A test that does 
they are (such as putting dog and fish into the category of animal). In contrast, members not discriminate against the members 
of the Kpelle tribe in Africa see intelligence demonstrated by grouping things according of any minority group. 
to what they do (grouping fish with swim). Similarly, children in the United States asked 
to memorize the position of objects on a chessboard perform better than African 
children living in remote villages if household objects familiar to the U.S. children are 
used. But if rocks are used instead of household objects, the African children do better. 

In short, it is difficult to produce a truly culture-fair test (Sandoval et al., 1998; Serpell, 
2000; Valencia & Suzuki, 2003). 

The efforts of psychologists to produce culture-fair measures of intelligence relate to 
a lingering controversy over differences in intelligence between members of different 
racial and ethnic groups. In attempting to identify whether there are differences 
between such groups, psychologists have had to confront the broader issue of 
determining the relative contribution to intelligence of genetic factors (heredity) and 
experience (environment)—the nature-nurture issue that is one of the basic issues of 
psychology. 

Richard Herrnstein, a psychologist, and Charles Murray, a sociologist, fanned the 
flames of the debate with the publication of their book The Bell Curve in the mid-1990s 
(Herrnstein & Murray, 1994). They argued that an analysis of IQ differences between 
whites and blacks demonstrated that although environmental factors played a role, 
there were also basic genetic differences between the two races. They based their 
argument on a number of findings. For instance, on average, whites score 15 points 
higher than blacks on traditional IQ tests even when socioeconomic status (SES) is 
taken into account. According to Herrnstein and Murray, middle- and upper-SES 
blacks score lower than middle- and upper-SES whites, just as lower-SES blacks score 
lower on average than lower-SES whites. Intelligence differences between blacks and 
whites, they concluded, could not be attributed to environmental differences alone. 


The Relative Influence of Genetics and 
Environment: Nature, Nurture, and IQ 





IQ AND HERITABILITY 


It is clear that intelligence in general shows a high degree of heritability, a measure heritability A measure of the degree 
of the degree to which a characteristic can be attributed to genetic, inherited factors. to which a characteristic is related to 
(e.g., Petrill, 2005; Miller & Penke, 2007; Plomin, 2009). As can be seen in Figure 1, genetic, inherited factors. 

the closer the genetic link between two related people, the greater the correspondence 


of IQ scores. Using data such as these, Herrnstein and Murray argued that differences y Alert 

between races in IQ scores were largely caused by genetically based differences in Remember that the differ- 

intelligence. ences in IQ scores are much 
However, many psychologists reacted strongly to the arguments laid out in The greater when comparing 

Bell Curve, refuting several of the book’s basic arguments. One criticism is that even individuals than when 


when attempts are made to hold socioeconomic conditions constant, wide variations comparing groups. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


302 Chapter 9 Intelligence 











Relationship Genetic overlap Rearing Correlation 
Monozygotic (identical) twins 100% Together 86 
. $ i 3 The difference between these 
Dizygotic (fraternal) twins 50% Together 62 +worcortaladons:shows:the 
Siblings 50% Together impact of the environment 
Siblings 50% Apart 
Parent-child 50% Together BS 
Parent-child 50% Apart Jl 
The relatively low correlation 
Adoptive parent-child 0% ‘Together 16 for unrelated children raised 
Unrelated children 0% Together (2) together shows the . 
importance of genetic factors 
Spouses 0% Apart 29 


FIGURE 1 The relationship between IQ and closeness of genetic relationship. In general, the 
more similar the genetic and environmental background of two people, the greater the 
correlation. Note, for example, that the correlation for spouses, who are genetically unrelated 
and have been reared apart, is relatively low, whereas the correlation for identical twins 
reared together is substantial. (Source: Adapted from Henderson, 1982.) 


remain among individual households. Furthermore, no one can convincingly assert 
that the living conditions of blacks and whites are identical even when their socio- 
economic status is similar. In addition, as we discussed earlier, there is reason to 
believe that traditional IO tests may discriminate against lower-SES urban blacks by 
asking for information pertaining to experience they are unlikely to have had (Amer- 
ican Psychological Association Task Force on Intelligence, 1996; Hall, 2002; Horn, 
2002; Nisbett, 2007). 

Moreover, blacks who are raised in economically enriched environments have 
IQ scores similar to whites in comparable environments. For example, in a study of 
black children who had been adopted at an early age by white middle-class families 
of above-average intelligence, the IQ scores of those children averaged 106—about 
15 points above the average IQ scores of unadopted black children in the study. Other 
research shows that the racial gap in IQ narrows considerably after a college educa- 
tion, and cross-cultural data demonstrate that when racial gaps exist in other cul- 
tures, the economically disadvantaged groups typically have lower scores. In short, 
the evidence that genetic factors play the major role in determining racial differences 
in IQ is not compelling (Scarr & Weinberg, 1976; Sternberg, Grigorenko, & Kidd, 2005; 
Fagan & Holland, 2007; Nisbett, 2009). 

Furthermore, drawing comparisons between different races on any dimension, 
including IQ scores, is an imprecise, potentially misleading, and often fruitless ven- 
ture. By far, the greatest discrepancies in IQ scores occur when comparing individuals, 
not when comparing mean IQ scores of different groups. There are blacks who score 
high on IQ tests and whites who score low, just are there are whites who score high 
and blacks who score low. For the concept of intelligence to aid in the betterment of 
society, we must examine how individuals perform and not the groups to which they 
belong (Angoff, 1988; Fagan & Holland, 2002, 2007). 


THE FLEXIBILITY OF INTELLIGENCE: THE FLYNN EFFECT 


Intelligence is also more flexible and modifiable than originally envisioned. For 
instance, researchers have been puzzled by data showing a long-term increase in IQ 
scores that has occurred since the early 1900s. Because the average person today gets 
more items correct on IQ tests than the average person several generations ago, 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 28 Group Differences in Intelligence: Genetic and Environmental Determinants 303 


scores have risen significantly—a phenomenon named the Flynn effect after its dis- 
coverer, psychologist James Flynn. The Flynn effect is not trivial, with the perfor- 
mance of the average 20-year-old today measuring some 15 points higher than the 
performance of the average 20-year-old in 1940 (Flynn, 2000, 2007). 

The cause of the Flynn effect is not clear. Flynn and a colleague, economist Wil- 
liam Dickens, argue that the higher scores have resulted as people have matched 
their genetic gifts with their environment. Because society is increasingly complex 
and driven by technology, it is increasingly making demands that people with high 
intelligence are particularly adept at meeting. Consequently, society provides greater 
opportunities for people with high IQs to excel, and that environment pushes them 
even more intellectually, creating an increase overall in IQ scores (Dickens & Flynn, 
2001; Flynn, 2007). 

Other explanations for the Flynn effect suggest that it may due to better nutri- 
tion, better parenting, or improvements in the general social environment, including 
education. Whatever the cause, the change in IQ scores over the century is not due 
to evolutionary changes in human genetics: The period over which the Flynn effect 
has occurred is far too short for people to have evolved into a more intelligent spe- 
cies (Loehlin, 2002; Sunder Borren, & Tambs, 2008; Lynn, 2009). 





PLACING THE HEREDITY-ENVIRONMENT QUESTION i DP ; 
IN PERSPECTIVE Social and economic inequality, as well 


as heredity and other environmental 
Ultimately, there is no final answer to the question of the degree to which intelligence factors, are associated with differences 


is influenced by heredity and by the environment. We are dealing with an issue for in intelligence. People who have greater 
which experiments to unambiguously determine cause and effect cannot be devised. educational opportunities and who 
The more critical question to ask, then, is not whether hereditary or environmen- suffer fewer economic constraints are 


tal factors primarily underlie intelligence, but whether there is anything we can do able to maximize their intelligence. 
to maximize the intellectual development of each individual. If we can find ways to 

do this, we will be able to make changes in the environment—which may take the 

form of enriched home and school environments—that can lead each person to reach 

his or her potential. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


304 Chapter 9 Intelligence 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 2. A(n) _______ test tries to use only questions appropri- 
ate to all the people taking the test. 
3. IQ tests can accurately determine the intelligence of entire 
groups of people. True or false? 
. Intelligence can be seen as a combination of 


Are traditional IQ tests culturally biased? 
e Traditional intelligence tests have frequently been criti- 
cized for being biased in favor of the white middle-class 
population. This controversy has led to attempts to de- 


A 


vise culture-fair tests, IQ measures that avoid questions and acre 
that depend on a particular cultural background. (p. 300) 
Are there racial differences in intelligence? EEE 
e Issues of racial differences in intelligence are very contro- 1. There’s been a steady rise in average IQ scores in the 
versial in part because of the difficulty of defining the United States over the last several decades. Why might 
concept of race scientifically and disagreement over this be the case? Do you think it represents an actual in- 
what constitutes an unbiased measure of race. (p. 301) crease in intelligence? 


2. From a college admissions officer's perspective: Imagine you 
notice that students who are members of minority groups 
systematically receive lower scores on standardized col- 
lege entrance exams. What suggestions do you have for 
helping these students improve their scores? What advice 
about their college applications would you give these stu- 
dents to help them be competitive applicants? 


To what degree is intelligence influenced by the environment 
and to what degree by heredity? 

e Attempting to distinguish environmental from heredi- 
tary factors in intelligence is probably futile and certainly 
misguided. Because individual IQ scores vary far more 
than group IQ scores, it is more critical to ask what can 
be done to maximize the intellectual development of 
each individual. (p. 301) 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 
Teyuowuoruə ÁIeppaIy p UBU 
EVALUATE TENprAarput Ur SUOLLA IpIM are areyy dnors Aue uyy ‘aouast[ayut 
JENPTIAIPuUI ainseoul o} pasn are $}sa} ÖJ ‘AsTey E eJ- MYND F ‘andy I 
1. Intelligence tests may be biased toward the prevailing cul- 
ture in such a way that minorities are put at a disadvan- 
tage when taking these tests. True or false? 


KEY TERMS 


culture-fair IQ test p. 301 
heritability p. 301 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Psychology on the Web 


1. Many sites on the web permit you to assess your IQ. Take at least two such tests and 
(a) compare your results, (b) indicate what mental qualities seemed to be tested on 
the tests, and (c) discuss your impression of the reliability and validity of the tests. 
Write up your conclusions. 

2. Find a way to assess at least one other of your multiple intelligences (that is, one not 
tested by the IQ tests you took) on the web. What sort of intelligence was the test 
supposed to be testing? What sorts of items were included? How valid and reliable do 
you think it was, both in and of itself and compared with the IQ tests you took? 


È D | | OQ U e We've just examined one of the most controversial areas 


of psychology— intelligence. Some of the most heated 
discussions in all of psychology focus on this topic, engaging educators, policymakers, 
politicians, and psychologists. The issues include the very meaning of intelligence, its 
measurement, individual extremes of intelligence, and, finally, the heredity-environment 
question. We saw that the quest to partition intelligence into hereditary factors versus 
environmental factors is generally pointless. In the area of intelligence, the focus of our 
efforts should be on making sure that every individual has the opportunity to achieve 
his or her potential. 

Before we leave the topic of intelligence, return to the stories of the two persons of 
widely different intellectual capabilities discussed in the Prologue, Chris Burke and Sho 
Yano. Consider the following questions on the basis of what you have learned about 
intelligence. 





1. Chris Burke's physicians concluded in his infancy that he would never be able to 
function effectively in society and should be immediately institutionalized. How do 
you think the physicians came to their conclusions, and why do you think they were 
proven wrong? 

2. In what ways would placing Burke in a separate educational program have helped or 
hurt his chances of reaching his full potential? 

3. How might the educational acceleration of Sho Yano help and hinder his later 
development? Do you think slowing down his educational progress might be 
beneficial? How? 

4. Based on research relating to individuals who have unusually high IQ scores, what do 
you think Sho's emotional intelligence is like? 





305 








CHAPTER 10 


Motivation and Emotion 











www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Key Concepts for Chapter 10 


MODULE 29 







How does motivation direct and energize Explaining Motivation 
nstinct Approaches: 


ior? 
behavior? Born to Be Motivated 


Drive-Reduction Approaches: 
Satisfying Our Needs 


Arousal Approaches: 
Beyond Drive Reduction 





ncentive Approaches: Motivation’s Pull 


Cognitive Approaches: 
The Thoughts Behind Motivation 


Maslow's Hierarchy: Ordering 
Motivational Needs 


Applying the Different 
Approaches to Motivation 


MODULE 30 






What biological and social factors underlie Human Needs and Motivation: 
Eat, Drink, and Be Daring 


The Motivation Behind 
Hunger and Eating 


hunger? @ How are needs relating to 


achievement, affiliation, and power 
Social Factors in Eating 


motivation exhibited? PsychWork: Nutritionist 
Eating Disorders 
Neuroscience in Your Life: Cognitive 
Processing in Anorexic Patients 


Becoming an Informed Consumer 
of Psychology: Dieting and Losing 
Weight Successfully 

The Need for Achievement: 
Striving for Success 

The Need for Affiliation: 

Striving for Friendship 





The Need for Power: Striving 
for Impact on Others 









MODULE 31 





What are emotions, and how do we Understanding Emotional 


Experiences 


experience them? @ What are the functions 7 l l 
he Functions of Emotions 


Determining the Range of Emotions: 
Labeling Our Feelings 


of emotions? @ What are explanations for 


emotions? ® How does nonverbal behavior 





The Roots of Emotions 


relate to the expression of emotions? Exploring Diversity: Do People in All 
Cultures Express Emotion Similarly? 


Applying Psychology in the 21st Century: 
Smiling Athletes—Do Their Facial 
Expressions Come Naturally? 


307 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Prologue Falling Off the Diet Wagon 


When Kirstie Alley stepped on the scale for the first time in 

15 months, she had a hunch that it wouldn't be pretty. “Il thought 

| weighed 190, but | got on the scale and started screaming,” 

recalls Alley. “It said 228 Ibs., which is my. highest weight ever.” 
But looking back, Alley, 58, is hardly shocked that her body... 

had ballooned beyond 200 Ibs. yet again. . À She had not worked 


out and even banished her gym equipment to the garage. As for 
her diet, her small, low-calorie portions gave way to Chinese 
takeout and pasta drenched in butter. 

“| fell off the horse,” says the 5'8” star... | just sort of went wild.” 
(Leonard, 2009, p. 50) 





Actress Kirstie Alley’s notorious up-and-down battle with 
obesity—at one point she dieted down to 145 pounds, only to 
gain back the weight—is just one very public example of the 
struggles that hundreds of millions of people have maintaining 
an appropriate weight. But why is it so difficult to maintain an 
appropriate weight? Why do the natural mechanisms that regulate 
our other bodily functions often fail when it comes to regulat- 
ing our eating behavior? These questions and many others are 
addressed by psychologists who study the topics of motivation 
and emotion. Psychologists who study motivation seek to discover 
the particular desired goals—the motives—that underlie behavior. 
Behaviors as basic as drinking to satisfy thirst and as inconsequential 
as taking a stroll to get exercise exemplify motives. Psychologists 
specializing in the study of motivation assume that such underlying 
motives steer our choices of activities. 

While motivation concerns the forces that direct future 
behavior, emotion pertains to the feelings we experience 


308 


throughout our lives. The study of emotions focuses on our 
internal experiences at any given moment. All of us feel a variety 
of emotions: happiness at succeeding at a difficult task, sadness 
over the death of a loved one, anger at being treated unfairly. 
Because emotions not only play a role in motivating our behavior 
but also act as a reflection of our underlying motivation, they 
play an important role in our lives. 

We begin this set of modules by focusing on the major concep- 
tions of motivation, discussing how different motives and needs 
jointly affect behavior. We consider motives that are biologically 
based and universal in the animal kingdom, such as hunger, as 
well as motives that are unique to humans, such as the need for 
achievement. 

We then turn to emotions. We consider the roles and functions 
that emotions play in people's lives and discuss several approaches 
that explain how people understand their emotions. Finally, we 
look at how nonverbal behavior communicates emotions. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 29 


In just a moment, 27-year-old Aron Ralston’s life changed. An 800-pound boulder dis- 
lodged in a narrow canyon where Ralston was hiking in an isolated Utah canyon, pinning 
his lower arm to the ground. 

For the next five days, Ralston lay in the dense, lonely forest, unable to escape. An 
experienced climber who had search-and-rescue training, he had ample time to consider 
his options. He tried unsuccessfully to chip away at the rock, and he rigged up ropes 
and pulleys around the boulder in a vain effort to move it. 

Finally, out of water and nearly dehydrated, Ralston reasoned there was only one 
option left short of dying. In acts of incredible bravery, Ralston broke two bones in his 
wrist, applied a tourniquet, and used a dull pen knife to amputate his arm beneath 
the elbow. 

Freed from his entrapment, Ralston climbed down from where he had been pinned 
and then hiked five miles to safety (Cox, 2003; Lofholm, 2003). 


What motivation lay behind Ralston’s resolve? 

To answer this question, psychologists employ the concept of motivation, the 
factors that direct and energize the behavior of humans and other organisms. Moti- 
vation has biological, cognitive, and social aspects, and the complexity of the concept 
has led psychologists to develop a variety of approaches. All seek to explain the 
energy that guides people’s behavior in specific directions. 


Instinct Approaches: 
Born to Be Motivated 


When psychologists first tried to explain motivation, they turned to instincts, inborn 
patterns of behavior that are biologically determined rather than learned. According 
to instinct approaches to motivation, people and animals are born preprogrammed 
with sets of behaviors essential to their survival. Those instincts provide the energy 
that channels behavior in appropriate directions. Hence, sexual behavior may be a 
response to an instinct to reproduce, and exploratory behavior may be motivated by 
an instinct to examine one’s territory. 

This conception presents several difficulties, however. For one thing, psycholo- 
gists do not agree on what, or even how many, primary instincts exist. One early 
psychologist, William McDougall (1908), suggested that there are 18 instincts. Other 
theorists came up with even more—with one sociologist (Bernard, 1924) claiming 
that there are exactly 5,759 distinct instincts! 

Furthermore, explanations based on the concept of instincts do not go very far 
toward explaining why one specific pattern of behavior, and not others, has appeared 
in a given species. In addition, although it is clear that much animal behavior is based 
on instincts, much of the variety and complexity of human behavior is learned and 
thus cannot be seen as instinctual. 

As a result of these shortcomings, newer explanations have replaced conceptions 
of motivation based on instincts. However, instinct approaches still play a role in 








How does motivation direct 
and energize behavior? 


L S 


Aron Ralston 


The factors that direct and 
energize the behavior of humans and 
other organisms. 


Inborn patterns of behavior 
that are biologically determined rather 
than learned. 


309 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


310 Chapter 10 Motivation and Emotion 


drive-reduction approaches to 
motivation Theories suggesting that a 
lack of a basic biological requirement 
such as water produces a drive to 
obtain that requirement (in this case, 
the thirst drive). 


drive Motivational tension, or arousal, 
that energizes behavior to fulfill a 
need. 


homeostasis The body’s tendency to 
maintain a steady internal state. 


ly Alert 


To remember the concept of 
homeostasis, keep in mind 
the analogy of a thermo- 
stat that regulates the 
temperature in a house. 


certain theories, especially those based on evolutionary approaches that focus on our 
genetic inheritance. Furthermore, Freud’s work suggests that instinctual drives of sex 
and aggression motivate behavior (Katz, 2001). 


Drive-Reduction Approaches: 
Satisfying Our Needs 


After rejecting instinct theory, psychologists first proposed simple drive-reduction 
theories of motivation to take its place (Hull, 1943). Drive-reduction approaches to 
motivation suggest that a lack of some basic biological requirement such as water 
produces a drive to obtain that requirement (in this case, the thirst drive). 

To understand this approach, we begin with the concept of drive. A drive is 
motivational tension, or arousal, that energizes behavior to fulfill a need. Many basic 
drives, such as hunger, thirst, sleep, and sex, are related to biological needs of the 
body or of the species as a whole. These are called primary drives. Primary drives 
contrast with secondary drives in which behavior fulfills no obvious biological need. 
In secondary drives, prior experience and learning bring about needs. For instance, 
some people have strong needs to achieve academically and professionally. We can 
say that their achievement need is reflected in a secondary drive that motivates their 
behavior (McKinley et al., 2004; Seli, 2007). 

We usually try to satisfy a primary drive by reducing the need underlying it. For 
example, we become hungry after not eating for a few hours and may raid the 
refrigerator, especially if the next scheduled meal is not imminent. If the weather 
turns cold, we put on extra clothing or raise the setting on the thermostat to keep 
warm. If our bodies need liquids to function properly, we experience thirst and seek 
out water. 





HOMEOSTASIS 


Homeostasis, the body’s tendency to maintain a steady internal state, underlies pri- 
mary drives. Using feedback loops, homeostasis brings deviations in body function- 
ing back to an optimal state, similar to the way a thermostat and a furnace work in 
a home heating system to maintain a steady temperature (see Figure 1). Receptor 
cells throughout the body constantly monitor factors such as temperature and nutri- 
ent levels. When deviations from the ideal state occur, the body adjusts in an effort 
to return to an optimal state. Many fundamental needs, including the needs for food, 
water, stable body temperature, and sleep, operate via homeostasis (Black, 2006; Shin, 
Zheng, & Berthoud, 2009; Vassalli & Dijk, 2009). 

Although drive-reduction theories provide a good explanation of how primary 
drives motivate behavior, they cannot fully explain a behavior in which the goal is 
not to reduce a drive but rather to maintain or even increase the level of excitement 
or arousal. For instance, some behaviors seem to be motivated by nothing more 
than curiosity, such as rushing to check e-mail messages. Similarly, many people 
pursue thrilling activities such as riding a roller coaster or steering a raft down the 
rapids of a river. Such behaviors certainly don’t suggest that people seek to reduce 
all drives as drive-reduction approaches would indicate (Begg & Langley, 2001; 
Rosenbloom & Wolf, 2002). 

Both curiosity and thrill-seeking behavior, then, shed doubt on drive-reduction 
approaches as a complete explanation for motivation. In both cases, rather than 
seeking to reduce an underlying drive, people and animals appear to be moti- 
vated to increase their overall level of stimulation and activity. To explain this 
phenomenon, psychologists have devised an alternative: arousal approaches to 
motivation. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 29 Explaining Motivation 311 


























Set point for heating 
When the room cools to 
this point, the regulatory 

system signals the furnace 
for heat. 


Set point for cooling 
When the room heats to 
this point, the regulatory 
system signals the air 

conditioner to cool. 


The sensor measures 
room temperature, 

providing feedback to 
the regulatory system. 













oS Se 





Air 
conditioner 





Controlled systems 


Arousal Approaches: 
Beyond Drive Reduction 


Arousal approaches seek to explain behavior in which the goal is to maintain or 
increase excitement. According to arousal approaches to motivation, each person 
tries to maintain a certain level of stimulation and activity. As with the drive- 
reduction model, this model suggests that if our stimulation and activity levels 
become too high, we try to reduce them. But, in contrast to the drive-reduction 
model, the arousal model also suggests that if levels of stimulation and activity are 
too low, we will try to increase them by seeking stimulation. 

People vary widely in the optimal level of arousal they seek out, with some people 
looking for especially high levels of arousal. For example, people who participate in 
daredevil sports, high-stakes gamblers, and criminals who pull off high-risk robberies 
may be exhibiting a particularly high need for arousal (see Figure 2 on page 312; 
Zuckerman & Kuhlman, 2000; Zuckerman, 2002; Cavenett & Nixon, 2006). 





Incentive Approaches: 
Motivation’s Pull 


When a luscious dessert appears on the table after a filling meal, its appeal has little 
or nothing to do with internal drives or the maintenance of arousal. Rather, if we 
choose to eat the dessert, such behavior is motivated by the external stimulus of the 





FIGURE 1 With homeostasis, a series of 
feedback loops is used to regulate body 
functions, similar to the way a thermostat 
turns on the furnace when a room's air 
temperature becomes too cool and turns 
it off when the air temperature becomes 
too warm. Similarly, when body tempera- 
ture becomes too low, the blood vessels 
constrict, and we shiver and seek warmth. 
When body temperature becomes too 
high, the blood vessels expand, and 

we sweat as our bodies try to lower the 
temperature. Can you think of other 
internal systems that operate via 
homeostasis? 


arousal approaches to motivation 
The belief that we try to maintain 
certain levels of stimulation and 
activity increasing or reducing them 
as necessary. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


312 Chapter 10 Motivation and Emotion 


FIGURE 2 Some people seek high 
levels of arousal, while others are more K 


easygoing. You can get a sense of your 
own preferred level of stimulation by 
completing this questionnaire. (Source: 
“Do You Seek Out Sensation?” questionnaire 
from Marvin Zuckerman, “The Search for 
High Sensation,” Psychology Today, February 
1978, pp. 30-46. Reprinted with permission 
from Psychology Today magazine, (Copyright 
© 1978 Sussex Publishers, LLC.) 


Do You Seek Out Sensation? 


How much stimulation do you crave in your everyday life? You will have an idea after you 
complete the following questionnaire, which lists some items from a scale designed to 
assess your sensation-seeking tendencies. Circle either A or B in each pair or statements. 


1. A I would like a job that requires a lot of travelling. 
| would prefer a job in one location. 
| am invigorated by a brisk, cold day. 
| can't wait to get indoors on a cold day. 
| get bored seeing the same old faces. 
| like the comfortable familiarity of everyday friends. 
| would prefer living in an ideal society in which everyone was safe, secure, 
and happy. 
| would have preferred living in the unsettled days of our history. 
| sometimes like to do things that are a little frightening. 
A sensible person avoids activities that are dangerous. 
| would not like to be hypnotized. 
| would like to have the experience of being hypnotized. 
The most important goal of life is to live it to the fullest and to experience as much 
as possible. 
The most important goal of life is to find peace and happiness. 
| would like to try parachute jumping. 
| would never want to try jumping out of a plane, with or without a parachute. 
| enter cold water gradually, giving myself time to get used to it. 
| like to dive or jump right into the ocean or a cold pool. 
When | go on a vacation, | prefer the comfort of a good room and bed. 
When | go on a vacation, | prefer the change of camping out. 
| prefer people who are emotionally expressive, even if they are a bit unstable. 
| prefer people who are calm and even-tempered. 
A good painting should shock or jolt the senses. 
A good painting should give one a feeling of peace and security. 
13. A People who ride motorcycles must have some kind of unconscious need to 
hurt themselves. 
B | would like to drive or ride a motorcycle. 


o 
raorwnrw rwrworwvy> 


S 
Wrnwnrvwrwrwvorw 


Scoring: Give yourself one point for each of the following responses: 1A, 2A, 3A, 4B, 5A, 6B, 7A, 8A, 
9B, 10B, 11A, 12A, 13B. Find your total score by adding up the number of points and then use the 
following scoring key. 


0-3 very low sensation seeking 

4-5 low 

6-9 average 

10-11 high 

12-13 very high 

Keep in mind, of course, that this short questionnaire, for which the scoring is based on the results 
of college students who have taken it, provides only a rough estimate of your sensation-seeking 
tendencies. Moreover, as people get older, their sensation-seeking scores tend to decrease. Still, 


the questionnaire will at least give you an indication of how your sensation-seeking tendencies 
compare with those of others. 


dessert itself, which acts as an anticipated reward. This reward, in motivational 
terms, is an incentive. 


incentive approaches to motivation 


Incentive approaches to motivation suggest that motivation stems from the 


Theories suggesting that motivation desire to obtain valued external goals, or incentives. In this view, the desirable prop- 
stems from the desire to obtain valued erties of external stimuli—whether grades, money, affection, food, or sex—account 
external goals, or incentives. for a person’s motivation (Festinger et al., 2009). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 29 Explaining Motivation 313 


Although the theory explains why we may succumb to an incentive (such as a 
mouth-watering dessert) even though we lack internal cues (such as hunger), it does 
not provide a complete explanation of motivation because organisms sometimes seek 
to fulfill needs even when incentives are not apparent. Consequently, many psy- 
chologists believe that the internal drives proposed by drive-reduction theory work 
in tandem with the external incentives of incentive theory to “push” and “pull” 
behavior, respectively. Thus, at the same time that we seek to satisfy our underlying 
hunger needs (the push of drive-reduction theory), we are drawn to food that appears 
very appetizing (the pull of incentive theory). Rather than contradicting each other, 
then, drives and incentives may work together in motivating behavior (Pinel, 
Assanand, & Lehman, 2000; Lowery, Fillingim, & Wright, 2003; Berridge, 2004). 


Cognitive Approaches: The 
Thoughts Behind Motivation 


Cognitive approaches to motivation suggest that motivation is a product of people’s 
thoughts, expectations, and goals—their cognitions. For instance, the degree to which 
people are motivated to study for a test is based on their expectation of how well 
studying will pay off in terms of a good grade. 

Cognitive theories of motivation draw a key distinction between intrinsic and 
extrinsic motivation. Intrinsic motivation causes us to participate in an activity for our 
own enjoyment rather than for any concrete, tangible reward that it will bring us. In 
contrast, extrinsic motivation causes us to do something for money, a grade, or some 
other concrete, tangible reward. For example, when a physician works long hours 
because she loves medicine, intrinsic motivation is prompting her; if she works hard 
to make a lot of money, extrinsic motivation underlies her efforts (Lepper, Corpus, 
& lyengar, 2005; Shaikholeslami & Khayyer, 2006; Finkelstein, 2009). 

We are more apt to persevere, work harder, and produce work of higher quality 
when motivation for a task is intrinsic rather than extrinsic. In fact, in some cases 
providing rewards for desirable behavior (thereby increasing extrinsic motivation) 
actually may decrease intrinsic motivation (Henderlong & Lepper, 2002; James, 2005; 
Grant, 2008). 





Maslow's Hierarchy: Ordering 
Motivational Needs 


What do Eleanor Roosevelt, Abraham Lincoln, and Albert Einstein have in common? 
The common thread, according to a model of motivation devised by psychologist 
Abraham Maslow, is that each of them fulfilled the highest levels of motivational 
needs underlying human behavior. 

Maslow’s model places motivational needs in a hierarchy and suggests that 
before more sophisticated, higher-order needs can be met, certain primary needs 
must be satisfied (Maslow, 1970, 1987). A pyramid can represent the model with the 
more basic needs at the bottom and the higher-level needs at the top (see Figure 3). 
To activate a specific higher-order need, thereby guiding behavior, a person must first 
fulfill the more basic needs in the hierarchy. 

The basic needs are primary drives: needs for water, food, sleep, sex, and the 
like. To move up the hierarchy, a person must first meet these basic physiological 
needs. Safety needs come next in the hierarchy; Maslow suggests that people need 
a safe, secure environment in order to function effectively. Physiological and safety 
needs compose the lower-order needs. 





cognitive approaches to motivation 
Theories suggesting that motivation 

is a product of people’s thoughts, ex- 
pectations, and goals—their cognitions. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


314 Chapter 10 Motivation and Emotion 


FIGURE 3 Maslow's hierarchy shows 
how our motivation progresses up the 
pyramid from the broadest, most 
fundamental biological needs to 
higher-order ones. Do you agree that 
lower-order needs must be satisfied 
before higher-order needs? Do hermits 
and monks who attempt to fulfill 
spiritual needs while denying basic 
physical needs contradict Maslow’s 
hierarchy? (After Maslow, 1970.) 


self-actualization A state of self- 
fulfillment in which people realize 
their highest potential in their own 
unique way. 





Only after meeting the basic lower-order needs can a person consider fulfilling 
higher-order needs, such as the needs for love and a sense of belonging, esteem, and 
self-actualization. Love and belongingness needs include the needs to obtain and give 
affection and to be a contributing member of some group or society. After fulfilling 
these needs, a person strives for esteem. In Maslow’s thinking, esteem relates to the 
need to develop a sense of self-worth by recognizing that others know and value 
one’s competence. 

Once these four sets of needs are fulfilled—no easy task—a person is able to 
strive for the highest-level need, self-actualization. Self-actualization is a state of 
self-fulfillment in which people realize their highest potentials in their own unique 
way. Although Maslow first suggested that self-actualization occurred in only a few 
famous individuals, he later expanded the concept to encompass everyday people. 
For example, a parent with excellent nurturing skills who raises a family, a teacher 
who year after year creates an environment that maximizes students’ opportunities 
for success, and an artist who realizes his creative potential all may be self-actualized. 
The important thing is that people feel at ease with themselves and satisfied that 
they are using their talents to the fullest. In a sense, achieving self-actualization 
reduces the striving and yearning for greater fulfillment that mark most people’s 
lives and instead provides a sense of satisfaction with the current state of affairs 
(Piechowski, 2003; Reiss & Havercamp, 2005; Laas, 2006). 

Although research has been unable to validate the specific ordering of Maslow’s 
stages, and it is difficult to measure self-actualization objectively, Maslow’s hierarchy 
of needs is important for two reasons: It highlights the complexity of human needs, 
and it emphasizes the idea that until more basic biological needs are met, people will 
be relatively unconcerned with higher-order needs. For example, if people are hun- 
gry, their first interest will be in obtaining food; they will not be concerned with 
needs such as love and self-esteem (Hanley & Abell, 2002; Samantaray, Srivastava, 
& Mishra, 2002; Ojha & Pramanick, 2009). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 29 Explaining Motivation 315 


Maslow’s hierarchy of needs has also spawned other approaches to motivation. 
For example, Edward Deci and Richard Ryan (2008) have considered human needs 
in terms of psychological well-being. They suggest in their self-determination theory 
that people have the three basic needs of competence, autonomy, and relatedness. 
Competence is the need to produce desired outcomes, while autonomy is the percep- 
tion that we have control over our own lives. Finally, relatedness is the need to be 
involved in close, warm relationships with others. In the view of self-determination 
theory, these three psychological needs are innate and universal across cultures, and 
they are essential as basic biological needs (Jang et al., 2009). 


Applying the Different 
Approaches to Motivation 


The various theories of motivation (summarized in Figure 4) give several different 
perspectives on it. Which provides the fullest account of motivation? Actually, many 
of the approaches are complementary rather than contradictory. In fact, employing 
more than one approach can help us understand motivation in a particular instance. 

Consider, for example, Aron Ralston’s accident while hiking (described earlier). 
His interest in climbing in an isolated and potentially dangerous area may be 
explained by arousal approaches to motivation. From the perspective of instinct 
approaches, we realize that Aron had an overwhelming instinct to preserve his life 













FIGURE 4 The major approaches to motivation. 






Use Figure 4 to learn the 
distinctions among the 
different explanations for 
motivation (instinct, drive 
reduction, arousal, incen- 


tive, cognitive, and Maslow’s 
hierarchy of needs). 





Cognitive 
Thoughts, 
expectations, and 
understanding of 
the world direct 
motivation. 





316 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 10 Motivation and Emotion 


at all costs. From a cognitive perspective, we see his careful consideration of various 
strategies to extricate himself from the boulder. 

In short, applying multiple approaches to motivation in a given situation pro- 
vides a broader understanding than we might obtain by employing only a single 
approach. We'll see this again when we consider specific motives—such as the needs 
for food, achievement, affiliation, and power—and draw on several of the theories 
for the fullest account of what motivates our behavior. 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 


How does motivation direct and energize behavior? 


Motivation relates to the factors that direct and energize 
behavior. (p. 309) 

Drive is the motivational tension that energizes behavior 
to fulfill a need. (p. 310) 

Homeostasis, the maintenance of a steady internal state, 
often underlies motivational drives. (p. 310) 

Arousal approaches suggest that we try to maintain a 
particular level of stimulation and activity. (p. 311) 
Incentive approaches focus on the positive aspects 

of the environment that direct and energize behavior. 
(p. 311) 

Cognitive approaches focus on the role of thoughts, 
expectations, and understanding of the world in 
producing motivation. (p. 313) 

Maslow’s hierarchy suggests that there are five basic 
needs: physiological, safety, love and belongingness, 
esteem, and self-actualization. Only after the more basic 
needs are fulfilled can a person move toward meeting 
higher-order needs. (p. 314) 


EVALUATE 


1. 


27 


are forces that guide a person’s behavior in a 
certain direction. 
Biologically determined, inborn patterns of behavior are 
known as 


KEY TERMS 


motivation p. 309 

instincts p. 309 

drive-reduction approaches to motivation p. 310 
drive p. 310 


homeostasis p. 310 


3. 


Your psychology professor tells you, “Explaining behavior 
is easy! When we lack something, we are motivated to get 
it.” Which approach to motivation does your professor 
subscribe to? 


4. By drinking water after running a marathon, a runner 


tries to keep his or her body at an optimal level of 
functioning. This process is called 


. [help an elderly person cross the street because doing a 


good deed makes me feel good. What type of motivation 
is at work here? What type of motivation would be at 
work if I were to help an elderly man across the street 
because he paid me $20? 


. According to Maslow, a person with no job, no home, and 


no friends can become self-actualized. True or false? 


RETHINK 


Al 


Which approaches to motivation are more commonly 
used in the workplace? How might each approach be used 
to design employment policies that can sustain or increase 
motivation? 

From the perspective of an educator: Do you think that giving 
students grades serves as an external reward that would 
decrease intrinsic motivation for the subject matter? Why 
or why not? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


m590 ued uopezTyenpe 
-JES a10Jaq paT[Y][NJ aq JSNUI spaeu JapIO-19aMOy ‘asTeJ *9 ‘OISUTI}Xa 
‘SISULUT 'G ‘SISe}SOSUIOY ‘p ‘UOTON pal 9ALIp *E ‘SJOUTISUT ‘Z ‘SOATJOU ‘L 


arousal approaches to motivation p. 311 
incentive approaches to motivation p. 312 
cognitive approaches to motivation p. 313 
self-actualization p. 314 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 30 


As a sophomore at the University of California, Santa Cruz, Lisa Arndt followed a 
menu of her own making: For breakfast she ate cereal or fruit with 10 diet pills and 

50 chocolate-flavored laxatives. Lunch was a salad or sandwich; dinner: chicken and 
rice. But it was the feast that followed that Arndt relished most. Almost every night at 
about 9 p.m., she would retreat to her room and eat an entire small pizza and a whole 
batch of cookies. Then she’d wait for the day’s laxatives to take effect. “It was extremely 
painful,” says Arndt of those days. . . . “But I was that desperate to make up for my 
binging. I was terrified of fat the way other people are afraid of lions or guns.” 
(Hubbard, O’Neill, & Cheakalos, 1999, p. 59) 


Lisa was one of the 10 million women (and 1 million men) who are estimated to 
suffer from an eating disorder. These disorders, which usually appear during ado- 
lescence, can bring about extraordinary weight loss and other forms of physical dete- 
rioration. Extremely dangerous, they sometimes result in death. 

Why are Lisa and others like her subject to such disordered eating, which 
revolves around the motivation to avoid weight gain at all costs? And why do so 
many other people engage in overeating, which leads to obesity? 

To answer these questions, we must consider some of the specific needs that 
underlie behavior. In this module, we examine several of the most important human 
needs. We begin with hunger, the primary drive that has received the most attention 
from researchers, and then we turn to secondary drives—those uniquely human 
endeavors based on learned needs and past experience that help explain why people 
strive to achieve, to affiliate with others, and to seek power over others. 


The Motivation Behind 
Hunger and Eating 


Two hundred million people in the United States—some two-thirds of the population— 
are overweight. Almost a quarter are so heavy that they have obesity, body weight 
that is more than 20 percent above the average weight for a person of a particular 
height. And the rest of the world is not far behind: A billion people around the globe 
are overweight or obese. The World Health Organization has said that worldwide 
obesity has reached epidemic proportions, producing increases in heart disease, dia- 
betes, cancer, and premature deaths (Hill, Catenacci, & Wyatt, 2005; Stephenson & 
Banet-Weiser, 2007). 

The most widely used measure of obesity is body mass index (BMI), which is based 
on a ratio of weight to height. People with a BMI greater than 30 are considered 
obese, whereas those with a BMI between 25 and 30 are overweight. (Use the for- 
mulas in Figure 1 to determine your own BMI.) 

Although the definition of obesity is clear from a scientific point of view, people’s 
perceptions of what an ideal body looks like vary significantly across different cultures 
and, within Western cultures, from one time period to another. For instance, many 
contemporary Western cultures stress the importance of slimness in women—a 





What biological and social 
factors underlie hunger? 


How are needs relating to 


achievement, affiliation, and 
power motivation exhibited? 


Body weight that is more than 


20% above the average weight for a 
person of a particular height. 


317 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


318 Chapter 10 Motivation and Emotion 


FIGURE 1 Use this procedure to find 
your body mass index. ~ 


To calculate your body mass index, follow these steps: 


1. Indicate your weight in pounds: —— pounds 
2. Indicate your height in inches: ___________ inches 
3. Divide your weight (item 1) by your height (item 2), and write the outcome here: 


4. Divide the result above (item 3) by your height (item 2), and write the outcome here: 


5. Multiply the number above by 703, and write the product here: . This 
is your body mass index. 


Example: 
For a person who weights 210 pounds and who is 6 feet tall, divide 210 pounds by 72 
inches, which equals 2.917. Then divide 2.917 by 72 inches (item 3), which yields .041. 
Multiplying .041 (from item 4) by 703 yields a BMI of 28.5. 


Interpretation: 
Underweight = less than 18.5 
Normal weight = 18.5-24.9 
Overweight = 25-29.9 
Obesity = BMI of 30 or greater 


Keep in mind that a BMI greater than 25 may or may not be due to excess body fat. For 
example, professional athletes may have little fat but weigh more than the average 
person because they have greater muscle mass. 


relatively recent view. In 19th century Hawaii, the most attractive women were those 
who were the heaviest. Furthermore, for most of the 20th century—except for periods 
in the 1920s and the most recent decades—the ideal female figure was relatively full. 
Even today, weight standards differ among different cultural groups. For instance, in 
some traditional Arab cultures, obese women are so prized as wives that parents 
force-feed their female children to make them more desirable (Naik, 2004; Blixen, 
Singh, & Xu, 2006; Marsh, Hau, & Sung, 2007). 

Regardless of cultural standards for appearance and weight, no one doubts that 
being overweight represents a major health risk. However, controlling weight is com- 
plicated because eating behavior involves a variety of mechanisms. In our discussion 
of what motivates people to eat, we'll start with the biological aspects of eating. 


BIOLOGICAL FACTORS IN 
THE REGULATION OF HUNGER 


In contrast to human beings, other species are unlikely to 
become obese. Internal mechanisms regulate not only the quan- 
tity of food they take in, but also the kind of food they desire. 
For example, rats that have been deprived of particular foods 
seek out alternatives that contain the specific nutrients their diet 
is lacking, and many species, given the choice of a wide variety 


















w i i @ ea 





p4 = of foods, select a well-balanced diet (Bouchard & Bray, 1996; 
A / Woods et al., 2000; Jones & Corp, 2003). 

PM — 7 Complex biological mechanisms tell organisms whether they 

dA Ee S É| require food or should stop eating. It’s not just a matter of an 

—— E a K AA empty stomach causing hunger pangs and a full one alleviating 

f o paataan those pangs. (Even individuals who have had their stomachs 


© Michael Maslin/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


“Gee, I had no idea you were married to a supermodel.” removed still experience the sensation of hunger.) One important 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 30 Human Needs and Motivation: Eat, Drink, and Be Daring 319 


Hypothalamus 





Pituitary gland 


FIGURE 2 The hypothalamus acts as the brain's “feeding center” and is primarily responsible 
for monitoring food intake. 


factor is changes in the chemical composition of the blood. For instance, changes in 
levels of glucose, a kind of sugar, regulate feelings of hunger. In addition, the hormone 
insulin leads the body to store excess sugar in the blood as fats and carbohydrates. 
Finally, the hormone ghrelin communicates to the brain feelings of hunger. The produc- 
tion of ghrelin increases according to meal schedules as well as the sight or smell of 
food, producing the feeling that tells us we’re hungry and should eat (Teff, 2007; Wren 
& Bloom, 2007; Kojima & Kangawa, 2008). 

The brain’s hypothalamus monitors glucose levels (see Figure 2). Increasing evi- 
dence suggests that the hypothalamus carries the primary responsibility for monitor- 
ing food intake. Injury to the hypothalamus has radical consequences for eating 
behavior, depending on the site of the injury. For example, rats whose lateral hypo- 
thalamus is damaged may literally starve to death. They refuse food when it is offered; 
unless they are force-fed, they eventually die. Rats with an injury to the ventromedial 
hypothalamus display the opposite problem: extreme overeating. Rats with this injury 
can increase in weight by as much as 400%. Similar phenomena occur in humans 
who have tumors of the hypothalamus (Woods & Seeley, 2002; Seymour, 2006; Fedeli 
et al., 2009). 

Although the important role the hypothalamus plays in regulating food intake 
is clear, the exact way this organ operates is still unclear. One hypothesis suggests 
that injury to the hypothalamus affects the weight set point, or the particular level 
of weight that the body strives to maintain, which in turn regulates food intake. 
Acting as a kind of internal weight thermostat, the hypothalamus calls for either 
greater or less food intake (Capaldi, 1996; Woods et al., 2000; Berthoud, 2002). 

In most cases, the hypothalamus does a good job. Even people who are not 
deliberately monitoring their weight show only minor weight fluctuations in spite of 
substantial day-to-day variations in how much they eat and exercise. However, injury 
to the hypothalamus can alter the weight set point, and a person then struggles to 
meet the internal goal by increasing or decreasing food consumption. Even tempo- 
rary exposure to certain drugs can alter the weight set point (Cabanac & Frankham, 
2002; Hallschmid et al., 2004; Khazaal et al., 2008). 

Genetic factors determine the weight set point, at least in part. People seem 
destined, through heredity, to have a particular metabolism, the rate at which food 
is converted to energy and expended by the body. People with a high metabolic rate 
can eat virtually as much as they want without gaining weight, whereas others with 
low metabolism may eat literally half as much yet gain weight readily (Jequier, 2002; 
Westerterp, 2006). 





weight set point The particular level 
of weight that the body strives to 
maintain. 


metabolism The rate at which food is 
converted to energy and expended by 
the body. 


320 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 10 Motivation and Emotion 


y Alert 


Akey point: Eating and 
hunger are influenced both 
by biological and social 
factors. 


NUTRITIONIST 


Name: Gail K. Rupert 
Position: Nutritionist 


Education: University of Pennsylvania, West 
Chester, Pennsylvania; BS in Nutrition and Dietetics 





Social Factors in Eating 


You’ve just finished a full meal and feel completely stuffed. Suddenly your host 
announced with great fanfare that he will be serving his “house specialty” dessert, 
bananas flambé, and that he has spent the better part of the afternoon preparing it. 
Even though you are full and don’t even like bananas, you accept a serving of his 
dessert and eat it all. 

Clearly, internal biological factors do not fully explain our eating behavior. Exter- 
nal social factors, based on societal rules and on what we have learned about appro- 
priate eating behavior, also play an important role. Take, for example, the simple fact 
that people customarily eat breakfast, lunch, and dinner at approximately the same 
times every day. Because we tend to eat on schedule everyday, we feel hungry as the 
usual hour approaches, sometimes quite independently of what our internal cues are 
telling us. 

Similarly, we put roughly the same amount of food on our plates everyday, 
even though the amount of exercise we may have had (and consequently our need 
for energy replenishment) varies from day to day. We also tend to prefer particular 
foods over others. Rats and dogs may be a delicacy in certain Asian cultures, but 
few people in Western cultures find them appealing despite their potentially high 
nutritional value. Even the amount of food we eat varies according to cultural 
norms. For instance, people in the United States eat bigger portions than people in 
France. In sum, cultural influences and our individual habits play important roles 
in determining when, what, and how much we eat (Miller & Pumariega, 2001; 
Rozin et al., 2003). 

Other social factors relate to our eating behavior as well. Some of us head toward 
the refrigerator after a difficult day seeking solace in a pint of Heath Bar Crunch ice 
cream. Why? Perhaps when we were children, our parents gave us food when we 
were upset. Eventually, we may have learned through the basic mechanisms of clas- 
sical and operant conditioning to associate food with comfort and consolation. Sim- 
ilarly, we may learn that eating, which focuses our attention on immediate pleasures, 
provides an escape from unpleasant thoughts. Consequently, we may eat when we 
feel distressed (Bulik et al., 2003; O’Connor & O’Connor, 2004; Elfhag, Tynelius, & 
Rasmussen, 2007; also see the PsychWork box). 


h k Obesity has become a serious health issue for many Ameri- 
Psyc Wo r cans, who struggle to maintain a healthy diet and weight. 


For Gail Rupert, a nutritionist with The Weight Loss Clinic 
in Harrisburg, PA, motivation is a key ingredient in losing 
weight. 

In her approach to getting people motivated, Rupert says, 
“It’s important to empathize with the patient so they don’t feel 
like they are on their own with no support. I tell them I under- 
stand that the weight-loss process can be frustrating and emo- 
tional, and it may take a lot of persistence and consistency before seeing any 
reduction in weight. 

“Each client is different. Some patients have absolutely no knowledge of nutrition, 
while others have at least some familiarity,” she explained. “For those with little 
knowledge, I have to break down more difficult concepts to help them better under- 
stand the procedure. 

“For all patients, I make sure to give a reason why I’m suggesting a change. Most 
patients wouldn’t change eating or exercise habits if I didn’t explain the positive 
consequences of their actions. For example, exercising three times per week will 
improve their metabolism and increase their weight loss,” she added. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 30 Human Needs and Motivation: Eat, Drink, and Be Daring 321 


THE ROOTS OF OBESITY 


Given that both biological and social factors influence eating behavior, determining 
the causes of obesity has proved to be a challenging task. Researchers have followed 
several paths. 

Some psychologists suggest that oversensitivity to external eating cues based on 
social factors, coupled with insensitivity to internal hunger cues, produce obesity. 
Others argue that overweight people have higher weight set points than other people 
do. Because their set points are unusually high, their attempts to lose weight by eat- 
ing less may make them especially sensitive to external, food-related cues and there- 
fore more apt to overeat and perpetuate their obesity (Tremblay, 2004; West, 
Harvey-Berino, & Raczynski, 2004). 

But why may some people’s weight set points be higher than those of others? 
One biological explanation is that obese individuals have a higher level of the 
hormone leptin, which appears to be designed, from an evolutionary standpoint, to 
“protect” the body against weight loss. The body’s weight-regulation system thus 
appears to be designed more to protect against losing weight than to protect against 
gaining it. Therefore, it’s easier to gain weight than to lose it (Ahiima & Osei, 2004; 
Zhang et al., 2005; Levin, 2006). 

Another biologically based explanation for obesity relates to fat cells in the body. 
Starting at birth, the body stores fat either by increasing the number of fat cells or 
by increasing the size of existing fat cells. Furthermore, any loss of weight past infancy 
does not decrease the number of fat cells; it only affects their size. Consequently, 
people are stuck with the number of fat cells they inherit from an early age, and the 
rate of weight gain during the first 4 months of life is related to being overweight 
during later childhood (Stettler et al, 2005. 

According to the weight-set-point hypothesis, the presence of too many fat cells 
from earlier weight gain may result in the set point’s becoming “stuck” at a higher 
level than desirable. In such circumstances, losing weight becomes a difficult propo- 
sition because one is constantly at odds with one’s own internal set point when 
dieting (Freedman, 1995; Leibel, Rosenbaum & Hirsch, 1995). 





Although obesity is reaching epidemic proportions in the United States, its exact causes 
remain unclear. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


322 Chapter 10 Motivation and Emotion 


anorexia nervosa A severe eating 
disorder in which people may refuse to 
eat while denying that their behavior 
and appearance—which can become 
skeleton-like—are unusual. 


bulimia A disorder in which a person 
binges on large quantities of food, 
followed by efforts to purge the food 
through vomiting or other means. 





Despite looking skeleton-like to others, 
people with the weight disorder 
anorexia nervosa see themselves as 
overweight. 


Not everyone agrees with the set-point explanation for obesity. Pointing to the 
rapid rise in obesity over the last several decades in the United States, some research- 
ers suggest that the body does not try to maintain a fixed weight set point. Instead, 
they suggest, the body has a settling point, determined by a combination of our 
genetic heritage and the nature of the environment in which we live. If high-fat foods 
are prevalent in our environment and we are genetically predisposed to obesity, we 
settle into an equilibrium that maintains relatively high weight. In contrast, if our 
environment is nutritionally healthier, a genetic predisposition to obesity will not be 
triggered, and we will settle into an equilibrium in which our weight is lower 
(Comuzzie & Allison, 1998; Pi-Sunyer, 2003). 


Eating Disorders 


Eating disorders are among the 10 most frequent causes of disability in young 
women. One devastating weight-related disorder is anorexia nervosa. In this severe 
eating disorder, people may refuse to eat while denying that their behavior and 
appearance—which can become skeleton-like—are unusual. Some 10% of people 
with anorexia literally starve themselves to death (Striegel-Moore & Bulik, 2007). 

Anorexia nervosa mainly afflicts females between the ages of 12 and 40, although 
both men and women of any age may develop it. People with the disorder are often 
successful, attractive, and relatively affluent. The disorder often begins after serious 
dieting, which somehow gets out of control. Life begins to revolve around food: 
Although people with the disorder eat little, they may cook for others, go shopping 
for food frequently, or collect cookbooks (Polivy, Herman, & Boivin, 2005; Myers, 
2007; Jacobs et al., 2009). 

A related problem, bulimia, from which Lisa Arndt (described earlier) suffered, 
is a disorder in which people binge on large quantities of food. For instance, they 
may consume an entire gallon of ice cream and a whole pie in a single sitting. After 
such a binge, sufferers feel guilt and depression and often induce vomiting or take 
laxatives to rid themselves of the food—behavior known as purging. Constant 
binging-and-purging cycles and the use of drugs to induce vomiting or diarrhea 
can lead to heart failure. Often, though, the weight of a person with bulimia remains 
normal (Mora-Giral et al., 2004; Couturier & Lock, 2006). 

Eating disorders represent a growing problem: Estimates show that between 1% 
and 4% of high school-age and college-age women have either anorexia nervosa or 
bulimia. As many as 10% of women suffer from bulimia at some point in their lives. 
Furthermore, an increasing number of men are diagnosed with eating disorders; an 
estimated 10% to 13% of all cases occur in males (Kaminski et al., 2005; Swain, 2006; 
Park, 2007). 

What are the causes of anorexia nervosa and bulimia? Some researchers suspect 
a biological cause such as a chemical imbalance in the hypothalamus or pituitary 
gland, perhaps brought on by genetic factors. Furthermore, brain scans of people 
with eating disorders show that they process information about food differently from 
healthy individuals (see Figure 3; Polivy & Herman, 2002; Santel et al., 2006; Klump 
& Culbert, 2007). 

Others believe that the cause has roots in society’s valuation of slenderness and 
the parallel notion that obesity is undesirable. These researchers maintain that people 
with anorexia nervosa and bulimia become preoccupied with their weight and take 
to heart the cliché that one can never be too thin. This may explain why eating 
disorders increase as countries become more developed and Westernized and dieting 
becomes more popular. Finally, some psychologists suggest that the disorders result 
from overly demanding parents or other family problems (Grilo et al., 2003; Couturier 
& Lock, 2006; Kluck, 2008). 

The complete explanations for anorexia nervosa and bulimia remain elusive. 
These disorders most likely stem from both biological and social causes, and 
successful treatment probably encompasses several strategies, including therapy and 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 30 Human Needs and Motivation: Eat, Drink, and Be Daring 323 


Cognitive Processing in Anorexic Patients 





FIGURE 3 In this study, participants—either patients with anorexia or healthy individuals— 
viewed images of different foods when hungry or full to examine their cognitive processing. 
Comparison of fMRI scans of patients with anorexia (left column a) and healthy participants 
(right column b) showed significant differences in their reactions to the food stimuli. The 
figures show that people with anorexia generate little additional brain activity when hungry 
(green) as compared to when full (red), whereas those without anorexia show additional 
(green) activity when hungry as compared to when full (red). The differences suggest that 
patients with anorexia differ from nonanorexics in terms of their attention to food stimuli, 
which in turn may be related to restrictions in the eating behavior of the anorexics. 

(Source: Santel et al., 2006, Figure 4.) 





dietary changes (Wilson, Grilo, & Vitousek, 2007; O’Brien & LeBow, 2007; Cooper & 
Shafran, 2008). 

If you or a family member needs advice or help with an eating problem, contact 
the American Anorexia Bulimia Association at www.aabainc.org or call (212) 575-6200. 
You can get more information at www.nlm.nih.gov/medlineplus/eatingdisorders.html. 


Although 60% of the people in the United States say they want BECOMING AN 
to lose weight, it’s a losing battle for most of them. Most people INFORMED CONSUMER 
who diet eventually regain the weight they lost, so they try 
again and get caught in a seemingly endless cycle of weight loss of P S YC h O | O g y 
and gain (Newport & Carroll, 2002; Parker-Pope, 2003; Cachelin Dieting and Losing Weight Successfully 
& Regan, 2006). 
You should keep several things in mind when trying to lose 
weight (Gatchel & Oordt, 2003; Heshka et al., 2003): 


e There is no easy route to weight control. You will have to make permanent changes 
in your life to lose weight without gaining it back. The most obvious strategy— 
cutting down on the amount of food you eat—is just the first step toward a 
lifetime commitment to changing your eating habits. 

e Keep track of what you eat and what you weigh. Unless you keep careful records, you 
won't really know how much you are eating and whether any diet is working. 

e Eat “big” foods. Eat fiber and foods that are bulky and heavy but low in calories, 
such as grapes and soup. Such foods trick your body into thinking you've eaten 
more and thus decrease hunger. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


324 Chapter 10 Motivation and Emotion 


e Cut out television. One reason for the epidemic of obesity is the number of hours 
people in the United States spend viewing television. Not only does watching 
television preclude other activities that burn calories (even walking around the 
house is helpful), people often gorge on junk food while watching TV (Hu et al., 2003). 

e Exercise. Exercise at least 30 consecutive minutes three times each week. When 
you exercise, you use up fat stored in your body as fuel for muscles, which is 
measured in calories. As you use up this fat, you will probably lose weight. 
Almost any activity helps burn calories (see Figure 4). 

e Decrease the influence of external social stimuli on your eating behavior. Serve yourself 

smaller portions of food, and leave the table before you see what is being served 

for dessert. Don’t even buy snack foods such as nachos and potato chips; if 
they're not readily available in the kitchen cupboard, you're not apt to eat them. 

Wrap refrigerated foods in aluminum foil so that you cannot see the contents and 

be tempted every time you open the refrigerator. 

Avoid fad diets. No matter how popular they are at a particular time, extreme 

diets, including liquid diets, usually don’t work in the long run and can be 

dangerous to your health. 

Avoid taking any of the numerous diet pills advertised on television that promise quick 

and easy results. 

e Maintain good eating habits. When you have reached your desired weight, main- 

tain the new habits you learned while dieting to avoid gaining back the weight 

you have lost. 

Set reasonable goals. Know how much weight you want to lose before you start to 

diet. Don’t try to lose too much weight too quickly, or you may doom yourself to 

failure. Even small changes in behavior—such as walking 15 minutes a day or 

eating a few less bites at each meal—can prevent weight gain (Kirk et al., 2003). 





FIGURE 4 Ways to burn 150 calories: People can expend 150 calories by spending more time 
at a less vigorous activity or spending less time at a more vigorous activity. (Source: Surgeon 
General Report, 1996.) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 30 Human Needs and Motivation: Eat, Drink, and Be Daring 325 


The Need for Achievement: 
Striving for Success 


Although hunger may be one of the more potent primary drives in our day-to-day 
lives, powerful secondary drives that have no clear biological basis also motivate us. 
Among the more prominent of these is the need for achievement. 

The need for achievement is a stable, learned characteristic in which a person 
obtains satisfaction by striving for and attaining a level of excellence (McClelland et 
al., 1953). People with a high need for achievement seek out situations in which they 
can compete against some standard—such as grades, money, or winning a game— 
and prove themselves successful. But they are not indiscriminate when it comes to 
picking their challenges: They tend to avoid situations in which success will come 
too easily (which would be unchallenging) and situations in which success is unlikely. 
Instead, people high in achievement motivation generally choose tasks that are of 
intermediate difficulty (Speirs-Neumeister & Finch, 2006). 

In contrast, people with low achievement motivation tend to be motivated 
primarily by a desire to avoid failure. As a result, they seek out easy tasks so they 
are sure to avoid failure, or they seek out very difficult tasks for which failure has 
no negative implications because almost anyone would fail at them. People with a 
high fear of failure will stay away from tasks of intermediate difficulty because 
they may fail where others have been successful (Martin & Marsh, 2002; Puca, 2005; 
Morrone & Pintrich, 2006). 

A high need for achievement generally produces positive outcomes, at least in a 
success-oriented culture such as Western society. For instance, people motivated by a 
high need for achievement are more likely to attend college than their low-achievement 
counterparts; once they are in college, they tend to receive higher grades in classes that 
are related to their future careers. Furthermore, high achievement motivation indicates 
future economic and occupational success (McClelland, 1985; Thrash & Elliot, 2002). 





MEASURING ACHIEVEMENT MOTIVATION 


How can we measure a person’s need for achievement? The measuring instrument used 
most frequently is the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) (Spangler, 1992). In the TAT, an 
examiner shows a series of ambiguous pictures, such as the one in Figure 5. The exam- 
iner tells participants to write a story that describes what is happening, who the people 
are, what led to the situation, what the people are thinking or wanting, and what will 
happen next. Researchers then use a standard scoring system to determine the amount 
of achievement imagery in people’s stories. For example, someone who writes a story 
in which the main character strives to beat an opponent, studies in order to do well at 
some task, or works hard in order to get a promotion shows clear signs of an achieve- 
ment orientation. The inclusion of such achievement-related imagery in the participants’ 
stories is assumed to indicate an unusually high degree of concern with—and therefore 
a relatively strong need for—achievement (Tuerlinckx, DeBoeck, & Lens, 2002). 


The Need for Affiliation: 
Striving for Friendship 


Few of us choose to lead our lives as hermits. Why? 

One main reason is that most people have a need for affiliation, an interest in 
establishing and maintaining relationships with other people. Individuals with a 
high need for affiliation write TAT stories that emphasize the desire to maintain or 
reinstate friendships and show concern over being rejected by friends. 








need for achievement A stable, 
learned characteristic in which a 
person obtains satisfaction by striving 
for and attaining a level of excellence. 


y Alert 


A key feature of people with a 

high need for achievement 
is that they prefer tasks of 
moderate difficulty. 





ta 


FIGURE 5 This ambiguous picture is 
similar to those used in the Thematic 
Apperception Test to determine people’s 
underlying motivation (© 1943 by the 
President and Fellows of Harvard 
College; 1971 by Henry A. Murray). What 
do you see? Do you think your response 
is related to your motivation? 


need for affiliation An interest in 
establishing and maintaining relation- 
ships with other people. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


326 Chapter 10 Motivation and Emotion 


need for power A tendency to seek 
impact, control, or influence over 
others and to be seen as a powerful 
individual. 


People who have higher affiliation needs are particularly sensitive to relation- 
ships with others. They desire to be with their friends more of the time and alone 
less often, compared with people who are lower in the need for affiliation. However, 
gender is a greater determinant of how much time is actually spent with friends: 
Regardless of their affiliative orientation, female students spend significantly more 
time with their friends and less time alone than male students do (Cantwell & 
Andrews, 2002; Johnson, 2004; Semykina & Linz, 2007). 


The Need for Power: Striving 
for Impact on Others 


If your fantasies include becoming president of the United States or running Micro- 
soft, your dreams may reflect a high need for power. The need for power, a ten- 
dency to seek impact, control, or influence over others and to be seen as a 
powerful individual, is an additional type of motivation (Lee-Chai & Bargh, 2001; 
Winter, 2007; Zians, 2007). 

As you might expect, people with strong needs for power are more apt to belong 
to organizations and seek office than are those low in the need for power. They also 
tend to work in professions in which their power needs may be fulfilled, such as 
business management and—you may or may not be surprised—teaching (Jenkins, 
1994). In addition, they seek to display the trappings of power. Even in college, they 
are more likely to collect prestigious possessions, such as electronic equipment and 
sports cars. 

Some significant gender differences exist in the display of need for power. Men 
with high power needs tend to show unusually high levels of aggression, drink heav- 
ily, act in a sexually exploitative manner, and participate more frequently in competi- 
tive sports—behaviors that collectively represent somewhat extravagant, flamboyant 
behavior. In contrast, women display their power needs with more restraint; this is 
congruent with traditional societal constraints on women’s behavior. Women with high 
power needs are more apt than men to channel those needs in a socially responsible 
manner, such as by showing concern for others or displaying highly nurturing behav- 
ior (Maroda, 2004; Winter, 1988, 1995, 2007; Schubert & Koole, 2009). 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 30 Human Needs and Motivation: Eat, Drink, and Be Daring 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


2. 


RECAP 


What biological and social factors underlie hunger? 

e Eating behavior is subject to homeostasis, as most 
people’s weight stays within a relatively stable range. 
The hypothalamus in the brain is central to the 
regulation of food intake. (p. 319) 

e Social factors, such as mealtimes, cultural food prefer- 
ences, and other learned habits, also play a role in the 
regulation of eating by determining when, what, and 
how much one eats. An oversensitivity to social cues and 
an insensitivity to internal cues may also be related to 
obesity. In addition, obesity may be caused by an 
unusually high weight set point—the weight the body 
attempts to maintain—and genetic factors. (p. 319) 


How are needs relating to achievement, affiliation, and power 

motivation exhibited? 

e Need for achievement refers to the stable, learned 
characteristic in which a person strives to attain a level 
of excellence. Need for achievement is usually mea- 
sured through the Thematic Apperception Test (TAT), a 
series of pictures about which a person writes a story. 
(p. 325) 

e The need for affiliation is a concern with establishing 
and maintaining relationships with others, whereas the 
need for power is a tendency to seek to exert an impact 
on others. (p. 325) 


EVALUATE 


1. Match the following terms with their definitions: 
1. Hypothalamus a. Leads to refusal of food and 


2. Lateral hypothalamic starvation 
damage b. Responsible for monitoring 
3. Ventromedial food intake 
hypothalamic c. Causes extreme overeating 
damage 


327 





The is the specific 
level of weight the body strives to maintain. 

is the rate at which the body produces and 
expends energy. 


. Julio is the type of person who constantly strives for 


excellence. He feels intense satisfaction when he is able to 
master a new task. Julio most likely has a high need for 


. Debbie’s Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) story depicts 


a young girl who is rejected by one of her peers and seeks 
to regain her friendship. What major type of motivation is 
Debbie displaying in her story? 

a. Need for achievement 

b. Need for motivation 

c. Need for affiliation 

d. Need for power 


RETHINK 


1. 


2. 


In what ways do societal expectations, expressed by 
television shows and commercials, contribute to both 
obesity and excessive concern about weight loss? How 
could television contribute to better eating habits and 
attitudes toward weight? Should it be required to do so? 
From the perspective of a human resources specialist: How 
might you use characteristics such as need for achieve- 
ment, need for power, and need for affiliation to select 
workers for jobs? What additional criteria would you have 
to consider? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


“D 'G JUDUA “p ‘UISTTOqejour g ‘urod yas 4310M “J 12- “L-Z ‘G-T “TL 


KEY TERMS 


obesity p.317 
weight set point p. 319 


bulimia p. 322 
need for achievement p. 325 


need for affiliation p. 325 
need for power p. 326 


metabolism p. 319 
anorexia nervosa p. 322 


What are emotions, and how 
do we experience them? 


What are the functions of 
emotions? 


What are the explanations for 
emotions? 


How does nonverbal behavior 
relate to the expression of 
emotions? 


Feelings that generally have 
both physiological and cognitive 
elements and that influence behavior. 


328 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Karl Andrews held in his hands the envelope he had been waiting for. It could be the 
ticket to his future: an offer of admission to his first-choice college. But what was it 
going to say? He knew it could go either way. His grades were pretty good, and he had 
been involved in some extracurricular activities, but his SAT scores had not been terrific. 
He felt so nervous that his hands shook as he opened the thin envelope (not a good 
sign, he thought). Here it comes. “Dear Mr. Andrews,” it read. “The Trustees of the 
University are pleased to admit you. . . .” That was all he needed to see. With a whoop 
of excitement, Karl found himself jumping up and down gleefully. A rush of emotion 
overcame him as it sank in that he had, in fact, been accepted. He was on his way. 


At one time or another, all of us have experienced the strong feelings that accompany 
both very pleasant and very negative experiences. Perhaps we have felt the thrill of 
getting a sought-after job, the joy of being in love, the sorrow over someone’s death, 
or the anguish of inadvertently hurting someone. Moreover, we experience such 
reactions on a less intense level throughout our daily lives with such things as the 
pleasure of a friendship, the enjoyment of a movie, and the embarrassment of breaking 
a borrowed item. 

Despite the varied nature of these feelings, they all represent emotions. Although 
everyone has an idea of what an emotion is, formally defining the concept has proved 
to be an elusive task. Here, we'll use a general definition: Emotions are feelings that 
generally have both physiological and cognitive elements and that influence behavior. 

Think, for example, about how it feels to be happy. First, we obviously experience 
a feeling that we can differentiate from other emotions. It is likely that we also experi- 
ence some identifiable physical changes in our bodies: Perhaps the heart rate increases, 
or—as in the example of Karl Andrews—we find ourselves “jumping for joy.” Finally, 
the emotion probably encompasses cognitive elements: Our understanding and evalu- 
ation of the meaning of what is happening prompts our feelings of happiness. 

It is also possible, however, to experience an emotion without the presence of 
cognitive elements. For instance, we may react with fear to an unusual or novel 
situation (such as coming into contact with an erratic, unpredictable individual), or 
we may experience pleasure over sexual excitation without having cognitive aware- 
ness or understanding of just what makes the situation exciting. 

Some psychologists argue that entirely separate systems govern cognitive responses 
and emotional responses. A current controversy focuses on whether the emotional 
response predominates over the cognitive response or vice versa. Some theorists 
suggest that we first respond to a situation with an emotional reaction and later try to 
understand it. For example, we may enjoy a complex modern symphony without at 
first understanding it or knowing why we like it. In contrast, other theorists propose 
that people first develop cognitions about a situation and then react emotionally. This 
school of thought suggests that we must think about and understand a stimulus or 
situation, relating it to what we already know, before we can react on an emotional 
level (Murphy & Zajonc, 1993; Lazarus, 1995; Oatley, Keltner, & Jenkins, 2006). 

Because proponents of both sides of this debate can cite research to support their 
viewpoints, the question is far from resolved. Perhaps the sequence varies from situation 
to situation with emotions predominating in some instances and cognitive processes 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 31 Understanding Emotional Experiences 


occurring first in others. Both sides agree that we can experience emotions that involve 
little or no conscious thought. We may not know why we're afraid of mice because we 
understand objectively that they represent no danger, but we may still be frightened 
when we see them. Neuroimaging studies of the brain may help resolve this debate as 
well as others about the nature of emotions (Barrett & Wager, 2006; Niedenthal, 2007; 
Karaszewski, 2008). 


The Functions of Emotions 


Imagine what it would be like if we didn’t experience emotion. We would have no 
depths of despair, no depression, and no remorse, but at the same time we would 
also have no happiness, joy, or love. Obviously, life would be considerably less 
satisfying and even dull if we lacked the capacity to sense and express emotion. 

But do emotions serve any purpose beyond making life interesting? Indeed they 
do. Psychologists have identified several important functions that emotions play in our 
daily lives (Frederickson & Branigan, 2005; Frijda, 2005; Gross, 2006; Siemer, Mauss, & 
Gross, 2007). Among the most important of those functions are the following: 





e Preparing us for action. Emotions act as a link between events in our environ- 
ment and our responses. For example, if you saw an angry dog charging 
toward you, your emotional reaction (fear) would be associated with physi- 
ological arousal of the sympathetic division of the autonomic nervous system, 
the activation of the “fight-or-flight” response. 

e Shaping our future behavior. Emotions promote learning that will help us make 
appropriate responses in the future. For instance, your emotional response to 
unpleasant events teaches you to avoid similar circumstances in the future. 

e Helping us interact more effectively with others. We often communicate the 
emotions we experience through our verbal and nonverbal behaviors, making 
our emotions obvious to observers. These behaviors can act as a signal to 
observers, allowing them to understand better what we are experiencing and 
to help them predict our future behavior. 


Determining the Range of 
Emotions: Labeling Our Feelings 


If we were to list the words in the English language that have been used to describe 
emotions, we would end up with at least 500 examples (Averill, 1975). The list would 
range from such obvious emotions as happiness and fear to less common ones, such 
as adventurousness and pensiveness. 

One challenge for psychologists has been to sort through this list to identify 
the most important, fundamental emotions. Theorists have hotly contested the 
issue of cataloging emotions and have come up with different lists, depending 
on how they define the concept of emotion. In fact, some reject the question 
entirely, saying that no set of emotions should be singled out as most basic and 
that emotions are best understood by breaking them down into their component 
parts. Other researchers argue for looking at emotions in terms of a hierarchy, 
dividing them into positive and negative categories and then organizing them 
into increasingly narrower subcategories (see Figure 1; Manstead, Frijda, & 
Fischer, 2003; Dillard & Shen, 2007). 

Still, most researchers suggest that a list of basic emotions would include, at a 
minimum, happiness, anger, fear, sadness, and disgust. Other lists are broader, 
including emotions such as surprise, contempt, guilt, and joy (Ekman, 1994a; Shweder, 
1994; Tracy & Robins, 2004). 





329 


330 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 10 Motivation and Emotion 


Emotions 


pe 


Positive Negative 


a a a a 


Love 


Fondness 


Infatuation 


Joy Anger Sadness Fear 
Bliss Pride Annoyance | Contempt Agony JA Horror 
Contentment Hostility Jealousy Grief Loneliness Worry 


FIGURE 1 One approach to organizing emotions is to use a hierarchy, which divides emotions 
into increasingly narrow subcategories. (Source: Adapted from Fischer, Shaver, & Carnochan, 1990.) 


One difficulty in defining a basic set of emotions is that substantial differences 
exist in descriptions of emotions among various cultures. For instance, Germans 
report experiencing schadenfreude, a feeling of pleasure over another person’s difficul- 
ties, and the Japanese experience hagaii, a mood of vulnerable heartache colored by 
frustration. In Tahiti, people experience musu, a feeling of reluctance to yield to 
unreasonable demands made by one’s parents. 

Finding schadenfreude, hagaii, or musu in a particular culture doesn’t mean that 
the members of other cultures are incapable of experiencing such emotions, of 
course. It suggests, though, that fitting a particular emotion into a linguistic cate- 
gory to describe that emotion may make it easier to discuss, contemplate, and 
perhaps experience (Russell & Sato, 1995; Li, Wang, & Fischer, 2004; Kuppens et 
al., 2006). 


The Roots of Emotions 


I’ve never been so angry before; I feel my heart pounding, and I’m trembling all over. . . . 





I don’t know how I'll get through the performance. I feel like my stomach is filled with 
butterflies. . . . That was quite a mistake I made! My face must be incredibly red. . . . When 
I heard the footsteps in the night, I was so frightened that I couldn’t catch my breath. 


If you examine our language, you will find that there are literally dozens of ways 
to describe how we feel when we experience an emotion and that the language we 
use to describe emotions is, for the most part, based on the physical symptoms that 
are associated with a particular emotional experience (Kobayashi, Schallert, & Ogren, 
2003; Manstead & Wagner, 2004; Spackman, Fujiki, & Brinton, 2006). 

Consider, for instance, the experience of fear. Pretend that it is late on New Year’s 
Eve. You are walking down a dark road, and you hear a stranger approaching behind 
you. It is clear that he is not trying to hurry by but is coming directly toward you. 
You think about what you will do if the stranger attempts to rob you or, worse, hurt 
you in some way. 

While these thoughts are running through your head, something dramatic will 
be happening to your body. The most likely reactions, which are associated with 
activation of the autonomic nervous system, include an increase in your rate of 
breathing, an acceleration of your heart rate, a widening of your pupils (to increase 
visual sensitivity), and a dryness in your mouth as the functioning of your salivary 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 31 Understanding Emotional Experiences 331 


glands and in fact of your entire digestive system ceases. At the same time, though, 
your sweat glands probably will increase their activity because increased sweating 
will help you rid yourself of the excess heat developed by any emergency activity 
in which you engage. 

Of course, all these physiological changes are likely to occur without your aware- 
ness. At the same time, though, the emotional experience accompanying them will 
be obvious to you: You most surely would report being fearful. 

Although it is easy to describe the general physical reactions that accompany 
emotions, defining the specific role that those physiological responses play in the 
experience of emotions has proved to be a major puzzle for psychologists. As we 
shall see, some theorists suggest that specific bodily reactions cause us to experience 
a particular emotion—we experience fear, for instance, because the heart is pounding 
and we are breathing deeply. In contrast, other theorists suggest that the physiolog- 
ical reaction results from the experience of an emotion. In this view, we experience 
fear, and as a result the heart pounds and our breathing deepens. 


THE JAMES-LANGE THEORY: DO GUT 
REACTIONS EQUAL EMOTIONS? 


To William James and Carl Lange, who were among the first researchers to explore 
the nature of emotions, emotional experience is, very simply, a reaction to instinctive 
bodily events that occur as a response to some situation or event in the environment. 
This view is summarized in James’s statement, “We feel sorry because we cry, angry 
because we strike, afraid because we tremble” (James, 1890). 

James and Lange took the view that the instinctive response of crying at a loss 
leads us to feel sorrow, that striking out at someone who frustrates us results in our 
feeling anger, that trembling at a menacing threat causes us to feel fear. They sug- 
gested that for every major emotion there is an accompanying physiological or “gut” 
reaction of internal organs—called a visceral experience. It is this specific pattern of 
visceral response that leads us to label the emotional experience. 

In sum, James and Lange proposed that we experience emotions as a result of 
physiological changes that produce specific sensations. The brain interprets these 
sensations as specific kinds of emotional experiences (see the first part of Figure 2). 
This view has come to be called the James-Lange theory of emotion (Laird & Bresler, 
1990; Cobos et al., 2002). 

The James-Lange theory has some serious drawbacks, however. For the theory 
to be valid, visceral changes would have to occur relatively quickly because we 
experience some emotions—such as fear upon hearing a stranger rapidly approaching 
on a dark night—almost instantaneously. Yet emotional experiences frequently occur 
even before there is time for certain physiological changes to be set into motion. 
Because of the slowness with which some visceral changes take place, it is hard to 
see how they could be the source of immediate emotional experience. 

The James-Lange theory poses another difficulty: Physiological arousal does not 
invariably produce emotional experience. For example, a person who is jogging has 
an increased heartbeat and respiration rate as well as many of the other physiological 
changes associated with certain emotions. Yet joggers typically do not think of such 
changes in terms of emotion. There cannot be a one-to-one correspondence, then, 
between visceral changes and emotional experience. Visceral changes by themselves 
may not be sufficient to produce emotion. 

Finally, our internal organs produce a relatively limited range of sensations. 
Although some types of physiological changes are associated with specific emo- 
tional experiences, it is difficult to imagine how each of the myriad emotions that 
people are capable of experiencing could be the result of a unique visceral change. 
Many emotions actually are associated with relatively similar sorts of visceral 
changes, a fact that contradicts the James-Lange theory (Davidson et al., 1994; 
Cameron, 2002). 


James-Lange theory of emotion 

The belief that emotional experience is 
a reaction to bodily events occurring 
as a result of an external situation (“I 
feel sad because I am crying”). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


332 Chapter 10 Motivation and Emotion 





Perceiving an emotion-inducing event or situation 
(such as a stranger following you) 


Cannon-Bard theory of emotion 

The belief that both physiological 
arousal and emotional experience are 
produced simultaneously by the same 
nerve stimulus. 






y Alert 


Use Figure 2 to distinguish 
the three classic theories of 
emotion (James-Lange, 
Cannon-Bard, and 
Schachter-Singer). 


James-Lange 
theory = 
Cannon-Bard 
theory = 


Schachter-Singer 
theory 


FIGURE 2 A comparison of three models of emotion. 







THE CANNON-BARD THEORY: PHYSIOLOGICAL 
REACTIONS AS THE RESULT OF EMOTIONS 


In response to the difficulties inherent in the James-Lange theory, Walter Cannon and 
later Philip Bard suggested an alternative view. In what has come to be known as the 
Cannon-Bard theory of emotion, they proposed the model illustrated in the second part 
of Figure 2 (Cannon, 1929). This theory rejects the view that physiological arousal alone 
leads to the perception of emotion. Instead, the theory assumes that both physiological 
arousal and the emotional experience are produced simultaneously by the same nerve 
stimulus, which Cannon and Bard suggested emanates from the thalamus in the brain. 

The theory states that after we perceive an emotion-producing stimulus, the 
thalamus is the initial site of the emotional response. Next, the thalamus sends a signal 
to the autonomic nervous system, thereby producing a visceral response. At the same 
time, the thalamus also communicates a message to the cerebral cortex regarding the 
nature of the emotion being experienced. Hence, it is not necessary for different 
emotions to have unique physiological patterns associated with them—as long as the 
message sent to the cerebral cortex differs according to the specific emotion. 

The Cannon-Bard theory seems to have been accurate in rejecting the view that 
physiological arousal alone accounts for emotions. However, more recent research 
has led to some important modifications of the theory. For one thing, we now under- 
stand that the hypothalamus and the limbic system, not the thalamus, play a major 
role in emotional experience. In addition, the simultaneous occurrence of the physi- 
ological and emotional responses, which is a fundamental assumption of the Cannon- 
Bard theory, has yet to be demonstrated conclusively. This ambiguity has allowed 
room for yet another theory of emotions: the Schachter-Singer theory. 


THE SCHACHTER-SINGER THEORY: EMOTIONS AS LABELS 


Suppose that as you are being followed down that dark street on New Year’s Eve, 
you notice a man being followed by another shady figure on the other side of the 
street. Now assume that instead of reacting with fear, the man begins to laugh and 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 31 Understanding Emotional Experiences 333 


act gleeful. Would the reactions of this other individual be sufficient to lay your fears 
to rest? Might you, in fact, decide there is nothing to fear and get into the spirit of 
the evening by beginning to feel happiness and glee yourself? 

According to an explanation that focuses on the role of cognition, the Schachter- 
Singer theory of emotion, this might very well happen. This approach to explain- 
ing emotions emphasizes that we identify the emotion we are experiencing by 
observing our environment and comparing ourselves with others (Schachter & 
Singer, 1962). 

Schachter and Singer’s classic experiment found evidence for this hypothesis. 
In the study, participants were told that they would receive an injection of a vita- 
min. In reality, they were given epinephrine, a drug that causes responses that 
typically occur during strong emotional reactions, such as an increase in physio- 
logical arousal, including higher heart and respiration rates and a reddening of the 
face. The members of both groups were then placed individually in a situation 
where a confederate of the experimenter acted in one of two ways. In one condition 
he acted angry and hostile; in the other condition he behaved as if he were exu- 
berantly happy. 

The purpose of the experiment was to determine how the participants would 
react emotionally to the confederate’s behavior. When they were asked to describe 
their own emotional state at the end of the experiment, the participants exposed to 
the angry confederate reported that they felt angry, while those exposed to the happy 
confederate reported feeling happy. In sum, the results suggest that participants 
turned to the environment and the behavior of others for an explanation of the 
physiological arousal they were experiencing. 

The results of the Schachter-Singer experiment, then, supported a cognitive view 
of emotions in which emotions are determined jointly by a relatively nonspecific kind 
of physiological arousal and the labeling of that arousal on the basis of cues from the 
environment (refer to the third part of Figure 2). Although later research has found 
that arousal is more specific than Schachter and Singer believed, they were right in 
assuming that when the source of physiological arousal is unclear, we may look to 
our surroundings to determine what we are experiencing. 


CONTEMPORARY PERSPECTIVES ON 
THE NEUROSCIENCE OF EMOTIONS 


When Schachter and Singer carried out their groundbreaking experiment in the early 
1960s, the ways in which they could evaluate the physiology that accompanies emo- 
tion were relatively limited. However, advances in the measurement of the nervous 
system and other parts of the body have allowed researchers to examine more closely 
the biological responses involved in emotion. As a result, contemporary research on 
emotion points to a revision of earlier views that physiological responses associated 
with emotions are undifferentiated. Instead, evidence is growing that specific patterns 
of biological arousal are associated with individual emotions (Franks & Smith, 1999; 
Vaitl, Schienle, & Stark, 2005; Woodson, 2006). 

For instance, researchers have found that specific emotions produce activation 
of very different portions of the brain. In one study, participants undergoing positron 
emission tomography (PET) brain scans were asked to recall events, such as deaths 
and funerals, that made them feel sad or events that made them feel happy, such as 
weddings and births. They also looked at photos of faces that appeared to be happy 
or sad. The results of the PET scans were clear: Happiness was related to a decrease 
in activity in certain areas of the cerebral cortex, whereas sadness was associated 
with increases in activity in particular portions of the cortex (George et al., 1995; 
Hamann et al., 2002; Prohovnik et al., 2004). 

In addition, the amygdala, in the brain’s temporal lobe, is important in the expe- 
rience of emotions for it provides a link between the perception of an emotion- 
producing stimulus and the recall of that stimulus later. For example, if we’ve once 


Schachter-Singer theory of emotion 
The belief that emotions are deter- 
mined jointly by a nonspecific kind of 
physiological arousal and its interpre- 
tation, based on environmental cues. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


334 Chapter 10 Motivation and Emotion 


FIGURE 3 Connections from the 
amygdala, seen here in red, allow it 

to mediate many of the autonomic 
expressions of emotional states through 
the hippocampus (purple) and visual 
cortex (orange). (Source: From Dolan, R. J. 
(2002). Emotion, cognition, and behavior. 
Science, 298, no. 5596, 1191-1194 (Figure 1, 
p. 1192). Drawing of brain reprinted with 
permission from AAAS.) 


y Alert 


It is important to understand 
the basic neuroscience of 
emotional experience. 


Visual cortex _ Amygdala 





Hippocampus 





been attacked by a vicious pit bull, the amygdala processes that information and 
leads us to react with fear when we see a pit bull later—an example of a classically 
conditioned fear response (Miller et al., 2005; Berntson et al., 2007; Kensinger, 2007; 
LaBar, 2007). 

Because neural pathways connect the amygdala, the visual cortex, and the 
hippocampus (which plays an important role in the consolidation of memories), 
some scientists speculate that emotion-related stimuli can be processed and 
responded to almost instantaneously (see Figure 3). This immediate response 
occurs so rapidly that higher-order, more rational thinking, which takes more time, 
seems not to be involved initially. In a slower but more thoughtful response to 
emotion-evoking stimuli, emotion-related sensory information is first evaluated 
and then sent on to the amygdala. It appears that the quicker system offers an 
immediate response to emotion-evoking stimuli, whereas the slower system helps 
confirm a threat and prepare a more thoughtful response (Dolan, 2002). 


MAKING SENSE OF THE MULTIPLE 
PERSPECTIVES ON EMOTION 


As new approaches to emotion continue to develop, it is reasonable to ask why 
so many theories of emotion exist and, perhaps more important, which one pro- 
vides the most complete explanation. Actually, we have only scratched the surface. 
There are almost as many explanatory theories of emotion as there are individual 
emotions (e.g., Manstead, Frijda, & Fischer, 2003; Frijda, 2005; Prinz, 2007; Herz- 
berg, 2009). 

Why are theories of emotion so plentiful? For one thing, emotions are not a 
simple phenomenon but are intertwined closely with motivation, cognition, neuro- 
science, and a host of related branches of psychology. For example, evidence from 
brain imaging studies shows that even when people come to supposedly rational, 
nonemotional decisions—such as making moral, philosophical judgments—emotions 
come into play (Greene et al., 2001). 

In short, emotions are such complex phenomena, encompassing both biological 
and cognitive aspects, that no single theory has been able to explain fully all the 
facets of emotional experience. Furthermore, contradictory evidence of one sort or 
another challenges each approach, and therefore no theory has proved invariably 
accurate in its predictions. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 31 Understanding Emotional Experiences 335 


This abundance of perspectives on emotion is not a cause for despair—or unhap- 
piness, fear, or any other negative emotion. It simply reflects the fact that psychology 
is an evolving, developing science. As we gather more evidence, the specific answers 
to questions about the nature of emotions will become clearer. 


a 


Consider, for a moment, the six photos displayed in Figure 4. Ex p | O ri nN g Ae’ Ss; 
Can you identify the emotions being expressed by the person in DIVERSITY Ad “a a 
Do People in All Cultures Express y GPa 


each of the photos? 
Emotion Similarly? 3 





If you are a good judge of facial expressions, you will f 
conclude that these expressions display six of the basic 
emotions: happiness, anger, sadness, surprise, disgust, and 
fear. Hundreds of studies of nonverbal behavior show that 
these emotions are consistently distinct and identifiable even by untrained observers 
(Ekman & O'Sullivan, 1991). 

Interestingly, these six emotions are not unique to members of Western cultures; 
rather, they constitute the basic emotions expressed universally by members of the 
human race, regardless of where individuals have been raised and what learning 
experiences they have had. Psychologist Paul Ekman convincingly demonstrated this 
point when he studied members of an isolated New Guinea jungle tribe who had had 
almost no contact with Westerners (Ekman, 1972). The people of the tribe did not 
speak or understand English, had never seen a movie, and had very limited experience 


FIGURE 4 These photos demonstrate six 
of the primary emotions: happiness, 
anger, sadness, surprise, disgust, and fear. 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


336 Chapter 10 Motivation and Emotion 


© Gahan Wilson/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


“And just exactly what is that expression intended to convey?” 


facial-affect program Activation of a 
set of nerve impulses that make the 
face display the appropriate 
expression. 


facial-feedback hypothesis 

The hypothesis that facial expressions 
not only reflect emotional experience 
but also help determine how people 
experience and label emotions. 





with Caucasians before Ekman’s arrival. Yet their nonverbal 
responses to emotion-evoking stories, as well as their ability 
to identify basic emotions, were quite similar to those of 
Westerners. 

Being so isolated, the New Guineans could not have 
learned from Westerners to recognize or produce similar 
facial expressions. Instead, their similar abilities and manner 
of responding emotionally appear to have been present 
innately. Although one could argue that similar experiences 
in both cultures led the members of each one to learn similar 
types of nonverbal behavior, this appears unlikely because 
the two cultures are so very different. The expression of basic 
emotions, thus, seems to be universal (Ekman, 1994b; Izard, 
1994; Matsumoto, 2002). 

Why do people across cultures express emotions similarly? 
A hypothesis known as the facial-affect program gives one 
explanation. The facial-affect program—which is assumed to 
be universally present at birth—is analogous to a computer 
program that is turned on when a particular emotion is 
experienced. When set in motion, the “program” activates a 
set of nerve impulses that make the face display an appropriate 
expression. Each primary emotion produces a unique set of 
muscular movements, forming the kinds of expressions shown in Figure 4. For example, 
the emotion of happiness is universally displayed by movement of the zygomatic major, 
a muscle that raises the corners of the mouth and form what we would call a smile 
(Ekman, 2003; Kohler et al., 2004; Kim, Kim, & Kim, 2007; Kendler et al., 2008). 

The importance of facial expressions is illustrated by an intriguing notion known as 
the facial-feedback hypothesis. According to this hypothesis, facial expressions not 
only reflect emotional experience, but they also help determine how people experience 
and label emotions. Basically put, “wearing” an emotional expression provides 
muscular feedback to the brain that helps produce an emotion congruent with that 
expression (Izard, 1990; Davis, Senghas, & Ochsner, 2009). 

For instance, the muscles activated when we smile may send a message to the brain 
indicating the experience of happiness—even if there is nothing in the environment 
that would produce that particular emotion. Some theoreticians have gone further by 
suggesting that facial expressions are necessary for an emotion to be experienced 
(Rinn, 1984, 1991). In this view, if no facial expression is present, the emotion cannot 
be felt. 

Support for this facial-feedback hypothesis comes from a classic experiment 
carried out by psychologist Paul Ekman and colleagues (Ekman, Levenson, & 
Friesen, 1983). In the study, professional actors were asked to follow very explicit 
instructions regarding the movements of muscles in their faces. You might try this 
example yourself: 


e Raise your brows and pull them together. 
e Raise your upper eyelids. 
e Now stretch your lips horizontally back toward your ears. 


After carrying out these directions—which, as you may have guessed, are meant to 
produce an expression of fear—the actors’ heart rates rose and their body temperatures de- 
clined, physiological reactions that characterize fear. Overall, facial expressions representing 
the primary emotions produced physiological effects similar to those accompanying the 
genuine emotions in other circumstances (Keillor et al., 2002; Soussignan, 2002). (Also see 
Applying Psychology in the 21st Century.) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Applying Psychology in the 21st Century 


Smiling Athletes: Do 
Their Facial Expressions 
Come Naturally? 


There’s no more stirring sight than medal- 
winning Olympic athletes standing on the 
podium as their national anthem is played. 
Have you ever wondered why—no matter 
what the winners’ native countries—their 
facial expressions are so readily under- 
standable? 

The question of whether the similarity 
of basic facial expressions are due to in- 
nate, inborn factors or if people learn them 
in similar ways across cultures has fasci- 
nated psychologists for decades. A new 
study sheds light on this issue, adding to 
the evidence that basic facial expressions 
are inborn rather than learned. 

The researchers used a clever tactic to 
approach this question: They examined the 
facial expressions of people blind from 
birth who, of course, could not have learned 
these behaviors by observing them in oth- 
ers. Judo athletes who won medals in the 
Olympic Games were photographed when 
they completed their matches, during the 
awards ceremony, and when they were on 
the medalists’ podium. Their facial expres- 
sions were compared with blind judo 
athletes—some of them blind from birth— 
who won medals in the Paralympic Games, 
a similar competition for athletes with dis- 
abilities. The blind medalists were photo- 
graphed at corresponding times to the 
sighted ones. Comparisons of the photo- 
graphs revealed that the blind athletes 
expressed anger, contempt, disgust, sadness, 
surprise, and smiling behavior in the same 
ways as the sighted athletes did (Ekman, 
2003; Matsumoto & Willingham, 2009). 

Most of the blind athletes smiled when 
they finished their matches and were 
standing on the podium, and nearly all 
produced genuine smiles when they were 
receiving their medals. The sighted athletes 
showed comparable frequency of smiling 
across these different contexts. More inter- 
estingly, of the blind athletes who won 
silver medals (who had lost the match and 
the gold), none smiled following the 
match, but most smiled politely while on 
the podium and while receiving their 
medals. This is also what the sighted silver 





Are basic facial expressions inborn or are they learned? 


medalists did, showing that all the athletes 
knew to put on a pleased expression at the 
appropriate time. 

The athletes who were blind from birth 
showed no differences from the athletes who 
became blind later in life. These findings col- 


ETHINK 


lectively show that blind and sighted people 
express emotion on their faces in the same 
ways across different contexts, suggesting 
that observation of others is not a precondi- 
tion for learning facial expression of emotion 
(Matsumoto & Willingham, 2009). 








e How could blind athletes have learned when they would be expected to “smile 
politely,” even if they did not feel happiness? 

e Although it seems like basic emotions are expressed similarly across cultures, can 
you think of examples of nonverbal behaviors that differ across cultures? 








337 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


338 Chapter 10 Motivation and Emotion 


RECAP 


What are emotions, and how do we experience them? 

e Emotions are broadly defined as feelings that may affect 
behavior and generally have both a physiological com- 
ponent and a cognitive component. Debate continues 
over whether separate systems govern cognitive and 
emotional responses and whether one has primacy over 
the other. (p. 328) 


What are the functions of emotions? 
e Emotions prepare us for action, shape future behavior 
through learning, and help us interact more effectively 
with others. (p. 329) 


What are the explanations for emotions? 
e Several theories explain emotions. The James-Lange 


theory suggests that emotional experience is a reaction to 
bodily, or visceral, changes that occur as a response to an 
environmental event and are interpreted as an emotional 


response. (p. 331) 

e In contrast, the Cannon-Bard theory contends that both 
physiological arousal and an emotional experience are 
produced simultaneously by the same nerve stimulus 
and that the visceral experience does not necessarily 
differ among differing emotions. (p. 332) 

e The Schachter-Singer theory suggests that emotions are 


determined jointly by a relatively nonspecific physiologi- 


cal arousal and the subsequent labeling of that arousal, 
using cues from the environment to determine how 
others are behaving in the same situation. (p. 333) 

e The most recent approaches to emotions focus on their 
biological origins. For instance, it now seems that 


specific patterns of biological arousal are associated with 


individual emotions. Furthermore, new scanning 
techniques have identified the specific parts of the brain 
that are activated during the experience of particular 
emotions. (p. 334) 


How does nonverbal behavior relate to the expression of 


emotions? 


e Aperson’s facial expressions can reveal emotions. In fact, 


members of different cultures understand others’ emo- 
tional expressions in similar ways. One explanation for 
this similarity is that an innate facial-affect program 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


activates a set of muscle movements representing the 
emotion being experienced. (p. 336) 

e The facial-feedback hypothesis suggests that facial 
expressions not only reflect, but also produce, emotional 
experiences. (p. 336) 


EVALUATE 


1. 


. The 


. According to the 


Emotions are always accompanied by a cognitive re- 
sponse. True or false? 

theory of emotion states that 
emotions are a response to instinctive bodily events. 
theory of 
emotion, both an emotional response and physiological 
arousal are produced simultaneously by the same nerve 
stimulus. 


. Your friend—a psychology major—tells you, “I was ata 


party last night. During the course of the evening, my 
general level of arousal increased. Since I was at a party 
where people were enjoying themselves, I assume I must 
have felt happy.” What theory of emotion does your 
friend subscribe to? 


. What are the six primary emotions that can be identified 


from facial expressions? 


RETHINK 


i, 


If researchers learned how to control emotional responses 
so that targeted emotions could be caused or prevented, 
what ethical concerns might arise? Under what circum- 
stances, if any, should such techniques be used? 


. From the perspective of an advertising executive: How might 


you use Schachter and Singer’s findings on the labeling of 
arousal to create interest in a product? Can you think of 
other examples whereby people’s arousal could be 
manipulated, which would lead to different emotional 
responses? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


Ieaj pue 4ysn8stp “Iasue ‘ssourddey 
‘ssoupes ‘astidins g ‘1aSurg-layypoeups ‘p ‘preg-uouues *¢ ‘asuey] 
-səwef ‘z ‘asuodsar aAytusoo e moyym M20 Áew sUOTJOUA /aSTLF 'T 


KEY TERMS 


emotions p. 328 
James-Lange theory of 
emotion p. 331 


facial-feedback 
hypothesis p. 336 


Cannon-Bard theory of 
emotion p. 332 


Schachter-Singer theory of 
emotion p. 333 
facial-affect program p. 336 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Psychology on the Web 


1. Find two different websites that deal with nonverbal behavior. One site should 
present a fairly “academic” discussion of the topic, and the other should be more 
informal. (Hint: The terms nonverbal behavior and nonverbal communication may lead 
you to more formal discussions of the topic, whereas body language may lead you to 
less formal discussions.) Compare and contrast your findings from the two sites. 

2. Find one or more websites that offer information on eating disorders. 


E © | | OQ U Q Motivation and emotions are two interrelated aspects of 


psychology. In these modules, we first considered the 
topic of motivation, which has spawned a great deal of theory and research examining 
primary and secondary drives. We then turned to a discussion of emotions, beginning 
with their functions and proceeding to a review of three major theories that seek to 
explain what emotions are and how they, and their associated physiological symptoms, 
emerge in the individual. Finally, we looked at cultural differences in the expression and 
display of emotions and discussed the facial-affect program, which seems to be innate 
and to regulate the nonverbal expression of the basic emotions. 

Return to the opening scenario of this group of modules, which describes actress 
Kirstie Alley’s problems with maintaining an appropriate weight. Using your knowledge 
of motivation and emotion, consider the following questions: 





1. How could Kirstie Alley’s eating problem be explained by each of the different 
approaches to motivation? 

2. What seem to be some of the social factors that encourage Alley to overeat? 

3. How might biological factors in the regulation of hunger explain the difficulty that 
Alley is having with maintaining her weight loss? 

4. What does Alley’s story seem to say about her need for achievement? 


339 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Key Concepts for Chapter 11 


MODULE 32 








What are the major differences between male Gender and Sex 


Gender Roles: Society's Expectations 


and female gender roles? for Women and Men 


Sexism on the Job 


Gender Differences: More 
Similar than Dissimilar 


Sources of Gender Differences: 

Where Biology and Society Meet 
Neuroscience in Your Life: When 

Brain and Socialization Meet 

Applying Psychology in the 21st Century: 
Trucks and Dolls: Does Fetal Exposure to 
Testosterone Affect How Children Play? 





MODULE 33 







Why, and under what circumstances, do we Understanding Human Sexual 


Response: The Facts of Life 


? 
become sexually aroused: The Basic Biology of Sexual Behavior 


Psychological Aspects of Sexual 
Excitement: What Turns People On? 
The Phases of Sexual Response: 
The Ups and Downs of Sex 





Exploring Diversity: Female 
Circumcision: A Celebration 
of Culture—or Genital Mutilation? 


MODULE 34 


What is “normal” sexual behavior? @ How The Diversity of Sexual Behavior 


do most people behave sexually? Approaches to Sexual Normality 
` Surveying Sexual Behavior: What's 


@ How prevalent are rape and other forms of Happening Behind Closed Doors? 


; ; 7 Heterosexuality 
nonconsenting sex, and what are their causes? Famo andl Benity 
@ What are the major sexually transmitted Transsexualism 


Sexual Difficulties 


! ae , ; 
infections? @ What sexual difficulties do Betanien informed tahisumet 


of Psychology: Lowering 


? 
people most frequently encounter? enue of owe rene 


341 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Prologue Trapped in a Male’s Body 





~ The images are iconic TV moments: precious little 
\ Chastity Bono, with her blonde hair and chubby 
\ cheeks, gracing her parents’ 1970s variety show 
|—The Sonny and Cher Comedy Hour with a family 
song or the sweet sign-off, “Goodnight every- 
body and God bless!” While girls at home may 
have been envious of Cher’s glittery outfits, 
Chastity, who always wore costumes to complement 
either that of her Mom's and Dad's, later'admitted, “I especially 
liked it when they put me in clothes that matched my father’s.” 










Chaz’s story illustrates the emotion and confusion that often 
characterize one of the most universal behaviors: sexuality. 
Exemplifying major personal as well as societal concerns, sex 
and the interrelated topic of gender are also key topics for 
psychologists in a variety of specialties. For instance, psychologists 
interested in motivation view sexuality in terms of sexual needs, 
drives, and gratification. Neuroscientists consider sexuality from 
the perspective of the relationship of the brain and nervous 
system to the functioning of the sexual organs. Social psycholo- 
gists and psychologists who specialize in the study of women 
focus on society’s rules of sexual conduct and the role sexual 
behavior plays in interpersonal behavior. 


342 


In fact, when Chastity came out as a lesbian to The Advocate in 
1995, she said, “As a kid | thought | was a little 
boy.” (Bartolomeo et al., 2009, p. 54) 
Chastity Bono never actually stopped 
thinking of herself as male. In spring 2009, 
at the age of 40, Chastity announced to 
the world that she was transitioning from 
female to male and would henceforth be 
known as Chaz. 








In this set of modules, we consider human sexuality from 
several of these vantage points. We begin by examining gender 
and discussing differences in societal expectations about how 
men and women should behave and the impact of those 
expectations on behavior and attitudes. Next we turn to sexual 
behavior. We describe the biological aspects of sexual excitement 
and arousal and then examine the variety of sexual activities in 
which people engage. We conclude with a discussion of non- 
consenting sex, sexually transmitted infections, and the psycho- 
logical aspects of sexual difficulties. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 32 


“Its a girl!” “It’s a boy!” 

One or some variant of these exclamations is typically the first sentence uttered 
upon the birth of a child. However, the consequences of whether we are born with 
male or female sex organs extend well beyond the moment of birth. Throughout our 
lives, the ways that others think of us, and even the ways we view ourselves are based 
to a large extent on whether society labels us as a woman or a man—our gender. 

Gender is the perception of being male or female. Although there is a good deal 
of overlap between the concepts of sex and gender, they are not the same: Sex typi- 
cally refers to sexual anatomy and sexual behavior, whereas gender refers to the sense 
of maleness or femaleness related to our membership in a given society. 


Gender Roles: Society's 


Expectations for Women 
and Men = —Ž o >€ y 


Our conclusions about what is or is not “appropriate” behavior for others and our- 
selves are based on gender roles. Gender roles are the set of expectations, defined by 
a particular society, that indicate what is appropriate behavior for men and women. 
If men’s and women’s gender roles were equivalent, they would have only a minor 
impact on our lives. However, expectations about men and women differ significantly, 
and these beliefs may result in favoritism toward members of one of the sexes. Gender 
roles also may produce stereotyping, judgments about individual members of a group on 
the basis of their membership in that group. Stereotypes about 
gender roles are reflected in sexism, negative attitudes and 
behavior toward a person that are based on that person’s gender. 
People in Western societies like ours generally hold well- 
defined stereotypes about men and women, which prevail 
regardless of age, economic status, and social and educational 
background. Men are more apt to be viewed as having traits 
involving competence, such as independence, objectivity, and 
competitiveness. In contrast, women tend to be seen as hav- 
ing traits involving warmth and expressiveness, such as gen- 
tleness and awareness of others’ feelings. Because Western 
society traditionally values competence more than warmth 
and expressiveness, the perceived differences between men 
and women are biased in favor of men (Eagly, Beall, & Stern- 
berg, 2005; Hyde, Mezulis, & Abramson, 2008; Kiefer, 2006). 
In addition, cross-cultural research finds remarkable simi- 
larity in the content of gender stereotypes in different societies. 
For example, a 25-nation study identified a core set of descrip- 
tors that were consistently used to describe men and women 


What are the major 
differences between male 
and female gender roles? 


The perception of being male 
or female. 


y Alert 


The distinction between 
gender and sex is subtle. 
Keep in mind that sex 

relates to anatomy, and 
gender relates to 
perception. 





The set of expectations, 
defined by a particular society, that 
indicate what is appropriate behavior 
for men and women. 

Negative attitudes and 
behavior toward a person based on 
that person’s gender. 





From the moment of birth, gender differences are defined by 
outside influences. Is it possible for a family to block these 
influences completely? Would it be desirable to do so? 


343 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


344 Chapter 11 Sexuality and Gender 


FIGURE 1 In spite of numerous 
differences among cultures, research has 
found strong similarities in the content 
of gender stereotypes. (Source: Williams & 
Best, 1990.) 


FIGURE 2 Gender discrepancies 
throughout the world. Each year, the 
World Economic Forum ranks countries 
on the basis of gender equality, 
considering educational attainment, 
economic participation and opportunity, 
political empowerment, and health and 
survival. Out of 134 countries, the 
United States ranks 31st worldwide. 
Iceland, Finland, and Norway are at the 
top of the list; Pakistan, Chad, and 
Yemen are at the bottom. (Source: 
Hausmann, Tyson, & Zahidi, 2009.) 


Words Used to Describe Males 


Active Initiative 
Adventurous 
Aggressive 
Ambitious 
Arrogant 

Assertive 

Autocratic 
Clear-thinki 


Words Used to Describe Females 


Affected 
A ionate 








rogressive 
tional 





Independent 


(Williams & Best, 1990). Women were seen as sentimental, submissive, and supersti- 
tious, whereas men were seen as adventurous, forceful, and independent (see Figure 1). 
These stereotypical similarities across cultures may be due to similarities in status 
between men and women across the cultures; in most, men receive somewhat higher 
status than women (Lips, 2003; Durik, Hyde, & Marks, 2006). 

Such stereotypes matter. By shaping beliefs about how men and women should 
behave, these stereotypes potentially keep inequalities between the genders alive. 
Stereotypes put pressure on people to fulfill the stereotypes, and they may lead 
people to perform in accordance with the stereotypes rather than in accordance with 
their own abilities. Attaining gender equality remains a significant challenge through- 
out the world (Lips, 2003; also see Figure 2). 


Country Rank Country Rank 
Iceland 1 Trinidad and Tobago 19 
Finland 2 Australia 20 
Norway 3 Barbados 21 
Sweden 4 Mongolia 22 
New Zealand 5 Ecuador 23 
South Africa 6 Argentina 24 
Denmark 7 Canada 25 
Ireland 8 Mozambique 26 
Philippines 9 Costa Rica 27 
Lesotho 10 Bahamas 28 
Netherlands 11 Cuba 29 
Germany 12 Lithuania 30 
Switzerland 13 United States 31 
Latvia 14 Namibia 32 
United Kingdom 15 Belgium 3B 
Sri Lanka 16 Belarus 34 
Spain 17 Guyana 35 
France 18 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Sexism on the Job 


Differences still exist regarding which occupations are deemed appropriate for men 
and for women. Women continue to be viewed as best suited for traditionally female 
jobs—that is, pink-collar jobs such as secretary, nurse, cashier, and jobs in other 
female-dominated professions that often feature low pay and low status. Men and 
women alike still report that they expect greater success when they enter a profession 
viewed as appropriate for their gender. Furthermore, women hold lower expectations 
than men about their entering and peak salaries (Crawford & Unger, 2004; MacPher- 
son, 2002; Casad, 2007). 

Those expectations reflect the reality that women, on average, earn less than men. 
Although the gap has been decreasing, women overall earn an average of 80¢ for 
every dollar that men earn. Women who are members of minority groups fare worse: 
Black women earn 69¢ for every dollar men make, and Hispanic and Latino women 
earn just 62¢ for every dollar men earn. Furthermore, even when they are in the same 
professions as men, women generally earn less than men in comparable positions 
(U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2009; see Figure 3). 

Despite these inequities, attitudes are shifting. Most people endorse gender 
equality in the workplace, and they believe that women should be given the same 
opportunities as men. For example, just about as many men as women endorse the 
idea that jobs should be family friendly by offering flexible work schedules. In addi- 
tion, polls today find that most men no longer believe that a woman’s most appro- 
priate role is to care for her home and children while men should be earning 
money—a shift from the 1970s, when the majority of men endorsed that traditional 
view (Bond et al., 2003; Barnett, 2004). 

Still, even when women are successful on the job and are promoted into upper- 
level, high-status positions, they may face significant hurdles in their efforts to move 
up the corporate ladder. Such obstacles are especially true for professional women 
who become mothers; they tend to be newly perceived as warm but less competent 
than they were prior to motherhood. The same thing doesn’t happen to men who 
become fathers; they are most likely to be viewed as warm and competent (Cuddy, 
Fiske, & Glick, 2004). 





90 


85 


80 


75 


70 


Percentage 


65 


60 


55 





gue ttt |] |] | | |} fj | ft | |I 


5 [| | | | | J 4 
1979 1985 1990 


| | | I 
1995 2000 


| | I 
2005 2008 





Year 


FIGURE 3 Although men’s wages have stagnated since 2001, women still earn an average 
of 80¢ for every dollar that men earn. The disparity is even greater for women who are 
members of minority groups. What factors account for the continuing gap between men’s 
and women’s wages? (Source: U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2009.) 


Module 32 


Gender and Sex 


345 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


346 Chapter 11 Sexuality and Gender 






i 


Sexual harassment can take many forms. 


Because of such stereotypic views, many women eventually hit what has come 
to be called the glass ceiling. The glass ceiling is an invisible barrier within an 
organization that may prevent women from being promoted beyond a certain level 
because of gender discrimination. The glass ceiling is even found in colleges and 
universities. For example, although women fill 29% of science and engineering 
jobs at U.S. educational institutions, they occupy only 15% of those positions at 
the top 50 research universities (Ripley, 2005; Lyness & Heilman, 2006; Sampson 
& Moore, 2008). 

As phenomena such as the glass ceiling make clear, male gender stereotypes are 
typically more positive than female stereotypes. Although such stereotypes reflect 
people’s perceptions and not necessarily the reality of the world, people often act as 
if they were real and modify their behavior to conform to the stereotypes. As a result, 
gender stereotypes limit both women’s and men’s behavior and ultimately lead to 
the unfortunate consequence of preferential treatment of men. 


SEXUAL HARASSMENT 


In addition to pay inequity and limited job advancement, women—even those in 
high-status professions—may face workplace sexism in the form of sexual harassment, 
which is defined as unwanted sexual attention, the creation of a hostile or abusive 
environment, or explicit coercion to engage in unwanted sexual activity. Sexual harass- 
ment is not a minor problem. One-fifth of women surveyed in polls say that they have 
been sexually harassed at work. And it is not just women who encounter harassment: 
Some 10% of men report experiencing sexual harassment on the job (Willness, Steel, 
& Lee, 2007; O’Leary-Kelly et al., 2009; Pina, Gannon, & Saunders, 2009). 

Sexual harassment is not just a workplace issue. Thirty percent of the female 
graduates of one large California university reported receiving some form of harass- 
ment. Such harassment begins early in life. In one survey, 81% of middle and high 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Received sexual 
comments, jokes, 
gestures, or looks 


Were touched, 
grabbed, or pinched 
in a sexual way 


Were intentionally 
brushed up against 
in a sexual way 


Sexual rumors were 
spread about them 


Clothing was pulled 
at in a sexual way 











Were shown, given, Girls 
or left sexual material 
Boys 
Sexual messages were 
written about them in 
public areas 
20 40 60 80 


Percentage of students experiencing each kind of behavior 


school students reported receiving some form of sexual harassment in school; 6 in 
10 experienced physical sexual harassment at some point in their school lives (see 
Figure 4). One-third of students are afraid of being sexually harassed; girls are more 
than twice as likely as boys to report concern. Overall, estimates suggest that one of 
every two women will be harassed at some point during her academic or working 
life (AAUW, 2001; Fitzgerald et al., 2003). 

Sexual harassment often has less to do with sex than with power (similar to the 
motivation behind many cases of rape, which we'll discuss in a later module). In this 
view, higher-status persons who engage in harassment may be less interested in 
receiving sexual favors than in demonstrating their power over their victims 
(O’Donohue, 1997; Huerta et al., 2006). 

In some cases, harassment stems from benevolent sexism, stereotyped and restric- 
tive attitudes that appear on the surface to be beneficial to women. For example, a 
male employer may compliment a woman on her attractiveness or offer her an easy 
job so that she won’t have to “work so hard.” The reality, however, is that such com- 
ments or “favors” may undermine the employee’s sense of competence, and she may 
feel that she is not being taken seriously (Glick et al., 2004; Forbes, Jung, & Haas, 
2006; Dardenne, Dumont, & Bollier, 2007). 

Regardless of the motivation that lies behind sexual harassment, the conse- 
quences for the victim are clear. Feelings of shame and embarrassment are standard 
and may be compounded by a sense of helplessness and powerlessness because 
targets of harassment are typically in lower-status positions. People in these situa- 
tions often suffer emotional and physical consequences, and the quality of their work 
may decline. Furthermore, they are less likely to believe they can attain higher posi- 
tions (Magley, 2002; Miner-Rubino & Cortina, 2007; Chiodo et al., 2009). 


Module 32 Gender and Sex 347 


FIGURE 4 Sexual harassment in school. 
The number of students who report 
experiencing unwelcomed sexual 
attention often or occasionally at school 
is significant. (Source: AAUW, 2001.) 


348 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 11 Sexuality and Gender 


Gender Differences: 

More Similar than Dissimilar 

Not surprisingly, gender stereotyping, combined with other factors, results in actual 
behavior differences between men and women. Before we consider the nature of 
gender differences, however, it is important to keep in mind men and women are 
more similar to one another in most respects than they are different. Furthermore, 
the differences that have been found reflect average male and female differences; this 
finding tells us little or nothing about any individual male or female. 

For example, even if we find that males are generally more talkative than females— 
as research shows they are despite the stereotype that women talk more—an individual 
man can be less talkative than most women. Similarly, an individual woman may be 
more talkative than most men. When we consider any single person, our focus should 


be on the individual rather than on his or her gender group. It is important to take this 
into account as we examine the findings on gender differences (Mehl et al., 2007). 





PERSONALITY FACTORS 


One of the most pronounced differences between men and women lies in their degree 
of aggressive behavior. By the time they are 2 years old, boys tend to display more 
aggression than girls do, and this higher level of aggression persists throughout the 
life span. Furthermore, compared with men, women experience greater anxiety and 
guilt about their aggressiveness and are more concerned about its effects on their 
victims (Feingold, 1994; Munroe et al., 2000; Hyde, Mezulis, & Abramson, 2008). 

Men generally have higher self-esteem than women do, although the difference 
is not large. Furthermore, women’s self-esteem is influenced primarily by their per- 
ception of their sense of interdependence and connection with others. In contrast, 
men’s self-esteem stems more from their assessment of their unique characteristics 
and abilities, traits that help them distinguish themselves from other people (Kling, 
Ryff, & Love, 2003; Lawrence, Ashford, & Dent, 2006; Gentile et al., 2009). 

Men and women differ in how positively they view their own abilities and 
how they estimate the probability of their future success. In general, women eval- 
uate themselves more harshly than men. For example, a survey of first-year college 
students compared men’s and women’s views of whether they were above or below 
average. As you can see in Figure 5, more men than women considered themselves 
above average in overall academic and mathematical ability, competitiveness, and 





Boys from age 2 tend to be more aggressive than girls, a pattern that 
continues throughout the life span. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Academic ability 


Mathematical ability 


Competitiveness 


Emotional health 











0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 


Percentage who rate themselves 
above average 


emotional health. Such self-perceptions matter because they influence motivation 
as well as academic and career choices. They even influence performance. For 
example, when women experience stereotype threat—the perception that a perfor- 
mance measure is sensitive to gender differences—their performance declines 
(Steele, Spencer, & Aronson, 2002; Keller, 2007; Carr & Steele, 2009). 

The content of men’s and women’s speech also differs. Women’s speech is more 
precise. However, their speech patterns lead others to view them as more tentative 
and less assertive. Women more often raise their pitch at the end of a sentence and 
add “tags” at the end of an opinion rather than stating the opinion outright. For 
example, instead of saying, “It’s awfully warm today,” a female speaker might say, 
“It’s awfully warm today, isn’t it?”—which makes her appear less certain of her 
opinion. When females use such tentative language, they are judged to be less com- 
petent and knowledgeable than they are when they speak assertively (Matlin, 1996; 
Popp, Donovan, & Crawford, 2003; Leaper & Ayres, 2007). 

Women’s and men’s nonverbal behavior differs as well in several significant 
respects. In conversations with people of the other sex, women look at their partners 
significantly more while listening than while speaking (thereby communicating coop- 
eration), whereas men look at their partners about the same length of time while 
listening and speaking (thereby communicating power). In addition, women are gen- 
erally better than men at decoding others’ facial expressions (Coats & Feldman, 1996; 
Burgoon & Bacue, 2003; LaFrance & Harris, 2004). 


COGNITIVE ABILITIES 


No general differences exist between men and women in overall IQ scores, learning, 
memory, problem solving, and concept-formation tasks. A few differences in more spe- 
cific cognitive areas have been identified, although more recent research has called into 
question the true nature of those differences—and even their existence (Halpern, 2000). 

When Eleanor Maccoby and Carol Jacklin carried out a pioneering study of sex 
differences in 1974, they concluded that girls outperformed boys in verbal abilities 
and that boys had superior quantitative and spatial abilities. Psychologists—and the 
public—widely accepted that conclusion as true. 

However, recent and more sophisticated analyses have questioned these earlier 
findings. In a ground-breaking study, psychologist Janet Hyde and colleagues 


Module 32 Gender and Sex 349 


FIGURE 5 Male first-year college students 
are much more likely than female first- 
year college students to rate themselves 
as above average in academic ability, 
mathematical ability, and emotional 
health. (Source: From Astin, A. W., Korn, W. S., 
& Berz, E. R. (2004). The American freshman: 
National norms for fall 2004. Los Angeles, CA: 
Higher Education Research Institute, Graduate 
School of Education, UCLA. © 2008 The 
Regents of the University of California. All 
Rights Reserved.) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


350 Chapter 11 Sexuality and Gender 


examined the mathematical performance of 7 million United States students in grades 
2 through 11—a huge sample of students. Contrary to traditional wisdom, the average 
mathematical performance of males and females did not differ. Even when they looked 
at only the most gifted students, they found no gender difference (Hyde et al., 2008). 
Similarly, cross-cultural data also find no difference between genders in math per- 
formance. For example, Japanese boys and girls perform quite similarly on math tests 
except for the single math area of probability; Japanese girls routinely outperform both 
boys and girls in the United States on comparable tests of mathematical ability. In short, 
there appear to be no meaningful differences between males and females in math 
performance (Angier & Chang, 2005; Lubinski & Benbow, 2006; Hyde & Mertz, 2009). 
Psychologists have drawn a similar conclusion about the extent of gender differ- 
ences in verbal skills. Despite the earlier view that women show greater verbal abil- 
ities than men, a more careful analysis of 165 studies of gender differences in verbal 
ability, which represents the testing of close to 1.5 million subjects, has led to the 
conclusion that verbal gender differences are insignificant. Furthermore, verbal SAT 
scores are quite similar for boys and girls (Angier & Chang, 2005; Hyde, 2005). 
Current evidence suggests, then, that gender differences in cognitive skills are 
minimal. On the other hand, particular tests of mathematical and verbal skills do elicit 
differences in performance, as in the example of the mathematics part of the SAT in 
which most of the very high scorers are male (Hyde, 2005; Lippa, 2005; Ripley, 2005). 
Furthermore, even when no differences are found in men’s and women’s per- 
formance on specific tasks, there may be underlying differences in how the brain 
processes information. For example, one study found that when men and women 
named particular objects, such as tools and plants, their brains were activated in 
different ways depending on their experience with the objects (see Figure 6). In short, 


When Brain 


and Socialization Meet 





FIGURE 6 Differences in how men and women process information reflect differences in 
learning and experience with those objects. In an fMRI study, (a) women showed greater 
activation in certain areas of the brain when they processed names for tools and (b) men 
showed greater activation in other areas of the brain when they processed names of plants. 
(Source: Garn et al., 2009.) 





(a) Areas more strongly activated in females to tools (b) Areas more strongly activated in males to plants 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


gender differences are complex, and we still do not have the full story on differences 
between men and women when it comes to their cognitive abilities (Hugdahl et 
al., 2006). 


Sources of Gender Differences: 
Where Biology and Society Meet 


If the identification of gender differences has presented a difficult challenge for 
researchers, the search for their causes has proved even more daunting. Given the 
indisputable fact that sex is a biological variable, it would seem reasonable to look 
at factors involving biological differences between men and women. It is also true 
that people are treated differently on the basis of their sex from the time they are 
born. Consequently, we must take into account both biological and social factors 
when we try to understand the source of gender differences. 

Although we'll consider biological and environmental variables separately, nei- 
ther alone can provide a full explanation for gender differences. Some combination 
of the two, interacting with each other, will ultimately provide us with an under- 
standing of why men and women may behave differently. 





BIOLOGICAL AND EVOLUTIONARY FACTORS 


Do differences between male and female brains underlie sex and gender differences? 
This intriguing hypothesis has been put forward by some psychologists studying 
brain structure and functioning. For instance, girls who were exposed before birth to 
unusually high levels of androgen, a male hormone, because their mothers acciden- 
tally took a drug containing that hormone while pregnant preferred different toys 
from those preferred by girls not exposed to androgens. Specifically, they were more 
likely to play with toys that boys stereotypically prefer (such as cars) and less likely 
to play with toys girls stereotypically prefer (such as dolls). Other research, discussed 
in Applying Psychology in the 21st Century, supports this finding. It is possible that 
exposure to particular hormones prior to birth may affect brain development, making 
children favor toys that involve certain kinds of skills, such as those related to spatial 
abilities (Mealey, 2000; Hines et al., 2002, Fink et al., 2007; Hines & Alexander, 2008). 

Similarly, some evidence suggests that women perform better on tasks involving 
verbal skill and muscular coordination during periods when their production of the 
female sex hormone estrogen is relatively high compared with periods when it is low. 
In contrast, they perform better on tasks involving spatial relationships when the 
estrogen level is relatively low (Kimura, 1999; Rosenberg & Park, 2002). 

Some psychologists argue that evolutionary forces lead to certain differences between 
men’s and women’s behavior. For example, David Buss and colleagues point to differ- 
ences in the nature of jealousy between men and women. Men are more jealous in cases 
of sexual infidelity than in cases of emotional infidelity; women are more jealous in cases 
of emotional infidelity than in cases of sexual infidelity (Buss et al., 1992; Buss, 2003). 

According to Buss’s controversial explanation, the root cause for the differences 
in jealousy lies in the evolutionary implications of sexual versus emotional infidelity 
for men and women. He argues that for males, sexual infidelity represents a threat 
to their ability to ensure that their children are actually their own (and are the ones 
who have inherited their genes). In contrast, females have no doubt that a child they 
carry through pregnancy is their own. However, their major concern is ensuring the 
male’s protection and support during child rearing. Thus, to females, maintaining 
males’ emotional attachment is crucial. 

Psychologists relying on the evolutionary approach also argue that similarities 
in the division of labor between men and women across different cultures suggest 


Module 32 


Gender and Sex 


351 


in 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


the 21st Century 





Trucks and Dolls: Does Fetal 
Exposure to Testosterone 
Affect How Children Play? 


In one case, the stereotypes are true: Boys 
typically like rough-and-tumble play and 
their toys of choice are cars and trucks, 
whereas girls prefer quieter activities and 
like to play with dolls. What is less clear is 
why these gender differences occur. Are 
they biological like sex itself, or are they 
learned as a consequence of how we treat 
boys and girls differently in society? 

This question may never be answered 
conclusively. It’s almost certainly the case 
that some combination of biological and 
environmental factors is at work. Recent 
research provides strong evidence for the 
importance of biological factors: It seems 
that the male hormone testosterone plays 
an important role in determining juvenile 
play behavior (Hines et al., 2002; Pasterski 
et al., 2005). 





352 


Ina recent large-scale study, researchers 
measured the concentration of testosterone 
in the amniotic fluid of more than 200 male 
and female fetuses as part of a long-term 
study on the effects of fetal testosterone on 
development. (Although testosterone is the 
primary androgen, or male sex hormone, it 
also is found to a lesser extent in females.) 


aspect is that these children were not un- 
usual in any way. For instance, they were 
not exposed to unnaturally high levels of 
prenatal hormones through drug use or dis- 
ease. They merely experienced normal vari- 
ation in the presence of fetal testosterone. 
That leaves few alternative explanations to 
account for the differences in their play be- 


Eight years later, when the children 
were in middle childhood, researchers 
gave their parents a questionnaire about 
the children’s play behavior. The results 
were clear: There was a strong relationship 
between prenatal testosterone concentra- 
tion and how their children played; higher 
concentrations of fetal testosterone pre- 
dicted more male-typical play behaviors 
in both boys and girls (Auyeung et al., 
2009). 

One interesting aspect of this study is 
that the influence of fetal testosterone on 
male-typical play was found in both 
genders—that is, higher levels of testoster- 
one produced more boyish kinds of play in 
both boys and girls. Another interesting 


havior that emerged later in development. 











e Could conformity pressures on boys and girls during middle childhood lead chil- 
dren to behave in more stereotypically “male” and “female” ways? 

e What should parents do, if anything, to affect the degree to which their children 
behave in ways that are consistent with stereotypes about male and female behavior? 








that sex differences are due to evolutionary factors. They argue that even in very 
different types of cultures, men tend to be more aggressive, competitive, and prone 
to taking risks than women are—traits that had an evolutionary advantage (Mealey, 
2000; Buss, 2003; Chang & Geary, 2007). 

However, many critics question the assumptions of the evolutionary approach. 
Rather than assuming that the differences are due to evolutionary forces, some psy- 
chologists suggest that differences in males’ and females’ beliefs about the meaning 
of infidelity are the actual cause of their jealousy differences. For instance, men may 
believe that women have sex only when they are in love. Consequently, sexual infi- 
delity may be seen as a sign that a woman is in love with another man and may 
produce more jealousy than emotional infidelity alone would. In contrast, women 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


may believe that men are capable of having sex without being in love; consequently 
they may find a man’s sexual infidelity less bothersome because it does not neces- 
sarily mean that he is in love with someone else (DeSteno et al., 2002; Fernandez, 
Vera-Villarroel, & Sierra, 2007). 

Psychologists Alice Eagly and Wendy Wood criticize evolutionary explanations 
for gender differences on different grounds. In their biosocial approach, they argue that 
one important source of gender differences is the difference in the physical capabili- 
ties of men and women. Consistently across cultures, the division of labor between 
men and women is based largely on men’s size, strength, and speed and women’s 
capacity for pregnancy and caring for children. However, the specific nature of activ- 
ities in which men and women specialize varies from one culture to another, depend- 
ing on the specifics of a culture (Wood & Eagly, 2002). 

The extent to which biological and evolutionary factors may underlie gender 
differences is an unanswered and highly controversial question. One thing is clear, 
however: Biological factors and evolutionary factors alone do not explain the com- 
plete range of differences between male and female behavior. To fully understand 
the source of gender differences, we also must consider the social environment, 
which we will do next. 


THE SOCIAL ENVIRONMENT 


From the moment of birth, with blue blankets for boys and pink ones for girls, most 
parents and other adults provide environments that differ in important respects 
according to gender. For example, boys and girls are given different kinds of toys, 
and—until protests recently brought the practice to an end—items in the largest toy 
store chain in the United States were laid out according to the gender appropriateness 
of particular toys (Bannon, 2000; Wood, Desmarais, & Gugula, 2002). 

Parents interact with their children differently, depending on their sex. Fathers 
play more roughly with their infant sons than with their infant daughters. Middle- 
class mothers tend to talk more to their daughters than to their sons. It is clear that 
adults in general frequently treat children differently on the basis of gender (McHale, 
Crouter, & Tucker, 1999; Tenenbaum & Leaper, 2002). 

Such differences in behavior (and there are many more) produce different social- 
ization experiences for men and women. Socialization is the process by which an 
individual learns the rules and norms of appropriate behavior. In this case, it refers 
to learning what society considers appropriate behavior for men and women. Accord- 
ing to the processes of social learning theory, boys and girls are taught and rewarded 
for performing the socially perceived appropriate behaviors for men and for women, 
respectively (Archer & Lloyd, 2002; Liben & Bigler, 2002; 
Leaper & Friedman, 2007). 

It is not just parents, of course, who provide socializa- 
tion experiences for children. Society as a whole commu- 
nicates clear messages to children as they are growing up. 
Children’s reading books traditionally have portrayed 
girls in stereotypically nurturing roles, whereas boys have 
been given more physical and action-oriented roles. Tele- 
vision, too, acts as a particularly influential source of 
socialization. Men outnumber women on television, and 
women are often cast in such stereotypical roles as house- 
wife, secretary, and mother. The potency of television as 
an agent of socialization is underscored by data indicating 
that the more television children watch, the more sexist 
they become (Turkel, 2002; Ogletree, Martinez, & Turner, 
2004; Abelman, 2007 Dill & Thill, 2007). 

Our educational system also treats boys and girls dif- 


Module 32 Gender and Sex 353 


y Alert 
Remember that gender 
differences are caused by 


a 


combination of biological, 
evolutionary, and social 
factors. 





ferently. For example, in elementary school, boys are five “Sometimes it would be helpful if you were a bit more androgynous.” 


© William Haefeli/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


All Rights Reserved. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


354 Chapter 11 Sexuality and Gender 


gender schema A mental framework 
that organizes and guides a child’s 
understanding of information relevant 
to gender. 





Androgynous people tend to defy stereotypes by combining the psychological and 
behavioral characteristics thought typical of both sexes. 


times more likely than girls to receive attention from classroom teachers. Boys receive 
significantly more praise, criticism, and remedial help than girls do. They are also 
more likely to be praised for the intelligence shown in their work, whereas girls are 
more apt to be commended for their neatness. Even in college classes, male students 
receive more eye contact from their professors than female students, men are called 
upon more frequently in class, and men are more apt to receive extra help from their 
professors (AAUW, 1992; Sadker & Sadker, 1994; Einarsson & Granstroem, 2002; 
Koch, 2003). 

Socialization produces a gender schema, a mental framework that organizes and 
guides a child’s understanding of information relevant to gender. On the basis of their 
schemas for appropriate and inappropriate behavior for males and females, children 
begin to behave in ways that reflect society’s gender roles. Hence, children who are 
offered the opportunity to sew a costume for a school play may evaluate the activity 
not in terms of the intrinsic components of the process (such as the mechanics of using 
a needle and thread) but in terms of whether the activity is compatible with their gen- 
der schema. Such evaluation may potentially limit the range of experiences to which 
children are exposed (Bem, 1998; Le Maner-Idrissi & Renault, 2006; Frawley, 2008). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 32 Gender and Sex 355 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 


What are the major differences between male and female gen- 
der roles? 


Gender is the perception of being male or female. 
Gender roles are the expectations, defined by society, of 
what is appropriate behavior for men and women. When 
gender roles reflect favoritism toward one sex, they lead 
to stereotyping and produce sexism. (p. 343) 

The gender-role stereotype for men suggests that they 
are endowed with competence-related traits, whereas 
women are seen in terms of their capacity for warmth 
and expressiveness. Actual sex differences are much less 
clear and of smaller magnitude than the stereotypes 
would suggest. The differences that do exist are pro- 
duced by a combination of biological and environmental 
factors. (p. 344) 

Biological causes of sex difference are reflected by evi- 
dence suggesting a possible difference in brain structure 
and functioning between men and women and may be 
associated with differential exposure to hormones before 
birth. (p. 350) 

An evolutionary approach explains gender differences in 
terms of different male-female concerns regarding the in- 
heritance of genes and the need for child rearing, but this 
approach is highly controversial. (p. 351) 

Socialization experiences produce gender schemas, men- 
tal frameworks that organize and guide a child’s under- 
standing of information relevant to gender. (p. 353) 


KEY TERMS 


gender p. 343 
gender roles p. 343 


sexism p. 343 
gender schema p. 354 


EVALUATE 


1. 


are sets of societal expectations about what is 
appropriate behavior for men and women. 
. Gender stereotypes seem to be much less prevalent today 
than they were several decades ago. True or false? 
. Which of the following statements about male-female 
differences in aggression is true? 
a. Males are physically more aggressive than females only 
during childhood. 
b. Male-female differences in aggression first become 
evident during adolescence. 
c. Males are more aggressive than females throughout the 
life span. 
d. Females and males feel equally anxious about their 
aggressive acts. 
are frameworks that organize understanding 
of gender-specific information. 


RETHINK 


1 


. The U.S. Congress has enacted laws prohibiting women in 
the armed forces from participating directly in combat in 
the interest of keeping them safe. Do you think such laws 
are protective or sexist? How might this be an example of 
“benevolent sexism”? 

. From the perspective of a business executive: Evidence shows 
that sexism in the workplace is widespread. If you wanted 
to end sexism in organizational settings, can you think of 
ways to narrow the gap between men and women in 
terms of occupations and salary? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


seuloyps Jopues “fF (OE ‘uazeaard [ys are Asay} ‘asyey Z ‘SaTOI Iapues `T 


MODULE 33 


Why, and under what 
circumstances, dowe become 
sexually aroused? 


Male sex hormones 
secreted by the testes. 


The male and female sex 
organs. 


Class of female sex 
hormones. 


A female sex hormone 
secreted by the ovaries. 


The point at which an egg 
is released from the ovaries. 


356 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


When I started “tuning out,” teachers thought I was sick—physically sick that is. They 
kept sending me to the school nurse to have my temperature taken. If I’d told them I 
was carrying on with Jennifer Lopez in their classes while supposedly learning my 
Caesar and my Latin vocabulary, they’d have thought I was—well, delirious. I was! 
(based on Coles & Stokes, 1985, pp. 18-19) 


Not everyone’s sexual fantasies are as consuming as those reported by this teenage 
boy. Yet sex is an important consideration in most people’s lives; although the phys- 
ical aspects of human sex are not all that different from those of other species, the 
meaning, values, and feelings that humans place on sexual behavior elevate it to a 
special plane. To fully appreciate this difference, however, it is necessary to under- 
stand the basic biology underlying sexual responses. 


The Basic Biology 
of Sexual Behavior 


Anyone who has seen two dogs mating knows that sexual behavior has a bio- 
logical basis. Their sexual behavior appears to occur naturally without much 
prompting on the part of others. A number of genetically controlled factors influ- 
ence the sexual behavior of nonhuman animals. For instance, animal behavior is 
affected by the presence of certain hormones in the blood. Moreover, female ani- 
mals are receptive to sexual advances only during certain relatively limited peri- 
ods of the year. 

Human sexual behavior, by comparison, is more complicated, although the 
underlying biology is not all that different from that of related species. In males, for 
example, the testes begin to secrete androgens, male sex hormones, at puberty. (See 
Figure 1 for the basic anatomy of the male and female genitals, or sex organs.) Not 
only do androgens produce secondary sex characteristics, such as the growth of 
body hair and a deepening of the voice, they also increase the sex drive. Because 
the level of androgen production by the testes is fairly constant, men are capable of 
(and interested in) sexual activities without any regard to biological cycles. Given 
the proper stimuli leading to arousal, male sexual behavior can occur at any time 
(Goldstein, 2000). 

Women show a different pattern. When they reach maturity at puberty, the 
two ovaries begin to produce estrogens and progesterone, female sex hormones. 
However, those hormones are not produced consistently; instead, their production 
follows a cyclical pattern. The greatest output occurs during ovulation, when an 
egg is released from the ovaries, making the chances of fertilization by a sperm 
cell highest. While in nonhumans the period around ovulation is the only time the 
female is receptive to sex, people are different. Although there are variations in 
reported sex drive, women are receptive to sex throughout their cycles (Leiblum 
& Chivers, 2007). 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 33 Understanding Human Sexual Response: The Facts of Life 357 





Ovary 


Uterus 





















Cervix 
Bladder 
Pubic bone 
Vagina 
Urethra 
Clitoris Anus 


Female 


Large intestine 


Bladder 


Seminal vesicle 





Pubic bone Ejaculatory duct 


Vas deferens 
Prostate 


Urethra Anüs 





Penis 






Testis 














my 
“) 
Glans-— 70 


Scrotum 





Male 


FIGURE 1 Cutaway side views of the female and male sex organs. 


In addition, some evidence suggests that males have a stronger sex drive than 
females, although the difference may be the result of society’s discouragement of 
female sexuality rather than of innate differences between men and women. It is clear 
that men think about sex more than women: while 54% of men report thinking about 
sex every day, only 19% of women report thinking about it on a daily basis (Men- 
delsohn, 2003; Gangestad et al., 2004; Baumeister & Stillman, 2006). 

Though biological factors “prime” people for sex, it takes more than hormones 
to motivate and produce sexual behavior. In animals the presence of a partner who 
provides arousing stimuli leads to sexual activity. Humans are considerably more 
versatile; not only other people but nearly any object, sight, smell, sound, or other 
stimulus can lead to sexual excitement. Because of prior associations, then, people 
may be turned on sexually by the smell of perfume or the sound of a favorite song 
hummed softly in their ears. The reaction to a specific, potentially arousing stimulus, 
as we shall see, is highly individual—what turns one person on may do just the 
opposite for another (Benson, 2003). 


358 Chapter 11 Sexuality and Gender 


erogenous zones Areas of the body 
that are particularly sensitive because 
of the presence of an unusually rich 
array of nerve receptors. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Physiological Aspects 
of Sexual Excitement: 
What Turns People On? 


If you were to argue that the major human sex organ is the brain, in a sense you 
would be right. Much of what is considered sexually arousing in our society has 
little or nothing to do with our genitals; instead, it is related to external stimuli 
that, through a process of learning, have come to be labeled as erotic, or sexually 
stimulating. 

For example, there are no areas of the body that automatically produce sexual 
arousal when touched. Areas of the body, called erogenous zones, that have an 
unusually rich array of nerve receptors are particularly sensitive not just to sexual 
touch but to any kind of touch. When a physician touches a breast or a penis, the 
information sent to the brain by the nerve cells is essentially the same as that sent 
when a sexual partner touches that spot. What differs is the interpretation given to 
the touch. Sexual arousal is likely only when a certain part of the body is touched 
in what people define as a sexual manner and when a person is receptive to sexual 
activity (Gagnon, 1977; Goldstein, 2000). 

Although people can learn to respond sexually to almost any stimulus, there is 
a good deal of agreement within a society or culture about what usually represents 
an erotic stimulus. In many Western societies breast size is often the standard by 
which female appeal is measured, but in many other cultures breast size is irrelevant 
(Rothblum, 1990; Furnham & Swami, 2007). 

Sexual fantasies also play an important role in producing sexual arousal. Not 
only do people have fantasies of a sexual nature during their everyday activities; 
about 60% of all people have fantasies during sexual intercourse. In fact, such fanta- 
sies often include having sex with someone other than one’s partner of the moment 
(Hicks & Leitenberg, 2001; Trudel, 2002; Critelli & Bivona, 2008). 

Men’s and women’s fantasies differ little from each other in terms of content or 
quantity, although men seem to fantasize about sex more than women do. But both 
men and women wish they kissed more, engaged in more oral sex, and had vaginal 
intercourse more frequently than they actually do (Hsu et al., 1994; Shulman & 
Horne, 2006; Santtila, et al., 2008). 





The Phases of Sexual 
Response: The Ups 
and Downs of Sex 


Although the kinds of stimuli that produce sexual arousal are to some degree unique 
to each individual, we all share some basic aspects of sexual responsiveness. Accord- 
ing to pioneering work done by William Masters and Virginia Johnson (1966), who 
studied sexual behavior in carefully controlled laboratory settings, sexual responses 
follow a regular pattern consisting of four phases: excitement, plateau, orgasm, and 
resolution. Although other researchers argue that sexual responses proceed some- 
what differently (e.g., Kaplan, 1974; Zilbergeld & Ellison, 1980), Masters and John- 
son’s research is the most widely accepted account of what happens when people 
become sexually excited (Masters & Johnson, 1994). 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 33 Understanding Human Sexual Response: The Facts of Life 359 




































Orgasm Orgasm 
Plateau 
Refractory D Plateau 
period & 
& 7 

2 Resolution 
U 

ğ 

Resolution Resolution Resolution 
ABC (C) (A) (B) 








Time 


a) Male pattern b) Female patterns 
p P 


FIGURE 2 A four-stage model of the sexual response cycle for males and females based on 
the findings of Masters and Johnson (1966). Note how the male pattern (a) includes a 
refractory period. Part (b) shows three possible female patterns. In A, the pattern is closest to 
the male cycle except that the woman has two orgasms in a row. In B, there is no orgasm, 
whereas in C orgasm is reached quickly and the woman rapidly returns to an unaroused 
state. (Source: After Masters & Johnson, 1966.) 


In the excitement phase, which can last from just a few minutes to more than 
an hour, an arousing stimulus begins a sequence that prepares the genitals for sexual 
intercourse. In the male, the penis becomes erect when blood flows into it. In the 
female, the clitoris swells because of an increase in the blood supply to that area, and 
the vagina becomes lubricated. Women may also experience a “sex flush,” a red rash 
that typically spreads over the chest and throat. 

Next comes the plateau phase, the body’s preparation for orgasm. During this 
stage, the maximum level of sexual arousal is attained as the penis and clitoris swell 
with blood. Women’s breasts and vaginas expand, heartbeat and blood pressure rise, 
and breathing rate increases. Muscle tension becomes greater as the body prepares 
itself for the next stage, orgasm. Although it is difficult to explain the sensation of 
orgasm beyond saying that it is an intense, highly pleasurable experience, the bio- 
logical events that accompany the feeling are fairly straightforward. When the orgasm 
stage is reached, rhythmic muscular contractions occur in the genitals every eight- 
tenths of a second. In the male, the contractions expel semen, a fluid containing 
sperm, from the penis—a process known as ejaculation. For women and men, breath- 
ing and heart rates reach their maximum. 

After orgasm, people move into the last stage of sexual arousal, the resolution 
stage. The body returns to its resting state, reversing the changes brought about by 
arousal. The genitals resume their unaroused size and shape; blood pressure, breath- 
ing, and heart rate return to normal. 

Male and female responses differ significantly during the resolution stage; these 
differences are depicted in Figure 2. Women are able to cycle back to the orgasm 
phase and experience repeated orgasms. Ultimately, of course, females enter the final 
resolution stage, and then they return to their prestimulation state. In contrast, it 
generally is thought that men enter a refractory period during the resolution stage. 
During the refractory period, men are unable to develop an erection and therefore 
are unable to have another orgasm and ejaculate. The refractory period may last 
from a few minutes to several hours; in the elderly it may continue for several days 
(Goldstein, 2000). 


Time 


y Alert 


Use Figure 2 to help remem- 
ber the different patterns of 
sexual response and how 
they differ between men 
and women. 


excitement phase The period in 
which an arousing stimulus begins a 
sequence that prepares the genitals for 
sexual intercourse. 


plateau phase The period in which the 
maximum level of arousal is attained, 
the penis and clitoris swell with blood, 
and the body prepares for orgasm. 


orgasm The peak of sexual excite- 
ment, during which rhythmic muscu- 
lar contractions occur in the genitals. 


resolution stage The interval after 
orgasm in which the body returns to 
its unaroused state, reversing the 
changes brought about by arousal. 


refractory period A temporary period 
that follows the resolution stage and 
during which the male cannot develop 
an erection again. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


360 Chapter 11 Sexuality and Gender 


AR 
j ©; 


Ex p | O ri n g Waris Dirie was just an innocent, unknowing child of 5 when 
DIVERSITY she begged her mother to let her be circumcised like virtually 


all females in Somalia. “When you've been told over and over 
that, until this happens, you’re filthy and no man would ever 
marry you, you believe what everybody says,” Dirie explains. 
“T just wanted to be like the other girls.” 

Months later her awful wish came true. As her mother held 
down the crying, blindfolded Diri, a gypsy performed the circumcision using a dirty, 
dull razor and no anesthetic. She sewed the ragged wound with thorns and thread. 
“It’s not a pain you forget,” says Dirie, in a whisper. She was left with only a tiny 
opening, and urinating became torture. Later, menstruation was so unbearable that 
Dirie routinely fainted. (Cheakalos & Heyn, 1998, p. 149) 

The operation in question—female circumcision—represents one of the most con- 
troversial procedures relating to sex throughout the world. In such an operation, the 
clitoris is removed, resulting in permanent inability to experience sexual pleasure. 

Some 80 million women living mostly in Africa and Asia have undergone female 
circumcision. More than 90% of Nigerian women have been circumcised during 
childhood, and more than 90% intend to circumcise their daughters. Furthermore, in 
some cases, more extensive surgery is carried out in which additional parts of the 
female genitals are removed or are sewn together with catgut or thorns (Lacey, 2002; 
Renshaw, 2006; Odeku, Rembe, & Anwo, 2009). 

Those who practice female circumcision say it upholds an ancient societal tradition 
and is no different from other cultural customs. Its purpose, they say, is to preserve 
virginity before marriage, keep women faithful to their husbands after marriage, and 
enhance a woman’s beauty. Furthermore, proponents believe that it differs little from 
the common Western practice of male circumcision in which the foreskin of the penis 
is surgically removed soon after birth. 

In contrast, critics argue that female circumcision is nothing less than female muti- 
lation. Not only does the practice permanently eliminate sexual pleasure, but it can 
also lead to constant pain, infection, and, in some cases, death. Furthermore, because 
the procedure is traditionally conducted in a ritualistic fashion using a razor blade, 
sawtooth knife, or glass and without an anesthetic, the circumcision itself can be 
physically traumatic (Dugger, 1996; Renshaw, 2006). 

The procedure raises some difficult issues that have been brought to light in var- 
ious court cases. For instance, a Nigerian immigrant, living temporarily in the United 
States, went to court to argue that she should be allowed to remain permanently. Her 
plea: If she and her young daughters were sent back to Nigeria, her daughters would 
face circumcision upon their return. The court agreed and permitted her to stay 
indefinitely (Gregory, 1994; Dugger, 1996). 

In reaction to the controversy about female circumcision, Congress made the prac- 
tice illegal in the United States. Still, some critics argue that female circumcision is a 
valued cultural custom, and that no one, particularly someone judging from the 
perspective of another culture, should prevent people from carrying out the customs 
they think are important. In addition, they point out that the practice of male circum- 
cision, in which the foreskin of the penis is surgically removed, is an accepted prac- 
tice in the United States, based largely on religious, social, and cultural traditions 
(Boyle et al. 2002; Johnsdotter & Essén, 2005; Renshaw, 2006). 


Female Circumcision: A Celebration 
of Culture—or Genital Mutilation? 





> 
By 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 33 Understanding Human Sexual Response: The Facts of Life 361 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 2. Men are generally thought to enter a ____ period 
f ich a ible f : i 
Why, and under what circumstances, do we become sexually e e a a op ae he 


aroused? 

e Biological factors, such as the presence of androgens 
(male sex hormones) and estrogens and progesterone 
(female sex hormones), prime people for sex. But almost 
any kind of stimulus can produce sexual arousal, 
depending on a person’s prior experience. (p. 356) 

e People’s sexual responses follow a regular pattern con- RETHINK 
sisting of four phases: excitement, plateau, orgasm, and 
resolution. (p. 356) 


3. Whereas men are interested in sexual activity regardless of 
their biological cycles, women are truly receptive to sex 
only during ovulation, when the production of their sex 
hormones is greatest. True or false? 


1. Why do you think humans differ from other species in 
their year-round receptivity to sex and in the number and 
variety of stimuli they perceive as sexual? What evolution- 

EVALUATE ary purpose might this difference serve in humans? 

2. From the perspective of a sex counselor: How do people learn 
to be aroused by the stimuli that their society considers 
erotic? When do they learn this, and where does the mes- 


1. Match the phase of sexual arousal with its characteristics. 
1. Excitement phase a. Maximum level of sexual arousal 


2. Plateau phase b. Erection and lubrication A 
; ; sage come from? 

3. Orgasm phase c. Rhythmic muscular contractions 

4, Resolution phase and ejaculation 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 
d. Return of body to resting state 
apo Ad Atay} ynoysnormyy 


aaydacar are UIWOM ‘aspey 'E !ÁIOPLIJƏI Z ‘P-P ‘0-€ “L-Z ‘G-T'T. 


KEY TERMS 


androgens p.356 progesterone p. 356 excitement phase p. 359 resolution stage p. 359 
genitals p. 356 ovulation p.356 plateau phase p. 359 refractory period p. 359 
estrogens p. 356 erogenous zones p. 358 orgasm p. 359 


~~ www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


|” 


What is “normal” sexual 


behavior? 


How do most people behave 
sexually? 


How prevalent are rape and 
other forms of nonconsenting 
sex, and what are their causes? 


What are the major sexually 
transmitted infections? 


What sexual difficulties do 
people most frequently 
encounter? 





362 


A boy who practices this habit can never be the best that Nature intended him to be. 
His wits are not so sharp. His memory is not so good. His power of fixing his attention 
on whatever he is doing is lessened . . . A boy like this is a poor thing to look at... 
[He is] untrustworthy, unreliable, untruthful, and probably even dishonest (Schofield & 


Vaughan-Jackson, 1913, pp. 30-42). 


The cause of this condition: masturbation—at least according to the authors of the 
early 1900s sex manual What Every Boy Should Know. The consequences of masturba- 
tion for women were considered no less severe. In the words of one 19th-century 
physician, “There is hardly an end to the diseases caused by masturbation: dyspep- 
sia, spinal disease, headache, epilepsy, various kinds of fits . . . impaired eyesight, 
palpitation of the heart, pain in the side and bleeding at the lungs, spasm of the 
heart, and sometimes sudden death” (Gregory, 1856). 

Such views may seem as bizarre and far-fetched to you as they do to contempo- 
rary experts on human sexual behavior. However, at one time they were considered 
perfectly sound by quite reasonable people. Indeed, trivia buffs might be interested 
to learn that corn flakes owe their invention to a 19th-century physician, J. W. Kel- 
logg, who believed that the enjoyment of tasty food provoked sexual excitation so 
an alternative of “unstimulating” grains was needed. 

Clearly, sex and sex-related behavior are influenced by expectations, attitudes, 
beliefs, and the state of medical and biological knowledge in a given period. Today 
we know that sexual behavior may take diverse forms, and much of what was once 
seen as “unnatural” and “lewd” is often more likely to be accepted in contemporary 
society. Similarly, sexual behavior that is commonplace in one culture is seen as 
appalling in others. For instance, seven societies are known in which kissing never 
occurs (Ford & Beach, 1951; Mason, 1995). In sum, distinctions between normal and 
abnormal sexual behavior are not easy to draw. This reality certainly hasn’t pre- 
vented people from trying to draw them, however. 


DOIN IT? 


FOR YOUTH B L-E p 
IT'S SAFE, FRIENDLY & 
WWW MIVGetTested.cor 


Ki EST. 1-800-758-0880 


= Ae 


Non Tal 


Times change: The veiled sexuality of Victorian-era paintings has given way to a more frank 
acknowledgment of sexual behavior. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 34 The Diversity of Sexual Behavior 363 


Approaches to Sexual Normality 


One approach is to define abnormal sexual behavior in terms of deviation from the 
average, or typical, behavior. To determine abnormality, we simply observe what 
behaviors are rare and uncommon in a society and label those deviations from the 
norm as abnormal. 

The difficulty with such an approach, however, is that some behaviors that are 
statistically unusual hardly seem worthy of concern. Even though most people have 
sexual intercourse in the bedroom, does the fact that someone prefers sex in the din- 
ing room imply abnormality? If some people prefer portly sexual partners, are they 
abnormal in a society that holds slimness in high regard? Since the answer to both 
of these questions is clearly no, an approach that defines sexual abnormality in terms 
of deviation from the average is inappropriate. (The same difficulties are encountered 
in considering definitions of psychological abnormality.) 

An alternative approach would be to compare sexual behavior against some 
standard or ideal. But here, again, there is a problem: What standard should we use? 
Some of us might suggest philosophy, some might turn to the Bible, and some might 
even consider psychology the ultimate determinant. The trouble is that none of these 
potential sources of standards is universally acceptable. Furthermore, since standards 
change radically with shifts in societal attitudes and new knowledge, such an 
approach is undesirable. For instance, 40 years ago, the American Psychiatric Asso- 
ciation labeled homosexuality a mental illness. However, as evidence to the contrary 
accumulated, in 1973 the organization determined that homosexuality should no lon- 
ger be considered a mental disorder. Obviously the behavior had not changed. Only 
the label the psychiatric profession gave it had been modified. 

In light of the difficulties with other approaches, probably the most reasonable 
definition of sexual normality is one that considers the psychological consequences of 
the behavior. In this approach, sexual behavior is considered abnormal if it produces 
a sense of distress, anxiety, or guilt—or if it is harmful to some other person. Accord- 
ing to this view, then, sexual behaviors can be seen as abnormal only when they have 
a negative impact on a person’s sense of well-being or if they hurt someone else. 

It is important to recognize that what is seen as normal and what is seen as 
abnormal sexual behavior are dictated primarily by societal values; there have been 








y Alert 


Remember the different 
approaches to sexual 
normality (deviation from 
the average, comparison 
against a standard, and 
psychological consequences). 


Same-sex marriages, already legal in 
some states, represent a significant 
social issue dividing people in the 
United States. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


364 Chapter 11 Sexuality and Gender 


dramatic shifts from one generation to another in definitions of what constitutes 
appropriate sexual behavior. People can and should make their own personal value 
judgments about what is appropriate in their own sex lives, but there are few uni- 
versally accepted absolute rights and wrongs. 


Surveying Sexual Behavior: 
What's Happening Behind 
Closed Doors? 


For most of recorded history, the vast variety of sexual practices remained shrouded 
in ignorance. However, in the late 1930s, biologist Alfred Kinsey launched a series 
of surveys on the sexual behavior of people in the United States. The result was the 
first comprehensive attempt to see what people were actually doing sexually and 
was highlighted by the publication of Kinsey’s landmark volumes, Sexual Behavior 
in the Human Male (Kinsey et al., 1948) and Sexual Behavior in the Human Female 
(Kinsey et al., 1953). 

Kinsey’s efforts represented the first major systematic approach to learning about 
human sexual behavior. Kinsey and his colleagues interviewed tens of thousands of 
individuals, and the interview techniques they devised are still regarded as exemplary 
because of their ability to elicit sensitive information without causing embarrassment. 

On the other hand, Kinsey’s samples reflected an overrepresentation of college 
students, young people, well-educated individuals, urban dwellers, and people liv- 
ing in Indiana and the northeast (Kirby, 1977). Furthermore, as with all surveys 
involving volunteer participants, it is unclear how representative his data are of 
people who refused to participate in the study. Similarly, because no survey observes 
behavior directly, it is difficult to assess how accurately people’s descriptions of what 
they do in private match their actual sexual practices. 

Kinsey’s work set the stage for later surveys. But due to political reasons (the 
use of government funding for sex surveys is controversial), surprisingly few com- 
prehensive, large-scale, representative surveys—either in the United States or in other 
countries—have been carried out since Kinsey did his initial work (Pinkerton et al., 
2003). However, by examining the common results gleaned from different samples 
of subjects, we now have a reasonably complete picture of contemporary sexual 
practices—to which we turn next. 





MASTURBATION: SOLITARY SEX 


masturbation Sexual self-stimulation. If you listened to physicians 75 years ago, you would have been told that masturbation, 
sexual self-stimulation often using the hand to rub the genitals, would lead to a wide 
variety of physical and mental disorders, ranging from hairy palms to insanity. If those 
physicians had been correct, however, most of us would be wearing gloves to hide the 
sight of our hair-covered palms because masturbation is one of the most frequently 
practiced sexual activities. Some 94% of all males and 63% of all females have mastur- 
bated at least once; among college students, the frequency ranges from “never” to “sev- 
eral times a day” (Hunt, 1974; Michael et al., 1994; Laqueur, 2003, Polonsky, 2006). 
Men and women typically begin to masturbate for the first time at different ages, 
as you can see in Figure 1. Furthermore, men masturbate considerably more often 
than women, although there are differences in frequency according to age. Male 
masturbation is most common in the early teens and then declines; females both 
begin and reach a maximum frequency later. There are also some racial differences: 
African-American men and women masturbate less than whites do (Oliver & Hyde, 
1993; Pinkerton et al, 2002; Das, Parish, & Laumann, 2009). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 34 The Diversity of Sexual Behavior 365 










The age of first masturbation is 
earlier for most males than it is 
for most females. 


50 
fa Males 


40 Females — 
30 


20 


Percentage of sample 





5-8 9-12 13-16 17-21 


Age at first masturbation 


Although masturbation is often considered an activity to engage in only if no 
other sexual outlets are available, this view bears little relationship to reality. Close 
to three-fourths of married men (age 20 to 40) report masturbating an average of 24 
times a year, and 68% of the married women in the same age group masturbate an 
average of 10 times a year (Hunt, 1974; Michael et al., 1994; Das, 2007). 

Despite the high incidence of masturbation, attitudes toward it still reflect some 
of the negative views of yesteryear. For instance, one survey found that around 10% 
of people who masturbated experienced feelings of guilt; 5% of the males and 1% of 
the females considered their behavior perverted (Arafat & Cotton, 1974). Despite 
these negative attitudes, however, most experts on sex view masturbation as a healthy 
and legitimate—and harmless—sexual activity. In addition, masturbation is seen as 
providing a means of learning about one’s own sexuality and a way of discovering 
changes in one’s body such as the emergence of precancerous lumps (Coleman, 2002; 
Levin, 2007; Herbenick et al., 2009). 


Heterosexuality 


People often believe that the first time they have sexual intercourse they have 
achieved one of life’s major milestones. However, heterosexuality, sexual attraction 
and behavior directed to the other sex, consists of far more than male-female inter- 
course. Kissing, petting, caressing, massaging, and other forms of sex play are all 
components of heterosexual behavior. Still, sex researchers’ focus has been on the act 
of intercourse, especially in terms of its first occurrence and its frequency. 





PREMARITAL SEX 


Until fairly recently, premarital sexual intercourse, at least for women, was con- 
sidered one of the major taboos in our society. Traditionally, women have been 
warned by society that “nice girls don’t do it”; men have been told that premari- 
tal sex is okay for them, but they should marry virgins. This view that premarital 
sex is permissible for males but not for females is called the double standard 
(Liang, 2007). 


FIGURE 1 The age at which a sample of 
college students first masturbated. The 
percentages are based on only those 
people who had experience with 
masturbation. (Source: Arafat & Cotton, 
1974.) 


heterosexuality Sexual attraction and 
behavior directed to the other sex. 


double standard The view that pre- 
marital sex is permissible for males but 
not for females. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


366 Chapter 11 Sexuality and Gender 


FIGURE 2 The percentage of adults 
who find premarital sex is not wrong at 
all has risen dramatically over the last 
several decades, whereas the percentage 
of adults who think premarital sex is 
always wrong has increased only slightly. 
(Source: Gallup News Service, 1998.) What 
changes in society account for this shift 
in attitudes toward intercourse? 


Percentage of people age 45-54 who say 
sex before marriage is not wrong at all 


Percentage of people age 18-24 who say it 
is always wrong to have sex before marriage 


Percentage 











0 | | | | l | | l | l | | | l | l | 
on tr wn n œ la] m 1N Ke] foe) D o = m +r so œ 
~ n ` ~ ~ œ oo fe) i) © io) D n fo fo n a 
oO [oN a oO a an oO a an a a a oO oo a oO oa 


Year 


As recently as the 1960s, the majority of adult Americans believed that pre- 
marital sex was always wrong. But there has been a dramatic change in public opin- 
ion since then. For example, as you can see in Figure 2, the percentage of middle-age 
people who say sex before marriage is “not wrong at all” has increased considerably, 
and overall 60% of Americans say premarital sex is okay. More than half say that 
living together before marriage is morally acceptable (Thornton & Young-DeMarco, 
2001; Harding & Jencks, 2003). 

Changes in attitudes toward premarital sex were matched by changes in actual 
rates of premarital sexual activity. For instance, the most recent figures show that just 
more than one-half of women between the ages of 15 and 19 have had premarital 
sexual intercourse. These figures are close to double the number of women in the 
same age range who reported having intercourse in 1970. Clearly, the trend over the 
last several decades has been toward more women engaging in premarital sexual 
activity (Jones, Darroch, & Singh, 2005). 

Males, too, have shown an increase in the incidence of premarital sexual inter- 
course, although the increase has not been as dramatic as it has been for females— 
probably because the rates for males were higher to begin with. For instance, the first 
surveys of premarital intercourse carried out in the 1940s showed an incidence of 
84% across males of all ages; recent figures are closer to 95%. Moreover, the average 
age of males’ first sexual experience has been declining steadily. Almost half of males 
have had sexual intercourse by the age of 18; by the time they reach age 20, 88% 
have had intercourse. There also are race and ethnicity differences: African Americans 
tend to have sex for the first time earlier than Puerto Ricans, who have sex earlier 
than whites. Racial and ethnic differences probably reflect differences in socioeco- 
nomic opportunities and family structure (Arena, 1984; Singh et al., 2000; Hyde, 
Mezulis, & Abramson, 2008). 

What may be most interesting about the patterns of premarital sex is that they 
show a convergence of male and female attitudes and behavior. But is the change 
sufficient to signal an end to the double standard? 

Probably. For many people, particularly younger individuals, the double stan- 
dard has been succeeded by a new view: permissiveness with affection. According to 
those holding this view, premarital intercourse is permissible for both men and 
women if it occurs within a long-term, committed, or loving relationship (DeGaton, 
Weed, & Jensen, 1996; Hyde, Mezulis, & Abramson, 2008). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 34 The Diversity of Sexual Behavior 367 


Still, the double standard has not disappeared completely. Where differing stan- 
dards remain, the attitudes are almost always more lenient toward the male than 
toward the female (Sprecher & Hatfield, 1996). 

Furthermore, there are substantial cultural differences regarding the incidence 
and acceptability of premarital intercourse. For instance, the proportions of male 
teenagers who have intercourse before their 17th birthday in Jamaica, the United 
States, and Brazil are about 10 times the level reported in the Philippines. And in 
some cultures, such as those in sub-Saharan Africa, women become sexually active 
at an earlier age than men—although this may be due to the fact that they marry at 
a younger age than men (Singh et al., 2000). 


MARITAL SEX 


To judge by the number of articles about sex in heterosexual marriages, one would 
think that sexual behavior was the number one standard by which marital bliss is 
measured. Married couples are often concerned that they are having too little sex, 
too much sex, or the wrong kind of sex (Harvey, Wenzel, & Sprecher, 2005). 

Although there are many different dimensions along which sex in marriage is 
measured, one is certainly the frequency of sexual intercourse. What is typical? As 
with most other types of sexual activities, there is no easy answer to the question 
because there are such wide variations in patterns between individuals. We do know 
that 43% of married couples have sexual intercourse a few times a month and 36% 
of couples have it two or three times a week. With increasing age and length of mar- 
riage, the frequency of intercourse declines. Still, sex continues into late adulthood 
with almost half of people reporting that they engage in high-quality sexual activity 
at least once a month (Michael et al., 1994; Powell, 2006; see Figure 3). 

Although early research found extramarital sex to be widespread, the current 
reality appears to be otherwise. According to surveys, 85% of married women and 
more than 75% of married men are faithful to their spouses. Furthermore, the median 
number of sex partners inside and outside of marriage since the age of 18 was six 
for men and two for women. Accompanying these numbers is a high, consistent 
degree of disapproval of extramarital sex, with nine of ten people saying that it is 
“always” or “almost always” wrong (Michael et al., 1994; Daines, 2006; Whisman & 
Snyder, 2007). 





Homosexuality and Bisexuality 


Homosexuals are sexually attracted to members of their own sex, whereas bisexuals 
are sexually attracted to people of the same sex and the other sex. Many male homo- 
sexuals prefer the term gay and female homosexuals prefer the term lesbian because 
they refer to a broader array of attitudes and lifestyles than the term homosexual, 
which focuses on the sexual act. 

The number of people who choose same-sex sexual partners at one time or 
another is considerable. Estimates suggest that around 20-25% of males and about 
15% of females have had at least one gay or lesbian experience during adulthood. The 
exact number of people who identify themselves as exclusively homosexual has 
proved difficult to gauge; some estimates are as low as 1.1% and some as high as 10%. 
Most experts suggest that 5-10% of both men and women are exclusively gay or 
lesbian during extended periods of their lives (Hunt, 1974; Sells, 1994; Firestein, 1996). 

Although people often view homosexuality and heterosexuality as two com- 
pletely distinct sexual orientations, the issue is not that simple. Pioneering sex 
researcher Alfred Kinsey acknowledged this when he considered sexual orientation 
along a scale or continuum with “exclusively homosexual” at one end and “exclu- 
sively heterosexual” at the other. In the middle were people who showed both homo- 
sexual and heterosexual behavior (see Figure 4). Kinsey’s approach suggests that 


Average incidence of sexual intercourse per month 


0 
18-24 25-34 35-44 45-54 55-65 








Age 


FIGURE 3 Once a couple is past their 
mid-30s, the average number of times 
per month they have sexual intercourse 
declines. Why do older couples have 
intercourse less frequently than younger 
ones? (Clements, 1994) 


extramarital sex Sexual activity 
between a married person and 
someone who is not his or her spouse. 


homosexuals Persons who are 
sexually attracted to members of 
their own sex. 


bisexuals Persons who are sexually 
attracted to people of the same sex 
and the other sex. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


368 Chapter 11 Sexuality and Gender 





FIGURE 4 The Kinsey scale is designed to define the degree to which sexual orientation is 
heterosexual, homosexual, or bisexual. Although Kinsey saw people as falling along a continuum, 
most people believe that they belonged to a specific category. (Source: From Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, 
W. B., & Martin, C. E. (1948). Sexual behavior in the human male, p. 638. Indiana University Press; originally 


published by W. B. Saunders Company, 1948. Reprinted by permission of The Kinsey Institute for Research in 
Sex, Gender, and Reproduction, Inc.) 


sexual orientation is dependent on a person’s sexual feelings and behaviors and 
romantic feelings (Weinberg, Williams, & Pryor, 1991). 

What determines whether people become homosexual or heterosexual? Although 
there are a number of theories, none has proved completely satisfactory. 

Some explanations for sexual orientation are biological, suggesting that there are 
genetic causes. Evidence for a genetic origin of sexual orientation comes from studies 
of identical twins. The studies found that when one twin identified himself or herself 
as homosexual, the occurrence of homosexuality in the other twin was higher than it 
was in the general population. Such results occur even for twins who have been sepa- 
rated early in life and who therefore are not necessarily raised in similar social environ- 
ments (Hamer et al., 1993; Turner, 1995; Kirk, Bailey, & Martin, 2000; Gooren, 2006). 

Hormones also may play a role in determining sexual orientation. For example, 
research shows that women exposed before birth to DES (diethylstilbestrol—a drug 
their mothers took to avoid miscarriage) were more likely to be homosexual or bisex- 
ual (Meyer-Bahlburg, 1997). 

Some evidence suggests that differences in brain structures may be related to 
sexual orientation. For instance, the structure of the anterior hypothalamus, an area of 
the brain that governs sexual behavior, differs in male homosexuals and heterosexuals. 
Similarly, other research shows that, compared with heterosexual men or women, gay 
men have a larger anterior commissure, which is a bundle of neurons connecting the 
right and left hemispheres of the brain (LeVay, 1993; Byne, 1996; Witelson et al., 2008). 

However, research suggesting that biological causes are at the root of homosexu- 
ality is not conclusive because most findings are based on only small samples of 
individuals. Still, the possibility is real that some inherited or biological factor exists 
that predisposes people toward homosexuality if certain environmental conditions are 
met (Veniegas, 2000; Teodorov et al., 2002; Rahman, Kumari, & Wilson, 2003). 

Little evidence suggests that sexual orientation is brought about by child-rearing 
practices or family dynamics. Although proponents of psychoanalytic theories once 
argued that the nature of the parent-child relationship can produce homosexuality 
(e.g., Freud, 1922/1959), research evidence does not support such explanations (Isay, 
1994; Roughton, 2002). 

Another explanation for sexual orientation rests on learning theory (Masters & 
Johnson, 1979). According to this view, sexual orientation is learned through rewards 
and punishments in much the same way that we may learn to prefer swimming over 
tennis. For example, a young adolescent who had an unpleasant heterosexual expe- 
rience might develop disagreeable associations with the other sex. If the same person 
had a rewarding, pleasant gay or lesbian experience, homosexuality might be incor- 
porated into his or her sexual fantasies. If such fantasies are used during later sexual 
activities—such as masturbation—they may be positively reinforced through orgasm, 
and the association of homosexual behavior and sexual pleasure eventually may 
cause homosexuality to become the preferred form of sexual behavior. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 34 The Diversity of Sexual Behavior 369 





Although the learning-theory explanation is plausible, several difficulties rule it 
out as a definitive explanation. Because our society has traditionally held homosexu- 
ality in low esteem, one ought to expect that the negative treatment of homosexual 
behavior would outweigh the rewards attached to it. Furthermore, children growing 
up with a gay or lesbian parent are statistically unlikely to become homosexual, 
which thus contradicts the notion that homosexual behavior may be learned from 
others (Golombok, 1995; Victor & Fish, 1995; Tasker, 2005). 

Because of the difficulty in finding a consistent explanation for sexual orienta- 
tion, we can’t answer the question of what determines it. Thus, it seems unlikely that 
any single factor orients a person toward homosexuality or heterosexuality. Instead, 
it seems reasonable to assume that a combination of biological and environmental 
factors is involved (Bem, 1996; Hyde, Mezulis, & Abramson, 2008). 

Although we don’t know at this point exactly why people develop a certain 
sexual orientation, one thing is clear: There is no relationship between sexual orienta- 
tion and psychological adjustment. Gays, lesbians, and bisexuals generally enjoy the 
same quality of mental and physical health that heterosexuals do, although the dis- 
crimination they experience may produce higher rates of some disorders, such as 
depression (Poteat & Espelage, 2007). Bisexuals and homosexuals also hold equivalent 
ranges and types of attitudes about themselves that are independent of sexual orien- 
tation. For such reasons, the American Psychological Association and other major 
mental health organizations have endorsed efforts to eliminate discrimination against 
gays and lesbians, such as revoking the ban against homosexuals in the military 
(Cochran, 2000; Perez, DeBord, & Bieschke, 2000; Morris, Waldo, & Rothblum, 2001). 


Transsexualism 


Transsexuals are people who believe they were born with the body of the other 
gender. In fundamental ways, transsexualism represents less of a sexual difficulty 
than a gender issue involving one’s sexual identity (Meyerowitz, 2004; Heath, 2006). 

Transsexuals (like Chaz Bono, whose story was discussed at the beginning of the 
chapter) sometimes seek sex-change operations in which their existing genitals are 
surgically removed and the genitals of the desired sex are fashioned. Several steps, 
including intensive counseling, hormone injections, and living as a member of the 
desired sex for several years, precede surgery, which is, not surprisingly, highly 





Extensive research has found that 
bisexuals and homosexuals enjoy the 
same overall degree of mental and 
physical health as heterosexuals. 


y Alert 


The determinants of sexual 
orientation have proven 
difficult to pinpoint. It is 
important to know the 
variety of explanations that 
have been put forward. 


transsexuals People who believe they 
were born with the body of the other 
gender. 


370 Chapter 11 Sexuality and Gender 






































Sn 


© Robert Mankoff/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


“Frankly, I’ve repressed my sexuality so long I’ve : 4 
actually forgotten what my orientation is.” organs, or the organs are ambiguous. It is an extremely 


rare condition found in one in 4,500 births. Intersexism 
involves a complex mix of physiological and psychological issues (Lehrman, 2007; 
Diamond, 2009). 


rape The act by which one person 
forces another person to submit to 
sexual activity. 


date rape Rape in which the rapist 
is either a date or a romantic 
acquaintance. 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


complicated. The outcome, though, can be quite positive 
(O'Keefe & Fox, 2003; Stegerwald & Janson, 2003; Lobato, 
Koff, & Manenti, 2006). 

Transsexualism is part of a broader category known 
as transgenderism. The term transgenderism encompasses 
not only transsexuals, but also people who view them- 
selves as a third gender, transvestites (who dress in the 
clothes of the other gender), or others who believe that 
traditional male-female gender classifications inadequately 
characterize them (Prince, 2005; Hyde, Mezulis, & 
Abramson, 2008). 

Transsexuals are distinct from individuals who are 
known as intersex or by the older term hermaphrodite. An 
intersex person is born with an atypical combination of 
sexual organs or chromosomal or gene patterns. In some 
cases, they are born with both male and female sexual 


Sexual Difficulties 


When sex—an activity that should be pleasurable, joyful, and intimate—is forced on 
someone, it becomes one of the ultimate acts of aggression and brutality, and few crimes 
produce such profound and long-lasting consequences. But sexual crimes are not the 
only category of serious problems related to sex. Few personal difficulties produce as 
much anxiety, embarrassment, and even shame as sexually transmitted infections and 
sexual dysfunctions. We now turn to the major types of problems related to sex. 





RAPE 


Rape occurs when one person forces another person to submit to sexual activity such 
as intercourse or oral-genital sex. Although it usually applies to a male forcing a 
female, rape can occur when members of either sex are forced into sexual activities 
without their consent. 

Most people think of rape as a rare crime committed by strangers. Unfortunately, 
they are wrong on both counts. In fact, rape occurs far more frequently than is com- 
monly thought, and rapists are typically acquaintances of their victims. Although it is 
hard to obtain reliable estimates, most research suggests that there is a 14-25% chance 
that a woman will be the victim of a rape during her lifetime. Furthermore, more than 
200,000 sexual assaults directed against women are reported to police in the United 
States each year. However, only about one-third of rapes and sexual assaults are reported 
to police, so the actual figure is much higher (Wiehe & Richards, 1995; Catalano, 2006). 

Furthermore, a national survey conducted at 35 universities revealed the startling 
finding that one out of eight female college students reported having been raped. 
Among the women who had been raped, about half said the rapists were first dates, 
casual dates, or romantic acquaintances—a phenomenon called date rape. Girls in 
high school also suffer both sexual and physical abuse: In one survey, 9% of 14- to 
18-year-old girls said they had been forced into sexual activity by a date, and in more 
than half of those cases the incident had been accompanied by hitting, slapping, or 
shoving (Koss, 1993; Ackard & Neumark-Sztainer, 2002; Aiello, 2006). 

Women in some segments of society are more at risk for rape than are others. 
Although the likelihood of sexual assault is considerably lower among Latino 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 34 


women in comparison to non-Latino white women, the rate for black women is 
slightly higher in comparison to white women. Such racial differences may stem 
from differing cultural views of women and male dominance (Koss, 1993; Black 
& Weisz, 2004). 

Although on the surface it might appear that rape is primarily a sex crime, other 
types of motivation also underlie the behavior. In many cases, the rapist uses sex as 
a means of demonstrating power and control over the victim. In such cases, there is 
little that is sexually satisfying about rape to the rapist; instead, the pleasure comes 
from forcing someone else to be submissive (Gowaty, 2003; Yamawaki, Darby, & 
Queiroz, 2007). 

In other cases of rape, the primary motivation is anger. Sexual behavior is used 
to show the rapist’s rage at women in general, usually because of some perceived 
rejection or hurt that he has suffered in the past. Such rapes are likely to include 
physical violence and degrading acts against the victim. 

Some rapes are based on a desire for sexual gratification. Some men hold the 
attitude that it is appropriate and desirable for them to actively seek out sex. To them, 
sexual encounters represent a form of “war” between the sexes—with winners and 
losers—and violence is sometimes considered an appropriate way to obtain what 
they want. According to their reasoning, using force to obtain sexual gratification is 
permissible (Malamuth et al., 1995; Hall, 1996; Mann & Hollin, 2007). 

Finally, there is a common, although unfounded, societal belief that many women 
offer token resistance to sex, saying no to sex when they mean yes. If a man holds 
such a view, he may ignore a woman’s protestations that she doesn’t want sex 
(Anderson, Cooper, & Okamura, 1997; Yost & Zurbriggen, 2006). 

The repercussions of rape are devastating for the victims. During a rape, women 
experience fear, terror, and physical pain. Later, victims report shock, disbelief, panic, 
extreme anxiety, and suspiciousness—reactions that are sometimes intensified by 
implications that somehow the victim was to blame because of her style of dress or 
her presence in the wrong neighborhood. 

The psychological reactions to rape are no different whether it is date rape or 
rape by a stranger. These feelings may continue for years, even though the victim 
outwardly appears to have recovered. However, immediate psychological interven- 
tion, such as that provided by rape crisis centers, may help diminish the long-term 
reactions to rape (Monnier et al., 2002; Zaslow, 2003; Ullman & Townsend, 2007; 
Campbell, 2008). 


CHILDHOOD SEXUAL ABUSE 


One form of sexual behavior that is surprisingly common yet little understood is the 
sexual abuse of children. Although it is difficult to make accurate estimates of the 
true incidence of child sexual abuse because most cases go unreported, experts esti- 
mate that each year a half million children are sexually abused. Between 5-10% of 
boys and 20% of girls will be abused at some point (Villarosa, 2002; Finkelhor & 
Jones, 2004; see Figure 5). 

Who commits child sexual abuse? In most cases it is a relative or acquaintance; 
in only about one-quarter of the cases is the abuse carried out by a stranger. The 
most vulnerable age for being molested is between the ages of 7 and 13; the abusers 
tend to be about 20 years older than their victims. In most instances, the abuser is a 
male heterosexual. Furthermore, there are increasing cases of child predators using 
the web to make contact with potential victims (Wolfe, 1999; Finkelhor, Ormrod, & 
Turner, 2005). 

The short- and long-term consequences of childhood sexual abuse can be 
extremely damaging. In terms of initial effects, victims report fear, anxiety, depres- 
sion, anger, and hostility. Long-term effects may include depression, self-destructive 
behavior, feelings of isolation, poor self-esteem, and substance abuse. Although they 


The Diversity of Sexual Behavior 


371 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


372 Chapter 11 Sexuality and Gender 


VICTIMS 
Every year, 500,000 children i 
sexually abused. The peak 


| or 2 of every 20 boys will be abused 


eeveree eee 00 0 0 OO 
19999999990 
4 of every 20 girls will be abused. 


| 


sexually transmitted infection 


(STI) A disease acquired through 


sexual contact. 







States are 70 to 90 90% 


ABUSERS ea 10% of abusers 
are women 






percent of 
abuse is 
committed 
by a person 
who knows 
the child. 


90% are men 





70% 


Family members make up 88% 
one-third to one-half of of childhood 
abusers of girls, and 10 to rape cases go 
20 percent of abusers of boys. unreported 


FIGURE 5 Studies have found that the sexual abuse of children often is committed by a 
person who knows the child, with almost a third of the abusers being family members. 
(Source: Finkelhor, 2002.) 


may experience sexual difficulties later in life, the victims are not more likely to 
become sexual abusers themselves. Ultimately, the consequences of childhood sexual 
abuse are related to the specific nature of the abuse. Experiences involving fathers, 
genital contact, and the use of force are the most damaging (Berliner & Elliott, 2002; 
Penza, Heim, & Nemeroff, 2006; Buzi, Weinman, & Smith, 2007). 


SEXUALLY TRANSMITTED INFECTIONS (STIs) 


Millions of people suffer the discomfort—not to mention the psychological distress— 
of a sexually transmitted infection (STI), a disease acquired through sexual contact. 
Estimates suggest that one in five people in the United States is infected with some 
form of STI, and at least one in four will probably contract a STI during their life- 
times. The United States has the highest rate of sexually transmitted infections of all 
the economically developed countries in the world in part because people in the 
United States don’t talk about sex as frankly or receive as much sex education as 
people in other developed nations (see Figure 6; CDC, 2008). 
These are the major STIs: 


e Chlamydia. The most widespread STI is chlamydia, a disease that in women 
initially produces no symptoms and in men causes a burning sensation 
during urination and a discharge from the penis. If it is left untreated, 
chlamydia can lead to pelvic inflammation, urethral damage, arthritis, and 
even sterility. There are almost 3 million new cases each year in the United 
States. Because it usually produces no symptoms in females, the Centers for 
Disease Control recommends that sexually active women under age 26 
should be tested for the disease every year. Once diagnosed, chlamydia can 
be cured with antibiotics, most often azithromycin or doxycycline (Ku et al., 
2002; CDC, 2004a). 

e Genital herpes. Genital herpes is a virus related to the cold sores that sometimes 
appear around the mouth. Herpes first appears as small blisters or sores 
around the genitals that later break open, causing severe pain. These sores heal 
after a few weeks, but the disease usually reappears typically four or five 
times in the year following infection. Later outbreaks are less frequent, but the 
infection, which cannot be cured, often causes psychological distress for those 
who know they are infected. During the active phases of the disease, it can be 
transmitted to sexual partners. Genital herpes is common among college-age 
students: About 17% of 20- to 29-year-olds have the infection (Farrell, 2005). 

e Trichomoniasis. Trichomoniasis is an infection in the vagina or penis. Caused by 
a parasite, it is often without symptoms, especially in men. Eventually, it can 
cause painful urination and intercourse, a discharge from the vagina, itching, 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 34 The Diversity of Sexual Behavior 


and an unpleasant odor. The 5 million cases reported each year can be 
treated with antibiotics. 

Gonorrhea. Gonorrhea is one of the STIs that has been recognized the longest 
by scientists. It often has no symptoms but can produce a burning sensation 
during urination or a discharge from the penis or vagina. The infection 

can lead to fertility problems and, in women, pelvic inflammatory 

disease. Although antibiotics usually can cure gonorrhea, a number of 
drug-resistant strains of the disease are growing, making treatment more 
difficult. 

Syphilis. If untreated, syphilis may affect the brain, the heart, and a develop- 
ing fetus. It can even be fatal. Syphilis first reveals itself through a small 
sore at the point of sexual contact. In its secondary stage, it may include a 
rash. Syphilis can be treated successfully with antibiotics if it is diagnosed 
early enough. 

Genital warts. Another common STI is genital warts (caused by the human 
papilloma virus). Genital warts are small, lumpy warts that form on or near 
the penis or vagina. The warts are easy to diagnose because of their distinctive 
appearance: They look like small cauliflower bulbs. They usually form 

about two months after exposure and can be treated with a drug called 
metronidazole. 

AIDS. In the last two decades, no sexually transmitted infection has had a 
greater impact on sexual behavior—and society as a whole—than acquired 
immune deficiency syndrome (AIDS). Although in the United States AIDS at 
first was found primarily in gay men, it has spread to other populations, such 
as intravenous drug users and heterosexuals. In the United States, AIDS is the 
leading cause of death among men 25-44 years of age and the third leading 
cause of death among women in that age range. The worldwide figures are 
even more daunting: Already, 25 million people have died from AIDS, and 
people living with the disease number 33 million worldwide (Quinn & 
Overbaugh, 2005; UNAIDS, 2007). 


Number of new cases reported in 2007 
(numbers in thousands 


The spread of AIDS is particularly pronounced among women, who now com- 
prise almost half the cases worldwide. Younger women and women of color are 
particularly vulnerable. For instance, the rate of AIDS diagnosis is around 25 times 
higher for African-American women than for white women, and four times higher 
for Hispanic women (Quinn & Overbaugh, 2005). 

The extent of the AIDS epidemic has led to significant changes in sexual behav- 
ior. People are less likely to engage in “casual” sex with new acquaintances, and the 
use of condoms during sexual intercourse has increased. Nonetheless, the only fool- 
proof method of avoiding AIDS is total abstinence—an alternative that many people 
find unrealistic. However, there are several ways to reduce the risk of contracting 
AIDS as well as other sexually transmitted infections; these methods have come to 
be called “safer sex” practices (Carr, 2002): 


e Know your sexual partner—iwell. Before having sex with someone, learn about 


his or her sexual history. 

Use condoms. For those in sexual relationships, condoms are the most reliable 
means of preventing transmission of the AIDS virus. 

Avoid the exchange of bodily fluids, particularly semen. In particular, avoid anal 
intercourse. The AIDS virus can spread through small tears in the rectum, 
making anal intercourse without condoms particularly dangerous. Oral sex, 
once thought relatively safe, is now viewed as potentially dangerous. 

Stay sober. Using alcohol and drugs impairs judgment and can lead to poor 
decisions—and it makes using a condom correctly more difficult. 

Consider the benefits of monogamy. People in long-term, monogamous rela- 
tionships with partners who have been faithful are at a lower risk of 
contracting AIDS. 








I Gonorrhea 


E Syphilis 





373 


Hl Chlamydia 


E Genital herpes 
I Genital warts 
I Trichomoniasis 





FIGURE 6 Estimates of the numbers of 
new cases annually of sexually transmitted 
infections in the United States. There are 
many more new cases of chlamydia each 
year than any other sexually transmitted 


infection. (Source: CDC, 2008.) 


acquired immune deficiency 
syndrome (AIDS) A sexually 
transmitted infection caused by 
a virus that destroys the body’s 
immune system. 


y Alert 


Use Figure 6 to learn the 
major types of sexually 
transmitted infections 
and their prevalence. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


374 Chapter 11 Sexuality and Gender 


erectile dysfunction A male’s inability 
to achieve or maintain an erection. 


premature ejaculation A male’s 
inability to delay orgasm as long as 
he wishes. 


inhibited ejaculation A male’s 
inability to ejaculate when he wants 
to, if at all. 


anorgasmia (an-or-GAZ-mee-uh) 
A female’s lack of orgasm. 


inhibited sexual desire A sexual 
dysfunction in which the motivation 
for sexual activity is restrained or 
lacking entirely. 


SEXUAL PROBLEMS 


Few people would feel embarrassed by a sprained ankle or broken arm. In contrast, 
sexual difficulties are often a source of concern and self-consciousness because of the 
importance that society places on “desirable” sexual conduct. And such difficulties 
are surprisingly common, with more than 40% of women and about one-third of men 
experiencing problems associated with sexual performance. Among the most wide- 
spread are the disorders we discuss below (Laumann, Paik, & Rosen, 1999; Goldstein, 
2000; Rosner, 2001). 

Erectile dysfunction is a male’s inability to achieve or maintain an erection. The 
rare case is a male who has never been able to have an erection, and the more com- 
mon case is a male who, though now unable to have an erection, has had one at least 
once in the past. Erectile dysfunction is not an uncommon problem, even among 
younger men—about 5-10% of men under 50 have erection problems. It is the rare 
man who has never experienced it at least once during his lifetime. This is hardly 
surprising because the ability to achieve and hold an erection is sensitive to alcohol, 
drugs, performance fears, anxiety, and a host of other factors. Erectile dysfunction 
becomes a more serious problem when it occurs more than occasionally (Mendelsohn, 
2003; Peterson & Fuerst, 2007). 

Drugs such as Viagra have brought about significant advances in the treatment 
of erectile dysfunction and have brought the disorder into the open through a con- 
stant barrage of commercials on television and in magazines. Viagra treats erectile 
dysfunction by increasing the flow of blood through a man’s penis and producing 
an erection relatively quickly. 

In premature ejaculation, a male is unable to delay orgasm as long as he wishes. 
Because “as long as he wishes” is dependent on a man’s—and his partner’s— 
attitudes and opinions about how long is appropriate, this is a difficult disorder to 
diagnose. Sometimes the problem can be resolved simply by having a male redefine 
how long he wants to delay ejaculation. Premature ejaculation is most often a psy- 
chological problem since there are rarely physical reasons for it. One cause may be 
early sexual learning: Because sexual experiences during adolescence are often 
accompanied by a fear of being caught, some men learn early in their lives to reach 
orgasm as quickly as possible (Astbury-Ward, 2002; Barnes & Eardley, 2007; Sadeghi- 
Nejad & Watson, 2008). 

Inhibited ejaculation is the opposite problem. In this case, the male is unable to 
ejaculate when he wants to, if at all. Sometimes learning general relaxation tech- 
niques is sufficient to allow men to overcome the difficulty. 

Some women experience anorgasmia, or a lack of orgasm. In primary orgasmic 
dysfunction, a woman has never experienced orgasm. In secondary orgasmic dysfunction, 
a woman has had an orgasm at some point but no longer does or does so only under 
certain conditions—such as during masturbation—but not during sexual intercourse. 
Because the lack of orgasm during sexual intercourse is so common (some one-third 
of women report they do not receive sufficient stimulation to reach orgasm during 
sexual intercourse), this condition is not dysfunctional at all but is merely a normal 
variation of female sexuality (McCabe, 2009). 

Finally, inhibited sexual desire occurs when the motivation for sexual activity 
is restrained or lacking entirely. When people with inhibited sexual desire find them- 
selves in circumstances that typically would evoke sexual feelings, they begin to turn 
off sexually and may even experience a kind of “sexual anesthesia.” Ultimately they 
may begin to avoid situations of a sexual nature, thereby forgoing intimacy with 
others (Meston, 2003; Hertlein, Weeks, & Gambescia, 2007). 

It is important to realize that many of the problems we have discussed turn up 
at one time or another in most people’s sex lives. It is only when these problems 
persist, cause undue anxiety, and turn sex from play into work that they are cause 
for concern. Furthermore, treatments for common sexual problems have a good rate 
of success (Masters & Johnson, 1994; Rosner, 2001). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


As we have seen, surveys of college women make clear that the 
greatest danger of rape comes not from some unknown assailant 
but from a fellow student. There are ways, however, to reduce 
the likelihood of date rape. The following suggestions provide 
some guidance for women and men (Shultz, Scherman, & 
Marshall, 2000; American College Health Association, 2006; 
Watkins, 2007): 


Module 34 The Diversity of Sexual Behavior 375 


BECOMING AN 
INFORMED CONSUMER 


of Psychology 


Lowering the Risks of Date Rape 


e Women should believe in their rights to set limits and communicate them clearly, 


firmly, and early on. They should say no when they mean no. 


e Women should be assertive in refusing to engage in an activity in which they 
don’t want to engage. They should keep in mind that men may interpret passiv- 


ity as permission. 


e Women should be aware of situations in which they are at risk. 


e Women should keep in mind that some men interpret certain kinds of dress as 
sexually provocative, and not all men subscribe to the same standards of sexual 


behavior as they do. 


e Women should keep close tabs on what they are given to drink in social situations; 
victims of date rape have sometimes been given mind-altering “date-rape” drugs. 


e Men should be aware of their dates’ views on sexual behavior. 


e Men should not hold the view that the goal of dating is to “score.” 
e The word No should be understood to mean no and not be interpreted as an invi- 


tation to continue. 


e Men should not assume that certain kinds of dress or flirtatious behavior are an 


invitation to sex. 


e Both men and women should understand that alcohol and drugs cloud judgment 


and hinder communication between them. 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 


What is “normal” sexual behavior? 

e There are a number of approaches to determining nor- 
mality: deviation from the average, comparison of sexual 
behavior with some standard or ideal, and consideration 
of the psychological and physical consequences of the 
behavior to the person and to others. (p. 363) 


How do most people behave sexually? 

e The frequency of masturbation is high, particularly for 
males. Although attitudes toward masturbation are 
increasingly liberal, they have traditionally been 
negative even though no negative consequences have 
been detected. (p. 364) 

e Heterosexuality, or sexual attraction to members of the 
other sex, is the most common sexual orientation. (p. 365) 

e The double standard by which premarital sex is thought 
to be more permissible for men than for women has 
declined, particularly among young people. For many 


people, the double standard has been replaced by 
endorsement of “permissiveness with affection,” the 
view that premarital intercourse is permissible if it 
occurs in the context of a loving and committed relation- 
ship. (p. 365) 


e The frequency of marital sex varies widely. However, 


younger couples tend to have sexual intercourse more 
frequently than older ones. In addition, most men and 
women do not engage in extramarital sex. (p. 367) 
Homosexuals are sexually attracted to members of 
their own sex; bisexuals are sexually attracted to people 
of the same sex and the other sex. No explanation for 
why people become homosexual has been confirmed; 
among the possibilities are genetic or biological factors, 
childhood and family influences, and prior learning 
experiences and conditioning. However, no relationship 
exists between sexual orientation and psychological 
adjustment. (p. 367) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


376 Chapter 11 Sexuality and Gender 


How prevalent are rape and other forms of nonconsenting 
sex, and what are their causes? 

e Rape occurs when one person forces another person to 
submit to sexual activity. Often the victim is acquainted 
with the rapist. Sexual gratification is sometimes the mo- 
tivation for rape. More frequently, however, it is power, 
aggression, or anger. (p. 370) 

e Childhood sexual abuse is surprisingly widespread. 
Most often the perpetrator is an acquaintance or a family 
member. (p. 371) 


What are the major sexually transmitted infections? 

e Acquired immune deficiency syndrome, or AIDS, is 
bringing about profound changes in people’s sexual 
practices. Other sexually transmitted infections include 
chlamydia, genital herpes, trichomoniasis, gonorrhea, 
syphilis, and genital warts. (p. 372) 


What sexual difficulties do people most frequently encounter? 
e Among the major sexual problems males report are erec- 
tile dysfunction, premature ejaculation, and inhibited 
ejaculation. For females, the major problem is anorgas- 
mia, or a lack of orgasm. Both men and women may suf- 
fer from inhibited sexual desire. (p. 372) 


EVALUATE 


1. The work carried out by in the 1930s was the 
first systematic study of sexual behavior ever undertaken. 

2. Although the incidence of masturbation among young 
adults is high, once men and women become involved in 
intimate relationships, they typically cease masturbating. 
True or false? 

3. The increase in premarital sex in recent years has been 
greater for women than for men. True or false? 


KEY TERMS 


masturbation p. 364 
heterosexuality p. 365 
double standard p. 365 
extramarital sex p. 367 
homosexuals p. 367 
bisexuals p. 367 


transsexuals p. 369 

rape p. 370 

date rape p. 370 

sexually transmitted 
infection (STI) p. 372 


4. refers to the view that 
premarital sex is acceptable within a loving, long-term 
relationship. 

5. Research comparing homosexuals and heterosexuals 
clearly demonstrates that there is no difference in the level 
of adjustment or psychological functioning between the 
two groups. True or false? 

6. Which of the following is unlikely to be a motivation for 
the act of rape? 

a. Need for power 

b. Desire for sexual intimacy 

c. Desire for sexual gratification 

d. Anger against women in general 

7. Which of the following STIs is the most widespread? 
a. Genital herpes 
b. Gonorrhea 
c. Chlamydia 
d. Syphilis 





RETHINK 


1. What societal factors have led to a reduction in the double 
standard by which sexuality in men and women is re- 
garded differently? Do you think the double standard has 
completely vanished? 

2. From the perspective of a politician: What responsibilities do 
people who learn they have a sexually transmitted infec- 
tion have to their sexual partners, and what responsibili- 
ties do public health officials have? Should legislation be 
designed to restrict sexual behavior for those who have 
sexually transmitted infections? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 
2'4 ‘q°9 ‘andy *g SuOTDaFFe 
UIIM ssouaatsstutted ‘p fans} 'g ‘UOTeqIMseUT JO dupu, penuTyUOS 
e moys Sdrysuoyejar panew ut əjdoəd usaa ‘asyej *z ‘AOSUDY “TL 


anorgasmia (an-or-GAZ- 
mee-uh) p. 374 
inhibited sexual 
desire p. 374 


acquired immune deficiency 
syndrome (AIDS) p. 373 
erectile dysfunction p. 374 
premature ejaculation p. 374 
inhibited ejaculation p. 374 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Psychology on the Web 


1. Find at least two recent news articles or discussions on the web dealing with the 
issue of gender equality in school or the workplace, especially unequal treatment of 
individuals because of their gender. Summarize in writing what you found, and 
discuss your own attitudes toward this issue. 

2. Find information on the web about date rape, including guidelines or suggestions 
published by other colleges for preventing rape among students. Summarize your 
findings, including recommendations for behavioral or policy changes that might be 
effective at your college. If your college’s policies toward or publications about this 
topic could benefit from your findings, bring them to the attention of the appropriate 
office at your institution and suggest specific changes. 


E © | | OQ U @ We have seen how psychologists are dealing with the 


topics of gender and sex, first with regard to gender 
roles, gender stereotyping, gender schemas, and sexism. Society's expectations regarding 
male and female characteristics and behaviors lead to inequities in the treatment of men 
and women in school, the workplace, and the rest of society—inequities that largely 
favor men. 

With regard to sexuality, we discussed the nature of the physical processes that 
surround sexuality and addressed the controversial issue of “normality” in sexual behav- 
ior. We saw the broad diversity of sexual behavior, and we discussed premarital sex and 
marital sex, as well as heterosexuality, homosexuality, and bisexuality. We also examined 
rape and other forms of nonconsenting sex, including childhood sexual abuse. 

Finally, we looked at the sexual problems that people may have, including STIs and 
sexual dysfunctions. We explored the nature of these problems and discussed ways to 
treat and deal with them. 

Before we turn to the subject of human development in the next few modules, 
return to the prologue of this chapter concerning Chaz Bono's sense that she is a male 
trapped in a female's body. Using your knowledge of gender and sex, consider the 
following questions. 





1. Why might Bono's family, friends, and fans react negatively to her decision to 
become a man? 

2. How is Bono's transsexualism different from lesbianism? 

3. Why do you think Bono believed herself to be a lesbian before deciding that she was 
really a man? 

4. What difficulties might Bono encounter in her journey to become male? 





377 


ld 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Key Concepts for Chapter 12 





MODULE 35 


How do psychologists study the degree to which Nature and Nurture: The Enduring 


: ‘ . . Developmental Issue 
development is an interaction of hereditary and 


Determining the Relative Influence of 


environmental factors? Na ene 
Developmental Research Techniques 







What is the nature of development before birth? Prenatal Development: 
Conception to Birth 


The Basics of Genetics 


@ What factors affect a child during the mother’s 










pregna ncy? The Earliest Development 


MODULE 37 








What are the major competencies of newborns? Infancy and Childhood 


, , The Extraordinary Newb 
@ What are the milestones of physical and social R A 
Neuroscience in Your Life: Young Infants 


Recognize Emotions Early in Life 
Infancy Through Childhood 
PsychWork: Child Protection Caseworker 


development during childhood? @ How does 


cognitive development proceed during childhood? 


MODULE 38 







What major physical, social, and cognitive transitions Adolescence: Becoming an Adult 


. Physical Development: 

? 
characterize adolescence? The Chanoag Adeles 
Moral and Cognitive Development: 
Distinguishing Right from Wrong 
Social Development: Finding One's Self 
in a Social World 
Exploring Diversity: Rites of Passage: 
Coming of Age Around the World 


MODULE 39 





Adulthood 
Physical Development: The Peak of Health 


What are the principal kinds of physical, social, 


and intellectual changes that occur in early and i i l 
Social Development: Working at Life 


middle adulthood, and what are their causes? @ Marriage, Children, and Divorce: Family Ties 


; š Changing Roles of Men and Women 
How does the reality of late adulthood differ from An . 

Later Years of Life: Growing Old 
the stereotypes about that period? @ How can we adjust to death? Physical Changes in Late 


Adulthood: The Aging Body 


Cognitive Changes: Thinking About— 
and During—Late Adulthood 


Applying Psychology in the 21st 
Century: Gaming in Late Adulthood: 
How Video Games May Improve 
Cognitive Functioning in Older Adults 


Becoming an Informed Consumer of 
Psychology: Adjusting to Death 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Prologue Twice-Born Baby 





Keri and Chad McCartney were looking forward to learning 
whether their baby was a boy or agirl. But the moment an image 
swam onto the screen at a Texas doctor's office last February 15, 
the technician fell silent. When Keri, 40, and Chad, 39, a pastor, 
peered closer, they saw a bulge below the fetus. “What's that?” 


they asked. “Well, it’s a mass,’ came the answer. 
\ 


\ 





But Keri and Chad’s daughter Marcie did survive. In a risky inter- 
vention, surgeons temporarily removed their daughter from Keri's 
uterus and cut out most of the tumor. They then put her back 
into her mother’s body to finish developing normally until she 
finally could be delivered—a second time—ten weeks later. 

The medical advances that permitted Marcie to survive are 
just some of the many that have improved children’s lives, not 
only in the prenatal period but throughout their life span. Marcie’s 
story also serves as an introduction to one of the broadest and 
most important areas of psychology: developmental psychology. 
Developmental psychology is the branch of psychology that 
studies the patterns of growth and change that occur through- 
out life. It deals with issues ranging from new ways of conceiving 
children, to learning how to raise children most sensibly, to 
understanding the milestones of life that we all face. 

Developmental psychologists study the interaction between 
the unfolding of biologically predetermined patterns of behavior 
and a constantly changing, dynamic environment. They ask how 
our genetic background affects our behavior throughout our 


380 


Then Dr. Debra Williams delivered difficult news: The tailbone 
attachment was a rare tumor, present in 1 of 35,000 pregnancies, 
that draws on the fetal blood supply. The baby’s odds of survival? 
Less than 10%. (Smolowe, 2008, p. 96) 


Ahead 





lives and whether heredity limits our potential. Similarly, they 
seek to understand the way in which the environment works 
with—or against—our genetic capabilities, how the world we 
live in affects our development, and how we can be encouraged 
to reach our full potential. 

We begin by examining the approaches psychologists use to 
study development change across the life span. Then we consider 
the very start of development, beginning with conception and 
the nine months of life before birth. We look at both genetic and 
environmental influences on the unborn individual and the way 
they can affect behavior throughout the remainder of the life 
cycle. 

Next, we examine development that occurs after birth: the 
enormous and rapid growth that takes place during the early 
stages of life and the physical, social, and cognitive change 
throughout infancy, toddlerhood, and middle childhood. We 
then move on to development from adolescence through 
adulthood. We end with a discussion of the ways in which 
people prepare themselves for death. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 35 


How many bald, six-foot-six, 250-pound volunteer firefighters in New Jersey wear 
droopy mustaches, aviator-style eyeglasses, and a key ring on the right side of the 
belt? The answer is two: Gerald Levey and Mark Newman. They are twins who were 
separated at birth. Each twin did not even know the other existed until they were 
reunited—in a fire station—by a fellow firefighter who knew Newman and was startled 
to see his double, Levey, at a firefighters’ convention. 

The lives of the twins, although separate, took remarkably similar paths. Levey 


How do psychologists study 
the degree to which develop- 
ment is an interaction of 
hereditary and environmental 
factors? 


went to college and studied forestry; Newman planned to study forestry in college 
but instead took a job trimming trees. Both had jobs in supermarkets. One had a job 


installing sprinkler systems; the other installed fire alarms. 


Both men are unmarried and find the same kind of woman—“tall, slender, long 


hair”—attractive. They share similar hobbies and enjoy hunting, fishing, going to the 


beach, and watching old John Wayne movies and professional wrestling. Both like 


Chinese food and drink the same brand of beer. Their mannerisms are also similar— 


for example, each one throws his head back when he laughs. And, of course, there is 


one more thing: They share a passion for fighting fires. 


The similarities we see in twins Gerald Levey and Mark Newman vividly raise one 
of the fundamental questions posed by developmental psychology, the study of the 


patterns of growth and change that occur throughout life. The question is this: How 
can we distinguish between the environmental causes of behavior (the influence of 
parents, siblings, family, friends, schooling, nutrition, and all the other experiences 


The branch of psychology that studies 
the patterns of growth and change 
that occur throughout life. 


to which a child is exposed) and hereditary causes (those based on an individual’s 
genetic makeup that influence growth and development throughout life)? This ques- 


tion embodies the nature-nurture issue. In this context, nature refers to hereditary 


factors, and nurture refers to environmental influences. 

Although the question was first posed as a nature-versus- 
nurture issue, developmental psychologists today agree that 
both nature and nurture interact to produce specific develop- 
mental patterns and outcomes. Consequently, the question has 
evolved into this: How and to what degree do environment and 
heredity both produce their effects? No one develops free of 
environmental influences or without being affected by his or 
her inherited genetic makeup. However, the debate over the 
comparative influence of the two factors remains active; dif- 
ferent approaches and different theories of development 
emphasize the environment or heredity to a greater or lesser 
degree (Gottesman & Hanson, 2005; Rutter, 2006; Belsky & 
Pluess, 2009). 

For example, some developmental theories rely on basic 
psychological principles of learning and stress the role learn- 
ing plays in producing changes in a developing child’s behav- 
ior. Such theories emphasize the role of the environment in 
development. In contrast, other developmental theories 
emphasize the influence of one’s physiological makeup and 


The issue of the 
degree to which environment and 
heredity influence behavior. 






a 


Gerald Levey and Mark Newman 


381 


382 Chapter 12 Development 


FIGURE 1 Characteristics influenced 
significantly by genetic factors. Although 
these characteristics have strong genetic 
components, they are also affected by 
environmental factors. 


y Alert 


The nature—nurture issue is a 
key question that is pervasive 
throughout the field of 
psychology. It explores 
how and to what degree 
environment and heredity 
produce their joint effects. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Emotional 
Physical Intellectual Characteristics 
Characteristics Characteristics and Disorders 
Height Memory Shyness 
Weight Intelligence Extraversion 
Obesity Age of language acquisition Emotionality 
Tone of voice Reading disability Neuroticism 


Blood pressure Mental retardation Schizophrenia 





Tooth decay 

Athletic ability 
Firmness of handshake 
Age of death 

Activity level 


Anxiety 
Alcoholism 


functioning on development. Such theories stress the role of heredity and maturation— 
the unfolding of biologically predetermined patterns of behavior—in producing 
developmental change. Maturation can be seen, for instance, in the development of 
sex characteristics (such as breasts and body hair) that occurs at the start of adoles- 
cence. Furthermore, the work of behavioral geneticists, who study the effects of 
heredity on behavior, and the theories of evolutionary psychologists, who identify 
behavior patterns that result from our genetic inheritance, have influenced develop- 
mental psychologists. Both behavioral geneticists and evolutionary psychologists 
have highlighted the importance of heredity in influencing human behavior (Buss, 
2003; Reif & Lesch, 2003; Iies, Arvey, & Bouchard, 2006). 

Despite their differences over theory, developmental psychologists concur on 
some points. They agree that genetic factors not only provide the potential for specific 
behaviors or traits to emerge, but also place limitations on the emergence of such 
behavior or traits. For instance, heredity defines people’s general level of intelligence 
and sets an upper limit that—regardless of the quality of the environment—people 
cannot exceed. Heredity also places limits on physical abilities; humans simply can- 
not run at a speed of 60 miles an hour or grow as tall as 10 feet, no matter the qual- 
ity of their environment (Pinker, 2004; Dodge, 2004). 

Figure 1 lists some of the characteristics most affected by heredity. As you con- 
sider these items, it is important to keep in mind that these characteristics are not 
entirely determined by heredity, for environmental factors also play a role. 

Developmental psychologists also agree that in most instances environmental 
factors play a critical role in enabling people to reach the potential capabilities that 
their genetic background makes possible. If Albert Einstein had received no intel- 
lectual stimulation as a child and had not been sent to school, it is unlikely that he 
would have reached his genetic potential. Similarly, a great athlete such as baseball 
star Derek Jeter would have been unlikely to display much physical skill if he had 
not been raised in an environment that nurtured his innate talent and gave him the 
opportunity to train and perfect his natural abilities. 

Clearly, the relationship between heredity and environment is far from simple. 
As a consequence, developmental psychologists typically take an interactionist 
position on the nature—nurture issue by suggesting that a combination of heredi- 
tary and environmental factors influences development. Developmental psycholo- 
gists face the challenge of identifying the relative strength of each of these 
influences on the individual as well as that of identifying the specific changes that 
occur over the course of development (McGregor & Capone, 2004; Moffitt, Caspi, 
& Rutter, 2006). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 35 Nature and Nurture: The Enduring Developmental Issue 383 


Determining the Relative 
Influence of Nature and Nurture 


Developmental psychologists use several approaches to determine the relative influ- 
ence of genetic and environmental factors on behavior. In one approach, researchers 
can experimentally control the genetic makeup of laboratory animals by carefully 
breeding them for specific traits. For instance, by observing animals with identical 
genetic backgrounds placed in varied environments, researchers can learn the effects 
of specific kinds of environmental stimulation. Although researchers must be careful 
when generalizing the findings of nonhuman research to a human population, find- 
ings from animal research provide important information that cannot be obtained for 
ethical reasons by using human participants. 

Human twins serve as another important source of information about the rela- 
tive effects of genetic and environmental factors. If identical twins (those who are 
genetically identical) display different patterns of development, those differences 
have to be attributed to variations in the environment in which the twins were 
raised. The most useful data come from identical twins (such as Gerald Levey and 
Mark Newman) who are adopted at birth by different sets of adoptive parents and 
raised apart in differing environments. Studies of nontwin siblings who are raised 
in totally different environments also shed some light on the issue. Because they 
have relatively similar genetic backgrounds, siblings who show similarities as adults 
provide strong evidence for the importance of heredity (Sternberg, 2002a; Vitaro, 
Brendgen, & Arseneault, 2009). 

Researchers can also take the opposite tack. Instead of concentrating on people 
with similar genetic backgrounds who are raised in different environments, they 
may consider people raised in similar environments who have totally dissimilar 
genetic backgrounds. For example, if they find similar courses of development in 
two adopted children who have different genetic backgrounds and have been 
raised in the same family, they have evidence for the importance of environmental 
influences on development. Moreover, psychologists can carry out research involv- 
ing animals with dissimilar genetic backgrounds; by experimentally varying the 
environment in which they are raised, they can determine the influence of envi- 
ronmental factors (independent of heredity) on development (Petrill & Deater- 
Deckard, 2004). 





Developmental Research 
Techniques 


Because of the demands of measuring behavioral change across different ages, devel- 
opmental researchers use several unique methods. The most frequently used, 
cross-sectional research, compares people of different ages at the same point in time. 
Cross-sectional studies provide information about differences in development 
between different age groups (Creasey, 2005; Huijie, 2006). 

Suppose, for instance, we were interested in the development of intellectual abil- 
ity in adulthood. To carry out a cross-sectional study, we might compare a sample 
of 25-, 45-, and 65-year-olds who all take the same IQ test. We then can determine 
whether average IQ test scores differ in each age group. 

Cross-sectional research has limitations, however. For instance, we cannot be 
sure that the differences in IQ scores we might find in our example are due to age 
differences alone. Instead, the scores may reflect differences in the educational 
attainment of the cohorts represented. A cohort is a group of people who grow up 
at similar times, in similar places, and in similar conditions. In the case of IQ 





identical twins Twins who are 
genetically identical. 


cross-sectional research A research 
method that compares people of 
different ages at the same point in time. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


384 Chapter 12 Development 


differences, any age differences we find in a cross-sectional study may reflect edu- 
cational differences among the cohorts studied: People in the older age group may 
belong to a cohort that was less likely to attend college than were the people in the 
younger groups. 

A longitudinal study, the second major research strategy developmental psy- 
chologists use, provides one way around this problem. Longitudinal research traces 
the behavior of one or more participants as the participants age. Longitudinal stud- 
ies assess change in behavior over time, whereas cross-sectional studies assess differ- 
ences among groups of people. 

For instance, consider how we might investigate intellectual development during 
adulthood by using a longitudinal research strategy. First, we might give an IQ test 
to a group of 25-year-olds. We’d then come back to the same people 20 years later 
and retest them at age 45. Finally, we’d return to them once more when they were 
65 years old and test them again. 

By examining changes at several points in time, we can clearly see how indi- 
viduals develop. Unfortunately, longitudinal research requires an enormous expen- 
diture of time as the researcher waits for the participants to get older. Participants 
who begin a study at an early age may drop out, move away, or even die as the 
research continues. Moreover, participants who take the same test at several points 
in time may become “test-wise” and perform better each time they take it because 
they have become more familiar with the test. 

To make up for the limitations in both cross-sectional and longitudinal research, 
investigators have devised an alternative strategy. Known as sequential research, it 
combines cross-sectional and longitudinal approaches by examining a number of 
different age groups at several points in time. For example, investigators might exam- 
ine a group of 3-, 5-, and 7-year-olds every six months for a period of several years. 
This technique allows a developmental psychologist to tease out the specific effects 
of age changes from other possibly influential factors. 


longitudinal research A research 
method that investigates behavior as 
participants age. 


ly Alert 


Be sure to be able to distin- 
guish the three different 
types of developmental 
research—cross-sectional 
(comparing people of 
different ages at the same time); 
longitudinal (studying participants 
as they age); and sequential 
(a combination of cross-sectional 
and longitudinal). 






sequential research A research 
method that combines cross-sectional 
and longitudinal research by consider- 
ing a number of different age groups 
and examining them at several points 
in time. 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP EVALUATE 


How do psychologists study the degree to which development 
is an interaction of hereditary and environmental factors? 
e Developmental psychology studies growth and change 2. 


1. Developmental psychologists are interested in the effects 
of both and on development. 
Environment and heredity both influence development 





throughout life. One fundamental question is how much 
developmental change is due to heredity and how much 
is due to environment—the nature-nurture issue. 
Heredity seems to define the upper limits of our growth 
and change, whereas the environment affects the degree 
to which the upper limits are reached. (p. 381) 

e Cross-sectional research compares people of different 
ages with one another at the same point in time. In con- 
trast, longitudinal research traces the behavior of one or 
more participants as the participants become older. 
Finally, sequential research combines the two methods 
by examining several different age groups at several 
points in time. (p. 383) 


with genetic potentials generally establishing limits on 
environmental influences. True or false? 


. By observing genetically similar animals in differing envi- 


ronments, we can increase our understanding of the influ- 
ences of hereditary and environmental factors in humans. 
True or false? 

research studies the same individuals over a 
period of time, whereas research 
studies people of different ages at the same time. 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 35 Nature and Nurture: The Enduring Developmental Issue 385 


RETHINK genetic influences might be involved? What recommen- 


dations might you make to the child’s parents about the 
1. When researchers find similarities in development i RA aget k 


between very different cultures, what implications might net! 
such findings have for the nature-nurture issue? Answers to Evaluate Questions 

2. From the perspective of a child-care provider: Consider what Teuoyoəs-ssox1 ‘Teurpnysuo] ‘p 
factors might determine why a child is not learning to ‘ony ‘g ‘ony z ‘(əmpnu Jo) zuəwuoauə ‘(əmzeu 10) ÁyrpəIəy 'T 


walk at the same pace as his peers. What kinds of envi- 
ronmental influences might be involved? What kinds of 


KEY TERMS 


developmental psychology p. 381 identical twins p. 383 longitudinal research p. 384 
nature—-nurture issue p. 381 cross-sectional research p. 383 sequential research p. 384 


MODULE 36 


What is the nature of 
development before birth? 


What factors affect a child 
during the mother’s 
pregnancy? 


Rod-shaped structures 
that contain all basic hereditary 
information. 

The parts of the chromosomes 
through which genetic information is 
transmitted. 


y Alert 


It's important to understand 
the basic building-blocks of 
genetics: chromosomes, 
which contain genes, 
which in turn are com- 
posed of sequences of DNA. 
386 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


When the Morrisons were expecting their second child, the young couple faced an 
anguishing dilemma. 

Their first child, a girl born in 2002, had a condition known as congential adrenal 
hyperplasia, or CAH, which can sometimes result in male-like genitals in female 
newborns. So when Mrs. Morrison became pregnant again, the couple was well aware 
the baby had a 1-in-8 chance of being born with the same disorder. 

There were choices. They could treat the fetus with a powerful steroid that would 
most likely avert the possibility of the genitals becoming malformed. But the couple 
wotried about doing this. There was little research on the long-term effects of treating a 
fetus with steroids, and statistically, there was a much greater chance that the baby 
wouldn't have the genital problem at all... . 

The couple decided to forgo the steroid treatment. “It was touch-and-go, but in the 
end I couldn’t expose the baby to the drugs,” says Mrs. Morrison. When the baby arrived, 
it was a girl and, like her older sister, was born with swollen genitalia (Naik, 2009, p. D1). 


The Morrisons will never know if they made the right choice. But their case shows 
the difficult choices that parents may encounter due to our increasing understanding 
of life spent inside a mother’s womb. 

Yet, our knowledge of the biology of conception—when a male’s sperm cell pen- 
etrates a female’s egg cell—and its aftermath makes the start of life no less of a 
miracle. Let’s consider how an individual is created by looking first at the genetic 
endowment that a child receives at the moment of conception. 


The Basics of Genetics 


The one-cell entity established at conception contains 23 pairs of chromosomes, rod- 
shaped structures that contain all basic hereditary information. One member of each 
pair is from the mother, and the other is from the father. 

Each chromosome contains thousands of genes—smaller units through which 
genetic information is transmitted. Either individually or in combination, genes pro- 
duce each person’s particular characteristics. Composed of sequences of DNA (deoxy- 
ribonucleic acid) molecules, genes are the biological equivalent of “software” that 
programs the future development of all parts of the body’s hardware. Humans have 
some 25,000 different genes (see Figure 1). 

Some genes control the development of systems common to all members of the 
human species—the heart, circulatory system, brain, lungs, and so forth; others shape 
the characteristics that make each human unique, such as facial configuration, height, 
and eye color. The child’s sex is also determined by a particular combination of genes. 
Specifically, a child inherits an X chromosome from its mother and either an X or a 
Y chromosome from its father. When it receives an XX combination, it is a female; 
with an XY combination, it develops as a male. Male development is triggered by a 
single gene on the Y chromosome; without the presence of that specific gene, the 
individual will develop as a female (see Figure 2). 

As behavioral geneticists have discovered, genes are also at least partially respon- 
sible for a wide variety of personal characteristics, including cognitive abilities, 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 36 Prenatal Development: Conception to Birth 387 





(a) Conception (b) 23 pairs of chromosomes (c) DNA sequence (d) Genes 
FIGURE 1 Every individual’s characteristics are determined by the individual’s specific genetic 

information. (a) At the moment of conception, (b) humans receive 23 pairs of chromosomes, 

half from the mother and half from the father. (c) These chromosomes are made up of coils 

of DNA. (d) Each chromosome contains thousands of genes that “program” the future 

development of the body. 


personality traits, and psychological disorders. Of course, few of these characteristics 
are determined by a single gene. Instead, most traits result from a combination of 
multiple genes that operate together with environmental influences (Plomin & 
McGuffin, 2003; Haberstick et al., 2005; Ramus, 2006). 


THE HUMAN GENOME PROJECT 


Our understanding of genetics took a giant leap forward in 2001, when scientists 
were able to map the specific location and sequence of every human gene as part 
of the massive Human Genome Project. The accomplishment was one of the most 


FIGURE 2 When an egg and a sperm 
Female meet at the moment of fertilization, the 
egg provides an X chromosome, and 
the sperm provides either an X or aY 
chromosome. If the sperm contributes 


The 23rd pai 
e 23rd pair an X chromosome, the child will have 


of chromosomes 


Uren pair of the sperm is an XX pairing on the 23rd chromosome 
of chromosomes comprised of i i i 

fe beatae M RE and will be a girl. If the sperm contributes 
comprised of XY a Y chromosome, the result will be an 
chromosomes XY pairing—a boy. 


XX 


‘x 


3 a 


Baby girl Baby boy 





388 Chapter 12 Development 





The complex sequence of genes can be 
harnessed in gene therapy in which 
genes are used to correct health 
problems. 


zygote The new cell formed by the 
union of an egg and sperm. 


embryo A developed zygote that has a 
heart, a brain, and other organs. 


fetus A developing individual from 
eight weeks after conception until 
birth. 


age of viability The point at which a 
fetus can survive if born prematurely. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


important in the history of biology (International Human Genome Sequencing 
Consortium, 2003). 

The success of the Human Genome Project started a revolution in health care 
because scientists can identify the particular genes responsible for genetically caused 
disorders. It is already leading not only to the identification of risk factors in chil- 
dren, but also to the development of new treatments for physical and psychological 
disorders. 

For example, in gene therapy, health-care providers inject genes to correct par- 
ticular diseases directly into a patient’s bloodstream. When the gene arrives at the 
location of a problem (or potential problem), it leads the body to produce chemicals 
that can alleviate the danger. In other cases, additional genes are inserted that replace 
missing or defective cells. Eventually, it also may be possible to “harvest” defective 
cells from a child prior to birth. These cells could be treated by gene therapy and 
reintroduced into the unborn child to repair the defect (Lymberis et al., 2004; Sato, 
Shimamura, & Takeuchi, 2007; Naldini, 2009). 

Although the promise of gene therapy is real, the number of diseases that can 
be treated today is fairly limited. Furthermore, the long-term success of gene ther- 
apy remains unknown. In fact, after they initially seem to be cured, some recipients 
of gene therapy have relapsed, and some have suffered from unpleasant side 
effects. Still, the potential uses of gene therapy are growing rapidly. For example, 
such disorders as AIDS, cancer, rheumatoid arthritis, and macular degeneration are 
strong candidates for the procedure (Feret et al., 2007; Rossi, June, & Kohn, 2007; 
Miller et al., 2008). 





The Earliest Development 


When an egg becomes fertilized by the sperm, the resulting one-celled entity, called 
a zygote, immediately begins to develop. The zygote starts out as a microscopic 
speck. Three days after fertilization, though, the zygote increases to around 32 cells; 
within a week it has grown to 100-150 cells. These first two weeks are known as the 
germinal period. 

Two weeks after conception, the developing individual enters the embryonic 
period, which lasts from week 2 through week 8; he or she is now called an embryo. 
As an embryo develops through an intricate, preprogrammed process of cell divi- 
sion, it grows 10,000 times larger by 4 weeks of age and attains a length of about 
one-fifth of an inch. At this point it has developed a rudimentary beating heart, a 
brain, an intestinal tract, and a number of other organs. Although all these organs 
are at a primitive stage of development, they are clearly recognizable. Moreover, 
by week 8, the embryo is about an inch long and has discernible arms, legs, and 
a face. 

From week 8 and continuing until birth, the developing individual enters the 
fetal period and is called a fetus. At the start of this period, it begins to respond to 
touch; it bends its fingers when touched on the hand. At 16 to 18 weeks, its move- 
ments become strong enough for the mother to sense them. At the same time, hair 
may begin to grow on its head, and the facial features become similar to those the 
child will display at birth. The major organs begin functioning, although the fetus 
could not be kept alive outside the mother. In addition, a lifetime’s worth of brain 
neurons are produced—although it is unclear whether the brain is capable of think- 
ing at this early stage. 

By week 24, a fetus has many of the characteristics it will display as a newborn. 
In fact, when an infant is born prematurely at this age, it can open and close its eyes; 
suck; cry; look up, down, and around; and even grasp objects placed in its hands. 

Within the womb the fetus continues to develop before birth. It begins to grow fatty 
deposits under the skin, and it gains weight. The fetus reaches the age of viability, the 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 36 Prenatal Development: Conception to Birth 389 





These remarkable photos of live fetuses display the degree of physical development at 
prenatal ages 4 and 15 weeks. 


point at which it can survive if born prematurely, at about prenatal age 22 weeks. At 
prenatal age 28 weeks, the fetus weighs less than 3 pounds and is about 16 inches long. 
It may be capable of learning: One study found that the infants of mothers who had 
repeatedly read aloud The Cat in the Hat by Dr. Seuss before the infants’ birth preferred 
the sound of that particular story to other stories after they were born (Spence & 
DeCasper, 1982; Schenone et al., 2010). 

Before birth, a fetus passes through several sensitive periods. A sensitive period is 
the time when organisms are particularly susceptible to certain kinds of stimuli. For 
example, fetuses are especially affected by their mothers’ use of drugs during certain 
sensitive periods before birth. If they are exposed to a particular drug before or after 
the sensitive period, it may have relatively little impact; if exposure comes during a 
critical period, the impact will be significant (Konig, 2005; Werker & Tees, 2005; 
Uylings, 2006). 

Sensitive periods can also occur after birth. Some language specialists suggest, 
for instance, that there is a period in which children are particularly receptive to 
developing language. If children are not exposed to appropriate linguistic stimuli, 
their language development may be impaired (Innocenti, 2007; Sohr-Preston & Scar- 
amella, 2006). In the final weeks of pregnancy, the fetus continues to gain weight 
and grow. At the end of the normal 38 weeks of pregnancy, the fetus typically 
weighs 7 pounds and is about 20 inches in length. However, the story is different 
for preterm infants, who are born before week 38. Because they have not been able 
to develop fully, they are at higher risk for illness, future problems, and even death. 
For infants who have been in the womb for more than 30 weeks, the prospects are 
relatively good. However, for those born before week 30, the story is often less 
positive. Such newborns, who may weigh as little as 2 pounds at birth, are in grave 
danger because they have immature organs; they have less than a 50-50 chance of 
survival. If they do survive—and it takes extraordinarily heroic (and expensive) 
medical intervention to assure this—they may later experience significant develop- 
mental delays. 


GENETIC INFLUENCES ON THE FETUS 


The process of fetal growth that we have just described reflects normal development, 
which occurs in 95-98% of all pregnancies. Some individuals are less fortunate; in 
the remaining 2-5% of cases, children are born with serious birth defects. A major 






y Alert 
Critical (or sensitive) periods, 
which can occur before or 
after birth, are important 
because they indicate the 
time that organisms are 
particularly susceptible to 
damage that may affect them for 
the rest of their lives. 


390 Chapter 12 Development 


teratogens Environmental agents such 
as a drug, chemical, virus, or other 
factor that produce a birth defect. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


cause of such defects is faulty genes or chromosomes. Here are some of the more 
common genetic and chromosomal difficulties. 


e Phenylketonuria (PKU). A child born with the inherited disease phenylketonuria 
cannot produce an enzyme that is required for normal development. This 
deficiency results in an accumulation of poisons that eventually cause 
profound mental retardation. The disease is treatable, however, if it is caught 
early. Most infants today are routinely tested for PKU, and children with the 
disorder can be placed on a special diet that allows them to develop normally 
(levers-Landis et al., 2005; Christ, Steiner, & Grange, 2006; Widaman, 2009). 

e Sickle-cell anemia. About 10% of the African-American population has the 
possibility of passing on sickle-cell anemia, a disease that gets its name from 
the abnormally shaped red blood cells it causes. Children with the disease 
may have episodes of pain, yellowish eyes, stunted growth, and vision 
problems (Taras & Potts-Datema, 2005; Selove, 2007). 

e Tay-Sachs disease. Children born with Tay-Sachs disease, a disorder most often 
found in Jews of Eastern European ancestry, usually die by age 3 or 4 because 
of the body’s inability to break down fat. If both parents carry the genetic 
defect that produces the fatal illness, their child has a 1 in 4 chance of being 
born with the disease (Leib et al., 2005; Weinstein, 2007). 

e Down syndrome. Down syndrome, one of the causes of mental retardation, 
occurs when the zygote receives an extra chromosome at the moment of 
conception. Down syndrome is often related to the mother’s age; mothers over 
35 and younger than 18 stand a higher risk than other women of having a 
child with the syndrome (Roizen & Patterson, 2003; Sherman et al., 2007). 


PRENATAL ENVIRONMENTAL INFLUENCES 


Genetic factors are not the only causes of difficulties in fetal development. Environmen- 
tal influences—the nurture part of the nature-nurture equation—also affect the fetus. 
Some of the more profound consequences are brought about by teratogens, environ- 
mental agents such as a drug, chemical, virus, or other factor that produce a birth defect. 
Among the major prenatal environmental influences on the fetus are the following: 


e Mother's nutrition. What a mother eats during her pregnancy can have impor- 
tant implications for the health of her baby. Seriously undernourished mothers 
cannot provide adequate nutrition to a growing fetus, and they are likely to 
give birth to underweight babies. Poorly nourished babies are also more 
susceptible to disease, and a lack of nourishment may have an adverse impact 
on their mental development (Zigler, Finn-Stevenson, & Hall, 2002; Najman et 
al., 2004; Everette, 2008). 

e Mother's illness. Several diseases that have a relatively minor effect on the 
health of a mother can have devastating consequences for a developing fetus if 
they are contracted during the early part of a pregnancy. For example, rubella 
(German measles), syphilis, diabetes, and high blood pressure may each 
produce a permanent effect on the fetus. The virus that causes AIDS can also 
be passed from mother to child before birth and through breast-feeding after 
birth (Nesheim et al., 2004; Magoni et al., 2005). 

e Mother's emotional state. A mother’s emotional state affects her baby. Mothers who 
are anxious and tense during the last months of their pregnancies are more apt 
to have irritable infants who sleep and eat poorly. The reason? The autonomic 
nervous system of the fetus becomes especially sensitive as a result of chemical 
changes produced by the mother’s emotional state (Relier, 2001; Hollins, 2007). 

e Mother's use of drugs. Mothers who take illegal, physically addictive drugs such 
as cocaine run the risk of giving birth to babies who are similarly addicted. 
Their newborns suffer painful withdrawal symptoms and sometimes show 
permanent physical and mental impairment. Even legal drugs taken by a 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 36 Prenatal Development: Conception to Birth 


Environmental Factor Possible Effect on Prenatal Development 


Rubella (German Blindness, deafness, heart abnormalities, stillbirth 

measles) 

Syphilis Mental retardation, physical deformities, maternal miscarriage 

Addictive drugs Low birth weight, addiction of infant to drug, with possible 
death after birth from withdrawal 

Nicotine Premature birth, low birth weight and length 

Alcohol Mental retardation, lower-than-average birth weight, small 


head, limb deformities 
Radiation from X-rays Physical deformities, mental retardation 


Inadequate diet Reduction in growth of brain, smaller-than-average weight 
and length at birth 


Mother's age—younger Premature birth, increased incidence of Down syndrome 
than 18 at birth of child 


Mother’s age—older Increased incidence of Down syndrome 
than 35 at birth of child 


DES (diethylstilbestrol) Reproductive difficulties and increased incidence of genital 
cancer in children of mothers who were given DES during 
pregnancy to prevent miscarriage 


AIDS Possible spread of AIDS virus to infant; facial deformities; 
growth failure 


Accutane Mental retardation and physical deformities 


FIGURE 3 A variety of environmental factors can play a role in prenatal development. 


pregnant woman (who may not know that she has become pregnant) can have 
a tragic effect (Ikonomidou et al, 2000; Schechter, Finkelstein, & Koren, 2005). 

e Alcohol. Alcohol is extremely dangerous to fetal development. For example, 1 out 
of every 750 infants is born with fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS), a condition resulting 
in below-average intelligence, growth delays, and facial deformities. FAS is now 
the primary preventable cause of mental retardation. Even mothers who use 
small amounts of alcohol during pregnancy place their child at risk. Fetal alcohol 
effects (FAE) is a condition in which children display some although not all of the 
problems of FAS due to their mother’s consumption of alcohol during pregnancy 
(Henderson, Kesmodel, & Gray, 2007; Niccols, 2007; Murthy et al., 2009). 

e Nicotine use. Pregnant mothers who smoke put their children at considerable 
risk. Smoking while pregnant can lead to miscarriage and infant death. For 
children who do survive, the negative consequences of mother’s tobacco use can 
last a lifetime (Haslam & Lawrence, 2004; Shea & Steiner, 2008; Rogers, 2009). 


Several other environmental factors have an impact on the child before and dur- 
ing birth (see Figure 3). Keep in mind, however, that although we have been discuss- 
ing the influences of genetics and environment separately, neither factor works alone. 
Furthermore, despite the emphasis here on some of the ways in which development 
can go wrong, the vast majority of births occur without difficulty. And in most 
instances, subsequent development also proceeds normally. 


ALTERNATIVE PATHS TO CONCEPTION 


For most couples, conception is routine, and pregnancy unfolds without incident. In 
other cases, though, conception represents a major challenge. Sometimes infertility, 
the inability to become pregnant, results because the male produces too few sperm. 


391 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


392 Chapter 12 Development 


In other cases, it is due to the advanced age of the parents, use of drugs, or previous 
cases of sexually transmitted disease. 

Scientists have devised several remedies to overcome infertility. One option is in 
vitro fertilization (IVF). IVF is a procedure in which a woman’s eggs are removed from 
her ovaries, and a man’s sperm is used to fertilize the eggs in a laboratory. A fertil- 
ized egg is then implanted in a woman’s uterus. Similarly, gamete intrafallopian trans- 
fer (GIFT) and zygote intrafallopian transfer (ZIFT) are procedures in which an egg and 
sperm or fertilized egg are implanted in a woman’s fallopian tubes. In IVF, GIFT, 
and ZIFT, the fertilized egg typically is implanted in the woman who provided the 
eggs. In some cases, eggs are implanted in a surrogate mother, a woman who agrees 
to carry the child to term. 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP EVALUATE 


1. Match each of the following terms with its definition: 
1. Zygote a. Smallest unit through which genetic 
2. Gene information is passed 
3. Chromosome b. Fertilized egg 
c. Rod-shaped structure containing 
genetic information 
2. Specific kinds of growth must take place during a 
period if the embryo is to develop normally. 
8. A is an environmental agent such as a drug, 
chemical, virus, or other factor that produces a birth defect. 


What is the nature of development before birth? 

e Each chromosome contains genes through which genetic 
information is transmitted. Genes, which are composed 
of DNA sequences, are the “software” that programs the 
future development of the body’s hardware. (p. 386) 

e Genes affect not only physical attributes, but also a wide 
array of personal characteristics such as cognitive 
abilities, personality traits, and psychological 
disorders. (p. 386) 

e At the moment of conception, a male’s sperm cell and a 
female’s egg cell unite; each contributes to the new indi- 
vidual’s genetic makeup. The union of sperm and egg 
produces a zygote, which contains 23 pairs of chromo- 
somes; one member of each pair comes from the father 


RETHINK 


1. Given the possible effects of the environment on a 





and the other comes from the mother. (p. 388) 

After two weeks the zygote becomes an embryo. By 
week 8, the embryo is called a fetus and is responsive to 
touch and other stimulation. At about week 22 it reaches 
the age of viability, which means it may survive if born 
prematurely. A fetus is normally born after 38 weeks of 
pregnancy; it weighs around 7 pounds and measures 
about 20 inches. (p. 388) 


What factors can affect a child during the mother’s pregnancy? 


Genetic abnormalities produce birth defects such as 
phenylketonuria (PKU), sickle-cell anemia, Tay-Sachs 
disease, and Down syndrome. (p. 390) 


developing fetus, do you think pregnant women should 
be prosecuted for the use of alcohol and other drugs that 
may do serious harm to their unborn children? Defend 
your position. 


. From the perspective of an educator: How would you 


utilize your knowledge of sensitive periods in language 
development to improve students’ learning? Would you 
want to teach children more than one language during 
this time? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 
uasoyeia} 'E EPD Z -Ç “L-Z ‘G-[ ° 


i=! 


e Among the environmental influences on fetal growth are 
the mother’s nutrition, illnesses, and drug intake. (p. 390) 


KEY TERMS 


chromosomes p. 386 
genes p. 386 


fetus p. 388 
age of viability p. 388 


zygote p. 388 teratogens p. 390 


embryo p. 388 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


His head was molded into a long melon shape and came to a point at the back .... He 
was covered with a thick greasy white material known as “vernix,” which made him 
slippery to hold and also allowed him to slip easily through the birth canal. In addition 
to a shock of black hair on his head, his body was covered with dark, fine hair known 
as “lanugo.” His ears, his back, his shoulders, and even his cheeks were furry . . . . His 
skin was wrinkled and quite loose, ready to scale in creased places such as his feet and 
hands... 
matted firmly forward on his cheek. His nose was flattened and pushed to one side by 


. His ears were pressed to his head in unusual positions—one ear was 


the squeeze as he came through the pelvis. (Brazelton, 1969, p. 3) 


What kind of creature is this? Although the description hardly fits that of the ador- 
able babies seen in advertisements for baby food, we are in fact talking about a 
normal, completely developed child just after the moment of birth. Called a neonate, 
a newborn arrives in the world in a form that hardly meets the standards of beauty 
against which we typically measure babies. Yet ask any parents: Nothing is more 
beautiful or exciting than the first glimpse of their newborn. 


The Extraordinary Newborn 


Several factors cause a neonate’s strange appearance. The trip through the mother’s 
birth canal may have squeezed the incompletely formed bones of the skull together 
and squashed the nose into the head. The skin secretes vernix, a white greasy cover- 
ing, for protection before birth, and the baby may have lanugo, a soft fuzz, over the 
entire body for a similar purpose. The infant’s eyelids may be puffy with an accu- 
mulation of fluids because of the upside-down position during birth. 

All these features change during the first two weeks of life 
as the neonate takes on a more familiar appearance. Even {$ 
more impressive are the capabilities a neonate begins to dis- & 
play from the moment of birth—capabilities that grow at an 
astounding rate over the ensuing months. 





REFLEXES 


A neonate is born with a number of reflexes—unlearned, 
involuntary responses that occur automatically in the presence 
of certain stimuli. Critical for survival, many of those reflexes 
unfold naturally as part of an infant’s ongoing maturation. 
The rooting reflex, for instance, causes neonates to turn their 
heads toward things that touch their cheeks—such as the 
mother’s nipple or a bottle. Similarly, a sucking reflex prompts 
infants to suck at things that touch their lips. Among other 
reflexes are a gag reflex (to clear the throat), the startle reflex 
(a series of movements in which an infant flings out the arms, 
fans the fingers, and arches the back in response to a sudden 


What are the major compe- 
tencies of newborns? 


What are the milestones of 
physical and social develop- 
ment during childhood? 


How does cognitive develop- 
ment proceed during 
childhood? 


neonate A newborn child. 


>s Unlearned, involuntary 
responses that occur automatically in 
the presence of certain stimuli. 


ae 





MARA : 
in £8 2% 


Many of the reflexes that a neonate is born with are critical for survival 
and unfold naturally as a part of an infant's ongoing maturation. Do 
you think humans have more or fewer reflexes than other animals? 


393 


394 Chapter 12 Development 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 








The basic reflexes— 
unlearned, involuntary 
responses—include the 
rooting reflex, the sucking 
reflex, the gag reflex, the 
startle reflex, and the 
Babinski reflex. 


support and finger 


FIGURE 1 Although at birth a neonate can make only jerky, limited voluntary movements, 
during the first year of life the ability to move independently grows enormously. The ages 
indicate the time when 50% of children are able to perform each skill. Remember, however, 
that the time when each skill appears can vary considerably. For example, 25% of children are 
able to walk well at age 11 months; by 15 months 90% of children are walking well. (Source: 
Frankenburg et al., 1992) 


noise), and the Babinski reflex (a baby’s toes fan out when the outer edge of the sole 
of the foot is stroked). 

Infants lose these primitive reflexes after the first few months of life and replace 
them with more complex and organized behaviors. Although at birth a neonate is 
capable of only jerky, limited voluntary movements, the ability to move indepen- 
dently grows enormously during the first year of life. The typical baby rolls over by 
the age of about 3 months, sits without support at about 6 months, stands alone at 
about 11 months, and walks at just over a year old. Not only does the ability to make 
large-scale movements improve during this time, but fine-muscle movements also 
become increasingly sophisticated (see Figure 1). 


DEVELOPMENT OF THE SENSES: TAKING IN THE WORLD 


When proud parents peer into the eyes of their neonate, is the child able to return 
their gaze? Although it was thought for some time that newborns can see only a 
hazy blur, most current findings indicate that neonates’ capabilities are far more 
impressive. Although their eyes have a limited capacity to focus on objects that are 
not within a seven- to eight-inch distance from the face, neonates can follow objects 
moving within their field of vision. They also show the rudiments of depth percep- 
tion as they react by raising their hands when an object appears to be moving rapidly 
toward the face (Gelman & Kit-Fong Au, 1996; Maurer et al., 1999). 

You might think that it would be hard to figure out just how well neonates can 
see because their lack of both language and reading ability clearly prevents them 
from saying what direction the E on a vision chart is facing. However, researchers 
have devised a number of ingenious methods that rely on the newborn’s biological 
responses and innate reflexes to test perceptual skills. 

For instance, infants who see a novel stimulus typically pay close attention to it; 
as a consequence, their heart rates increase. But if they repeatedly see the same 
stimulus, their attention to it decreases, as indicated by a return to a slower heart 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 37 Infancy and Childhood 395 





rate. This phenomenon is known as habituation, the decrease in the response to a 
stimulus that occurs after repeated presentations of the same stimulus. By studying 
habituation, developmental psychologists can tell when a child who is too young to 
speak can detect and discriminate a stimulus (Grunwald et al., 2003; Hannon & 
Johnson, 2005; del Rosal, Alonso, & Moreno, 2006). 

Researchers have developed many other methods for measuring neonate and 
infant perception. One technique, for instance, involves babies sucking on a nipple 
attached to a computer. A change in the rate and vigor with which the babies suck 
helps researchers infer that babies can perceive variations in stimuli. Other 
approaches include examining babies’ eye movements and observing which way 
babies move their heads in response to a visual stimulus (George, 1999; Franklin, 
Pilling, & Davies, 2005). 

Through the use of such research techniques, we now know that infants’ visual 
perception is remarkably sophisticated from the start of life. At birth, babies prefer 
patterns with contours and edges over less distinct patterns, indicating that they can 
respond to the configuration of stimuli. Furthermore, even newborns are aware of 
size constancy because they are apparently sensitive to the phenomenon by which 
objects stay the same size even though the image on the retina may change size as 
the distance between the object and the retina varies (Norcia et al., 2005; Moore, 
Goodwin, & George, 2007). 

In fact, neonates can discriminate facial expressions—and even imitate them. As 
you can see in Figure 2, newborns can produce a good imitation of an adult’s expres- 
sions. Even very young infants, then, can respond to the emotions and moods that 
their caregivers’ facial expressions reveal. This capability provides the foundation for 
social interaction skills in children (Meltzoff, 1996; Lavelli & Fogel, 2005; Grossmann, 
Striano, & Friederici, 2007). 

Other visual abilities grow rapidly after birth. By the end of their first month, 
babies can distinguish some colors from others; after 4 months they can focus on 


FIGURE 2 This newborn infant is clearly 
imitating the expressions of the adult 
model in these amazing photos. How 
does this ability contribute to social 
development? (Source: Courtesy of 

Dr. Tiffany Field.) 


habituation The decrease in the 
response to a stimulus that occurs after 
repeated presentations of the same 
stimulus. 


396 


Chapter 12 Development 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Young Infants 


Recognize Emotions Early in Life 





FIGURE 3 Using a method that measures electrophysiological responses in the brain 
(event-related potentials), researchers have found that by 7 months of age infants show 
distinct responses to happy, fearful, and neutral facial expressions using different regions 

of their brain (specifically, the medial, semimedial, and lateral regions). For example, 
approximately 400 milliseconds after seeing a fearful face (see arrow), infants show greater 
brain activity than when seeing either a neutral or happy face. This demonstrates that infants 
recognize differences between these facial expressions. (Source: Leppanen et al., 2007, Figure 2.) 


—40 uV 
Left medial Right medial 





100 300 500 700 900 100 300 500 700 900 
—40 uV 
Left Right 
semi-medial semi-medial 


100 300 500 700 900 100 300 500 700 90 





Left lateral Right lateral 





100 300 500 700 900 100 300 500 700 900 


= Fearful 


— Happy 
— Neutral 


near or far objects. By the age of 4 or 5 months, they are able to recognize two- and 
three-dimensional objects, and they can perceive the gestalt organizing principles 
discovered by psychologists who study perception. By the age of 7 months, neural 
systems related to the processing of information about facial expressions are highly 
sophisticated and cause babies to respond differently to specific facial expressions 
(see Figure 3). Overall, their perceptual abilities rapidly improve: Sensitivity to visual 
stimuli, for instance, becomes three to four times greater at 1 year of age than it was 
at birth Johnson, 2004; Striano & Vaish, 2006; Leppanen et al., 2007). 

In addition to vision, infants display other impressive sensory capabilities. 
Newborns can distinguish different sounds to the point of being able to recognize 
their own mothers’ voices at the age of 3 days. They can also make the subtle 
perceptual distinctions that underlie language abilities. For example, at 2 days of 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 37 


age, infants can distinguish between their native tongue and foreign languages, and 
they can discriminate between such closely related sounds as ba and pa when they 
are 4 days old. By 6 months of age, they can discriminate virtually any difference 
in sound that is relevant to the production of language. Moreover, they can recog- 
nize different tastes and smells at a very early age. There even seems to be some- 
thing of a built-in sweet tooth: Neonates prefer liquids that have been sweetened 
with sugar over their unsweetened counterparts (Cohen & Cashon, 2003; Rivera- 
Gaxiola et al., 2005). 


Infancy Through Childhood 


It was during the windy days of March that the problem in the day-care center first 





arose. Its source: 10-month-old Russell Ruud. Otherwise a model of decorum, Russell 

had somehow learned how to unzip the Velcro chin strap to his winter hat. He would 
remove the hat whenever he got the urge, seemingly oblivious to the potential health 

problems that might follow. 

But that was just the start of the real difficulty. To the chagrin of the teachers in 
the day-care center, not to speak of the children’s parents, soon other children were 
following his lead, removing their own caps at will. Russell’s mother, made aware of 
the anarchy at the day-care center—and the other parents’ distress over Russell’s 
behavior—pleaded innocent. “I never showed Russell how to unzip the Velcro,” claimed 
his mother, Judith Ruud, an economist with the Congressional Budget Office in 
Washington, DC. “He learned by trial and error, and the other kids saw him do it one 
day when they were getting dressed for an outing.” (Goleman, 1993, C10) 


At the age of 10 months, Russell asserted his personality, illustrating the tremendous 
growth that occurs in a variety of domains during the first year of life. Throughout 
the remainder of childhood, moving from infancy into middle childhood and the 
start of adolescence around age 11 or 12, children develop physically, socially, and 
cognitively in extraordinary ways. In the remainder of this module, we’ll consider 
this development. 


PHYSICAL DEVELOPMENT 


Children’s physical growth provides the most obvious sign of development. During 
the first year of life, children typically triple their birthweight, and their height 
increases by about half. This rapid growth slows down as the child gets older—think 
how gigantic adults would be if that rate of growth was constant. From age 3 to the 
beginning of adolescence at around age 13, growth averages a gain of about 5 pounds 
and 3 inches a year (see Figure 4). 

The physical changes that occur as children develop are not just a matter of 
increasing growth; the relationship of the size of the various body parts to one another 
changes dramatically as children age. As you can see in Figure 5, the head of a fetus 
(and a newborn) is disproportionately large. However, the head soon becomes more 
proportional in size to the rest of the body as growth occurs mainly in the trunk 
and legs. 


DEVELOPMENT OF SOCIAL BEHAVIOR: 
TAKING ON THE WORLD 


As anyone who has seen infants smiling at the sight of their mothers can guess, at 
the same time that infants grow physically and hone their perceptual abilities, they 
also develop socially. The nature of a child’s early social development provides the 
foundation for social relationships that will last a lifetime. 


Infancy and Childhood 


397 


398 Chapter 12 Development 


FIGURE 4 The average heights and 
weights of males and females in the 
United States from birth through age 20. 
At what ages are girls typically heavier 
and taller than boys? (Source: National 
Center for Health Statistics, 2000.) 


FIGURE 5 As development progresses, 
the size of the head relative to the rest of 
the body decreases until the individual 
reaches adulthood. Why do you think the 
head starts out so large? (Source: Adapted 
from Figure 5 from W. J. Robbins, Growth. 
Copyright © 1928 Yale University Press. Used 
by permission of Yale University Press.) 


attachment The positive emotional 
bond that develops between a child 
and a particular individual. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 













160 
—— Boys (50th il ie 
7o = = = Boys (50th percentile) ah 
68 F = Girls (50th percentile) 7 7145 
LE 7 | 
66 140 
E 135 
saa 130 
62 - 125 
60 + 120 
E 115 
-E Height 110 
56 105 
~ 54H = 100 
£ H | 95 < 
wv 52} E 
È 5 Weight J 90 > 
eo {86 
48 + 4 80 
L + 75 
46 i ape 
-i 4 65 
42 H + 60 
L 4 55 
40 H 
R = 50 
38 - J45 
36 Fb + 40 
34 4 35 
= 30 
32 a Pease 








30 





20 
2345 67 8 9 1011 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 
Age (years) 





Attachment, the positive emotional bond that develops between a child and a 
particular individual, is the most important form of social development that occurs 
during infancy. The earliest studies of attachment were carried out by animal ethologist 
Konrad Lorenz (1966). Lorenz focused on newborn goslings, which under normal 
circumstances instinctively follow their mother, the first moving object they perceive 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 37 Infancy and Childhood 399 


after birth. Lorenz found that goslings whose eggs were raised in an incubator and 
which viewed him immediately after hatching would follow his every movement as 
if he were their mother. He labeled this process imprinting, behavior that takes place 
during a critical period and involves attachment to the first moving object that is 
observed. 

Our understanding of attachment progressed when psychologist Harry Harlow, 
in a classic study, gave infant monkeys the choice of cuddling a wire “monkey” 
that provided milk or a soft, terry-cloth “monkey” that was warm but did not 
provide milk. Their choice was clear: They spent most of their time clinging to the 
warm cloth “monkey,” although they made occasional forays to the wire monkey 
to nurse. Obviously, the cloth monkey provided greater comfort to the infants; milk 
alone was insufficient to create attachment (Harlow & Zimmerman, 1959; Blum, 
2002; see Figure 6). 

Building on this pioneering work with nonhumans, developmental psychologists 
have suggested that human attachment grows through the responsiveness of infants’ 
caregivers to the signals the babies provide, such as crying, smiling, reaching, and 
clinging. The greater the caregiver’s responsiveness to the child’s signals, the more 
likely it is that the child will become securely attached. Full attachment eventually 
develops as a result of the complex series of interactions between caregiver and child. 
In the course of these interactions, the infant plays as critical and active of a role as 
the caregiver in the formation of the bond. Infants who respond positively to a 
caregiver produce more positive behavior on the caregiver’s part, which, in turn, 
produces an even stronger degree of attachment in the child. 


Assessing Attachment. Developmental psychologists have devised a quick and 
direct way to measure attachment. Developed by Mary Ainsworth, the Ainsworth 
strange situation consists of a sequence of events involving a child and (typically) 
his or her mother. Initially, the mother and baby enter an unfamiliar room, and 
the mother permits the baby to explore while she sits down. An adult stranger 
then enters the room; after this the mother leaves. The mother returns, and the 
stranger leaves. The mother once again leaves the baby alone, and the stranger 
returns. Finally, the stranger leaves, and the mother returns (Ainsworth et al., 
1978; Izard & Abe, 2004; Combrink-Graham & McKenna, 2006). 

Babies’ reactions to the experimental situation vary drastically, depending, accord- 
ing to Ainsworth, on their degree of attachment to the mother. One-year-old children 
who are securely attached employ the mother as a kind of home base; they explore 
independently but return to her occasionally. When she leaves, they exhibit distress, 
and they go to her when she returns. Avoidant children do not cry when the mother 
leaves, and they seem to avoid her when she returns as if indifferent to her. Ambivalent 
children display anxiety before they are separated and are upset when the mother 
leaves, but they may show ambivalent reactions to her return, such as seeking close 
contact but simultaneously hitting and kicking her. A fourth reaction is disorganized- 
disoriented; these children show inconsistent and often contradictory behavior. 

The nature of attachment between children and their mothers has far-reaching 
consequences for later development. For example, children who are securely attached 
to their mothers tend to be more socially and emotionally competent than are their 
less securely attached peers, and others find them more cooperative, capable, and 
playful. Furthermore, children who are securely attached at age 1 show fewer psy- 
chological difficulties when they grow older compared with avoidant and ambivalent 
youngsters. As adults, children who are securely attached tend to have more success- 
ful romantic relationships. On the other hand, being securely attached at an early 
age does not guarantee good adjustment later; conversely, children who lack secure 
attachment do not always have difficulties later in life (Mikulincer & Shaver, 2005; 
Roisman et al., 2005; Hardy, 2007). 


The Father’s Role. Although early developmental research focused largely on the 
mother-child relationship, more recent research has highlighted the father’s role in 





FIGURE 6 Although the wire “mother” 
dispensed milk to the hungry infant 
monkey, the infant preferred the soft, 
terry-cloth “mother.” Do you think 
human babies would react the same 
way? What does this experiment tell us 
about attachment? (Source: Harry Harlow 
Primate Laboratory/University of Wisconsin.) 


Alert 


Attachment—the positive 
emotional bond that devel- 
ops between a child and a 
particular individual—is a 
key concept in understand- 
ing the social development of 
children. 





400 


Chapter 12 Development 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


parenting—and with good reason: The number of fathers who are primary caregivers 
for their children has grown significantly, and fathers play an increasingly important 
role in their children’s lives. For example, in almost 13% of families with children, 
the father is the parent who stays at home to care for preschoolers (Parke, 2004; Day 
& Lamb, 2004; Halford, 2006). 

When fathers interact with their children, their play often differs from mothers’ 
play. Fathers engage in more physical, rough-and-tumble sorts of activities, whereas 
mothers play more verbal and traditional games, such as peekaboo. Despite such 
behavioral differences, the nature of attachment between fathers and children com- 
pared with that between mothers and children can be similar. In fact, children can 
form multiple attachments simultaneously (Borisenko, 2007; Pellis & Pellis, 2007; 
Diener et al., 2008). 


Social Relationships with Peers. By the time they are 2 years old, children become 
less dependent on their parents, more self-reliant, and increasingly prefer to play 
with friends. Initially, play is relatively independent: Even though they may be sitting 
side by side, 2-yearolds pay more attention to toys than to one another when playing. 
Later, however, children actively interact, modify one another’s behavior, and 
exchange roles during play (Lindsey & Colwell, 2003; Colwell & Lindsey, 2005). 

Cultural factors also affect children’s styles of play. For example, Korean-American 
children engage in less pretend play than their Anglo-American counterparts (Bai, 
2005; Drewes, 2005; Suizzo & Bornstein, 2006). 

As children reach school age, their social interactions begin to follow set patterns 
and become more frequent. They may engage in elaborate games involving teams 
and rigid rules. This play serves purposes other than mere enjoyment. It allows 
children to become increasingly competent in their social interactions with others. 
Through play they learn to take the perspective of other people and to infer others’ 
thoughts and feelings, even when those thoughts and feelings are not directly 
expressed (Royzman, Cassidy, & Baron, 2003). 

In short, social interaction helps children interpret the meaning of others’ behav- 
ior and develop the capacity to respond appropriately. Furthermore, children learn 
physical and emotional self-control: They learn to avoid hitting a playmate who 
beats them at a game. They learn to be polite and to control their emotional displays 
and facial expressions (e.g., smiling even when receiving a disappointing gift). Situ- 
ations that provide children with opportunities for social interaction, then, may 
enhance their social development (Feldman, 1993; Talukdar & Shastri, 2006; White- 
bread et al., 2009). 


The Consequences of Child Care Outside the Home. Research on the importance 
of social interaction is corroborated by work that examines the benefits of child care 
out of the home, which is an important part of an increasing number of children’s 
lives. For instance, almost 30% of preschool children whose mothers work outside 
the home spend their days in child-care centers. By the age of 6 months, almost 
two-thirds of infants are cared for by people other than their mothers for part of the 
day. Most of these infants begin child care before the age of 4 months and are cared 
for by people other than their mothers almost 30 hours per week (NICHD Early 
Child Care Research Network, 2006; National Research Council, 2001; see Figure 7). 

Do child-care arrangements outside the home benefit children’s development? If 
the programs are of high quality, they can. According to the results of a large study 
supported by the U.S. National Institute of Child Health and Development, children 
who attend high-quality child-care centers may not only do as well as children who 
stay at home with their parents, but in some respects they may actually do better. 
Children in child care are generally more considerate and sociable than other children 
are, and they interact more positively with teachers. They may also be more compliant 
and regulate their own behavior more effectively. And their mothers show increased 
sensitivity to their children (NICHD Early Child Care Research Network, 1999, 2001). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 37 Infancy and Childhood 401 












































11% 12% 54% 12% 7% 4% 
4⁄2 Years ites | | 
: a 
5 21% 20% 31% 13% 8% 7% 
So 
< 
36% 22% 9% 13% 10% 10% 
| | | | | | | ] T 
0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 


Percentage of Children 


| Mother Child care home Center 


a Father Ea Grandparent In home 


In addition, especially for children from poor or disadvantaged homes, child care 
in specially enriched environments—those with many toys, books, a variety of chil- 
dren, and high-quality providers—often proves to be more intellectually stimulating 
than the home environment. Such child care can lead to increased intellectual achieve- 
ment, demonstrated in higher IQ scores, and better language development. In fact, 
children in care centers sometimes are found to score higher on tests of cognitive 
abilities than those who are cared for by their mothers or by sitters or home day-care 
providers—effects that last into adulthood (Wilgoren, 1999; Burchinal, Roberts, & 
Riggins, 2000; Dearing, McCartney, & Taylor, 2009). 

However, child care outside the home does not have universally positive out- 
comes. Children may feel insecure after placement in low-quality child care or in 
multiple child-care settings. Furthermore, some research suggests that infants who 
are involved in outside care more than 20 hours a week in the first year show less 
secure attachment to their mothers than do those who have not been in child care 
outside the home. Finally, children who spent long hours in child care as infants and 
preschoolers may have a reduced ability to work independently and to manage their 
time effectively when they reach elementary school (NICHD Early Child Care 
Research Network, 2001; Vandell et al., 2005; Pluess & Belsky, 2009). 

The key to the success of nonparental child care is its quality. High-quality child 
care produces benefits; low-quality child care provides little or no gain and may even 
hinder children’s development. In short, significant benefits result from the social 
interaction and intellectual stimulation provided by high-quality child-care centers— 
especially for children from impoverished environments (NICHD Early Child Care 
Research Network, 2000, 2002; National Association for the Education of Young Chil- 
dren, 2005; Zaslow, Halle, & Martin, 2006). 


Parenting Styles and Social Development. Parents’ child-rearing practices are 
critical in shaping their children’s social competence. According to classic research 
by developmental psychologist Diana Baumrind, four main categories describe 
different parenting styles (Figure 8). Rigid and punitive, authoritarian parents 
value unquestioning obedience from their children. They have strict standards and 
discourage expressions of disagreement. Permissive parents give their children 
relaxed or inconsistent direction and, although they are warm, require little of 


FIGURE 7 According to a study by the 
National Institute of Child Health and 
Human Development, children were 
more likely to spend time in some kind 
of child care outside the home or family 
as they got older. (Source: NICHD, 2006). 


authoritarian parents Parents who are 
rigid and punitive and value unques- 
tioning obedience from their children. 


permissive parents Parents who give 
their children relaxed or inconsistent 
direction and, although they are warm, 
require little of them. 


402 Chapter 12 Development 





authoritative parents Parents who are 
firm, set clear limits, reason with their 
children, and explain things to them. 


uninvolved parents Parents who 
show little interest in their children 
and are emotionally detached. 


> Study Alert 


Know the four major types of 
child-rearing practices— 
authoritarian, permissive, 
authoritative, and 
uninvolved—and their 
effects. 


temperament Basic, innate disposition. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Type of Behavior 


Parenting Style Parent Behavior Produced in Child 


Authoritarian Rigid, punitive, strict standards (example: Unsociable, unfriendly, withdrawn 
“If you don't clean your room, I'm going 
to take away your iPod for good and 
ground you.’) 

Permissive Lax, inconsistent, undemanding (example: Immature, moody, dependent, 
“It might be good to clean your room, but low self-control 
| guess it can wait.’) 

Authoritative Firm, sets limits and goals, uses reasoning, Good social skills, likable, 
encourages independence (example:"You'll self-reliant, independent 
need to clean your room before we can 
go out to the restaurant. As soon as you 
finish, we'll leave.) 

Uninvolved Detached emotionally, sees role only as Indifferent, rejecting behavior 


providing food, clothing, and shelter 
(example: “I couldn't care less if your room 


is a pigsty.’) 


FIGURE 8 According to developmental psychologist Diana Baumrind (1971), four main 
parenting styles characterize child rearing. 


them. In contrast, authoritative parents are firm and set limits for their children. 
As the children get older, these parents try to reason and explain things to them. 
They also set clear goals and encourage their children’s independence. Finally, 
uninvolved parents show little interest in their children. Emotionally detached, 
they view parenting as nothing more than providing food, clothing, and shelter 
for children. At their most extreme, uninvolved parents are guilty of neglect, a 
form of child abuse (Baumrind, 2005; Winsler, Madigan, & Aquilino, 2005; Lagacé- 
Séguin & d’Entremont, 2006). 

As you might expect, the four kinds of child-rearing styles seem to produce very 
different kinds of behavior in children (with many exceptions, of course). Children 
of authoritarian parents tend to be unsociable, unfriendly, and relatively withdrawn. 
In contrast, permissive parents’ children show immaturity, moodiness, dependence, 
and low self-control. The children of authoritative parents fare best: With high social 
skills, they are likable, self-reliant, independent, and cooperative. Worst off are the 
children of uninvolved parents; they feel unloved and emotionally detached, and 
their physical and cognitive development are impeded (Saarni, 1999; Snyder, Cramer, 
& Afrank, 2005; Berk, 2005). 

Before we rush to congratulate authoritative parents and condemn authoritarian, 
permissive, and uninvolved ones, it is important to note that in many cases non- 
authoritative parents also produce perfectly well-adjusted children. Moreover, children 
are born with a particular temperament—a basic, innate disposition. Some children 
are naturally easygoing and cheerful, whereas others are irritable and fussy or pen- 
sive and quiet. The kind of temperament a baby is born with may in part bring about 
specific kinds of parental child-rearing styles (Majdandzic & van den Boom, 2007; 
Miner & Clarke-Stewart, 2008; Coplan, Reichel, & Rowan, 2009). 

In addition, children vary considerably in their degree of resilience, the ability to 
overcome circumstances that place them at high risk for psychological or even phys- 
ical harm. Highly resilient children have temperaments that evoke positive responses 
from caregivers. Such children display unusual social skills: outgoingness, intelli- 
gence, and a feeling that they have control over their lives. In a sense, resilient chil- 
dren try to shape their own environment rather than being victimized by it (Luthar, 
Cicchetti, & Becker, 2000; Deater-Deckard, Ivy, & Smith, 2005; Vellacott, 2007). (Also 
see PsychWork.) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 37 Infancy and Childhood 403 


Children are among the most vulnerable members of society. 
When they face abuse or neglect, a child protective services Psyc hWo rk 


agency intervenes, and a caseworker is assigned to advocate CHILD PROTECTION CASEWORKER 


for the child. 
Jason Larson, a child protection caseworker with the Child 
and Family Services Division of the Montana Department of 


Public Health and Human Services, notes that every case is Name o onia on 


unique. “The age of the children, the physical condition of the Position: Child Protection Caseworker 
parents or caretakers, the physical condition of the home, Education: BS in Human Services, Montana 


whether the alleged perpetrator is living in the home or has State University 
access to the child are all factors to be considered,” he said. 

“In addition, whether or not I can corroborate the information quickly enough to 
gauge whether or not a child can stay in the home or needs to be removed is impor- 
tant as well,” he added. 

Larson, who has been a caseworker for the past 15 years, works in a remote area 
of Montana which provides its own set of challenges. “Living and serving the rural 
part of Montana I have to be very creative with the services we have, as we are very 
limited in a lot of small towns across Montana,” Larson noted. “As a result, we use 
many different approaches to protect children.” 

One of those approaches, according to Larson, is a process called Family Group 
Decision Making, meetings in which the parents and all the service providers are at 
one table discussing the children and the family’s situation. 

“Everyone goes around the room and explains their concerns, and once this is 
complete, a plan is made for them to follow,” he notes. “These meetings are very effec- 
tive in assisting the family as a whole so that we can apply all the resources available.” 


We also need to keep in mind that these findings regarding child rearing styles 
apply primarily to U.S. society, which highly values children’s growing indepen- 
dence and diminishing reliance on their parents. In contract, Japanese parents encour- 
age dependence to promote the values of cooperation and community life. These 
differences in cultural values result in very different philosophies of child rearing. 
For example, Japanese mothers believe it is a punishment to make a young child 
sleep alone; thus, many children sleep next to their mothers throughout infancy and 
toddlerhood (Kawasaki et al., 1994; Dennis et al., 2002; Jones, 2007). 

In sum, a child’s upbringing results from the child-rearing philosophy parents 
hold, the specific practices they use, and the nature of their own and their child’s 
personalities. As is the case with other aspects of development, then, behavior is a 
function of a complex interaction of environmental and genetic factors. 


Erikson’s Theory of Psychosocial Development. In tracing the course of social 
development, some theorists have considered how the challenges of society and cul- 
ture change as an individual matures. Following this path, psychoanalyst Erik Erik- 
son developed one of the more comprehensive theories of social development. 
Erikson (1963) viewed the developmental changes that occur throughout life as a 
series of eight stages of psychosocial development; of these, four occur during child- 
hood. Psychosocial development involves changes in our interactions and under- 
standing of one another as well as in our knowledge and understanding of ourselves 
as members of society. 

Erikson suggests that passage through each of the stages necessitates the resolu- 
tion of a crisis or conflict. Accordingly, Erikson represents each stage as a pairing of 
the most positive and most negative aspects of the crisis of that period. Although 
each crisis is never resolved entirely—life becomes increasingly complicated as we 
grow older—it has to be resolved sufficiently to equip us to deal with demands made 
during the following stage of development. 






psychosocial development Develop- 
ment of individuals’ interactions and 
understanding of each other and of 
their knowledge and understanding of 
themselves as members of society. 


404 


© Lee Lorenz/The New Yorker Collection/www. 


cartoonbank.com. 


Chapter 12 Development 


“Please, Jason. Don’t you want to grow up 
to be an autonomous person?” 


trust-versus-mistrust stage According 
to Erikson, the first stage of psycho- 
social development, occurring from 
birth to age 1% years, during which 
time infants develop feelings of trust 
or lack of trust. 


autonomy-versus-shame-and-doubt 
stage The period during which, 
according to Erikson, toddlers (ages 
1% to 3 years) develop independence 
and autonomy if exploration and 
freedom are encouraged or shame and 
self-doubt if they are restricted and 
overprotected. 


initiative-versus-guilt stage Accord- 
ing to Erikson, the period during 
which children ages 3 to 6 years 
experience conflict between indepen- 
dence of action and the sometimes 
negative results of that action. 


industry-versus-inferiority stage 
According to Erikson, the last stage of 
childhood, during which children age 6 
to 12 years may develop positive social 
interactions with others or may feel 
inadequate and become less sociable. 


y Alert 


Four of Erikson’s stages of 
psychosocial development 
occur during childhood: 
trust-versus-mistrust, 
autonomy-versus-shame- 


and-doubt, initiative-versus-guilt, 
and industry-versus inferiority. 


cognitive development The process 
by which a child’s understanding of 
the world changes as a function of 
age and experience. 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


In the first stage of psychosocial development, the trust-versus-mistrust 
stage (ages birth to 1% years), infants develop feelings of trust if their physical 
requirements and psychological needs for attachment are consistently met and 
their interactions with the world are generally positive. In contrast, inconsistent 
care and unpleasant interactions with others can lead to mistrust and leave an 
infant unable to meet the challenges required in the next stage of development. 

In the second stage, the autonomy-versus-shame-and-doubt stage (ages 114 
to 3 years), toddlers develop independence and autonomy if exploration and 
freedom are encouraged, or they experience shame, self-doubt, and unhappi- 
ness if they are overly restricted and protected. According to Erikson, the key 
to the development of autonomy during this period is that the child’s caregiv- 
ers provide the appropriate amount of control. If parents provide too much 
control, children cannot assert themselves and develop their own sense of con- 
trol over their environment; if parents provide too little control, the children 
become overly demanding and controlling. 

Next, children face the crises of the initiative-versus-guilt stage (ages 3 to 6). In 
this stage, children’s desire to act independently conflicts with the guilt that comes 
from the unintended and unexpected consequences of such behavior. Children in this 
period come to understand that they are persons in their own right, and they begin 
to make decisions about their behavior. If parents react positively to children’s 
attempts at independence, they will help their children resolve the initiative-versus- 
guilt crisis positively. 

The fourth and last stage of childhood is the industry-versus-inferiority stage 
(ages 6 to 12). During this period, increasing competency in all areas, whether social 
interactions or academic skills, characterizes successful psychosocial development. In 
contrast, difficulties in this stage lead to feelings of failure and inadequacy. 

Erikson’s theory suggests that psychosocial development continues throughout 
life, and he proposes four more crises that are faced after childhood (described in 
the next module). Although his theory has been criticized on several grounds—such 
as the imprecision of the concepts he employs and his greater emphasis on male 
development than female development—it remains influential and is one of the few 
theories that encompass the entire life span. 


COGNITIVE DEVELOPMENT: CHILDREN’S 
THINKING ABOUT THE WORLD 


Suppose you had two drinking glasses of different shapes—one short and broad and 
one tall and thin. Now imagine that you filled the short, broad one with soda about 
halfway and then poured the liquid from that glass into the tall one. The soda would 
appear to fill about three-quarters of the second glass. If someone asked you whether 
there was more soda in the second glass than there had been in the first, what would 
you say? 

You might think that such a simple question hardly deserves an answer; of 
course, there is no difference in the amount of soda in the two glasses. However, 
most 4-year-olds would be likely to say that there is more soda in the second glass. 
If you then poured the soda back into the short glass, they would say there is now 
less soda than there was in the taller glass. 

Why are young children confused by this problem? The reason is not immedi- 
ately obvious. Anyone who has observed preschoolers must be impressed by how 
far they have progressed from the early stages of development. They speak with ease, 
know the alphabet, count, play complex games, use computers, tell stories, and com- 
municate ably. Yet despite this seeming sophistication, there are deep gaps in chil- 
dren’s understanding of the world. Some theorists have suggested that children 
cannot understand certain ideas and concepts until they reach a particular stage of 
cognitive development—the process by which a child’s understanding of the world 
changes as a function of age and experience. In contrast to the theories of physical 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 37 


Cognitive Stage Approximate Age Range 


Sensorimotor Birth—2 years 
Preoperational 2-7 years 
Concrete operational 7—|2 years 


Formal operational 12 years—adulthood 





FIGURE 9 According to Piaget, all children pass through four stages of cognitive development. 


and social development discussed earlier (such as those of Erikson), theories of 
cognitive development seek to explain the quantitative and qualitative intellectual 
advances that occur during development. 


Piaget’s Theory of Cognitive Development. No theory of cognitive development 
has had more impact than that of Swiss psychologist Jean Piaget. Piaget (1970) sug- 
gested that children around the world proceed through a series of four stages in a 
fixed order. He maintained that these stages differ not only in the quantity of infor- 
mation acquired at each stage but in the quality of knowledge and understanding as 
well. Taking an interactionist point of view, he suggested that movement from one 
stage to the next occurs when a child reaches an appropriate level of maturation and 
is exposed to relevant types of experiences. Piaget assumed that, without having such 
experiences, children cannot reach their highest level of cognitive growth. 

Piaget proposed four stages: the sensorimotor, preoperational, concrete opera- 
tional, and formal operational (see Figure 9). Let’s examine each of them and the 
approximate ages that they span. 

Sensorimotor Stage: Birth to 2 Years. During the sensorimotor stage, children base 
their understanding of the world primarily on touching, sucking, chewing, shaking, 
and manipulating objects. In the initial part of the stage, children have relatively little 
competence in representing the environment by using images, language, or other 
kinds of symbols. Consequently, infants lack what Piaget calls object permanence, the 
awareness that objects—and people—continue to exist even if they are out of sight. 

How can we know that children lack object permanence? Although we cannot 
ask infants, we can observe their reactions when a toy they are playing with is hid- 
den under a blanket. Until the age of about 9 months, children will make no attempt 
to locate the hidden toy. However, soon after that age they will begin an active 
search for the missing object, indicating that they have developed a mental repre- 
sentation of the toy. Object permanence, then, is a critical development during the 
sensorimotor stage. 

Preoperational Stage: 2 to 7 Years. The most important development during the 
preoperational stage is the use of language. Children develop internal representa- 
tional systems that allow them to describe people, events, and feelings. They even use 
symbols in play, pretending, for example, that a book pushed across the floor is a car. 

Although children use more advanced thinking in this stage than they did in the 
earlier sensorimotor stage, their thinking is still qualitatively inferior to that of adults. 
We see this when we observe a preoperational child using egocentric thought, a way 
of thinking in which the child views the world entirely from his or her own perspective. 
Preoperational children think that everyone shares their perspective and knowledge. 


Infancy and Childhood 405 


Major Characteristics 


Development of object permanence, 
development of motor skills, little 

or no capacity for symbolic 
representation 


Development of language and 
symbolic thinking, egocentric thinking 


Development of conservation, mastery 
of concept of reversibility 


Development of logical and abstract 
thinking 







y Alert 


Use Figure 9 to help remem- 
ber Piaget's stages of 
cognitive development. 


sensorimotor stage According to 
Piaget, the stage from birth to 2 years, 
during which a child has little compe- 
tence in representing the environment 
by using images, language, or other 
symbols. 


object permanence The awareness 
that objects—and people—continue to 
exist even if they are out of sight. 


preoperational stage According to 
Piaget, the period from 2 to 7 years of 
age that is characterized by language 
development. 


egocentric thought A way of thinking 
in which a child views the world 
entirely from his or her own perspective. 


406 Chapter 12 Development 





Children who have not mastered the 
principle of conservation assume that 
the volume of a liquid increases when it 
is poured from a short, wide container 
to a tall, thin one. What other tasks 
might a child under age 7 have difficulty 
comprehending? 


principle of conservation The 
knowledge that quantity is unrelated 
to the arrangement and physical 
appearance of objects. 


concrete operational stage According 
to Piaget, the period from 7 to 12 years 
of age that is characterized by logical 
thought and a loss of egocentrism. 


formal operational stage According 
to Piaget, the period from age 12 to 
adulthood that is characterized by 
abstract thought. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Thus, children’s stories and explanations to adults can be maddeningly uninformative 
because they are delivered without any context. For example, a preoperational child 
may start a story with, “He wouldn't let me go,” neglecting to mention who “he” is or 
where the storyteller wanted to go. We also see egocentric thinking when children at 
the preoperational stage play hiding games. For instance, 3-year-olds frequently hide 
with their faces against a wall and covering their eyes—although they are still in plain 
view. It seems to them that if they cannot see, then no one else will be able to see them 
because they assume that others share their view. 

In addition, preoperational children have not yet developed the ability to under- 
stand the principle of conservation, which is the knowledge that quantity is unre- 
lated to the arrangement and physical appearance of objects. Children who have not 
mastered this concept do not know that the amount, volume, or length of an object 
does not change when its shape or configuration changes. The question about the 
two glasses—one short and broad and the other tall and thin—with which we began 
our discussion of cognitive development illustrates this point clearly. Children who 
do not understand the principle of conservation invariably state that the amount of 
liquid changes as it is poured back and forth. They cannot comprehend that a trans- 
formation in appearance does not imply a transformation in amount. Instead, it 
seems as reasonable to the child that there is a change in quantity as it does to the 
adult that there is no change. 

In a number of other ways, some quite startling, the failure to understand the 
principle of conservation affects children’s responses. Research demonstrates that chil- 
dren during the preoperational period may completely misunderstand principles that 
are obvious to and unquestioned by adults and that children do not grasp the concept 
of conservation until the next stage of cognitive development (see Figure 10). 

Concrete Operational Stage: 7 to 12 Years. Mastery of the principle of conservation 
marks the beginning of the concrete operational stage. However, children do not 
fully understand some aspects of conservation—such as conservation of weight and 
volume—for a number of years. 

During the concrete operational stage, children develop the ability to think in a 
more logical manner and begin to overcome some of the egocentrism characteristic 
of the preoperational period. One of the major principles children learn during this 
stage is reversibility, the idea that some changes can be undone by reversing an 
earlier action. For example, they can understand that when someone rolls a ball of 
clay into a long sausage shape, that person can recreate the original ball by reversing 
the action. Children can even conceptualize this principle in their heads without 
having to see the action performed before them. 

Although children make important advances in their logical capabilities during 
the concrete operational stage, their thinking still displays one major limitation: They 
are largely bound to the concrete, physical reality of the world. For the most part, 
they have difficulty understanding questions of an abstract or hypothetical nature. 

Formal Operational Stage: 12 Years to Adulthood. The formal operational stage 
produces a new kind of thinking that is abstract, formal, and logical. Thinking is no 
longer tied to events that individuals observe in the environment but makes use of 
logical techniques to resolve problems. 

The way in which children approach the “pendulum problem” devised by Piaget 
(Piaget & Inhelder, 1958) illustrates the emergence of formal operational thinking. The 
problem solver is asked to figure out what determines how fast a pendulum swings. 
Is it the length of the string, the weight of the pendulum, or the force with which the 
pendulum is pushed? (For the record, the answer is the length of the string.) 

Children in the concrete operational stage approach the problem haphazardly 
without a logical or rational plan of action. For example, they may simultaneously 
change the length of the string, the weight on the string, and the force with which 
they push the pendulum. Because they are varying all the factors at once, they can- 
not tell which factor is the critical one. In contrast, people in the formal operational 
stage approach the problem systematically. Acting as if they were scientists conducting 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Conservation Modality Change in Average age 
of... physical appearance at full mastery 
Number Number of elements Rearranging or 6-7 years 
in a collection dislocating elements 
eeeeeeecece 0000000000 
eoeeeeecece eoeeeeeece 
Substance Amount of a Altering shape 7-8 years 
(mass) malleable substance 
(e.g., clay 
or liquid) C a 
Length Length of a line or Altering shape or 7-8 years 
object configuration 
eeoeeeeeecce £} $ 
Area Amount of surface Rearranging the figures 8-9 years 
covered by a set of 
plane figures E] m 
al eh 
Ses ET lu 
Weight Weight of an object Altering shape 9-10 years 
= 
pe i | | 
= 
Volume Volume of an object Altering shape 14-15 years 


(in terms of water 
displacement) 




















an experiment, they examine the effects of changes in one variable at a time. This 
ability to rule out competing possibilities characterizes formal operational thought. 

Although formal operational thought emerges during the teenage years, some 
individuals use this type of thinking only infrequently. Moreover, it appears that 
many individuals never reach this stage at all; most studies show that only 40-60% 
of college students and adults fully reach it, with some estimates running as low as 
25% of the general population. In addition, in certain cultures—particularly those 
that are less technically oriented than Western societies—almost no one reaches the 
formal operational stage (Keating & Clark, 1980; Super, 1980; Genovese, 2006). 

Stages Versus Continuous Development: Is Piaget Right? No other theorist has given 
us as comprehensive of a theory of cognitive development as Piaget. Still, many 
contemporary theorists suggest that a better explanation of how children develop 
cognitively can be provided by theories that do not involve a stage approach. For 
instance, children are not always consistent in their performance of tasks that—if 
Piaget’s theory is accurate—ought to be performed equally well at a particular stage 
(Feldman, 2003, 2004). 

Furthermore, some developmental psychologists suggest that cognitive develop- 
ment proceeds in a more continuous fashion than Piaget’s stage theory implies. They 


Module 37 Infancy and Childhood 407 


FIGURE 10 These tests are frequently 
used to assess whether children have 
learned the principle of conservation 
across a variety of dimensions. Do you 
think children in the preoperational stage 
can be taught to avoid conservation 
mistakes before the typical age of 
mastery? 


408 Chapter 12 Development 


information processing The way in 
which people take in, use, and store 
information. 


metacognition An awareness and 
understanding of one’s own cognitive 
processes. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


propose that cognitive development is primarily quantitative rather than qualitative. 
They argue that although there are differences in when, how, and to what extent a 
child can use specific cognitive abilities—reflecting quantitative changes, the under- 
lying cognitive processes change relatively little with age (Gelman & Baillargeon, 
1983; Case & Okamoto, 1996). 

Piaget also underestimated the age at which infants and children can understand 
specific concepts and principles; in fact, they seem to be more sophisticated in their 
cognitive abilities than Piaget believed. For instance, some evidence suggests that 
infants as young as 5 months have rudimentary mathematical skills (Wynn, Bloom, 
& Chiang, 2002; McCrink & Wynn, 2007; van Marle & Wynn, 2009). 

Despite such criticisms, most developmental psychologists agree that although 
the processes that underlie changes in cognitive abilities may not unfold in the man- 
ner Piaget’s theory suggests, he has generally provided us with an accurate account 
of age-related changes in cognitive development. Moreover, his theory has had an 
enormous influence in education. For example, Piaget suggests that individuals can- 
not increase their cognitive performance unless both cognitive readiness brought 
about by maturation and appropriate environmental stimulation are present. This 
view has inspired the nature and structure of educational curricula and teaching 
methods. Researchers have also used Piaget’s theory and methods to investigate 
issues surrounding animal cognition, such as whether primates show object perma- 
nence (they seem to; Hauser, 2000; Egan, 2005; Cunningham, 2006). 


INFORMATION-PROCESSING APPROACHES: 
CHARTING CHILDREN’S MENTAL PROGRAMS 


If cognitive development does not proceed as a series of stages as Piaget suggested, 
what does underlie the enormous growth in children’s cognitive abilities that even 
the most untutored eye can observe? To many developmental psychologists, changes 
in information processing, the way in which people take in, use, and store informa- 
tion, account for cognitive development (Lacerda, von Hofsten, & Heimann, 2001; 
Cashon & Cohen, 2004; Munakata, 2006). 

According to this approach, quantitative changes occur in children’s ability to 
organize and manipulate information. From this perspective, children become 
increasingly adept at information processing, much as a computer program may 
become more sophisticated as a programmer modifies it on the basis of experience. 
Information-processing approaches consider the kinds of “mental programs” that 
children invoke when approaching problems. 

Several significant changes occur in children’s information-processing capabili- 
ties. For one thing, speed of processing increases with age as some abilities become 
more automatic. The speed at which children can scan, recognize, and compare stim- 
uli increases with age. As they grow older, children can pay attention to stimuli 
longer and discriminate between different stimuli more readily, and they are less 
easily distracted (Myerson et al., 2003; Van den Wildenberg & Van der Molen, 2004). 

Memory also improves dramatically with age. Preschoolers can hold only two 
or three chunks of information in short-term memory, 5-year-olds can hold four, and 
7-year-olds can hold five. (Adults are able to keep seven, plus or minus two, chunks 
in short-term memory.) The size of the chunks also grows with age, as does the 
sophistication and organization of knowledge stored in memory (see Figure 11). Still, 
memory capabilities are impressive at a very early age: Even before they can speak, 
infants can remember for months events in which they actively participated (Cowan 
et al., 2003; Bayliss et al., 2005a). 

Finally improvement in information processing relates to advances in metacognition, 
an awareness and understanding of one’s own cognitive processes. Metacognition 
involves the planning, monitoring, and revising of cognitive strategies. Younger chil- 
dren, who lack an awareness of their own cognitive processes, often do not realize 
their incapabilities. Thus, when they misunderstand others, they may fail to recognize 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 37 Infancy and Childhood 409 





10 
a 
© 8 
3 Numb 
a m 
a 5 
e} 
& 4 
ao 
5 3 
= 
c 2 
a 
oO 
= l 
0 








44 
2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 II I2 Adults 


Age (years) 


their own errors. It is only later, when metacognitive abilities become more sophisti- 
cated, that children are able to know when they don’t understand. Such increasing 
sophistication reflects a change in children’s theory of mind, their knowledge and 
beliefs about the way the mind operates (Bernstein, Loftus, & Meltzoff, 2005; Matthews 
& Funke, 2006; Lockl & Schneider, 2007). 


Vygotsky’s View of Cognitive Development: Considering Culture. According to 
Russian developmental psychologist Lev Vygotsky, the culture in which we are raised 
significantly affects our cognitive development. In an increasingly influential view, 
Vygotsky suggests that the focus on individual performance of both Piagetian and 
information-processing approaches is misplaced. Instead, he holds that we cannot 
understand cognitive development without taking into account the social aspects of 
learning (Vygotsky, 1926/1997; Maynard & Martini, 2005; Rieber & Robinson, 2006). 

Vygotsky argues that cognitive development occurs as a consequence of social 
interactions in which children work with others to jointly solve problems. Through 
such interactions, children’s cognitive skills increase, and they gain the ability to 
function intellectually on their own. More specifically, he suggests that children’s 
cognitive abilities increase when they encounter information that falls within their 
zone of proximal development. The zone of proximal development (ZPD) is the 
level at which a child can almost, but not fully, comprehend or perform a task on 
his or her own. When children receive information that falls within the ZPD, they 
can increase their understanding or master a new task. In contrast, if the information 
lies outside children’s ZPD, they will not be able to master it (see Figure 12). 


Performance 
Performance 

















Unaided Aided 
Child A 


Unaided Aided 


Child B 


FIGURE 11 Memory span increases 
with age for both numbers and letters. 
(Source: Adapted from Dempster, 1981.) 


zone of proximal development 
(ZPD) According to Vygotsky, the 
level at which a child can almost, but 
not fully, comprehend or perform a 
task on his or her own. 


FIGURE 12 Although the performances 
of the two children working at a task 
without aid are similar, the second child 
benefits more from aid and thus has a 
larger zone of proximal development 
(ZPD). 


410 Chapter 12 Development 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


In short, cognitive development occurs when parents, teachers, or skilled peers 
assist a child by presenting information that is both new and within the ZPD. This 
type of assistance, called scaffolding, provides support for learning and problem solv- 
ing that encourages independence and growth. Vygotsky claims that scaffolding not 
only promotes the solution of specific problems, but also aids in the development of 
overall cognitive abilities (Schaller & Crandall, 2004). 

More than other approaches to cognitive development, Vygotsky’s theory consid- 
ers how an individual’s specific cultural and social context affects intellectual growth. 
The way in which children understand the world grows out of interactions with 
parents, peers, and other members of a specific culture (John-Steiner & Mahn, 2003; 
Kozulin et al., 2003). 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 


What are the major competencies of newborns? 
e Newborns, or neonates, have reflexes—unlearned, 
involuntary responses that occur automatically in the 


presence of certain stimuli. (p. 393) 


e Sensory abilities also develop rapidly; infants can distin- 
guish color, depth, sound, tastes, and smells relatively 


soon after birth. (p. 394) 


e After birth, physical development is rapid; children 
typically triple their birthweight in a year. (p. 397) 


What are the milestones of physical and social development 


during childhood? 


e Attachment—the positive emotional bond between a 


to 2 years), the preoperational stage (2 to 7 years), the 
concrete operational stage (7 to 12 years), and the formal 
operational stage (12 years to adulthood). (p. 404) 

e Information-processing approaches suggest that quanti- 
tative changes occur in children’s ability to organize and 
manipulate information about the world, such as signifi- 
cant increases in speed of processing, attention span, and 
memory. In addition, children advance in metacognition, 
the awareness and understanding of one’s own cognitive 
processes. (p. 408) 

e Vygotsky argued that children’s cognitive development 
occurs as a consequence of social interactions in which 
children and others work together to solve problems. 

(p. 409) 


child and a particular individual—marks social develop- 


ment in infancy. Measured in the laboratory by means of 


EVALUATE 


the Ainsworth stranger situation, attachment relates to 


later social and emotional adjustment. (p. 398) 


1. Researchers studying newborns use , or the 


e As children become older, the nature of their social inter- 


actions with peers changes. Initially play occurs rela- 
tively independently, but it becomes increasingly 
cooperative. (p. 400) 

The different child-rearing styles include authoritarian, 
permissive, authoritative, and uninvolved. (p. 401) 
According to Erikson, eight stages of psychosocial devel- 
opment involve people’s changing interactions and 
understanding of themselves and others. During child- 
hood, the four stages are trust-versus-mistrust (birth to 
1% years), autonomy-versus-shame-and-doubt (1% to 

3 years), initiative-versus-guilt (3 to 6 years), and industry- 
versus-inferiority (6 to 12 years). (p. 403) 


How does cognitive development proceed during childhood? 


Piaget’s theory suggests that cognitive development 
proceeds through four stages in which qualitative 
changes occur in thinking: the sensorimotor stage (birth 


. Erikson’s theory of 


decrease in the response to a stimulus that occurs after re- 
peated presentations of the same stimulus, as an indicator 
of a baby’s interest. 


. The emotional bond that develops between a child and its 


caregiver is known as 


. Match the parenting style with its definition: 


1. Permissive a. Rigid; highly punitive; 

2. Authoritative demanding obedience 

3. Authoritarian b. Gives little direction; lax on 

4, Uninvolved obedience 

c. Firm but fair; tries to explain paren- 
tal decisions 

d. Emotionally detached and unloving 

development involves a 
series of eight stages, each of which must be resolved for a 
person to develop optimally. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


5. Match the stage of development with the thinking style 
characteristic of that stage: 
1. Egocentric thought a. Sensorimotor 
2. Object permanence b. Formal operational 
3. Abstract reasoning c. Preoperational 
4. Conservation; d. Concrete operational 

reversibility 

6. - theories of development suggest 
that the way in which a child handles information is criti- 
cal to his or her development. 





7. According to Vygotsky, information that is within a child’s 


is most 





likely to result in cognitive development. 


KEY TERMS 


trust-versus-mistrust 
stage p. 404 

autonomy-versus-shame- 
and-doubt stage p. 404 

initiative-versus-guilt 


neonate p. 393 

reflexes p. 393 

habituation p. 395 
attachment p. 398 
authoritarian parents p. 401 


Module 37 Infancy and Childhood 411 


RETHINK 


1. Do you think the widespread use of IQ testing in the 
United States contributes to parents’ views that their chil- 
dren’s academic success is due largely to the children’s in- 
nate intelligence? Why? Would it be possible (or desirable) 
to change this view? 

2. From the perspective of a child-care provider: If a parent was 
not sure whether to enroll his or her child in your pro- 
gram, what advice would you give about the possible pos- 
itive and negative consequences about day care? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 
yuaurtdojaasp 
yeurrxoid jo əuoz *z ‘Sutssad01d-uoyeuosut "9 ‘p-p ‘q-¢ 2-7 
‘2-1 °g ‘Teosoypdsd ‘p ‘p-p ‘e-¢ o-z ‘q-q 'e QuauTYpeHe ‘z ‘uoyenyqey T 


sensorimotor stage p. 405 formal operational 


object permanence p. 405 stage p. 406 
preoperational information 
stage p.405 processing p. 408 


egocentric thought p. 405 metacognition p. 408 


permissive parents p. 401 
authoritative parents p. 402 
uninvolved parents p. 402 
temperament p. 402 
psychosocial 

development p. 403 


stage p. 404 
industry-versus-inferiority 
stage p. 404 


cognitive development p. 404 


principle of 
conservation p. 406 

concrete operational 
stage p. 406 


zone of proximal 
development 
(ZPD) p. 409 


What major physical, social, 
and cognitive transitions 
characterize adolescence? 


adolescence The developmental stage 
between childhood and adulthood. 


412 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Joseph Charles, Age 13: Being 13 is very hard at school. I have to be bad in order to be 
considered cool. I sometimes do things that aren’t good. I have talked back to my 
teachers and been disrespectful to them. I do want to be good, but it’s just too hard. 
(Gibbs, 2005, p. 51) 


Trevor Kelson, Age 15: “Keep the Hell Out of my Room!” says a sign on Trevor’s 
bedroom wall, just above an unmade bed, a desk littered with dirty T-shirts and candy 
wrappers, and a floor covered with clothes. Is there a carpet? “Somewhere,” he says 
with a grin. “I think it’s gold.” (Fields-Meyer, 1995, p. 53) 


Lauren Barry, Age 18: “I went to a National Honor Society induction. The parents were 
just staring at me. I think they couldn’t believe someone with pink hair could be smart. 
I want to be a high-school teacher, but I’m afraid that, based on my appearance, they 
won't hire me.” (Gordon et al., 1999, p. 47) 


Although Joseph, Trevor, and Lauren have never met, they share anxieties that are 
common to adolescence—concerns about friends, parents, appearance, independence, 
and their futures. Adolescence, the developmental stage between childhood and 
adulthood, is a crucial period. It is a time of profound changes and, occasionally, 
turmoil. Considerable biological change occurs as adolescents attain sexual and phys- 
ical maturity. At the same time and rivaling these physiological changes, important 
social, emotional, and cognitive changes occur as adolescents strive for independence 
and move toward adulthood. 

Because many years of schooling precede most people’s entry into the workforce 
in Western societies, the stage of adolescence is fairly long; it begins just before the 
teenage years and ends just after them. Adolescents are no longer children, yet soci- 
ety doesn’t quite consider them adults. They face a period of rapid physical, cogni- 
tive, and social change that affects them for the rest of their lives. 

Dramatic changes in society also affect adolescents’ development. More than half 
of all children in the United States will spend all or some of their childhood and 
adolescence in single-parent families. Furthermore, adolescents spend considerably 
less time with their parents and more with their peers than they did several decades 
ago. Finally, the ethnic and cultural diversity of adolescents as a group is increasing 
dramatically. A third of all adolescents today are of non-European descent; by the 
year 2050 the number of adolescents of Hispanic, African-American, Native-American, 
and Asian origin collectively will surpass that of whites (National Adolescent Health 
Information Center, 2003). 


Physical Development: 
The Changing Adolescent 


If you think back to the start of your own adolescence, the most dramatic changes 
you probably remember are physical. A spurt in height, the growth of breasts in 
girls, deepening voices in boys, the development of body hair, and intense sexual 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 38 Adolescence: Becoming an Adult 413 


Height spurt 


Penis growth 


First 
ejaculation 


Pubic hair 








Age (years) 


Average male 


Height spurt 


Onset of 
menstruation 


Breast 
development 


Pubic hair 








Age (years) 


Average female 


feelings cause curiosity, interest, and sometimes embarrassment for individuals 
entering adolescence. 

The physical changes that occur at the start of adolescence result largely from 
the secretion of various hormones, and they affect virtually every aspect of an ado- 
lescent’s life. Not since infancy has development been so dramatic. Weight and height 
increase rapidly because of a growth spurt that typically begins around age 10 for 
girls and age 12 for boys. Adolescents may grow as much as 5 inches in one year. 

Puberty, the period at which maturation of the sexual organs occurs, begins at 
about age 11 or 12 for girls, when menstruation starts. However, there are wide 
variations (see Figure 1). For example, some girls begin to menstruate as early as age 
8 or 9 or as late as age 16. Furthermore, in Western cultures, the average age at which 
adolescents reach sexual maturity has been steadily decreasing over the last century 
most likely as a result of improved nutrition and medical care. Sexual attraction to 
others begins even before the maturation of the sexual organs at around age 10 (see 
Figure 1; Tanner, 1990; Finlay, Jones, & Coleman, 2002). 

For boys, the onset of puberty is marked by their first ejaculation, known as 
spermarche. Spermarche usually occurs around the age of 13 (see Figure 1). At first, 
relatively few sperm are produced during an ejaculation, but the amount increases 
significantly within a few years. 

The age at which puberty begins has implications for the way adolescents feel 
about themselves—as well as the way others treat them. Early-maturing boys have 
a distinct advantage over later-maturing boys. They do better in athletics, are gener- 
ally more popular with peers, and have more positive self-concepts (Ge et al., 2003; 
Becker & Luthar, 2007). 


FIGURE 1 The range of ages during 
which major sexual changes occur during 
adolescence is shown by the colored 
bars. (Source: Based on Tanner, 1978.) 


puberty The period at which matura- 
tion of the sexual organs occurs, 
beginning at about age 11 or 12 for 
girls and 13 or 14 for boys. 


414 Chapter 12 Development 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


The picture differs for girls. Although early-maturing girls 
are more sought after as dates and have better self-esteem than 
later-maturing girls, some consequences of early physical mat- 
uration may be less positive. For example, early breast devel- 
opment may set them apart from their peers and be a source 
of ridicule (Franko & Striegel-Moore, 2002; Olivardia & Pope, 
2002; Nadeem & Graham, 2005). 

Late physical maturation may produce certain psycho- 
logical difficulties for both boys and girls. Boys who are 
smaller and less coordinated than their more mature peers 
tend to feel ridiculed and less attractive. Similarly, late-matur- 
ing girls are at a disadvantage in middle school and early high 
school. They hold relatively low social status and may be over- 
looked in dating (Lanza & Collins, 2002). 

Clearly, the rate at which physical changes occur during 


Although puberty begins around 11 or 12 for girls and 13 or 14 for adolescence can affect the way in which people are viewed by 
boys, there are wide variations. What are some of the advantages others and the way they view themselves. Just as important 


and disadvantages of early puberty? 


as physical changes, however, are the psychological and social 
changes that unfold during adolescence. 


Moral and Cognitive 
Development: Distinguishing 
Right from Wrong 





In a European country, a woman is near death from a special kind of cancer. The one drug 
that the doctors think might save her is a medicine that a medical researcher has recently 
discovered. The drug is expensive to make, and the researcher is charging ten times the 
cost, or $5,000, for a small dose. The sick woman’s husband, Henry, approaches everyone 
he knows in hope of borrowing money, but he can get together only about $2,500. He tells 
the researcher that his wife is dying and asks him to lower the price of the drug or let him 
pay later. The researcher says, “No, I discovered the drug, and I’m going to make money 
from it.” Henry is desperate and considers stealing the drug for his wife. 

What would you tell Henry to do? 


KOHLBERG’S THEORY OF MORAL DEVELOPMENT 


In the view of psychologist Lawrence Kohlberg, the advice you give Henry reflects 
your level of moral development. According to Kohlberg, people pass through a 
series of stages in the evolution of their sense of justice and in the kind of reasoning 
they use to make moral judgments (Kohlberg, 1984). Largely because of the various 
cognitive limitations that Piaget described, preadolescent children tend to think 
either in terms of concrete, unvarying rules (“It is always wrong to steal” or “TIl be 
punished if I steal”) or in terms of the rules of society (“Good people don’t steal” or 
“What if everyone stole?”). 

Adolescents, however, have typically reached Piaget’s formal operational stage of 
cognitive development and can reason on a higher plane. Because they are able to com- 
prehend broad moral principles, they can understand that morality is not always black 
and white and that conflict can exist between two sets of socially accepted standards. 

Kohlberg (1984) suggests that the changes in moral reasoning can be understood 
best as a three-level sequence (see Figure 2). His theory assumes that people move 
through the levels in a fixed order and that they cannot reach the highest level until 
about age 13—primarily because of limitations in cognitive development before that 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Level 


Level | Preconventional 
morality: At this level, the 
concrete interests of the 
individual are considered in 
terms of rewards and 
punishments. 


Level 2 Conventional morality: 


At this level, people approach 
moral problems as members 
of society. They are interested 
in pleasing others by acting as 
good members of society. 
Level 3 Postconventional 


morality: At this level, people 
use moral principles which are 


Module 38 Adolescence: Becoming an Adult 


Sample Moral Reasoning of Subjects 


In Favor of Stealing the Drug 


“If you let your wife die, you will get 
in trouble. You'll be blamed for not 
spending the money to save her, 
and there'll be an investigation of 
you and the druggist for your wife's 
death.” 





“If you let your wife die, you'll never 
be able to look anybody in the face 
again.” 


“If you don't steal the drug, and if 
you let your wife die, you'll always 
condemn yourself for it afterward. 


seen as broader than those of 
any particular society. 


You won't be blamed and you'll 


the law, but you won't have lived 
up to your own conscience and 
standards of honesty.” 


FIGURE 2 Developmental psychologist Lawrence Kohlberg theorized that people move 
through a three-level sequence of moral reasoning in a fixed order. However, he contended 
that few people ever reach the highest level of moral reasoning. 


age. However, many people never reach the highest level of moral reasoning. In fact, 
Kohlberg found that only a relatively small percentage of adults rise above the sec- 
ond level of his model (Kohlberg & Ryncarz, 1990; Hedgepeth, 2005; Powers, 2006). 

Although Kohlberg’s theory has had a substantial influence on our understand- 
ing of moral development, the research support is mixed. One difficulty with the 
theory is that it pertains to moral judgments, not moral behavior. Knowing right from 
wrong does not mean that we will always act in accordance with our judgments. In 
addition, the theory applies primarily to Western society and its moral code; cross- 
cultural research conducted in cultures with different moral systems suggests that 
Kohlberg’s theory is not necessarily applicable (Coles, 1997; Damon, 1999; Nucci, 
2002; Barandiaran, Pascual, & Samaniego, 2006). 


MORAL DEVELOPMENT IN WOMEN 


One glaring shortcoming of Kohlberg’s research is that he primarily used male par- 
ticipants. Furthermore, psychologist Carol Gilligan (1996) argues that because of men’s 
and women’s distinctive socialization experiences, a fundamental difference exists in 
the way each gender views moral behavior. According to Gilligan, men view morality 
primarily in terms of broad principles, such as justice and fairness. In contrast, women 
see it in terms of responsibility toward individuals and willingness to make sacrifices 
to help a specific individual within the context of a particular relationship. Compassion 
for individuals is a more salient factor in moral behavior for women than it is for men. 

Because Kohlberg’s model defines moral behavior largely in terms of abstract 
principles such as justice, Gilligan finds that it inadequately describes females’ moral 
development. She suggests that women’s morality centers on individual well-being 
and social relationships—a morality of caring. In her view, compassionate concern 
for the welfare of others represents the highest level of morality. 


have lived up to the outside rule of 


Against Stealing the Drug 


“You shouldn't steal the drug because 
you'll be caught and sent to jail if 

you do. If you do get away, your 
conscience will bother you thinking 
how the police will catch up with you 
at any minute.’ 


“After you steal the drug, you'll feel 
bad thinking how you've brought 
dishonor on your family and yourself; 
you won't be able to face anyone 
again.” 


“If you steal the drug, you won't be 
blamed by other people, but you'll 
condemn yourself because you won't 
have lived up to your own conscience 
and standards of honesty.’ 


415 


416 Chapter 12 Development 


y Alert 
The difference between the 
Kohlberg and Gilligan 
approaches to moral 


development is significant. 


Kohlberg’s theory focuses 
on stages and Gilligan's rests 
on gender differences. 


identity-versus-role-confusion 

stage According to Erikson, a time in 
adolescence of major testing to 
determine one’s unique qualities. 


identity The distinguishing character 
of the individual: who each of us is, 
what our roles are, and what we are 
capable of. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


The fact that Gilligan’s conception of morality differs greatly from Kohlberg’s 
suggests that gender plays an important role in determining what a person sees as 
moral. Although the research evidence is not definitive, it seems plausible that their 
differing conceptions of what constitutes moral behavior may lead men and women 
to regard the morality of a specific behavior in different ways (Jorgensen, 2006; 
Sherblom, 2008; Walker & Frimer, 2009). 


Social Development: Finding 
One's Self in a Social World 


“Who am 1?” “How do I fit into the world?” “What is life all about?” 

Questions such as these assume special significance during the teenage years, as 
adolescents seek to find their place in the broader social world. As we will see, this 
quest takes adolescents along several routes. 





ERIKSON’S THEORY OF PSYCHOSOCIAL 
DEVELOPMENT: THE SEARCH FOR IDENTITY 


Erikson’s theory of psychosocial development emphasizes the search for identity 
during the adolescent years. As noted earlier, psychosocial development encom- 
passes the way people’s understanding of themselves, one another, and the world 
around them changes during the course of development (Erikson, 1963). 

The fifth stage of Erikson’s theory (summarized, with the other stages, in Figure 3), 
the identity-versus-role-confusion stage, encompasses adolescence. During this stage, 
a time of major testing, people try to determine what is unique about themselves. They 
attempt to discover who they are, what their strengths are, and what kinds of roles 
they are best suited to play for the rest of their lives—in short, their identity. A person 
confused about the most appropriate role to play in life may lack a stable identity, 


Stage 


|. Trust-vs.-mistrust 


confusion 





8. Ego-integrity-vs.- 


despair 


2. Autonomy-vs.-shame- 
and-doubt 

3. Initiative-vs.-guilt 

4. Industry-vs.-inferiority 

5. Identity-vs.-role- 


6. Intimacy-vs.-isolation 


7. Generativity-vs.- 
stagnation 


Approximate Age 
Birth—|'/2 years 


| '/2-3 years 

3—6 years 

6-12 years 
Adolescence 
Early adulthood 
Middle adulthood 


Late adulthood 


Positive Outcomes 


Feelings of trust from 
environmental support 


Self-sufficiency if exploration is 
encouraged 


Discovery of ways to initiate 
actions 


Development of sense of 
competence 


Awareness of uniqueness of self, 
knowledge of role to be followed 


Development of loving, sexual 
relationships and close friendships 


Sense of contribution to 
continuity of life 


Sense of unity in life's 
accomplishments 


Negative Outcomes 
Fear and concern 
regarding others 
Doubts about self, lack 
of independence 

Guilt from actions and 
thoughts 

Feelings of inferiority, 
no sense of mastery 
Inability to identify 
appropriate roles in life 
Fear of relationships 
with others 
Trivialization of one's 
activities 

Regret over lost 
opportunities of life 


FIGURE 3 Erikson’s stages of psychosocial development. According to Erikson, people 
proceed through eight stages of psychosocial development across their lives. He suggested 
that each stage requires the resolution of a crisis or conflict and may produce both positive 


and negative outcomes. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 38 Adolescence: Becoming an Adult 417 


adopt an unacceptable role such as that of a social deviant, or have 
difficulty maintaining close personal relationships later in life | 
(Updegraff et al., 2004; Vleioras & Bosma, 2005; Goldstein, 2006). i 

During the identity-versus-role-confusion period, an adolescent 
feels pressure to identify what to do with his or her life. Because these 
pressures come at a time of major physical changes as well as impor- 
tant changes in what society expects of them, adolescents can find the 
period an especially difficult one. The identity-versus-role-confusion 
stage has another important characteristic: declining reliance on adults 
for information with a shift toward using the peer group as a source 
of social judgments. The peer group becomes increasingly important, 
enabling adolescents to form close, adult-like relationships and help- 
ing them clarify their personal identities. According to Erikson, the 
identity-versus-role-confusion stage marks a pivotal point in psycho- 
social development, paving the way for continued growth and the 
future development of personal relationships. 

During early adulthood, people enter the intimacy-versus- 
isolation stage. Spanning the period of early adulthood (from post- 
adolescence to the early 30s), this stage focuses on developing close 
relationships with others. Difficulties during this stage result in feel- 
ings of loneliness and a fear of such relationships; successful reso- 
lution of the crises of this stage results in the possibility of forming relationships 
that are intimate on a physical, intellectual, and emotional level. 

Development continues during middle adulthood as people enter the generativity- 
versus-stagnation stage. Generativity is the ability to contribute to one’s family, 
community, work, and society and to assist the development of the younger genera- 
tion. Success in this stage results in a person’s feeling positive about the continuity 
of life; difficulties in this stage lead a person to feel that his or her activities are 
trivial or stagnant and have done nothing for upcoming generations. In fact, if a 
person has not successfully resolved the identity crisis of adolescence, he or she may 
still be foundering, for example, in identifying an appropriate career. 

Finally, the last stage of psychosocial development, the ego-integrity-versus- 
despair stage, spans later adulthood and continues until death. Now a sense of 
accomplishment signifies success in resolving the difficulties presented by this stage 
of life; failure to resolve the difficulties results in regret over what might have been 
achieved but was not. 

Notably, Erikson’s theory suggests that development does not stop at adoles- 
cence but continues throughout adulthood. A substantial amount of research now 
confirms this view. For instance, a 22-year study by psychologist Susan Whitbourne 
found considerable support for the fundamentals of Erikson’s theory; the study 
determined that psychosocial development continues through adolescence and adult- 
hood. In sum, adolescence is not an end point but rather a way station on the path 
of psychosocial development (Whitbourne et al., 1992; McAdams et al., 1997). 

Although Erikson’s theory provides a broad outline of identity development, 
critics have pointed out that his approach is anchored in male-oriented concepts of 
individuality and competitiveness. In an alternative conception, psychologist Carol 
Gilligan suggests that women may develop identity through the establishment of 
relationships. In her view, a primary component of women’s identity is the construc- 
tion of caring networks among themselves and others (Gilligan, 2004). 


Lest gener ia 
the world- 


STORMY ADOLESCENCE: MYTH OR REALITY? 


Does puberty invariably foreshadow a stormy, rebellious period of adolescence? 

At one time, psychologists thought that most children entering adolescence were 
beginning a period fraught with stress and unhappiness. However, research now 
shows that this characterization is largely a myth, that most young people pass 


ee! We buy you the VS 





THE WORLD'S FIRST GENETICALLY ENGINEERED 
HUMAN HITS ADOLESCENCE 


Bchecking “genius” De 


on the order form = 


Ano Mow Look/ 





© Roz Chast/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


intimacy-versus-isolation 

stage According to Erikson, a period 
during early adulthood that focuses on 
developing close relationships. 


generativity-versus-stagnation 

stage According to Erikson, a period 
in middle adulthood during which we 
take stock of our contributions to 
family and society. 


ego-integrity-versus-despair 

stage According to Erikson, a period 
from late adulthood until death during 
which we review life’s accomplish- 
ments and failures. 


418 Chapter 12 Development 


y Alert 
The characterization of a 
stormy adolescence is a 
myth for most adolescents. 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


through adolescence without appreciable turmoil in their lives, and that parents 
speak easily—and fairly often—with their children about a variety of topics (van Wel, 
Linssen, & Abma, 2000; Granic, Hollenstein, & Dishion, 2003). 

Not that adolescence is completely calm! In most families with adolescents, the 
amount of arguing and bickering clearly rises. Most young teenagers, as part of their 
search for identity, experience tension between their attempts to become independent 
from their parents and their actual dependence on them. They may experiment with 
a range of behaviors and flirt with a variety of activities that their parents, and even 
society as a whole, find objectionable. Happily, though, for most families such ten- 
sions stabilize during middle adolescence—around age 15 or 16—and_ eventually 
decline around age 18 (Smetana, Daddis, & Chuang, 2003; Smetana, 2005). 

One reason for the increase in discord during adolescence appears to be the pro- 
tracted period in which children stay at home with their parents. In prior historical 
periods—and in some non-Western cultures today—children leave home immediately 
after puberty and are considered adults. Today, however, sexually mature adolescents 
may spend as many as seven or eight years with their parents. Current social trends 
even hint at an extension of the conflicts of adolescence beyond the teenage years 
because a significant number of young adults—known as boomerang children—return 
to live with their parents, typically for economic reasons, after leaving home for some 
period. Although some parents welcome the return of their children, others are less 
sympathetic, which opens the way to conflict (Bianchi & Casper, 2000; Lewin, 2003). 

Another source of strife with parents lies in the way adolescents think. Adolescence 
fosters adolescent egocentrism, a state of self-absorption in which a teenager views the 
world from his or her own point of view. Egocentrism leads adolescents to be highly 
critical of authority figures, unwilling to accept criticism, and quick to fault others. It 
also makes them believe that they are the center of everyone else’s attention, which 
leads to self-consciousness. Furthermore, they develop personal fables, the belief that their 
experience is unique, exceptional, and shared by no one else. Such personal fables may 
make adolescents feel vulnerable to the risks that threaten others (Tucker Blackwell, 
2006; Alberts, Elkind, & Ginsberg, 2007; Schwartz, Maynard, & Uzelac, 2008). 

Finally, parent-adolescent discord occurs because adolescents are much more apt 
to engage in risky behavior than later in life. In large part, their riskiness is due to 
the immaturity of brain systems that regulate impulse control, some of which do not 
fully develop until people are in their 20s (Steinberg, 2007). 


ADOLESCENT SUICIDE 


Although the vast majority of teenagers pass through adolescence without major 
psychological difficulties, some experience unusually severe psychological problems. 
Sometimes those problems become so extreme that adolescents 
take their own lives. Suicide is the third leading cause of death for 
adolescents (after accidents and homicide) in the United States. 
More teenagers and young adults die from suicide than from can- 
cer, heart disease, AIDS, birth defects, stroke, pneumonia and influ- 
enza, and chronic lung disease combined (CDC, 2004b). 

A teenager commits suicide every 90 minutes. Furthermore, the 
reported rate of suicide may actually be understated because medical 
personnel hesitate to report suicide as a cause of death. Instead, they 
frequently label a death as an accident in an effort to protect the sur- 
vivors. Overall, as many as 200 adolescents may attempt suicide for 
every one who actually takes his or her own life (CDC, 2000; Brausch 
& Gutierrez, 2009). 

Male adolescents are five times more likely to commit suicide 


These students are kneeling by a friend’s tombstone. The rate than females, although females attempt suicide more often than 


of suicide among teenagers has risen significantly over the males. The rate of adolescent suicide is significantly greater 
last few decades. Can you think of any reasons for this among whites than among nonwhites. However, the suicide rate 
phenomenon? of African-American males has increased much more rapidly than 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 38 Adolescence: Becoming an Adult 


Family problems 
Peer relationships 
Self-esteem 

Just to talk 

Drugs and alcohol 
Abuse 

Sexuality 

School problems 
Death 

Mental illness 
Spirituality 
Pregnancy 

Other 

Legal issue 

Eating disorder 


AIDS 











o 
om 
o 


15 20 25 30 


Percentage of calls mentioning the concern 


FIGURE 4 According to a review of phone calls to one telephone help line, adolescents who 
were considering suicide most often mentioned family, peer relationships, and self-esteem 
problems. (Source: Boehm & Campbell, 1995.) 


that of white males over the last two decades. Native Americans have the highest 
suicide rate of any ethnic group in the United States, and Asian Americans have the 
lowest rate (CDC, 2004b; Gutierrez et al., 2005; Boden, Fergusson, & Horwood, 2007). 

Although the rate of suicide has slowly declined, the rates are still higher for 
adolescents than any other age group except for the elderly. Some psychologists sug- 
gest that the sharp rise in stress that teenagers experience—in terms of academic and 
social pressure, alcoholism, drug abuse, and family difficulties—provokes the most 
troubled adolescents to take their own lives. However, that is not the whole story 
because the suicide rate for other age groups has remained fairly stable in the last 
few decades. It is unlikely that stress has increased only for adolescents and not for 
the rest of the population (Lubell et al., 2004). 

Although the question of why adolescent suicide rates are so high remains unan- 
swered, several factors put adolescents at risk. One factor is depression, characterized 
by unhappiness, extreme fatigue, and—a variable that seems especially important—a 
profound sense of hopelessness. In other cases, adolescents who commit suicide are 
perfectionists who are inhibited socially and prone to extreme anxiety when they face 
any social or academic challenge (see Figure 4; CDC, 2004b; Richardson et al., 2005; 
Caelian, 2006). 

Family background and adjustment difficulties are also related to suicide. A long- 
standing history of conflicts between parents and children may lead to adolescent 
behavior problems, such as delinquency, dropping out of school, and aggressive 
tendencies. In addition, teenage alcoholics and abusers of other drugs have a relatively 
high rate of suicide (Winstead & Sanchez, 2005; Bagge & Sher, 2008; Hardt et al., 2008). 


35 


40 


45 


419 


420 Chapter 12 Development 


Around the World 





Rites of Passage: Coming of Age 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Several warning signs indicate when a teenager’s problems may be severe 
enough to warrant concern about the possibility of a suicide attempt. They include 
the following: 


e School problems, such as missing classes, truancy, and a sudden change in grades 

e Frequent incidents of self-destructive behavior, such as careless accidents 

e Loss of appetite or excessive eating 

e Withdrawal from friends and peers 

e Sleeping problems 

e Signs of depression, tearfulness, or overt indications of psychological difficulties, 
such as hallucinations 

e A preoccupation with death, an afterlife, or what would happen “if I died” 

e Putting affairs in order, such as giving away prized possessions or making 
arrangements for the care of a pet 

e An explicit announcement of thoughts of suicide 


If you know someone who shows signs that he or she is suicidal, urge that per- 
son to seek professional help. You may need to take assertive action, such as enlist- 
ing the assistance of family members or friends. Talk of suicide is a serious signal 
for help and not a confidence to be kept. 

For immediate help with a suicide-related problem, call (800) 273-8255, a national 
hotline staffed with trained counselors, or access www.suicidepreventionlifeline.org. 


i It is not easy for male members of the Awa tribe in New Guinea 
Exploring DIVERSITY 


to make the transition from childhood to adulthood. First come 
whippings with sticks and prickly branches both for the boys’ 
own past misdeeds and in honor of those tribesmen who were 
killed in warfare. In the next phase of the ritual, adults jab 
sharpened sticks into the boys’ nostrils. Then they force a five- 
foot length of vine into the boys’ throats until they gag and vomit. Finally, tribesmen 
cut the boys’ genitals, causing severe bleeding. 

Although the rites that mark the coming of age of boys in the Awa tribe sound 
horrifying to Westerners, they are comparable to those in other cultures. In some, 
youths must kneel on hot coals without displaying pain. In others, girls must toss wads 
of burning cotton from hand to hand and allow themselves to be bitten by hundreds of 
ants (Selsky, 1997). 

Other cultures have less fearsome although no less important ceremonies that mark 
the passage from childhood to adulthood. For instance, when a girl first menstruates in 
traditional Apache tribes, the event is marked by dawn-to-dusk chanting. Western 
religions, too, have several types of celebrations, including bar mitzvahs and bat 
mitzvahs at age 13 for Jewish boys and girls, respectively, and confirmation ceremonies 
for children in many Christian denominations (Magida, 2006). 

In most societies, males are the focus of coming-of-age ceremonies. The renowned 
anthropologist Margaret Mead remarked, only partly in jest, that the preponderance of 
male ceremonies might reflect the fact that “the worry that boys will not grow up to be 
men is much more widespread than that girls will not grow up to be women” (1949, 
p. 195). Said another way, it may be that in most cultures men traditionally have higher 
status than women, and therefore those cultures regard boys’ transition into adulthood 
as more important. 

However, another fact may explain why most cultures place greater emphasis on 
male rather than female rites. For females, the transition from childhood is marked by 
a definite biological event: menstruation. For males, in contrast, no single event can be 
used to pinpoint entry into adulthood. Thus, men are forced to rely on culturally 
determined rituals to acknowledge their arrival into adulthood. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 38 Adolescence: Becoming an Adult 421 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 3. ______ proposed a set of three levels of moral 
development ranging from reasoning based on rewards 


What major physical, social, and cognitive transitions charac- ancl parnighnaranis io EEE O imeobane aonanale 


terize adolescence? 


; of justice. 
$ Adolescence the developmental Peene ca 4. Erikson believed that during adolescence, people must 
hood and adulthood, is marked by the onset of puberty, A 
search for —— ~ , whereas during the early adult- 


the point at which sexual maturity occurs. The age at 
which puberty begins has implications for the way 
people view themselves and the way others see 
them. (p. 412) RETHINK 

e Moral judgments during adolescence increase in sophis- 
tication, according to Kohlberg’s three-level model. 
Although Kohlberg’s levels provide an adequate de- 
scription of males’ moral judgments, Gilligan suggests 
that women view morality in terms of caring for individ- 
uals rather than in terms of broad general principles of 
justice. (p. 414) 

e According to Erikson’s model of psychosocial develop- 
ment, adolescence may be accompanied by an identity 
crisis. Adolescence is followed by three more stages of 
psychosocial development that cover the remainder 


hood, the major task is 


1. In what ways do school cultures help or hurt teenage 
students who are going through adolescence? What school 
policies might benefit early-maturing girls and late- 
maturing boys? Explain how same-sex schools could help 
students going through adolescence, as some have argued. 

2. From the perspective of a social worker: How might you 
determine if an adolescent is at risk for suicide? What 
strategies would you use to prevent the teen from 
committing suicide? Would you use different strategies 
depending on the teenager’s gender? 


of the life span. (p. 416) Answers to Evaluate Questions 
e Suicide is the third leading cause of death in Aeuur ‘ApHUApt p BION *¢ 127 amyeur 
adolescents. (p. 418) Aau} FI Jayyns syusosapope eyeuray pue sfeur y0q ‘asTey Z ‘Ayszaqnd ‘Tf 
EVALUATE 
T. is the period during which the sexual organs 


begin to mature. 
2. Delayed maturation typically provides both males and 
females with a social advantage. True or false? 


KEY TERMS 


adolescence p. 412 identity p. 416 generativity-versus- ego-integrity-versus-despair 
puberty p. 413 intimacy-versus-isolation stagnation stage p. 417 stage p. 417 
identity-versus-role- stage p. 417 


confusion stage p. 416 


What are the principal kinds 

of physical, social, and intel- 

lectual changes that occur in 
early and middle adulthood, 
and what are their causes? 


How does the reality of late 
adulthood differ from the 
stereotypes about that 
period? 


How can we adjust to death? 


emerging adulthood The period 
beginning in the late teenage years and 
extending into the mid-20s. 


422 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


I thought I got better as I got older. I found out that wasn’t the case in a real hurry last 
year. After going twelve years in professional football and twelve years before that in 
amateur football without ever having surgery performed on me, the last two seasons of 
my career I went under the knife three times. It happened very quickly and without 
warning, and I began to ask myself, “Is this age? Is this what’s happening?” Because up 
until that moment, I’d never realized that I was getting older. (Kotre & Hall, 1990, 

pp. 257, 259-260) 


As a former professional football player, Brian Sipes intensely felt the changes in his 
body brought about by aging. But the challenges he experienced are part of a normal 
process that affects all people as they move through adulthood. 

Psychologists generally agree that early adulthood begins around age 20 and 
lasts until about age 40 to 45 when middle adulthood begins and continues until 
around age 65. Despite the enormous importance of these periods of life in terms 
of both the accomplishments that occur in them and their overall length (together 
they span some 44 years), they have been studied less than has any other stage. 
For one reason, the physical changes that occur during these periods are less 
apparent and more gradual than those at other times during the life span. In 
addition, the diverse social changes that arise during this period defy simple 
categorization. 

The variety of changes that occur in early adulthood have led many develop- 
mental psychologists to view the start of the period as a transitional phase called 
emerging adulthood. Emerging adulthood is the period beginning in the late teen- 
age years and extending into the mid-20s. During emerging adulthood, people are 
no longer adolescents, but they haven’t fully taken on the responsibilities of adult- 
hood. Instead, they are still engaged in determining who they are and what their 
life and career paths should be (Schwartz, Côté, & Arnett, 2005; Bukobza, 2009; 
Lamborn & Groh, 2009). 

The view that adulthood is preceded by an extended period of emerging 
adulthood reflects the reality that the economies of industrialized countries have 
shifted away from manufacturing to an economy that focuses on technology and 
information and thus requires increases in time spent in educational training. Fur- 
thermore, the age at which most people marry and have children has risen sig- 
nificantly (Arnett, 2007). 

There’s also an increasing ambivalence about reaching adulthood. When people 
in their late teens and early 20s are asked if they feel they have reached adulthood, 
most say “yes and no” (see Figure 1). In short, emerging adulthood is an age of 
identity exploration in which individuals are more self-focused and uncertain than 
they will be later in early adulthood (Arnett, 2000, 2006). 

As we discuss the changes that occur through emerging adulthood, early adult- 
hood, middle adulthood, and ultimately late adulthood, keep in mind the demarca- 
tions between the periods are fuzzy. However, the changes are certainly no less 
profound than they were in earlier periods of development. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


100 


E Yes 
90 E No 
E Yes and no 


Percentage 
UI 
te] 





40 
30 
20 
10 
18-25 26-35 36-55 
Age 


FIGURE 1 Evidence of a period of emerging adulthood is provided by the responses to 
a questionnaire asking, “Do you feel that you have reached adulthood?” Most people 
between the ages of 18 and 25 were ambivalent, responding “yes and no.” Later, this 
ambivalence disappeared, with most people 26-35 saying “yes.” (Source: Arnett, 2000). 


Physical Development: 
The Peak of Health 


For most people, early adulthood marks the peak of physical health. From about 18 
to 25 years of age, people’s strength is greatest, their reflexes are quickest, and their 
chances of dying from disease are quite slim. Moreover, reproductive capabilities are 
at their highest level. 

Around age 25, the body becomes slightly less efficient and more susceptible to 
disease. Overall, however, ill health remains the exception; most people stay remark- 
ably healthy during early adulthood. (Can you think of any machine other than the 
body that can operate without pause for so long a period?) 

During middle adulthood, people gradually become aware of changes in their 
bodies. They often experience weight gain (although they can avoid such increases 
through diet and exercise). Furthermore, the sense organs gradually become less 
sensitive, and reactions to stimuli are slower. But generally, the physical declines 
that occur during middle adulthood are minor and often unnoticeable (DiGiovanna, 
1994). The major biological change that does occur during middle adulthood per- 
tains to reproductive capabilities. On average, during their late 40s or early 50s, 
women begin menopause, during which they stop menstruating and are no longer 
fertile. Because menopause is accompanied by a significant reduction in the pro- 
duction of estrogen, a female hormone, women sometimes experience symptoms 
such as hot flashes, sudden sensations of heat. Many symptoms can be treated 
through hormone therapy (HT) in which menopausal women take the hormones 
estrogen and progesterone. 

However, hormone therapy poses several dangers, such as an increase in the 
risk of breast cancer, blood clots, and coronary heart disease. These uncertainties 
make the routine use of HT controversial. Currently, the medical consensus seems 
to be that younger women with severe menopausal symptoms ought to consider 





Module 39 Adulthood 423 


menopause The period during which 
women stop menstruating and are no 
longer fertile. 


424 Chapter 12 Development 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


HT on a short-term basis. On the other hand, HT is less 
appropriate for older women after menopause (Rossouw et 
al., 2007; Lindh-Astrand, Brynhildsen, & Hoffmann, 2007; 
MacLennan, 2009). 

Menopause was once blamed for a variety of psycho- 
logical symptoms, including depression and memory loss. 
However, if such difficulties occur, they may be caused by 
women’s expectations about reaching an “old” age in a soci- 
ety that highly values youth. For example, women’s reactions 
to menopause vary significantly across cultures. The more a 
society values old age, the less difficulty its women have dur- 
ing menopause (Elliot, Berman, & Kim, 2002; Beyene, Gilliss, 
& Lee, 2007). 

For men, the aging process during middle adulthood is 
somewhat subtler. There are no physiological signals of 
increasing age equivalent to the end of menstruation in 
women; that is, no male menopause exists. In fact, men remain 
fertile and capable of fathering children until well into late 
adulthood. However, some gradual physical decline occurs. 
Sperm production decreases, and the frequency of orgasm 
tends to decline. Once again, though, any psychological dif- 


Women’s reactions to menopause vary significantly across cultures. ficulties associated with these changes are usually brought 
According to one study, the more a society values old age, the less = about by an aging individual’s inability to meet the exagger- 
difficulty its women have during menopause. Why do you think ated standards of youthfulness and not by the person’s phys- 


this would be the case? 


ical deterioration. 


Social Development: 
Working at Life 


Whereas physical changes during adulthood reflect development of a quantitative 
nature, social developmental transitions are qualitative and more profound. During 
this period, people typically launch themselves into careers, marriage, and families. 

The entry into early adulthood is usually marked by leaving one’s childhood 
home and entering the world of work. People envision life goals and make career 
choices. Their lives often center on their careers, which form an important part of 
their identity (Vaillant & Vaillant, 1990; Levinson, 1990, 1992). 

In their early 40s, however, people may begin to question their lives as they enter 
a period called the midlife transition. The idea that life will end at some point can 
become more influential in their thinking, which leads them to question their past 
accomplishments (Gould, 1978). 

Although some psychologists—and popular opinion—suggest that physical 
aging and dissatisfaction with one’s life mark a so-called “midlife crisis,” there 
is little evidence for such a “crisis.” In fact, the passage into middle age is rela- 
tively calm for most people. Most 40-year-olds view their lives and accomplish- 
ments positively enough to proceed relatively smoothly through midlife, and the 
40s and 50s are often a particularly rewarding period. Rather than looking to the 
future, people concentrate on the present; their involvement with their families, 
friends, and other social groups takes on new importance. A major developmen- 
tal thrust of this period is coming to terms with one’s circumstances (Whitbourne, 
2000, 2010). 

Finally, during the last stages of adulthood, people become more accepting of 
others and of their own lives and are less concerned about issues or problems that 
once bothered them. They come to accept the fact that death is inevitable, and they 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


try to understand their accomplishments in terms of the broader meaning of life. 
Although people may begin for the first time to label themselves as “old,” many also 
develop a sense of wisdom and feel freer to enjoy life (Baltes & Kunzmann, 2003; 
Miner-Rubino, Winter, & Stewart, 2004; Ward-Baker, 2007). 


Marriage, Children, and Divorce: 
Family Ties 


In the typical fairy tale, a dashing young man and a beautiful young woman marry, 
have children, and live happily ever after. However, that scenario does not match 
the realities of love and marriage in the 21st century. Today, it is just as likely that 
the man and woman would first live together, then get married and have children, 
but ultimately get divorced. 

The percentage of U.S. households made up of unmarried couples has increased 
dramatically over the last two decades. At the same time, the average age at which 
marriage takes place is higher than at any time since the turn of the last century. 
These changes have been dramatic, and they suggest that the institution of marriage 
has changed considerably from earlier historical periods. 

When people do marry, the probability of divorce is high, especially for younger 
couples. Even though divorce rates have been declining since they peaked in 1981, 
about half of all first marriages end in divorce. Before they are 18 years old, two-fifths 
of children will experience the breakup of their parents’ marriages. Moreover, the 
rise in divorce is not just a U.S. phenomenon: The divorce rate has accelerated over 
the last several decades in most industrialized countries. In some countries, the 
increase has been enormous. In South Korea, for example, the divorce rate quadru- 
pled from 11% to 47% in the 12-year period ending in 2002 (Schaefer, 2000; Lankov, 
2004; Olson & DeFrain, 2005). 

Changes in marriage and divorce trends have doubled the number of single- 
parent households in the United States over the last two decades. Almost 25% of 
all family households are now headed by one parent compared with 13% in 1970. 
If present trends continue, almost three-fourths of American children will spend 
some portion of their lives in a single-parent family before they turn 18. For chil- 
dren in minority households, the numbers are even higher. Almost 60% of all 
black children and more than a third of all Hispanic children live in homes with 
only one parent. Furthermore, in most single-parent families, the children live 
with the mother rather than the father—a phenomenon that is consistent across 
racial and ethnic groups throughout the industrialized world (U.S. Bureau of the 
Census, 2000). 

What are the economic and emotional consequences for children living in 
homes with only one parent? Single-parent families are often economically less 
well off, and this economic disadvantage has an impact on children’s opportuni- 
ties. More than a third of single-mother families with children have incomes below 
the poverty line. In addition, good child care at an affordable price is often hard 
to find. Furthermore, for children of divorce, the parents’ separation is often a 
painful experience that may result in obstacles to establishing close relationships 
later in life. Children may blame themselves for the breakup or feel pressure to 
take sides (U.S. Bureau of the Census, 2000; Wallerstein et al., 2000; Liu, He, & 
Wu, 2007). 

Most evidence suggests, however, that children from single-parent families are 
no less well adjusted than those from two-parent families. In fact, children may be 
more successful growing up in a harmonious single-parent family than in a two- 
parent family that engages in continuous conflict (Harold et al., 1997; Clarke-Stewart 
et al., 2000; Kelly, 2000; Olson & DeFrain, 2005). 





Module 39 Adulthood 


425 


426 


Chapter 12 Development 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Changing Roles of Men and 
Women 


One of the major changes in family life in the last two decades has been the evolu- 
tion of men’s and women’s roles. More women than ever before act simultaneously 
as wives, mothers, and wage earners—in contrast to women in traditional marriages 
in which the husband is the sole wage earner and the wife assumes primary respon- 
sibility for care of the home and children. 

Close to 75% of all married women with school-age children are now employed 
outside the home, and 55% of mothers with children under age 6 are working. In 
the mid-1960s, only 17% of mothers of 1-year-olds worked full-time; now, more than 
half are in the labor force (U.S. Bureau of the Census, 2001; Halpern, 2005). 

Most married working women are not free of household responsibilities. Even 
in marriages in which the spouses hold jobs that have similar status and require 
similar hours, the distribution of household tasks between husbands and wives has 
not changed substantially. Working wives are still more likely than husbands to feel 
responsible for traditional homemaking tasks such as cooking and cleaning. In con- 
trast, husbands still view themselves as responsible primarily for household tasks 
such as repairing broken appliances and doing yardwork (Ganong & Coleman, 1999; 
Juster, Ono, & Stafford, 2002). 





WOMEN’S “SECOND SHIFT” 


Working mothers can put in a staggering number of hours. One survey, for instance, 
found that if we add the number of hours worked on the job and in the home, 
employed mothers of children under 3 years of age put in an average of 90 hours 
per week! The additional work women perform is sometimes called the “second 
shift.” National surveys show women who are both employed and mothers put in 
an extra month of 24-hour days during the course of a year. Researchers see similar 
patterns in many developing societies throughout the world, with women working 
at full-time jobs and also having primary responsibilities for child care (Hochschild, 
2001; Jacobs & Gerson, 2004; Bureau of Labor Statistics, 2007). 

Consequently, rather than careers being a substitute for what women do at home, 
they are often an addition to the role of homemaker. It is not surprising that some 
wives feel resentment toward husbands who spend less time on child care and 
housework than the wives had expected before the birth of their children (Kiecolt, 
2003; Gerstel, 2005; Fagan & Press, 2008). 


Later Years of Life: Growing Old 


I’ve always enjoyed doing things in the mountains—hiking or, more recently, active 





cliff-climbing. The more difficult the climb, the more absorbing it is. The climbs I really 
remember are the ones I had to work on. Maybe a particular section where it took two 
or three tries before I found the right combination of moves that got me up easily—and, 
preferably, elegantly. It’s a wonderful exhilaration to get to the top and sit down and 
perhaps have lunch and look out over the landscape and be so grateful that it’s still 
possible for me to do that sort of thing. (Lyman Spitzer, age 74, quoted in Kotre & Hall, 
1990, pp. 358-359) 


If you can’t quite picture a 74-year-old rock-climbing, some rethinking of your view 
of late adulthood may be in order. In spite of the societal stereotype of “old age” as 
a time of inactivity and physical and mental decline, gerontologists, specialists who 
study aging, are beginning to paint a very different portrait of late adulthood. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





100 
Percentage of the U.S. 
population 65 or older 
80 
v 
S 60 Percentage of the U.S. 
F population younger 
2 than 65 
2 40 
20 








1910 1930 1950 1970 1990 2010 2030 2050 


Year 


By focusing on the period of life that starts at around age 65, gerontologists are 
making important contributions to clarifying the capabilities of older adults. Their 
work is demonstrating that significant developmental processes continue even dur- 
ing old age. And as life expectancy increases, the number of people who reach older 
adulthood will continue to grow substantially (see Figure 2). Consequently, develop- 
ing an understanding of late adulthood has become a critical priority for psycholo- 
gists (Birren, 1996; Moody, 2000, Schaie, 2005b). 


Physical Changes in Late 
Adulthood: The Aging Body 


Napping, eating, walking, conversing. It probably doesn’t surprise you that these 
relatively nonstrenuous activities represent the typical pastimes of late adulthood. 
But it is striking that these activities are identical to the most common leisure activ- 
ities reported in a survey of college students (Harper, 1978). Although the students 
cited more active pursuits—such as sailing and playing basketball—as their favorite 
activities, in actuality they engaged in such sports relatively infrequently and spent 
most of their free time napping, eating, walking, and conversing. 

Although the leisure activities in which older adults engage may not differ all 
that much from the ones that younger people pursue, many physical changes are, of 
course, brought about by the aging process. The most obvious are those of appear- 
ance—hair thinning and turning gray, skin wrinkling and folding, and sometimes a 
slight loss of height as the thickness of the disks between vertebrae in the spine 
decreases. But subtler changes also occur in the body’s biological functioning. For 
example, sensory capabilities decrease as a result of aging: Vision, hearing, smell, 
and taste become less sensitive. Reaction time slows, and physical stamina changes 
(Stenklev & Laukli, 2004; Schieber, 2006; Madden, 2007). 

What are the reasons for these physical declines? Genetic preprogramming 
theories of aging suggest that human cells have a built-in time limit to their repro- 
duction. These theories suggest that after a certain time cells stop dividing or 
become harmful to the body—as if a kind of automatic self-destruct button had 
been pushed. In contrast, wear-and-tear theories of aging suggest that the mechan- 
ical functions of the body simply work less efficiently as people age. Waste by- 
products of energy production eventually accumulate, and mistakes are made 
when cells divide. Eventually the body in effect wears out like an old automobile 
(Ly et al., 2000; Miquel, 2006; Hayflick, 2007). 





Module 39 Adulthood 427 


FIGURE 2 Projections suggest that by 
the year 2050, 20% of the U.S. 
population will be over age 65. What 
implications does this statistic have for 
U.S. society? 


genetic preprogramming theories of 
aging Theories that suggest that human 
cells have a built-in time limit to their 
reproduction and that they are no 
longer able to divide after a certain time. 


wear-and-tear theories of aging 
Theories that suggest that the mechan- 
ical functions of the body simply stop 
working efficiently. 


428 Chapter 12 Development 


y Alert 


Two major theories of aging— 
the genetic preprogramming 
and the wear-and-tear 
views—explain some of 
the physical changes that 
take place in older adults. 


FIGURE 3 Age-related changes in 
intellectual skills vary according to the 
specific cognitive ability in question. 
(Source: Schaie, K. W. (2005). Longitudinal 
studies. In Developmental influences on adult 
intelligence: The Seattle Longitudinal Study, 
Figure 5.7a (p. 127). Copyright © 2005 by 
Oxford University Press, Inc. By permission of 
Oxford University Press, Inc. www.oup.co.uk) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Evidence supports both the genetic preprogramming and the wear-and-tear 
views, and it may be that both processes contribute to natural aging. It is clear, how- 
ever, that physical aging is not a disease but a natural biological process. Many 
physical functions do not decline with age. For example, sex remains pleasurable 
well into old age (although the frequency of sexual activity decreases), and some 
people report that the pleasure they derive from sex increases during late adulthood 
(Gelfand, 2000; DeLamater & Sill, 2005). 


Cognitive Changes: Thinking 
About—and During— 
Late Adulthood 


At one time, many gerontologists would have agreed with the popular view that 
older adults are forgetful and confused. Today, however, most research indicates that 
this assessment is far from an accurate one of older people’s capabilities. 

One reason for the change in view is that more sophisticated research techniques 
exist for studying the cognitive changes that occur in late adulthood. For example, 
if we were to give a group of older adults an IQ test, we might find that the average 
score was lower than the score achieved by a group of younger people. We might 
conclude that this signifies a decline in intelligence. Yet, if we looked a little more 
closely at the specific test, we might find that the conclusion was unwarranted. For 
instance, many IQ tests include portions based on physical performance (such as 
arranging a group of blocks) or on speed. In such cases, poorer performance on the 
IQ test may be due to gradual decreases in reaction time—a physical decline that 
accompanies late adulthood and has little or nothing to do with older adults’ intel- 
lectual capabilities. 

Other difficulties hamper research into cognitive functioning during late adulthood. 
For example, older people are often less healthy than younger ones; when only healthy 
older adults are compared to healthy younger adults, intellectual differences are far less 


























55 
Total Sample 
50 
wn 
2 
o 
9 
n 
45 
c 
a 
o 
pz 
40 + = inductive reasoning 
— Spacial orientation 
= Perceptual speed 
= Numeric ability 
= Verbal ability 
= Verbal memory 
a l l | 


25 32 39 46 53 60 67 74 8l 88 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





in the 21st Century 


Gaming in Late Adulthood: 
How Video Games May 
Improve Cognitive Functioning 
in Older Adults 


Have you ever had the experience of frit- 
tering away an afternoon—or maybe even 
an entire day—playing a video game such 
as World of Warcraft or Grand Theft Auto? A 
lot of people do. Perhaps you’ve told your- 
self that you were doing something to im- 
prove yourself beyond just whiling away 
the time, such as increasing your eye-hand 
coordination or honing your mental skills. 
You may have been right, according to 
some recent research that looked at the ben- 
efits of playing video games. And that may 
be especially true if you were an older adult. 


tion are generally helpful, up to now it 
hasn’t been clear if video games provide 
the right kind of stimulation in the right 
amounts to do the trick (Tsai et al., 2008; 
Nunes & Kramer, 2009). 

Recent studies are encouraging, how- 
ever. In one study, older adults played a 
real-time strategic video game called Rise of 
Nations, which involves managing and de- 
fending an empire of cities, for almost 24 
hours over several sessions. Not only did 
their skill at playing the game improve 
over time, but they also showed improve- 
ments in tests of real-world skills such as 
task switching, short-term memory, and 
reasoning (Basak et al., 2008). 

In another study, groups of older and 
younger adults received five weeks of 
computerized cognitive skills training that 


Claims of skill enhancement and even 
improvements in cognitive ability from 
playing video games are not new. There are 
even games marketed specifically as “men- 
tal workouts” that supposedly sharpen 


became progressively more challenging. 
After the training, both groups showed no- 
ticeable increases in cognitive functioning 


both in tests and in their daily lives. What’s 
more, the effects tended to last and could 
be measured several months after the train- 
ing had concluded (Westerberg et al., 2008). 

The potential benefits to those in late 
adulthood of cognitive improvement 
through video games are so promising that 
the National Science Foundation recently 
awarded a team of researchers at North 
Carolina State University and Georgia Tech 
a 4-year, $1 million grant to study the effects 
on older adults of playing selected Nintendo 
Wii games. The researchers will investigate 
not only what cognitive improvements can 
be realized from gaming, but also what as- 
pects of playing games (such as the novelty 
of the games or the social interaction they 
require) are responsible for cognitive im- 
provements. Ultimately they hope to create 
new games based on their research that 
maximizes the cognitive benefits to those in 
late adulthood (Hamilton, 2009). 





brain skills. But researchers have only re- RETHINK 

cently become interested in the possible e What aspects of video games do you think would be most helpful in preventing 
use of video games to help slow the normal cognitive declines in late adulthood? 

declines of cognitive functioning in late e What other sorts of activities do you think would be helpful in averting such cogni- 


adulthood. While research has found that tive deterioration? 


activities that involve cognitive stimula- 











evident. Furthermore, the average number of years in school is often lower in older 
adults (for historical reasons) than in younger ones, and older adults may be less moti- 
vated to perform well on intelligence tests than younger people. Finally, traditional IQ 
tests may be inappropriate measures of intelligence in late adulthood. Older adults 
sometimes perform better on tests of practical intelligence than younger individuals do 
(Willis & Schaie, 1994; Dixon & Cohen, 2003). 

Still, some declines in intellectual functioning during late adulthood do occur, 
although the pattern of age differences is not uniform for different types of cognitive 
abilities (see Figure 3). In general, skills relating to fluid intelligence (which involves 
information-processing skills such as memory, calculations, and analogy solving) show 
declines in late adulthood. In contrast, skills relating to crystallized intelligence (intelli- 
gence based on the accumulation of information, skills, and strategies learned through 
experience) remain steady and in some cases actually improve (Rozencwajg et al., 2005; 
van Hooren, Valentijn, & Bosma, 2007; Kaufman, Johnson, & Liu, 2008). 

Even when changes in intellectual functioning occur during later adulthood, 
people often are able to compensate for any decline. They can still learn what they 
want to learn; it may just take more time. Furthermore, teaching older adults strate- 
gies for dealing with new problems can prevent declines in performance (Saczynski, 
Willis, & Schaie, 2002; Cavallini, Pagnin, & Vecchi, 2003; Peters et al., 2007; also see 
Applying Psychology in the 21st Century). 


429 


430 Chapter 12 Development 





Although there are declines in fluid intelligence in late 
adulthood, skills relating to crystallized intelligence 
remain steady and may actually improve. 


Alzheimer’s disease A progressive 
brain disorder that leads to a gradual 
and irreversible decline in cognitive 
abilities. 





y Alert 


It’s important to be able to 

describe the nature of intel- 
lectual changes during late 
adulthood. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MEMORY CHANGES IN LATE ADULTHOOD: 
ARE OLDER ADULTS FORGETFUL? 


One of the characteristics most frequently attributed to late adulthood is 
forgetfulness. How accurate is this assumption? 

Most evidence suggests that memory change is not an inevitable part 
of the aging process. For instance, research shows that older people in 
cultures in which older adults are held in high esteem, such as mainland 
China, are less likely to show memory losses than those living in cultures 
in which the expectation is that memory will decline. Similarly, when older 
people in Western societies are reminded of the advantages of age (for 
example, “age brings wisdom”), they tend to do better on tests of memory 
(Levy, 1996; Hess, Hinson, & Statham, 2004; Dixon, Rust, & Feltmate, 2007). 

Even when people show memory declines during late adulthood, their 
deficits are limited to certain types of memory. For instance, losses tend to 
be limited to episodic memories that relate to specific experiences in peo- 
ple’s lives. Other types of memories, such as semantic memories (that refer 
to general knowledge and facts) and implicit memories (memories of which 
we are not consciously aware) are largely unaffected by age (Fleischman et 
al., 2004; Mitchell & Schmitt, 2006; St. Jacques & Levine, 2007). 

Declines in episodic memories can often be traced to changes in older 
adults’ lives. For instance, it is not surprising that a retired person, who 
may no longer face the same kind of consistent intellectual challenges 
encountered on the job, may have less practice in using memory or even 
be less motivated to remember things, which leads to an apparent decline 
in memory. Even in cases in which long-term memory declines, older 
adults can profit from training that targets memory skills (Fritsch et al., 
2007; West, Bagwell, & Dark-Freudeman, 2007). 

In the past, older adults with severe cases of memory decline accompanied by 
other cognitive difficulties were said to suffer from senility. Senility is a broad, impre- 
cise term typically applied to older adults who experience progressive deterioration 
of mental abilities, including memory loss, disorientation to time and place, and 
general confusion. Once thought to be an inevitable state that accompanies aging, 
senility is now viewed by most gerontologists as a label that has outlived its useful- 
ness. Rather than senility being the cause of certain symptoms, the symptoms are 
deemed to be caused by some other factor. 

Some cases of memory loss, however, are produced by actual disease. For 
instance, Alzheimer’s disease is a progressive brain disorder that leads to a gradual 
and irreversible decline in cognitive abilities. More than 5 million Americans have 
the disease, and one in eight people age 65 and older are afflicted. Unless a cure is 
found, some 14 million people will experience Alzheimer’s by 2050 (Feinberg, 2002; 
Hurt et al., 2005; Rogers, 2007; Alzheimer’s Association, 2009). 

Alzheimer’s occurs when production of the beta amyloid precursor protein goes 
awry, producing large clumps of cells that trigger inflammation and deterioration of 
nerve cells. The brain shrinks, neurons die, and several areas of the hippocampus 
and frontal and temporal lobes deteriorate. So far, there is no effective treatment 
(Wolfe, 2006; Medeiros et al., 2007; Behrens, Lendon, & Roe, 2009). 

In other cases, cognitive declines may be caused by temporary anxiety and 
depression, which can be treated successfully, or may even be due to overmedication. 
The danger is that people with such symptoms may be left untreated, thereby con- 
tinuing their decline (Selkoe, 1997; Sachs-Ericsson et al., 2005). 

In sum, declines in cognitive functioning in late adulthood are, for the most 
part, not inevitable. The key to maintaining cognitive skills may lie in intellectual 
stimulation. Like the rest of us, older adults need a stimulating environment in 
order to hone and maintain their skills (Bosma et al., 2003; Glisky, 2007; Hertzog 
et al., 2008). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Maintaining interests and activities one had during middle age can contribute to a more 
successful late adulthood, according to the activity theory of aging. 


THE SOCIAL WORLD OF LATE ADULTHOOD: 
OLD BUT NOT ALONE 


Just as the view that old age predictably means mental decline has proved to be 
wrong, so has the view that late adulthood inevitably brings loneliness. People in 
late adulthood most often see themselves as functioning members of society; only a 
small number of them report that loneliness is a serious problem (Binstock & George, 
1996; Jylha, 2004). 

Certainly, late adulthood brings significant challenges. People who have spent their 
adult lives working and then enter retirement bring about a major shift in the role they 
play. Moreover, many people must face the death of their spouse. Especially if the mar- 
riage has been a long and good one, the death of a partner means the loss of a com- 
panion, confidante, and lover. It can also bring about changes in economic well-being. 

There is no single way to age successfully. According to the disengagement 
theory of aging, aging produces a gradual withdrawal from the world on physical, 
psychological, and social levels. However, such disengagement serves an important 
purpose of providing an opportunity for increased reflectiveness and decreased emo- 
tional investment in others at a time of life when social relationships will inevitably 
be ended by death (Adams, 2004; Wrosch, Bauer, & Scheier, 2005). 

Because little research supports disengagement theory, alternative theories have 
been suggested. According to the activity theory of aging, people who age most 
successfully are those who maintain the interests, activities, and level of social inter- 
action they experienced during middle adulthood. Activity theory argues that late 
adulthood should reflect a continuation, as much as possible, of the activities in 
which people participated during the earlier part of their lives (Crosnoe & Elder, 
2002; Nimrod & Kleiber, 2007). 

Although most research supports activity theory, not all people in late adulthood 
need a life filled with activities and social interaction to be happy; as in every stage 
of life, some older adults are just as satisfied leading a relatively inactive, solitary 
existence. What may be more important is how people view the aging process: Evi- 
dence shows that positive self-perceptions of aging are associated with increased 
longevity (Levy et al., 2002; Levy & Myers, 2004). 


Module 39 Adulthood 431 


disengagement theory of aging 

A theory that suggests that aging 
produces a gradual withdrawal from 
the world on physical, psychological, 
and social levels. 


activity theory of aging A theory that 
suggests that the elderly who are most 
successful while aging are those who 
maintain the interests and activities 
they had during middle age. 


432 Chapter 12 Development 


life review The process by which 
people examine and evaluate their 
lives. 


BECOMING AN 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Regardless of how people age, most engage in a process of life review in which 
they examine and evaluate their lives. Remembering and reconsidering what has 
occurred in the past, people in late adulthood often come to a better understanding 
of themselves. They sometimes resolve lingering problems and conflicts and face 
their lives with greater wisdom and serenity. 

Clearly, people in late adulthood are not just marking time until death. Rather, 
old age is a time of continued growth and development as important as any other 
period of life. 


At some time in our lives, we all face death—certainly our own 


INFORMED CONSUMER as well as the deaths of friends, loved ones, and even strangers. 
Although there is nothing more inevitable in life, death remains 


of Psychology a frightening, emotion-laden topic. Certainly, little is more 


Adjusting to Death 


© Roz Chast/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 





stressful than the death of a loved one or the contemplation of 
our own imminent death, and preparing for death is one of our 
most crucial developmental tasks (Aiken, 2000). 

A few generations ago, talk of death was taboo. The topic was never mentioned to 
dying people, and gerontologists had little to say about it. That changed, however, 
with the pioneering work of Elisabeth Kübler-Ross (1969), who brought the subject of 
death into the open with her observation that those facing impending death tend to 
move through five broad stages: 


e Denial. In this stage, people resist the idea that they are dying. Even if told that 
their chances for survival are small, they refuse to admit that they are facing 
death. 

e Anger. After moving beyond the denial stage, dying people become angry— 
angry at people around them who are in good health, angry at medical profes- 
sionals for being ineffective, angry at God. 

e Bargaining. Anger leads to bargaining in which the dying try to think of ways to 
postpone death. They may decide to dedicate their lives to religion if God saves 
them. They may say, “If only I can live to see my son married, I will accept 
death then.” 

e Depression. When dying people come to feel that bargaining is no use, they move 
to the next stage: depression. They realize that their lives really are coming to an 
end, which leads to what Kübler-Ross calls “prepara- 
tory grief” for their own deaths. 

e Acceptance. In this stage, people accept impending 
death. Usually they are unemotional and uncom- 
municative; it is as if they have made peace with 
themselves and are expecting death with no 
bitterness. 


It is important to keep in mind that not everyone 
experiences each of these stages in the same way. In fact, 
Kiibler-Ross’s stages pertain only to people who are fully 
aware that they are dying and have the time to evaluate their 
impending death. Furthermore, vast differences occur in the 
way individuals react to impending death. The specific cause 
and duration of dying, as well as the person’s sex, age, 
personality, and the type of support received from family and 
friends, all have an impact on how people respond to death 
(Carver & Scheier, 2002; Coyle, 2006). 

Few of us enjoy the contemplation of death. Yet awareness 
of its psychological aspects and consequences can make its 
inevitable arrival less anxiety producing and perhaps more 
understandable. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 39 Adulthood 433 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 


What are the principal kinds of physical, social, and intellec- 
tual changes that occur in early and middle adulthood, and 


what are their causes? 


Early adulthood marks the peak of physical health. 
Physical changes occur relatively gradually in men and 
women during adulthood. (p. 422) 

One major physical change occurs at the end of middle 
adulthood for women: They begin menopause after 
which they are no longer fertile. (p. 423) 

During middle adulthood, people typically experience a 
midlife transition in which the notion that life will end 
becomes more important. In some cases this may lead to 
a midlife crisis, although the passage into middle age is 
typically relatively calm. (p. 424) 

As aging continues during middle adulthood, people 
realize in their 50s that their lives and accomplishments 
are fairly well set, and they try to come to terms with 
them. (p. 425) 

Among the important developmental milestones during 
adulthood are marriage, family changes, and divorce. 
Another important determinant of adult development is 
work. (p. 425) 


How does the reality of late adulthood differ from the stereo 
types about that period? 


Old age may bring marked physical declines caused by 
genetic preprogramming or physical wear and tear. 
Although the activities of people in late adulthood are 
not all that different from those of younger people, older 
adults experience declines in reaction time, sensory 
abilities, and physical stamina. (p. 427) 

Intellectual declines are not an inevitable part of aging. 
Fluid intelligence does decline with age, and long-term 
memory abilities are sometimes impaired. In contrast, 
crystallized intelligence shows slight increases with age, 
and short-term memory remains at about the same 
level. (p. 428) 


e Although disengagement theory sees successful aging as 
a process of gradual withdrawal from the physical, 
psychological, and social worlds, there is little research 
supporting this view. Instead, activity theory, which 
suggests that the maintenance of interests and activities 
from earlier years leads to successful aging, is a more 
accurate explanation. (p. 431) 


How can we adjust to death? 


e According to Kübler-Ross, dying people move through 
five stages as they face death: denial, anger, bargaining, 
depression, and acceptance. (p. 432) 


EVALUATE 


il, 


Rob recently turned 40 and surveyed his goals and accom- 
plishments to date. Although he has accomplished a lot, 
he realized that many of his goals will not be met in his 
lifetime. This stage is called a 





. In households where both partners have similar jobs, the 


division of labor that generally occurs is the same as in 
“traditional” households where the husband works and 
the wife stays at home. True or false? 

theories suggest that there is a 
maximum time span in which cells are able to reproduce. 
This time limit explains the eventual breakdown of the 
body. 





. Lower IQ test scores during late adulthood do not neces- 


sarily mean a decrease in intelligence. True or false? 


. During old age, a person’s ______ intelligence contin- 
ues to increase, whereas ______ intelligence may 
decline. 

. In Kiibler-Ross’s _____ stage, people resist the idea 
of death. In the ______ stage, they attempt to make 
deals to avoid death, and in the _______ stage, they 


passively await death. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


434 Chapter 12 Development 


RETHINK about how to deal with aging? How would you handle 
someone who believed that getting older had only 


1. Is the possibility that life may be extended for several de- ; 5 
negative consequences? 


cades a mixed blessing? What societal consequences 
might an extended life span bring about? 

2. From the perspective of a health-care provider: What sorts of 
recommendations would you make to your older patients 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 
aourydaoor ‘Surute8ieq ‘TeTUap '9 Pmj POZA *g 
‘ony p ‘Sutunues8oidard syoues *¢ fans} z ‘UOISUeIy pppu T 


KEY TERMS 


emerging adulthood p. 422 wear-and-tear theories of aging p. 427 activity theory of aging p. 431 
menopause p. 423 Alzheimer’s disease p. 430 life review p. 432 
genetic preprogramming disengagement theory of aging p. 431 


theories of aging p. 427 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Psychology on the Web 


1. Find information on the web about gene therapy. What recent advances in gene 
therapy have been made by researchers? What developments appear to be on the 
horizon? What ethical issues have been raised regarding the use of gene therapy to 
produce children with characteristics specified by their parents? 

2. Find different answers to the question, “Why do people die?” Search the web for 
scientific, philosophical, and spiritual/religious answers. Write a summary in which you 
compare the different approaches to this question. How does the thinking in any one 
realm influence the thinking in the others? 


È D | | OQ U e We have traced major events in the development of 


physical, social, and cognitive growth throughout the life 
span. Clearly, people change throughout their lives. 

As we explored each area of development, we encountered anew the nature- 
nurture issue, concluding in every significant instance that both nature and nurture 
contribute to a person's development of skills, personality, and interactions. Specifically, 
our genetic inheritance—nature—lays down general boundaries within which we can 
advance and grow; our environment—nurture—helps determine the extent to which we 
take advantage of our potential. 

Before proceeding to the next set of modules, turn once again to the prologue at 
the beginning of this chapter that discussed that the McCartneys’ decision to remove 
Marcie’s tumor prior to birth. Using your knowledge of human development, consider 
the following questions. 





1. Why was Marcie McCartney put back into her mother's womb after her tumor was 
removed? 

2. Describe the stages of development that Marcie progressed through from conception 
to birth. 

3. What genetic influences might have acted on Marcie in the womb? 

4. What environmental influences might have acted on Marcie in the womb? 





435 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Key Concepts for Chapter 13 


MODULE 40 







How do psychologists define and use the Psychodynamic Approaches 
to Personality 


ity? 
concept of personality? @ What do the Freud's: Psychoanalytic Théory: 


theories of Freud and his successors tell us Mapping thë Uriconscigus Mind 


The Neo-Freudian Psychoanalysts: 
about the structure and development of Building on Freud 
personality? 


MODULE 41 






What are the major aspects of trait, learning, Trait, Learning, Biological and 
. , , o Evolutionary, and Humanistic 
biological and evolutionary, and humanistic Approaches to Personality 


Trait Approaches: Placing 
Labels on Personality 


approaches to psychology? 


Applying Psychology in the 21st 
Century: Judged at First Sight 
Learning Approaches: We Are 
What We've Learned 

Biological and Evolutionary Approaches: 
Are We Born with Personality? 
Neuroscience in Your Life: 
Wired for Thrills—The Biological 
Underpinnings of Personality 
Humanistic Approaches: 

The Uniqueness of You 





Comparing Approaches to Personality 









MODULE 42 





How can we most accurately assess Assessing Personality: Determining 


What Makes Us Distinctive 


Exploring Diversity: Should Race and 
Ethnicity Be Used to Establish Norms? 


personality? @ What are the major types of 


personality measures? 
Self-Report Measures of Personality 


Projective Methods 
Behavioral Assessment 
PsychWork: Human Resources Manager 


Becoming an Informed Consumer 
of Psychology: Assessing 
Personality Assessments 


437 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Prologue Who Is the Real Madoff? 


To some, Bernard L. Madoff was an affable, charismatic man who 
moved comfortably among power. brokers on Wall Street and in 
Washington. He secured a long-standing role as an elder states- 
man on Wall Street, allowing him to land on important boards 
and commissions where his opinions helped shape securities 
regulations. And his employees say he treated them like family. 


There was, of course, another side to Mr. Madoff. Reclusive, at 
times standoffish and aloof, this Bernard rarely rubbed elbows in 
Manhattan's cocktail circuit or at Palm Beach balls. This Bernard 
was quiet, controlled, and closely attuned to his image down to 
the most minute details. (Creswell & Landon, 2009, p. 1) 





Which was the real Bernard Madoff? Was he the powerful, 
charismatic Wall Street businessman? Or was he the self- 
conscious, detail-oriented recluse? And perhaps most impor- 
tant, were there any signs that Madoff was secretly operating 
a fraudulent investment scheme that ultimately cheated 
thousands of people out of billions of dollars? 

Many people, like Madoff, have different sides to their person- 
alities, appearing one way to some and quite differently to others. 
Determining who a person truly is falls to a branch of psychology 
that seeks to understand the characteristic ways people behave— 
personality psychology. 

Personality is the pattern of enduring characteristics that 
produce consistency and individuality in a given person. Person- 
ality encompasses the behaviors that make each of us unique 
and that differentiate us from others. Personality also leads us to 
act consistently in different situations and over extended periods 
of time. 


438 


We will consider a number of approaches to personality. For 
historical reasons, we begin with psychodynamic theories of 
personality, which emphasize the importance of the uncon- 
scious. Next, we consider approaches that concentrate on 
identifying the most fundamental personality traits; theories 
that view personality as a set of learned behaviors; biological 
and evolutionary perspectives on personality; and approaches, 
known as humanistic theories, that highlight the uniquely 
human aspects of personality. We end our discussion by 
focusing on how personality is measured and how personality 
tests can be used. 


personality The pattern of enduring characteristics that produce 
consistency and individuality in a given person. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 40 


The college student was intent on making a good first impression on an attractive 
woman he had spotted across a crowded room at a party. As he walked toward her, 
he mulled over a line he had heard in an old movie the night before: “I don’t believe 
we've been properly introduced yet.” To his horror, what came out was a bit different. 
After threading his way through the crowded room, he finally reached the woman and 
blurted out, “I don’t believe we’ve been properly seduced yet.” 


Although this student’s error may seem to be merely an embarrassing slip of the 
tongue, according to some personality theorists such a mistake is not an error at all 
(Motley, 1987). Instead, psychodynamic personality theorists might argue that the error 
illustrates one way in which behavior is triggered by inner forces that are beyond 
our awareness. These hidden drives, shaped by childhood experiences, play an 
important role in energizing and directing everyday behavior. 

Psychodynamic approaches to personality are based on the idea that personal- 
ity is motivated by inner forces and conflicts about which people have little aware- 
ness and over which they have no control. The most important pioneer of the 
psychodynamic approach was Sigmund Freud. A number of Freud’s followers, 
including Carl Jung, Karen Horney, and Alfred Adler, refined Freud’s theory and 
developed their own psychodynamic approaches. 


Freud’s Psychoanalytic Theory: 
Mapping the Unconscious Mind 


Sigmund Freud, an Austrian physician, developed psychoanalytic theory in the early 
1900s. According to Freud’s theory, conscious experience is only a small part of our 
psychological makeup and experience. He argued that much of our behavior is moti- 
vated by the unconscious, a part of the personality that contains the memories, 
knowledge, beliefs, feelings, urges, drives, and instincts of which the individual is 
not aware. 

Like the unseen mass of a floating iceberg, the contents of the unconscious far 
surpass in quantity the information in our conscious awareness. Freud maintained 
that to understand personality, it is necessary to expose what is in the unconscious. 
But because the unconscious disguises the meaning of the material it holds, the con- 
tent of the unconscious cannot be observed directly. It is therefore necessary to inter- 
pret clues to the unconscious—slips of the tongue, fantasies, and dreams—to 
understand the unconscious processes that direct behavior. A slip of the tongue such 
as the one quoted earlier (sometimes termed a Freudian slip) may be interpreted as 
revealing the speaker’s unconscious sexual desires. 

To Freud, much of our personality is determined by our unconscious. Some of 
the unconscious is made up of the preconscious, which contains material that is not 
threatening and is easily brought to mind, such as the knowledge that 2 + 2 = 4. 
But deeper in the unconscious are instinctual drives—the wishes, desires, demands, 
and needs that are hidden from conscious awareness because of the conflicts and 





How do psychologists define 
and use the concept of 
personality? 


What do the theories of Freud 
and his successors tell us 
about the structure and 
development of personality? 


Approaches that assume 
that personality is motivated by inner 
forces and conflicts about which 
people have little awareness and over 
which they have no control. 


Freud’s theory 
that unconscious forces act as determi- 
nants of personality. 


A part of the personality 
that contains the memories, knowl- 
edge, beliefs, feelings, urges, drives, 
and instincts of which the individual 
is not aware. 


439 


440 Chapter 13 Personality 





Conscious 


FIGURE 1 In Freud's personality model, 
there are three major components: the 
id, the ego, and the superego. As the 
iceberg analogy shows, only a small 
portion of personality is conscious. Why 
do you think that only the ego and 
superego have conscious components? 


y Alert 


Remember that the three 
parts of personality in 
Freud’s theory—the id, the 
ego, and the superego— 
are abstract conceptions 
that don't exist as physical structures 
in the brain. 


id The raw, unorganized, inborn part 
of personality whose sole purpose is to 
reduce tension created by primitive 
drives related to hunger, sex, aggres- 
sion, and irrational impulses. 


ego The part of the personality that 
provides a buffer between the id and 
the outside world. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


pain they would cause if they were part of our everyday lives. The unconscious 
provides a “safe haven” for our recollections of threatening events. 


STRUCTURING PERSONALITY: ID, EGO, AND SUPEREGO 


To describe the structure of personality, Freud developed a comprehensive theory that 
held that personality consists of three separate but interacting components: the id, the 
ego, and the superego. Freud suggested that the three structures can be diagrammed 
to show how they relate to the conscious and the unconscious (see Figure 1). 

Although the three components of personality Freud described may appear to 
be actual physical structures in the nervous system, they are not. Instead, they rep- 
resent abstract conceptions of a general model of personality that describes the inter- 
action of forces that motivate behavior. 

If personality consisted only of primitive, instinctual cravings and longings, it 
would have just one component: the id. The id is the raw, unorganized, inborn part 
of personality. From the time of birth, the id attempts to reduce tension created by 
primitive drives related to hunger, sex, aggression, and irrational impulses. Those 
drives are fueled by “psychic energy,” which we can think of as a limitless energy 
source constantly putting pressure on the various parts of the personality. 

The id operates according to the pleasure principle in which the goal is the imme- 
diate reduction of tension and the maximization of satisfaction. However, in most 
cases, reality prevents the fulfillment of the demands of the pleasure principle: We 
cannot always eat when we are hungry, and we can discharge our sexual drives only 
when the time and place are appropriate. To account for this fact of life, Freud sug- 
gested a second component of personality, which he called the ego. 

The ego, which begins to develop soon after birth, strives to balance the desires 
of the id and the realities of the objective, outside world. In contrast to the pleasure- 
seeking id, the ego operates according to the reality principle in which instinctual 
energy is restrained to maintain the individual’s safety and to help integrate the 
person into society. In a sense, then, the ego is the “executive” of personality: It makes 
decisions, controls actions, and allows thinking and problem solving of a higher 
order than the id’s capabilities permit. 





Freud suggests that the superego, the part of personality that represents the rights and wrongs 
of society, develops from direct teaching from parents, teachers, and other significant individuals. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 40 Psychodynamic Approaches to Personality 


Stage Age 

Oral Birth to 12—18 months 
Anal 12-18 months to 3 years 
Phallic 3 to 5—6 years 

Latency 5—6 years to adolescence 


Genital Adolescence to adulthood 





FIGURE 2 Freud's theory of personality development suggests that there are several distinct 
stages. 


The superego, the final personality structure to develop in childhood, represents 
the rights and wrongs of society as taught and modeled by a person’s parents, teach- 
ers, and other significant individuals. The superego includes the conscience, which 
prevents us from behaving in a morally improper way by making us feel guilty if 
we do wrong. The superego helps us control impulses coming from the id, making 
our behavior less selfish and more virtuous. 

Both the superego and the id are unrealistic in that they do not consider the 
practical realities imposed by society. The superego, if left to operate without restraint, 
would create perfectionists unable to make the compromises that life requires. An 
unrestrained id would create a primitive, pleasure-seeking, thoughtless individual 
seeking to fulfill every desire without delay. As a result, the ego must mediate 
between the demands of the superego and the demands of the id. 


DEVELOPING PERSONALITY: PSYCHOSEXUAL STAGES 


Freud also provided us with a view of how personality develops through a series of 
five psychosexual stages during which children encounter conflicts between the 
demands of society and their own sexual urges (in which sexuality is more about 
experiencing pleasure and less about lust). According to Freud, failure to resolve the 
conflicts at a particular stage can result in fixations, conflicts or concerns that persist 
beyond the developmental period in which they first occur. Such conflicts may be 
due to having needs ignored or (conversely) being overindulged during the earlier 
period. 

The sequence Freud proposed is noteworthy because it explains how experiences 
and difficulties during a particular childhood stage may predict specific characteris- 
tics in the adult personality. This theory is also unique in associating each stage with 
a major biological function, which Freud assumed to be the focus of pleasure in a 
given period. (See Figure 2 for a summary of the stages.) 

In the first psychosexual stage of development, called the oral stage, the baby’s 
mouth is the focal point of pleasure. During the first 12 to 18 months of life, children 
suck, eat, mouth, and bite anything they can put into their mouths. To Freud, this 
behavior suggested that the mouth is the primary site of a kind of sexual pleasure 
and that weaning (withdrawing the breast or bottle) represents the main conflict 
during the oral stage. If infants are either overindulged (perhaps by being fed every 
time they cry) or frustrated in their search for oral gratification, they may become 
fixated at this stage. For example, fixation might occur if an infant’s oral needs were 


441 


Major Characteristics 


Interest in oral gratification from sucking, 
eating, mouthing, biting 


Gratification from expelling and 
withholding feces; coming to terms with 
society's controls relating to toilet training 


Interest in the genitals, coming to terms 
with Oedipal conflict leading to 
identification with same-sex parent 


Sexual concerns largely unimportant 


Reemergence of sexual interests and 
establishment of mature sexual 
relationships 


superego According to Freud, the 
final personality structure to develop; 
it represents the rights and wrongs of 
society as handed down by a person’s 
parents, teachers, and other important 
figures. 


psychosexual stages Developmental 
periods that children pass through 
during which they encounter conflicts 
between the demands of society and 
their own sexual urges. 


fixations Conflicts or concerns that 
persist beyond the developmental 
period in which they first occur. 






Alert 


The five psychosexual stages 
of personality development 
in Freud’s theory—oral, 
anal, phallic, latency, and 
genital— indicate how 
personality develops as 
people age. 


oral stage According to Freud, a stage 
from birth to age 12 to 18 months, in 
which an infant’s center of pleasure is 
the mouth. 


442 Chapter 13 Personality 





According to Freud, a child goes 
through the anal stage from age 12 to 
18 months until 3 years of age. Toilet 
training is a crucial event at this 
stage—one that psychoanalytic theory 
claims directly influences the formation 
of an individual's personality. 


anal stage According to Freud, a stage 
from age 12 to 18 months to 3 years of 
age, in which a child’s pleasure is 
centered on the anus. 


phallic stage According to Freud, a 
period beginning around age 3 during 
which a child’s pleasure focuses on the 
genitals. 


Oedipal conflict A child’s sexual 
interest in his or her opposite-sex 
parent, typically resolved through 
identification with the same-sex 
parent. 


identification The process of wanting 
to be like another person as much as 
possible, imitating that person’s 
behavior and adopting similar beliefs 
and values. 


latency period According to Freud, 
the period between the phallic stage 
and puberty during which children’s 
sexual concerns are temporarily put 
aside. 


genital stage According to Freud, the 
period from puberty until death, 
marked by mature sexual behavior 
(that is, sexual intercourse). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


constantly gratified immediately at the first sign of hunger rather than if the infant 
learned that feeding takes place on a schedule because eating whenever an infant 
wants to eat is not always realistic. Fixation at the oral stage might produce an adult 
who was unusually interested in oral activities—eating, talking, smoking—or who 
showed symbolic sorts of oral interests such as being “bitingly” sarcastic or very 
gullible (“swallowing” anything). 

From around age 12 to 18 months until 3 years of age—a period when the 
emphasis in Western cultures is on toilet training—a child enters the anal stage. At 
this point, the major source of pleasure changes from the mouth to the anal region, 
and children obtain considerable pleasure from both retention and expulsion of feces. 
If toilet training is particularly demanding, fixation might occur. Fixation during the 
anal stage might result in unusual rigidity, orderliness, punctuality—or extreme dis- 
orderliness or sloppiness—in adulthood. 

At about age 3, the phallic stage begins. At this point there is another major shift 
in the child’s primary source of pleasure. Now interest focuses on the genitals and 
the pleasures derived from fondling them. During this stage the child must also 
negotiate one of the most important hurdles of personality development: the Oedipal 
conflict. According to Freudian theory, as children focus attention on their genitals, 
the differences between male and female anatomy become more salient. Furthermore, 
according to Freud, at this time the male unconsciously begins to develop a sexual 
interest in his mother, starts to see his father as a rival, and harbors a wish to kill 
his father—as Oedipus did in the ancient Greek tragedy. But because he views his 
father as too powerful, he develops a fear that his father may retaliate drastically by 
removing the source of the threat: the son’s penis. The fear of losing one’s penis leads 
to castration anxiety, which ultimately becomes so powerful that the child represses 
his desires for his mother and identifies with his father. Identification is the process 
of wanting to be like another person as much as possible, imitating that person’s 
behavior and adopting similar beliefs and values. By identifying with his father, a 
son seeks to obtain a woman like his unattainable mother. 

For girls, the process is different. Freud reasoned that girls begin to experience sexual 
arousal toward their fathers and begin to experience penis envy. They wish they had the 
anatomical part that, at least to Freud, seemed most clearly “missing” in girls. Blaming 
their mothers for their lack of a penis, girls come to believe that their mothers are respon- 
sible for their “castration.” (This aspect of Freud’s theory later provoked accusations that 
he considered women to be inferior to men.) Like males, though, they find that they can 
resolve such unacceptable feelings by identifying with the same-sex parent, behaving like 
her, and adopting her attitudes and values. In this way, a girl’s identification with her 
mother is completed. 

At this point, the Oedipal conflict is said to be resolved, and Freudian theory 
assumes that both males and females move on to the next stage of development. If 
difficulties arise during this period, however, all sorts of problems are thought to 
occur, including improper sex-role behavior and the failure to develop a conscience. 

After the resolution of the Oedipal conflict, typically around age 5 or 6, children 
move into the latency period, which lasts until puberty. During this period, sexual 
interests become dormant, even in the unconscious. Then, during adolescence, sexual 
feelings re-emerge, which marks the start of the final period, the genital stage, which 
extends until death. The focus during the genital stage is on mature, adult sexuality, 
which Freud defined as sexual intercourse. 


DEFENSE MECHANISMS 


Freud’s efforts to describe and theorize about the underlying dynamics of personal- 
ity and its development were motivated by very practical problems that his patients 
faced in dealing with anxiety, an intense, negative emotional experience. According 
to Freud, anxiety is a danger signal to the ego. Although anxiety can arise from 
realistic fears—such as seeing a poisonous snake about to strike—it can also occur 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 40 Psychodynamic Approaches to Personality 


Freud’s Defense Mechanisms 





Defense Mechanism Explanation 

Repression Unacceptable or unpleasant impulses 
are pushed back into the unconscious. 

Regression People behave as if they were at an 


earlier stage of development. 


Displacement The expression of an unwanted feeling 
or thought is redirected from a more 
threatening powerful person to a 


weaker one. 


Rationalization People provide self-justifying 
explanations in place of the actual, but 


threatening, reason for their behavior. 


Denial People refuse to accept or acknowledge 
an anxiety-producing piece of 
information. 

Projection People attribute unwanted impulses 
and feelings to someone else. 

Sublimation People divert unwanted impulses into 


socially approved thoughts, feelings, or 
behaviors. 


Reaction formation Unconscious impulses are expressed as 


their opposite in consciousness. 


FIGURE 3 According to Freud, people are able to use a wide range of defense mechanisms to 
cope with anxieties. 


in the form of neurotic anxiety in which irrational impulses emanating from the id 
threaten to burst through and become uncontrollable. 

Because anxiety is obviously unpleasant, Freud believed that people develop a 
range of defense mechanisms to deal with it. Defense mechanisms are unconscious 
strategies that people use to reduce anxiety by concealing its source from themselves 
and others. 

The primary defense mechanism is repression in which unacceptable or unpleas- 
ant id impulses are pushed back into the unconscious. Repression is the most direct 
method of dealing with anxiety; instead of handling an anxiety-producing impulse 
on a conscious level, we simply ignore it. For example, a college student who feels 
hatred for his mother may repress those personally and socially unacceptable feel- 
ings. The feelings remain lodged within the unconscious because acknowledging 
them would provoke anxiety. Similarly, memories of childhood abuse may be 
repressed. Although such memories may not be consciously recalled, according to 
Freud they can affect later behavior, and they may be revealed through dreams or 
slips of the tongue or symbolically in some other fashion. 

If repression is ineffective in keeping anxiety at bay, we might use other defense 
mechanisms. Freud and later his daughter Anna Freud (who became a well-known 
psychoanalyst) formulated an extensive list of potential defense mechanisms. The 
major defense mechanisms are summarized in Figure 3 (Conte, Plutchik & Draguns, 
2004; Hentschel et al., 2004; Cramer, 2007). 

All of us employ defense mechanisms to some degree, according to Freudian 
theory, and they can serve a useful purpose by protecting us from unpleasant 


Example 


A woman is unable to recall that she 
was raped. 


A boss has a temper tantrum when an 
employee makes a mistake. 


A brother yells at his younger sister 
after a teacher gives him a bad grade. 


A student who goes out drinking the 
night before a big test rationalizes his 
behavior by saying the test isn’t all that 
important. 


A student refuses to believe that he has 
flunked a course. 


A man who is unfaithful to his wife and 
feels guilty suspects that his wife is 
unfaithful. 


A person with strong feelings of 
aggression becomes a soldier. 


A mother who unconsciously resents 
her child acts in an overly loving way 
toward the child. 


defense mechanisms In Freudian 
theory, unconscious strategies that 
people use to reduce anxiety by 
concealing the source of it from 
themselves and others. 


repression The primary defense 
mechanism in which unacceptable or 
unpleasant id impulses are pushed 
back into the unconscious. 


dy Alert 


Use Figure 3 to remember the 
most common defense 
mechanisms (unconscious 
strategies used to reduce 
anxiety by concealing its 
source from ourselves and 
others). 


443 


444 Chapter 13 Personality 


ota Ss 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


information. Yet some people fall prey to them to such an extent 
that they must constantly direct a large amount of psychic energy 
toward hiding and rechanneling unacceptable impulses. When this 
occurs, everyday living becomes difficult. In such cases, the result 
is a mental disorder produced by anxiety—what Freud called “neu- 
rosis.” (Psychologists rarely use this term today, although it endures 
in everyday conversation.) 


EVALUATING FREUD’S LEGACY 


Freud’s theory has had a significant impact on the field of psychology— 
and even more broadly on Western philosophy and literature. Many 
people have accepted the ideas of the unconscious, defense mecha- 
nisms, and childhood roots of adult psychological difficulties. 

However, many contemporary personality psychologists have 
leveled significant criticisms against psychoanalytic theory. Among 
the most important is the lack of compelling scientific data to support 
it. Although individual case studies seem supportive, we lack conclu- 
sive evidence that shows the personality is structured and operates 
along the lines Freud laid out. The lack of evidence is due, in part, 
to the fact that Freud’s conception of personality is built on unobserv- 
able abstract concepts. Moreover, it is not clear that the stages of 
personality Freud laid out provide an accurate description of person- 
ality development. We also know now that important changes in per- 
sonality can occur in adolescence and adulthood—something that 
Freud did not believe happened. Instead, he argued that personality 
largely is set by adolescence. 

The vague nature of Freud’s theory also makes it difficult to pre- 
dict how an adult will display certain developmental difficulties. For 


Imitating a person's behavior and adopting similar beliefs instance, if a person is fixated at the anal stage, according to Freud, he 
and values are part of Freud’s concept of identification. How Fr she may be unusually messy—or unusually neat. Freud’s theory 
can this concept be applied to the definition of gender offers no way to predict how the difficulty will be exhibited (Macmil- 


roles? Is identification similar in all cultures? 


lan, 1996; Crews, 1996). Furthermore, Freud can be faulted for seeming 

to view women as inferior to men because he argued that women have 
weaker superegos than men and in some ways unconsciously yearn to be men (the 
concept of penis envy). 

Finally, Freud made his observations and derived his theory from a limited pop- 
ulation. His theory was based almost entirely on upper-class Austrian women living 
in the strict, puritanical era of the early 1900s, who had come to him seeking treat- 
ment for psychological and physical problems. How far one can generalize beyond 
this population is a matter of considerable debate. For instance, in some Pacific Island 
societies, the mother’s oldest brother and not the father plays the role of disciplinar- 
ian. In such a culture, it is unreasonable to argue that the Oedipal conflict will prog- 
ress in the same way that it did in Austrian society in which the father typically was 
the major disciplinarian. In short, a cross-cultural perspective raises questions about 
the universality of Freud’s view of personality development (Doi, 1990; Altman, 1996; 
Spielberger, 2006). 

Still, Freud generated an important method of treating psychological distur- 
bances called psychoanalysis. As we will see when we discuss treatment approaches 
to psychological disorders, psychoanalysis remains in use today (Messer & McWil- 
liams, 2003; Heller, 2005; Riolo, 2007). 

Moreover, Freud’s emphasis on the unconscious has been partially supported by 
current research on dreams and implicit memory. As we first noted when we dis- 
cussed dreaming, advances in neuroscience are consistent with some of Freud’s argu- 
ments. For example, the fact that some behavior is motivated by occurrences that 
apparently have been forgotten, as well as the discovery of neural pathways relating 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 40 Psychodynamic Approaches to Personality 445 


to emotional memories, supports the notion of repression. Furthermore, cognitive 
and social psychologists have found increasing evidence that unconscious processes 
help us think about and evaluate our world, set goals, and choose a course of action. 
The degree to which future neuroscientific advances will support the criticisms lev- 
eled against Freud’s psychoanalytic theory remains to be seen (Derryberry, 2006; 
Litowitz, 2007; Turnbull & Solms, 2007). 


The Neo-Freudian 
Psychoanalysts: 
Building on Freud 


Freud laid the foundation for important work done by a series of successors who 
were trained in traditional Freudian theory but later rejected some of its major points. 
These theorists are known as neo-Freudian psychoanalysts. 

The neo-Freudians placed greater emphasis than Freud on the functions of the 
ego by suggesting that it has more control than the id over day-to-day activities. 
They focused more on the social environment and minimized the importance of sex 
as a driving force in people’s lives. They also paid greater attention to the effects of 
society and culture on personality development. 





JUNG’S COLLECTIVE UNCONSCIOUS 


Carl Jung (pronounced “yoong”), one of the most influential neo-Freudians, rejected 
Freud’s view of the primary importance of unconscious sexual urges. Instead, he 
looked at the primitive urges of the unconscious more positively and argued that 
they represented a more general and positive life force that encompasses an inborn 
drive motivating creativity and more positive resolution of conflict (Lothane, 2005; 
Cassells, 2007). 

Jung suggested that we have a universal collective unconscious, a common set of 
ideas, feelings, images, and symbols that we inherit from our relatives, the whole 
human race, and even nonhuman animal ancestors from the distant past. This collective 
unconscious is shared by everyone and is displayed in behavior that is common across 
diverse cultures—such as love of mother, belief in a supreme being, and even behavior 
as specific as fear of snakes (Oehman & Mineka, 2003; Drob, 2005; Hauke, 2006). 

Jung went on to propose that the collective unconscious contains archetypes, 
universal symbolic representations of a particular person, object, or experience. For 





In terms of Jung's theory, Harry Potter and Voldemort represent the archetypes, or universally 
recognizable symbols, of good and evil. 


neo-Freudian psychoanalysts Psycho- 
analysts who were trained in tradi- 
tional Freudian theory but who later 
rejected some of its major points. 


collective unconscious According to 
Jung, a common set of ideas, feelings, 
images, and symbols that we inherit 
from our ancestors, the whole human 
race, and even animal ancestors from 
the distant past. 

archetypes According to Jung, uni- 
versal symbolic representations of a 
particular person, object, or experience 
(such as good and evil). 


446 Chapter 13 Personality 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


instance, a mother archetype, which contains reflections of our ances- 
tors’ relationships with mother figures, is suggested by the prevalence 
of mothers in art, religion, literature, and mythology. (Think of the Vir- 
gin Mary, Earth Mother, wicked stepmothers in fairy tales, Mother’s 
Day, and so forth!) Jung also suggested that men possess an unconscious 
feminine archetype that affects how they behave, whereas women have 
an unconscious male archetype that colors their behavior (Jung, 1961; 
Bair, 2003; Smetana, 2007). 

To Jung, archetypes play an important role in determining our day-to- 
day reactions, attitudes, and values. For example, Jung might explain the 
popularity of the Star Wars movies as being due to their use of broad 
archetypes of good (Luke Skywalker) and evil (Darth Vader). 

Although no reliable research evidence confirms the existence of 
the collective unconscious—and even Jung acknowledged that such 
evidence would be difficult to produce—Jung’s theory has had 
significant influence in areas beyond psychology. For example, person- 
ality types derived from Jung’s personality approach form the basis 
for the Myers-Briggs personality test, which is widely used in business 
and industry to provide insights into how employees make decisions 
and perform on the job (Gladwell, 2004; Bayne, 2005; Furnham & 
Crump, 2005). 


HORNEY’S NEO-FREUDIAN PERSPECTIVE 


Karen Horney was one of the earliest proponents of 
women’s issues. 


inferiority complex According to 
Adler, a problem affecting adults who 
have not been able to overcome the 
feelings of inferiority that they devel- 
oped as children, when they were 
small and limited in their knowledge 
about the world. 


Karen Horney (pronounced “HORN-eye”) was one of the earliest psy- 

chologists to champion women’s issues and is sometimes called the first 
feminist psychologist. Horney suggested that personality develops in the context of 
social relationships and depends particularly on the relationship between parents 
and child and how well the child’s needs are met. She rejected Freud’s suggestion 
that women have penis envy; she asserted that what women envy most in men is 
not their anatomy but the independence, success, and freedom women often are 
denied (Horney, 1937; Miletic, 2002; Smith, 2007). 

Horney was also one of the first to stress the importance of cultural factors in 
the determination of personality. For example, she suggested that society’s rigid gen- 
der roles for women lead them to experience ambivalence about success because they 
fear they will make enemies if they are too successful. Her conceptualizations, devel- 
oped in the 1930s and 1940s, laid the groundwork for many of the central ideas of 
feminism that emerged decades later (Eckardt, 2005; Jones, 2006). 


ADLER AND THE OTHER NEO-FREUDIANS 


Alfred Adler, another important neo-Freudian psychoanalyst, also considered Freud- 
ian theory’s emphasis on sexual needs misplaced. Instead, Adler proposed that the 
primary human motivation is a striving for superiority, not in terms of superiority 
over others but in a quest for self-improvement and perfection. 

Adler used the term inferiority complex to describe situations in which adults 
have not been able to overcome the feelings of inferiority they developed as children, 
when they were small and limited in their knowledge about the world. Early social 
relationships with parents have an important effect on children’s ability to outgrow 
feelings of personal inferiority and instead to orient themselves toward attaining 
more socially useful goals, such as improving society. 

Other neo-Freudians included Erik Erikson, whose theory of psychosocial devel- 
opment we discussed in earlier modules, and Freud’s daughter, Anna Freud. Like 
Adler and Horney, they focused less than Freud on inborn sexual and aggressive 
drives and more on the social and cultural factors behind personality. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 40 Psychodynamic Approaches to Personality 447 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP EVALUATE 
How do psychologists define and use the concept of | 1. _____ approaches state that behavior is motivated 
personality? primarily by unconscious forces. 
e Personality is the pattern of enduring characteristics that 2. Match each section of the personality (according to Freud) 
produce consistency and individuality in a given person. with its description: 
(p. 438) 1. Ego a. Determines right from wrong on 
2. Id the basis of cultural standards. 


What do the theories of Freud and his successors tell us about 
the structure and development of personality? 

e According to psychodynamic approaches to personality, 
much behavior is caused by parts of personality that are 
found in the unconscious and of which we are unaware. 
(p. 439) 

e Freud’s psychoanalytic theory, one of the psycho- 
dynamic approaches, suggests that personality is com- 
posed of the id, the ego, and the superego. The id is the 
unorganized, inborn part of personality whose purpose is 
to immediately reduce tensions relating to hunger, sex, 
aggression, and other primitive impulses. The ego re- 
strains instinctual energy to maintain the individual’s 
safety and to help the person be a member of society. The 
superego represents society’s rights and wrongs and in- 
cludes the conscience. (p. 440) 

e Freud’s psychoanalytic theory suggests that personality RETHINK 
develops through a series of psychosexual stages (oral, 
anal, phallic, latency, and genital), each of which is asso- 
ciated with a primary biological function. (p. 441) 

e Defense mechanisms, according to Freudian theory, are 
unconscious strategies with which people reduce anxi- 
eties relating to impulses from the id. (p. 443) 

e Freud’s psychoanalytic theory has provoked a number of 
criticisms, including a lack of supportive scientific data, 
the theory’s inadequacy in making predictions, and its Answers to Evaluate Questions 
reliance on a highly restricted population. On the other suIstueYpauT asuajap ‘p "E ‘L-g -7 ‘q-] ‘Z TueUApoypAsd 1 
hand, recent neuroscience research has offered some 
support for the concept of the unconscious. (p. 444) 

e Neo-Freudian psychoanalytic theorists built on Freud’s 
work, although they placed greater emphasis on the role 
of the ego and paid more attention to the role of social 
factors in determining behavior. (p. 445) 


3. Superego b. Operates according to the 
“reality principle”; energy is 
redirected to integrate the 
person into society. 

c. Seeks to reduce tension brought 
on by primitive drives. 
3. Which of the following represents the proper order of per- 
sonality development, according to Freud? 

a. Oral, phallic, latency, anal, genital 

b. Anal, oral, phallic, genital, latency 

c. Oral, anal, phallic, latency, genital 

d. Latency, phallic, anal, genital, oral 

4. is the term Freud used to describe 
unconscious strategies used to reduce anxiety. 





1. Can you think of ways in which Freud’s theories of un- 
conscious motivations are commonly used in popular cul- 
ture? How accurately do you think such popular uses of 
Freudian theories reflect Freud’s ideas? 

2. From the perspective of an advertising executive: How might 
you use Jung’s concept of archetypes in designing your 
advertisements? Which of the archetypes would you use? 


KEY TERMS 


personality p. 438 ego p.440 phallic stage p. 442 neo-Freudian 
psychodynamic approaches superego p. 441 Oedipal conflict p. 442 psychoanalysts p. 445 
to personality p. 439 psychosexual identification p. 442 collective 
psychoanalytic stages p. 441 latency period p. 442 unconscious p. 445 
theory p. 439 fixations p. 441 genital stage p. 442 archetypes p. 445 
unconscious p. 439 oral stage p. 441 defense mechanisms p.443 inferiority 


id p. 440 anal stage p. 442 repression p. 443 complex p. 446 


MODULE 41 


What are the major aspects of 
trait, learning, biological and 
evolutionary, and humanistic 
approaches to personality? 





SINVddSY STIVA 404 dn0Y9 ILVYV dS 
SOSA ONE = CB 
CEANN dchW/ Sila 
WNYOISLNVUldSVSS9/SN WVY9S 131 
OL8S960lL tr8l8St/sdnos6/w09 q 
SPIM VAJI PYG MMC) Voy 
1893131 3 ddysyeyA q4 uo Sp Ulf 


2 


i 


A model of personality 
that seeks to identify the basic traits 
necessary to describe personality. 


Consistent personality charac- 
teristics and behaviors displayed in 
different situations. 


448 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


“Tell me about Nelson,” said Johnetta. 


“Oh, he’s just terrific. He’s the friendliest guy I know—goes out of his way to be nice 
to everyone. He hardly ever gets mad. He’s just so even-tempered, no matter what's 
happening. And he’s really smart, too. About the only thing I don’t like is that he’s 
always in such a hurry to get things done. He seems to have boundless energy, much 
more than I have.” 


“He sounds great to me, especially in comparison to Rico,” replied Johnetta. “He is so 
self-centered and arrogant that it drives me crazy. I sometimes wonder why I ever 
started going out with him.” 


Friendly. Even-tempered. Smart. Energetic. Self-centered. Arrogant. 

The above exchange is made up of a series of trait characterizations of the speak- 
ers’ friends. In fact, much of our own understanding of others’ behavior is based on 
the premise that people possess certain traits that are consistent across different situ- 
ations. For example, we generally assume that if someone is outgoing and sociable 
in one situation, he or she is outgoing and sociable in other situations (Gilbert et al., 
1992; Gilbert, Miller, & Ross, 1998; Mischel, 2004). 

Dissatisfaction with the emphasis in psychoanalytic theory on unconscious—and 
difficult to demonstrate—processes in explaining a person’s behavior led to the devel- 
opment of alternative approaches to personality, including a number of trait-based 
approaches. Other theories reflect established psychological perspectives, such as 
learning theory, biological and evolutionary approaches, and the humanistic approach. 


Trait Approaches: Placing 
Labels on Personality 


If someone asked you to characterize another person, like Johnetta and her friend 
you probably would come up with a list of that individual’s personal qualities as 
you see them. But how would you know which of those qualities are most important 
to an understanding of that person’s behavior? 

Personality psychologists have asked similar questions. To answer them, they 
have developed a model of personality known as trait theory. Trait theory seeks to 
explain in a straightforward way the consistencies in individuals’ behavior. Traits are 
consistent personality characteristics and behaviors displayed in different situations. 

Trait theorists do not assume that some people have a trait and others do not; 
rather, they propose that all people possess certain traits but the degree to which a 
particular trait applies to a specific person varies and can be quantified. For instance, 
you may be relatively friendly, whereas I may be relatively unfriendly. But we both 
have a “friendliness” trait, although your degree of “friendliness” is higher than 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 41 Trait, Learning, Biological and Evolutionary, and Humanistic Approaches to Personality 449 


mine. The major challenge for trait theorists taking this approach has been to identify 
the specific primary traits necessary to describe personality. As we shall see, different 
theorists have come up with surprisingly different sets of traits. 


ALLPORT’S TRAIT THEORY: IDENTIFYING 
BASIC CHARACTERISTICS 


When personality psychologist Gordon Allport systematically pored over an unabridged 
dictionary in the 1930s, he came up with some 18,000 separate terms that could be 
used to describe personality. Although he was able to pare down the list to a mere 
4,500 descriptors after eliminating words with the same meaning, he was left with a 
problem crucial to all trait approaches: Which of those traits were the most basic? 

Allport eventually answered this question by suggesting that there are three 
fundamental categories of traits: cardinal, central, and secondary (Allport, 1961, 
1966). A cardinal trait is a single characteristic that directs most of a person’s activities. 
For example, a totally selfless woman may direct all her energy toward humanitarian 
activities; an intensely power-hungry person may be driven by an all-consuming 
need for control. 

Most people, however, do not develop a single, comprehensive cardinal trait. 
Instead, they possess a handful of central traits that make up the core of personality. 
Central traits, such as honesty and sociability, are an individual’s major characteristics; 
they usually number from five to ten in any one person. Finally, secondary traits are 
characteristics that affect behavior in fewer situations and are less influential than 
central or cardinal traits. For instance, a reluctance to eat meat and a love of modern 
art would be considered secondary traits (Nicholson, 2003; Glicksohn & Nahari, 2007). 


CATTELL AND EYSENCK: FACTORING OUT PERSONALITY 


Later attempts to identify primary personality traits have centered on a statistical tech- 
nique known as factor analysis. Factor analysis is a statistical method of identifying 
associations among a large number of variables to reveal more general patterns. For 
example, a personality researcher might administer a questionnaire to many participants 
that asks them to describe themselves by referring to an extensive list of traits. By sta- 
tistically combining responses and computing which traits are associated with one 
another in the same person, a researcher can identify the most fundamental patterns or 
combinations of traits—called factors—that underlie participants’ responses. 

Using factor analysis, personality psychologist Raymond Cattell (1965) suggested 
that 16 pairs of source traits represent the basic dimensions of personality. Using those 
source traits, he developed the Sixteen Personality Factor Questionnaire, or 16 PF, a 
measure that provides scores for each of the source traits (Cattell, Cattell, & Cattell, 
1993; 2000). 

Another trait theorist, psychologist Hans Eysenck (1995), also used factor analy- 
sis to identify patterns of traits, but he came to a very different conclusion about the 
nature of personality. He found that personality could best be described in terms of 
just three major dimensions: extraversion, neuroticism, and psychoticism. The extraver- 
sion dimension relates to the degree of sociability, whereas the neurotic dimension 
encompasses emotional stability. Finally, psychoticism refers to the degree to which 
reality is distorted. By evaluating people along these three dimensions, Eysenck was 
able to predict behavior accurately in a variety of situations. Figure 1 lists specific 
traits associated with each of the dimensions. 


THE BIG FIVE PERSONALITY TRAITS 


For the last two decades, the most influential trait approach contends that five traits or 
factors—called the “Big Five”—lie at the core of personality. Using modern factor analytic 
statistical techniques, a host of researchers have identified a similar set of five factors 





Alert 


All trait theories explain 
personality in terms of traits 
(consistent personality 
characteristics and behav- 
iors), but they differ in terms 
of which and how many traits are 
seen as fundamental. 





Psychoticism 
+ Aggressive 
e Cold 
e Egocentric 


e Impersonal 
e Impulsive 


FIGURE 1 According to Eysenck, person- 
ality could best be described in terms of 
just three major dimensions: extraversion, 
neuroticism, and psychoticism. Eysenck 
was able to predict behavior accurately 

in a variety of types of situations by 
evaluating people along these three 
dimensions. (Source: Eysenck, 1990). 


450 Chapter 13 Personality 


FIGURE 2 Five broad trait factors, 
referred to as the “Big Five,’ are 
considered to be the core of personality. 
(Source: Adapted from Pervin, 1990, Chapter 3, 
and McCrae & Costa, 1986, p. 1002.) 


dy Alert 


You can remember the “Big 
Five” set of personality traits 
by using the acronym 
OCEAN (openness to expe- 
rience, conscientiousness, 
extraversion, agreeableness, and 
neuroticism). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


The Big Five Personality Factors and Dimensions of Sample Traits 


Openness to experience 
Independent—Conforming 
Imaginative—Practical 

Preference for variety—Preference for routine 


Conscientiousness 
Careful—Careless 
Disciplined—Impulsive 
Organized—Disorganized 
Extraversion 
Talkative—Quiet 
Fun-loving—Sober 
Sociable—Retiring 


Agreeableness 
Sympathetic—Fault-finding 
Kind—Cold 
Appreciative—Unfriendly 


Neuroticism (Emotional Stability) 
Stable—Tense 

Calm—Anxious 

Secure—Insecure 


that underlie personality. The five factors, described in Figure 2, are openness to experience, 
conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism (emotional stability). 

The Big Five emerge consistently across a number of domains. For example, fac- 
tor analyses of major personality inventories, self-report measures made by observers 
of others’ personality traits, and checklists of self-descriptions yield similar factors. 
In addition, the Big Five emerge consistently in different populations of individuals, 
including children, college students, older adults, and speakers of different lan- 
guages. Cross-cultural research conducted in areas ranging from Europe to the Mid- 
dle East to Africa also has been supportive. Finally, studies of brain functioning show 
that Big Five personality traits are related to the way the brain processes information 
(McCrae et al., 2005; Schmitt, Allik, & McCrae, 2007; Schmitt et al., 2008). 

In short, a growing consensus exists that the Big Five represent the best description 
of personality traits we have today. Still, the debate over the specific number and kinds 
of traits—and even the usefulness of trait approaches in general—remains a lively one. 


EVALUATING TRAIT APPROACHES TO PERSONALITY 


Trait approaches have several virtues. They provide a clear, straightforward explana- 
tion of people’s behavioral consistencies. Furthermore, traits allow us to readily com- 
pare one person with another. Because of these advantages, trait approaches to 
personality have had an important influence on the development of several useful 
personality measures (Funder, 1991; Wiggins, 2003; Larsen & Buss, 2006). 

However, trait approaches also have some drawbacks. For example, we have 
seen that various trait theories describing personality come to very different conclu- 
sions about which traits are the most fundamental and descriptive. The difficulty in 
determining which of the theories is the most accurate has led some personality 
psychologists to question the validity of trait conceptions of personality in general. 

Actually, there is an even more fundamental difficulty with trait approaches. 
Even if we are able to identify a set of primary traits, we are left with little more 
than a label or description of personality—rather than an explanation of behavior. If 
we say that someone who donates money to charity has the trait of generosity, we 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Judged at First Sight 


Imagine that you've just showed up to an 
important job interview dressed in your 
best business attire, and the personnel di- 
rector greets you and ushers you into her 
office. Or perhaps you've arrived at a 
party where there are many other stu- 
dents from your campus that you'd like to 
get to know. In either case, you might be 
focused on how well you’re coming across 
and thinking that you have only a few 
minutes to make a good first impression— 
and that’s where you're wrong. Whatever 
impression you were going to make, you 
did so already. 

At least that’s the surprising finding of 
recent research on how people make judg- 
ments of others’ personality. For instance, 
in one study, participants were shown pho- 
tographs of faces of unfamiliar people and 
asked to rate those people on a number of 
characteristics, such as attractiveness, 
trustworthiness, and competence. These 
participants showed high agreement in 
their judgments of these characteristics. 
Then another group of participants were 
shown the same photographs, but they 
were asked to make the same judgments 
under time constraints. In some cases the 





Research has found that first impressions 
are made almost instantaneously. 


with no time constraints, the researchers 
found that the judgments were extremely 
similar. Moreover, the length of the time 


constraint made no difference—the judg- 
ments made in Yo of a second were just as 
accurate as those made in a % or 1 second. 

What these findings suggest is that 
people make virtually instantaneous 
judgments about others essentially the 
moment that they lay eyes on them. This 
may be particularly true of judgments 
about attractiveness and trustworthiness, 
which the participants were able to assess 
most quickly. But even other types of 
judgments—such as a person’s sexual 
orientation—are made extremely quickly; 
sometimes such quick judgments are 
more accurate than those made more 
thoughtfully and deliberately (Rule, 
Amabady, & Hallett, 2009). 

Researchers have theorized that the 
ability to judge characteristics quickly and 
accurately may have evolved in humans 
because it once had survival value. Today 
it means, for better or for worse, that we 
accurately size each other up at just a 
glance (Todorov & Duchaine, 2008; Oveis 
et al., 2009). 








judgment had to be made in 1 second, in 
others in 1⁄2 of a second, and in still others 
in just “’o of a second (Willis & Todorov, 
2006; Oosterhof & Todorov, 2008). 

When the time-constrained judgments 
were compared to the judgments made 


an evolutionary perspective? 


e Do you think people should attempt to override their first judgments about others’ 
personalities and make more thoughtful judgments? Why or why not? 
e Why would the ability to judge personality characteristics quickly be valuable from 








still do not know why that person became generous in the first place or the reasons 
for displaying generosity in a specific situation. In the view of some critics, then, 
traits do not provide explanations for behavior; they merely describe it. On the other 
hand, laypersons certainly use traits to make judgments of others, as we can see in 
Applying Psychology in the 21st Century. 


Learning Approaches: 
We Are What We've Learned 


The psychodynamic and trait approaches we’ve discussed concentrate on the “inner” 
person—the fury of an unobservable but powerful id or a hypothetical but critical set 
of traits. In contrast, learning approaches to personality focus on the “outer” person. 
To a strict learning theorist, personality is simply the sum of learned responses to the 
external environment. Internal events such as thoughts, feelings, and motivations are 





451 


452 Chapter 13 Personality 


social cognitive approaches to 
personality Theories that emphasize 
the influence of a person’s cognitions— 
thoughts, feelings, expectations, and 
values—as well as observation of 
others’ behavior, in determining 
personality. 


self-efficacy Belief in one’s personal 
capabilities. Self-efficacy underlies 
people’s faith in their ability to carry 
out a particular behavior or produce a 
desired outcome. 


ae t 
zt 

rua 

s “A 


EUN 





Self-efficacy, the belief in one’s own 
capabilities, leads to higher aspirations 
and greater persistence. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


ignored. Although the existence of personality is not denied, learning theorists say 
that it is best understood by looking at features of a person’s environment. 


SKINNER’S BEHAVIORIST APPROACH 


According to the most influential learning theorist, B. F. Skinner (who carried out 
pioneering work on operant conditioning), personality is a collection of learned 
behavior patterns (Skinner, 1975). Similarities in responses across different situations 
are caused by similar patterns of reinforcement that have been received in such situ- 
ations in the past. If I am sociable both at parties and at meetings, it is because I 
have been reinforced for displaying social behaviors—not because I am fulfilling an 
unconscious wish based on experiences during my childhood or because I have an 
internal trait of sociability. 

Strict learning theorists such as Skinner are less interested in the consistencies in 
behavior across situations than in ways of modifying behavior. Their view is that humans 
are infinitely changeable through the process of learning new behavior patterns. If we are 
able to control and modify the patterns of reinforcers in a situation, behavior that other 
theorists would view as stable and unyielding can be changed and ultimately improved. 
Learning theorists are optimistic in their attitudes about the potential for resolving per- 
sonal and societal problems through treatment strategies based on learning theory. 


SOCIAL COGNITIVE APPROACHES TO PERSONALITY 


Not all learning theories of personality take such a strict view in rejecting the impor- 
tance of what is “inside” a person by focusing solely on the “outside.” Unlike other 
learning approaches to personality, social cognitive approaches to personality empha- 
size the influence of cognition—thoughts, feelings, expectations, and values—as well 
as observation of others’ behavior, on personality. According to Albert Bandura, one 
of the main proponents of this point of view, people can foresee the possible outcomes 
of certain behaviors in a specific setting without actually having to carry them out. 
This understanding comes primarily through observational learning—viewing the 
actions of others and observing the consequences (Bandura, 1986, 1999). 

For instance, children who view a model behaving in, say, an aggressive manner 
tend to copy the behavior if the consequences of the model’s behavior are seen as 
positive. If, in contrast, the model’s aggressive behavior has resulted in no conse- 
quences or negative consequences, children are considerably less likely to act aggres- 
sively. According to social cognitive approaches, then, personality develops through 
repeated observation of others’ behavior. 


SELF-EFFICACY 


Bandura places particular emphasis on the role played by self-efficacy, belief in one’s 
personal capabilities. Self-efficacy underlies people’s faith in their ability to carry out 
a specific task or produce a desired outcome. People with high self-efficacy have 
higher aspirations and greater persistence in working to attain goals and ultimately 
achieve greater success than those with lower self-efficacy (Bandura & Locke, 2003; 
Glickler, 2006; Betz, 2007). 

How do we develop self-efficacy? One way is by paying close attention to our 
prior successes and failures. If we try snowboarding and experience little success, 
we'll be less likely to try it again. However, if our initial efforts appear promising, 
we'll be more likely to attempt it again. Direct reinforcement and encouragement 
from others also play a role in developing self-efficacy (Devonport & Lane, 2006; 
Buchanan & Selmon, 2008). 

Compared with other learning theories of personality, social cognitive approaches 
are distinctive in their emphasis on the reciprocity between individuals and their 
environment. Not only is the environment assumed to affect personality, but people’s 
behavior and personalities are also assumed to “feed back” and modify the environ- 
ment (Bandura, 1999, 2000). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 41 Trait, Learning, Biological and Evolutionary, and Humanistic Approaches to Personality 453 


HOW MUCH CONSISTENCY EXISTS IN PERSONALITY? 


Another social cognitive theorist, Walter Mischel, takes a different approach to per- 
sonality from that of Albert Bandura. He rejects the view that personality consists of 
broad traits that lead to substantial consistencies in behavior across different situa- 
tions. Instead, he sees personality as considerably more variable from one situation 
to another (Mischel, 2009). 

In this view, particular situations give rise to particular kinds of behavior. Some 
situations are especially influential (think of a movie theater, where everyone dis- 
plays pretty much the same behavior by sitting quietly and watching the film). Other 
situations permit much variability in behavior (think of a party, for example, where 
some people may be dancing, while others are eating and drinking). 

From this perspective, personality cannot be considered without taking the par- 
ticular context of the situation into account—a view known as situationism. In his 
cognitive-affective processing system (CAPS) theory, Mischel argues that people’s 
thoughts and emotions about themselves and the world determine how they view, 
and then react, in particular situations. Personality is thus seen as a reflection of how 
people’s prior experiences in different situations affect their behavior (Shoda & 
Mischel, 2006; Mischel & Shoda, 2008). 


SELF-ESTEEM 


Our behavior also reflects the view we have of ourselves and the way we value the 
various parts of our personalities. Self-esteem is the component of personality that 
encompasses our positive and negative self-evaluations. Unlike self-efficacy, which 
focuses on our views of whether we are able to carry out a task, self-esteem relates 
to how we feel about ourselves. 

Although people have a general level of self-esteem, it is not unidimensional. 
We may see ourselves positively in one domain but negatively in others. For exam- 
ple, a good student may have high self-esteem in academic domains but lower self- 
esteem in sports (Swann, Chang-Schneider, & Larsen McClarty, 2007; Salmela-Aro & 
Nurmi, 2007; Gentile et al., 2009). 

Self-esteem has strong cultural components. For example, having high relation- 
ship harmony—a sense of success in forming close bonds with other people—is more 
important to self-esteem in Asian cultures than it is in more individualistic Western 
societies (Spencer-Rodgers et al., 2004; Lun & Bond, 2006; Cheng & Kwan, 2008). 

Although almost everyone goes through periods of low self-esteem (for instance, 
after an undeniable failure), some people are chronically low in self-esteem. For them, 
failure seems to be an inevitable part of life. In fact, low self-esteem may lead to a 
cycle of failure in which past failure breeds future failure. 

For example, consider students with low self-esteem who are studying for a test. 
Because of their low self-esteem, they expect to do poorly on the test. In turn, this 
belief raises their anxiety level, which makes it increasingly difficult to study and 
perhaps even leading them not to work as hard. Because of these attitudes, they do, 
in fact, ultimately perform badly on the test. Ultimately, the failure reinforces their 
low self-esteem, and the cycle is perpetuated as illustrated in Figure 3. In short, low 
self-esteem can lead to a self-destructive cycle of failure. 


Low = Actual failure 


a amy 


self-esteem The component of person- 
ality that encompasses our positive and 
negative self-evaluations. 


FIGURE 3 The cycle of low self-esteem 
begins with an individual already having 
low self-esteem. As a consequence, the 
person will have low performance 
expectations and expect to fail a test, 
thereby producing anxiety and reduced 
effort. As a result, the person will actually 
fail, and failure in turn reinforces low 
self-esteem. 


454 Chapter 13 Personality 


biological and evolutionary 
approaches to personality Theories 
that suggest that important compo- 
nents of personality are inherited. 


Biological and evolutionary approaches 
to personality seek to explain the 
consistencies in personality that are 
found in some families. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


EVALUATING LEARNING APPROACHES TO PERSONALITY 


Because they ignore the internal processes that are uniquely human, traditional learning 
theorists such as Skinner have been accused of oversimplifying personality to such an 
extent that the concept becomes meaningless. Their critics think that reducing behavior 
to a series of stimuli and responses and excluding thoughts and feelings from the realm 
of personality leaves behaviorists practicing an unrealistic and inadequate form of science. 

Of course, some of these criticisms are blunted by social cognitive approaches, 
which explicitly consider the role of cognitive processes in personality. Still, learning 
approaches tend to share a highly deterministic view of human behavior, which main- 
tains that behavior is shaped primarily by forces beyond the individual’s control. As 
in psychoanalytic theory (which suggests that personality is determined by the 
unconscious forces) and trait approaches (which views personality in part as a mix- 
ture of genetically determined traits), learning theory’s reliance on deterministic prin- 
ciples de-emphasizes people’s ability to pilot their own course through life. 

Nonetheless, learning approaches have had a major impact on the study of per- 
sonality. For one thing, they have helped make personality psychology an objective, 
scientific venture by focusing on observable behavior and the effects of their environ- 
ments. In addition, they have produced important, successful means of treating a 
variety of psychological disorders. The degree of success of these treatments is 
a testimony to the merits of learning theory approaches to personality. 


Biological and Evolutionary 
Approaches: Are We Born 
with Personality? 


Approaching the question of what determines personality from a different direction, 
biological and evolutionary approaches to personality suggest that important com- 
ponents of personality are inherited. Building on the work of behavioral geneticists, 








www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 41 Trait, Learning, Biological and Evolutionary, and Humanistic Approaches to Personality 455 


researchers using biological and evolutionary approaches argue that personality is 
determined at least in part by our genes in much the same way that our height is 
largely a result of genetic contributions from our ancestors. The evolutionary per- 
spective assumes that personality traits that led to our ancestors’ survival and repro- 
ductive success are more likely to be preserved and passed on to subsequent 
generations (Buss, 2001, 2009). 

Twin studies illustrate the importance of genetic factors in personality. For 
instance, personality psychologists Auke Tellegen and colleagues at the University 
of Minnesota examined the personality traits of pairs of twins who were genetically 
identical but were raised apart from each other (Tellegen et al., 1988; Bouchard, et 
al., 2004). In the study, each twin was given a battery of personality tests, including 
one that measured 11 key personality characteristics. 

The results of the personality tests indicated that in major respects the twins were 
quite similar in personality, despite having separated at an early age. Moreover, cer- 
tain traits were more heavily influenced by heredity than others. For example, social 
potency (the degree to which a person assumes mastery and leadership roles in social 
situations) and traditionalism (the tendency to follow authority) had particularly 
strong genetic components, whereas achievement and social closeness had relatively 
weak genetic components (see Figure 4). 

Furthermore, it is increasingly clear that the roots of adult personality emerge 
in the earliest periods of life. Infants are born with a specific temperament, an innate 
disposition. Temperament encompasses several dimensions, including general 
activity level and mood. For instance, some individuals are quite active, while others 
are relatively calm. Similarly, some are relatively easygoing, while others are irritable, 


Social potency 
Traditionalism 
Stress reaction 

Absorption 


Alienation ” 
out to get me 






y Alert 


Remember that biological and 
evolutionary approaches 
focus on the way in which 
people's genetic heritage 
affects personality. 


temperament The innate disposition 
that emerges early in life. 


Is masterful; a forceful leader who likes to be 
the center of attention 


Follows rules and authority; endorses high 
moral standards and strict discipline 


Feels vulnerable and sensitive; is given to 
worrying and easily upset 


Has a vivid imagination readily captured by 
rich experience; relinquishes sense of reality 


Feels mistreated and used, that “the world is 


Has a cheerful disposition; feels confident and 


Well-being 


Harm avoidance 


Aggression 


Achievement 


Control 


Social closeness 


optimistic 


Shuns the excitement of risk and danger; 
prefers the safe route even if it is tedious 


Is physically aggressive and vindictive; has taste 
for violence; is “out to get the world” 


Works hard; strives for mastery; puts work 
and accomplishment ahead of other things 


Is cautious and plodding; is rational and sensible; 
likes carefully planned events 


Prefers emotional intimacy and close ties; turns 
to others for comfort and help 








o 
o 
N 
© 


30 40 


wu 
oO 
fon 
oO 
N 
o 


Percentage 


FIGURE 4 The inherited roots of personality. The percentages indicate the degree to which 
11 personality characteristics reflect the influence of heredity. (Source: Tellegen et al., 1988.) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


456 Chapter 13 Personality 


~~ 





2 ia 


Infants are born with particular temperaments—dispositions that are consistent throughout 
childhood. 


easily upset, and difficult to soothe. Temperament is quite consistent, with signifi- 
cant stability from infancy well into adolescence (Wachs et al., 2004; Kagan et al., 
2007; Evans & Rothbart, 2007, 2009). 

Some researchers contend that specific genes are related to personality. For 
example, people with a longer dopamine-4 receptor gene are more likely to be 
thrill-seekers than those without such a gene. These thrill-seekers tend to be extro- 
verted, impulsive, quick-tempered, and always in search of excitement and novel 
situations. Furthermore, the structure of their brains may reflect their thrill-seeking 
tendencies (see Neuroscience in Your Life, Figure 5; Robins, 2005; Golimbet et al., 2007; 
Ray et al., 2009). 

Does the identification of specific genes linked to personality, coupled with the 
existence of temperaments from the time of birth, mean that we are destined to 
have certain types of personalities? Hardly. First, it is unlikely that any single gene 
is linked to a specific trait. For instance, the dopamine-4 receptor accounts for only 
around 10% of the variation in novelty seeking between different individuals. 
The rest of the variation is attributable to other genes and environmental factors 
(Keltikangas-Jarvinen et al., 2004; Lahti et al., 2005). 

More important, genes interact with the environment. As we see in discussions 
of the heritability of intelligence and the nature-nurture issue, it is impossible to 
completely divorce genetic factors from environmental factors. Although studies of 
identical twins raised in different environments are helpful, they are not definitive 
because it is impossible to assess and control environmental factors fully. Further- 
more, estimates of the influence of genetics are just that—estimates—and apply to 
groups, not individuals. Consequently, findings such as those shown in Figure 4 must 
be regarded as approximations. 

Finally, even if more genes are found to be linked to specific personality charac- 
teristics, genes still cannot be viewed as the sole cause of personality. For one thing, 
genetically determined characteristics may not be expressed if they are not “turned 
on” by particular environmental experiences. Furthermore, behaviors produced by 
genes may help to create a specific environment. For instance, a cheerful, smiley baby 
may lead her parents to smile more and be more responsive, thereby creating a 
supportive, pleasant environment. In contrast, the parents of a cranky, fussy baby 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 41 Trait, Learning, Biological and Evolutionary, and Humanistic Approaches to Personality 457 


Wired for Thrills— 


The Biological Underpinnings of Personality 





FIGURE 5 Research suggests that personality characteristics may be related to differences in 
brain structure. Scientists used magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) to look for relationships 
between personality traits such as novelty-seeking and differences in the number of neurons 
in various parts of the brain. In this MRI, the red areas of the frontal and posterior cingulate 
are related to novelty-seeking. This MRI helps us understand that those who are more likely 
to seek novel experiences have a greater number of neurons in the highlighted areas of the 
brain. (Source: Gardini et al., 2009, Figure 1.) 





may be less inclined to smile at the child; in turn, the environment in which that 
child is raised will be less supportive and pleasant. In a sense, then, genes not only 
influence a person’s behavior—they also help produce the environment in which a 
person develops (Scarr, 1998; Plomin & Caspi, 1999; Kim-Cohen, Caspi, & Moffitt, 
2003; Kim-Cohen et al., 2005). 

Although an increasing number of personality theorists are taking biological and 
evolutionary factors into account, no comprehensive, unified theory that considers 
biological and evolutionary factors is widely accepted. Still, it is clear that certain 
personality traits have substantial genetic components and that heredity and environ- 
ment interact to determine personality (Ebstein, Benjamin, & Belmaker, 2003; 
Bouchard, 2004; South & Krueger, 2008). 


Humanistic Approaches: 
The Uniqueness of You 


In all the approaches to personality that we have discussed, where is an explanation for 
the saintliness of a Mother Teresa, the creativity of a Michelangelo, and the brilliance 
and perseverance of an Einstein? An understanding of such unique individuals—as well 
as more ordinary sorts of people who have some of the same attributes—comes from 
humanistic theory. 
According to humanistic theorists, all the approaches to personality we have 
discussed share a fundamental misperception in their views of human nature. Instead 
of seeing people as controlled by unconscious, unseen forces (psychodynamic 
approaches), a set of stable traits (trait approaches), situational reinforcements and humanistic approaches to personality 
punishments (learning theory), or inherited factors (biological and evolutionary Theories that emphasize people’s 
approaches), humanistic approaches to personality emphasize people’s inherent innate goodness and desire to achieve 
goodness and their tendency to move toward higher levels of functioning. It is this higher levels of functioning. 





458 Chapter 13 Personality 





_ | Mee PSYCHOLOGIST || 


© Robert Mankoff/The New Yorker Collection/www. 


cartoonbank.com 
| 
\ 
\ 
\ 
\ 
\ 
\ 
\ 
\ 


“So, while extortion, racketeering, and murder may be bad acts, 
they don t make you a bad person.” 


self-actualization A state of self- 
fulfillment in which people realize 
their highest potential, each in a 
unique way. 


unconditional positive regard An 
attitude of acceptance and respect on 
the part of an observer, no matter what 
a person says or does. 


FIGURE 6 According to the humanistic 
view of Carl Rogers, people have a basic 
need to be loved and respected. If you 
receive unconditional positive regard 
from others, you will develop a more 
realistic self-concept; but if the response 
is conditional, it may lead to anxiety and 
frustration. 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


conscious, self-motivated ability to change and improve, 
along with people’s unique creative impulses, that human- 
istic theorists argue make up the core of personality. 


ROGERS AND THE NEED 
FOR SELF-ACTUALIZATION 


The major proponent of the humanistic point of view is 
Carl Rogers (1971). Along with other humanistic theorists, 
such as Abraham Maslow, Rogers maintains that all people 
have a fundamental need for self-actualization, a state of 
self-fulfillment in which people realize their highest poten- 
tial, each in a unique way. He further suggests that people 
develop a need for positive regard that reflects the desire 
to be loved and respected. Because others provide this 
positive regard, we grow dependent on them. We begin to 
see and judge ourselves through the eyes of other people, relying on their values and 
being preoccupied with what they think of us. 

According to Rogers, one outgrowth of placing importance on others’ opinions is 
that a conflict may grow between people’s experiences and their self-concepts, the set of 
beliefs they hold about what they are like as individuals. If the discrepancies are minor, 
so are the consequences. But if the discrepancies are great, they will lead to psycho- 
logical disturbances in daily functioning, such as the experience of frequent anxiety. 

Rogers suggests that one way of overcoming the discrepancy between experience 
and self-concept is through the receipt of unconditional positive regard from another 
person—a friend, a spouse, or a therapist. Unconditional positive regard refers to 
an attitude of acceptance and respect on the observer’s part, no matter what a person 
says or does. This acceptance, says Rogers, gives people the opportunity to evolve 
and grow both cognitively and emotionally and to develop more realistic self- 
concepts. You may have experienced the power of unconditional positive regard 
when you confided in someone, revealing embarrassing secrets because you knew 
the listener would still love and respect you even after hearing the worst about you 
(Snyder, 2002; Marshall, 2007). 

In contrast, conditional positive regard depends on your behavior. In such cases, 
others withdraw their love and acceptance if you do something of which they don’t 
approve. The result is a discrepancy between your true self and what others wish 
you would be, which leads to anxiety and frustration (see Figure 6). 


MANKOFF 


EVALUATING HUMANISTIC APPROACHES 


Although humanistic theories suggest the value of providing unconditional positive 
regard toward people, unconditional positive regard toward humanistic theories has 
been less forthcoming. The criticisms have centered on the difficulty of verifying the 
basic assumptions of the approach as well as on the question of whether uncondi- 
tional positive regard does, in fact, lead to greater personality adjustment. 


Others’ behavior 
toward you 


Your response 





Conditional 
positive regard 


Anxiety and 
frustration 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 41 Trait, Learning, Biological and Evolutionary, and Humanistic Approaches to Personality 459 


Humanistic approaches have also been criticized for making the assumption that 
people are basically “good”—a notion that is unverifiable—and, equally important, 
for using nonscientific values to build supposedly scientific theories. Still, humanistic 
theories have been important in highlighting the uniqueness of human beings and 
guiding the development of a significant form of therapy designed to alleviate psy- 
chological difficulties (Cain, 2002; Bauman & Kopp, 2006; Elkins, 2009). 


Comparing Approaches 
to Personality 


In light of the multiple approaches we have discussed, you may be wondering which 
of the theories provides the most accurate description of personality. That question 
cannot be answered precisely. Each theory is built on different assumptions and 
focuses on somewhat different aspects of personality (see Figure 7). Furthermore, 
there is no clear way to scientifically test the various approaches and their assump- 
tions against one another. Given the complexity of every individual, it seems reason- 
able that personality can be viewed from a number of perspectives simultaneously 
(Pervin, 2003). 





Conscious Versus 


Theoretical Unconscious Nature (Hereditary 
Approach and Determinants of Factors) Versus Nurture 
Major Theorists Personality (Environmental Factors) Free Will Versus Determinism Stability Versus Modifiability 





FIGURE 7 The multiple perspectives of personality. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


460 Chapter 13 Personality 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 


What are the major aspects of trait, learning, biological and 
evolutionary, and humanistic approaches to personality? 


Trait approaches have been used to identify relatively 
enduring dimensions along which people differ from 
one another—dimensions known as traits. (p. 448) 
Learning approaches to personality concentrate on 
observable behavior. To a strict learning theorist, person- 
ality is the sum of learned responses to the external 
environment. (p. 451) 

Social cognitive approaches concentrate on the role of 
cognition in determining personality. Those approaches 
pay particular attention to self-efficacy and self-esteem 
in determining behavior. (p. 452) 

Biological and evolutionary approaches to personality 
focus on the way in which personality characteristics are 
inherited. (p. 454) 

Humanistic approaches emphasize people’s inherent 
goodness. They consider the core of personality in terms 
of a person’s ability to change and improve. (p. 457) 

The major personality approaches differ substantially 
from one another; the differences may reflect both their 
focus on different aspects of personality and the overall 
complexity of personality. (p. 459) 


EVALUATE 


1. 


Carl’s determination to succeed is the dominant force in 
all his activities and relationships. According to Gordon 


Allport’s theory, this is an example of a _______ trait. 
In contrast, Cindy’s fondness for old western movies is an 
example of a _______trait. 


KEY TERMS 


trait theory p. 448 self-efficacy p. 452 

traits p. 448 self-esteem p. 453 

social cognitive biological and evolutionary 
approaches to approaches to 


personality p. 452 personality p. 454 


2 


Eysenck might describe a person who enjoys activities 
such as parties and hang-gliding as high on what trait? 


. Proponents of which approach to personality would be 


most likely to agree with the statement, “Personality can 
be thought of as learned responses to a person’s upbring- 
ing and environment”? 

a. Humanistic 

b. Biological and evolutionary 

c. Learning 

d. Trait 


. Bandura would rate a person who would make the state- 


ment, “I know I can’t do it” as low on 


. Which approach to personality emphasizes the innate 


goodness of people and their desire to grow? 
a. Humanistic 

b. Psychodynamic 

c. Learning 

d. Biological and evolutionary 


RETHINK 


1. 


If personality traits are merely descriptive and not explan- 
atory, what use are they? Can assigning a trait to a person 
be harmful—or helpful? Why or why not? 


. From the perspective of an educator: How might you encour- 


age your students’ development of self-esteem and self- 
efficacy? What steps would you take to ensure that their 
self-esteem did not become over-inflated? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


eg ‘Aovoyya-Jfas “pf 19 g /UOISIaARI}Xxe *Z ‘ATepUODS ‘[eUTPIeD “TL 


temperament p. 455 self-actualization p. 458 
humanistic approaches to unconditional positive 
personality p. 457 regard p.458 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


You have a need for other people to like and admire you. 
You have a tendency to be critical of yourself. 
You have a great deal of unused potential that you have not turned to your advantage. 


Although you have some personality weaknesses, you generally are able to compensate 
for them. 


Relating to members of the opposite sex has presented problems for you. 


Although you appear to be disciplined and self-controlled to others, you tend to be 
anxious and insecure inside. 


At times you have serious doubts about whether you have made the right decision or 
done the right thing. 


You prefer a certain amount of change and variety and become dissatisfied when 
hemmed in by restrictions and limitations. 


You do not accept others’ statements without satisfactory proof. 
You have found it unwise to be too frank in revealing yourself to others. 


If you think these statements provide a surprisingly accurate account of your per- 
sonality, you are not alone: Most college students think that these descriptions are 
tailored just to them. In fact, the statements were designed intentionally to be so 
vague that they apply to just about anyone (Forer, 1949; Russo, 1981). 

The ease with which we can agree with such imprecise statements underscores 
the difficulty in coming up with accurate and meaningful assessments of people’s 
personalities. Psychologists interested in assessing personality must be able to 
define the most meaningful ways of discriminating between one person’s person- 
ality and another’s. To do this, they use psychological tests, standard measures 
devised to assess behavior objectively. With the results of such tests, psychologists 
can help people better understand themselves and make decisions about their 
lives. Researchers interested in the causes and consequences of personality also 
employ psychological tests (Aiken, 2000; Kaplan & Saccuzzo, 2001; Hambleton, 
2006). 

Like the assessments that seek to measure intelligence, all psychological tests 
must have reliability and validity. Reliability refers to a test's measurement consis- 
tency. If a test is reliable, it yields the same result each time it is administered to a 
specific person or group. In contrast, unreliable tests give different results each time 
they are administered. 

For meaningful conclusions to be drawn, tests also must be valid. Tests have 
validity when they actually measure what they are designed to measure. If a test is 
constructed to measure sociability, for instance, we need to know that it actually 
measures sociability and not some other trait. 


How can we most accurately 
assess personality? 


What are the major types of 
personality measures? 


Standard mea- 
sures devised to assess behavior 
objectively; used by psychologists to 
help people make decisions about their 
lives and understand more about 
themselves. 


> Study Alert 


The distinction between reli- 
ability and validity is impor- 
tant. For instance, a test 
that measures trustfulness 
is reliable if it yields the 
same results each time it is adminis- 
tered, while it would be valid if it 
measures trustfulness accurately. 


461 


462 





Chapter 13 Personality 


b 


Should Race and Ethnicity 
Be Used to Establish Norms? 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Finally, psychological tests are based on norms, standards of test performance 
that permit the comparison of one person’s score on a test with the scores of others 
who have taken the same test. For example, a norm permits test-takers who have 
received a certain score on a test to know that they have scored in the top 10% of all 
those who have taken the test. 

Norms are established by administering a specific test to a large number of 
people and determining the typical scores. It is then possible to compare a single 
person’s score with the scores of the group, which provides a comparative 
measure of test performance against the performance of others who have taken 
the test. 

The establishment of appropriate norms is not a simple endeavor. For instance, 
the specific group that is employed to determine norms for a test has a profound 
effect on the way an individual's performance is evaluated. In fact, as we discuss 
next, the process of establishing norms can take on political overtones. 


eg i à j The passions of politics may confront the objectivity of science 
Exploring DIVERSITY 


P 
' 


when test norms are established, at least in the realm of 
standardized tests that are meant to predict future job 
performance. In fact, a national controversy has developed 
around the question of whether different norms should be 
established for members of various racial and ethnic groups 
(Manly, 2005, 2006; Manly & Echemendia, 2007; Pedraza & 
Mungas, 2008). 

The test that sparked the controversy was the U.S. government’s General Aptitude 
Test Battery, a test that measures a broad range of abilities from eye-hand coordination 
to reading proficiency. The problem was that African Americans and Hispanics tend to 
score lower on the test, on average, than members of other groups. The lower scores 
often are due to a lack of prior relevant experience and job opportunities, which in turn 
has been due to prejudice and discrimination. 

To promote the employment of minority racial groups, the government developed 
a separate set of norms for African Americans and Hispanics. Rather than using the 
pool of all people who took the tests, the scores of African-American and Hispanic 
applicants were compared only with the scores of other African Americans and 
Hispanics. Consequently, a Hispanic who scored in the top 20% of the Hispanics taking 
the test was considered to have performed equivalently to a white job applicant who 
scored in the top 20% of the whites who took the test, even though the absolute score 
of the Hispanic might be lower than that of the white. 

Critics of the adjusted norming system suggested that such a procedure 
discriminates in favor of certain racial and ethnic groups at the expense of others, 
thereby fanning the flames of racial bigotry. The practice was challenged legally; with 
the passage of the Civil Rights Act in 1991, race norming on the General Aptitude Test 
Battery was discontinued (Galef, 2001). 

However, proponents of race norming continue to argue that norming procedures 
that take race into account are an affirmative action tool that simply permits minority 
job-seekers to be placed on an equal footing with white job-seekers. Furthermore, a 
panel of the National Academy of Sciences supported the practice of adjusting test 
norms. It suggested that the unadjusted test norms are not very useful in predicting job 
performance and that they would tend to screen out otherwise qualified minority 
group members (Fleming, 2000). 

Job testing is not the only area in which issues arise regarding norms and the 
meaning of test scores. The issue of how to treat racial differences in IQ scores is also 
controversial and divisive. Clearly, race norming raises profound and intense feelings 
that may come into conflict with scientific objectivity (Leiter & Leiter, 2003; Rushton & 
Jensen, 2006; Davis, 2009). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 42 Assessing Personality: Determining What Makes Us Distinctive 463 


The issue of establishing norms for tests is further complicated by the existence of a 
wide array of personality measures and approaches to assessment. We next consider 
some of these measures. 


Self-Report Measures 
of Personality 


If someone wanted to assess your personality, one possible approach would be to 
carry out an extensive interview with you to determine the most important events 
in your childhood, your social relationships, and your successes and failures. Obvi- 
ously, though, such a technique would take extraordinary time and effort. 

It is also unnecessary. Just as physicians draw only a small sample of your blood 
to test it, psychologists can utilize self-report measures that ask people about a 
relatively small sample of their behavior. This sampling of self-report data is then 
used to infer the presence of particular personality characteristics. For example, a 
researcher who was interested in assessing a person’s orientation to life might admin- 
ister the questionnaire shown in Figure 1. Although the questionnaire consists of only 
a few questions, the answers can be used to generalize about personality character- 
istics. (Try it yourself!) 

One of the best examples of a self-report measure, and one of the most frequently 
used personality tests, is the Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 
(MMPI-2). Although the original purpose of this measure was to identify people with 
specific sorts of psychological difficulties, it has been found to predict a variety of 


Se V ONO 


The Life Orientation Test-Revised 


Use the following scale to answer the items below: 
0 1 2 3 4 
Strongly disagree Disagree Neutral Agree Strongly agree 





In uncertain times, | usually expect the best. 

It's easy for me to relax. 

If something can go wrong for me, it will. 

I'm always optimistic about my future. 

| enjoy my friends a lot. 

It's important for me to keep busy. 

| hardly ever expect things to go my way. 

| don't get upset too easily. 

| rarely count on good things happening to me. 

Overall, | expect more good things to happen to me than bad. 


S &9 E S) ON Sr es UI I 


sy 


Scoring. First, reverse your answers to questions 3, 7, and 9. Do this by changing a 0 to a 4, a 1 to 
a 3, a 3 to a 1, and a 4 to a 0 (answers of 2 stay as 2). Then sum the reversed scores, and add 
them to the scores you gave to questions 1, 4, and 10. (Ignore questions 2, 5, 6, and 8, which are 
filler items.) 

The total score you get is a measure of a particular orientation to life: your degree of 
optimism. The higher your scores, the more positive and hopeful you generally are about life. For 
comparison purposes the average score for college students is 14.3, according to the results of a 
study by Scheier, Carver, and Bridges (1994). People with a higher degree of optimism generally 
deal with stress better than do those with lower scores. 


self-report measures A method of 
gathering data about people by asking 
them questions about a sample of their 
behavior. 


Minnesota Multiphasic Personality 
Inventory-2 (MMPI-2) A widely used 
self-report test that identifies people 
with psychological difficulties and is 
employed to predict some everyday 
behaviors. 


FIGURE 1 The Life Orientation Test- 
Revised. Complete this test by indicating 
the degree to which you agree with 
each of the 10 statements using the 
scale from 0 to 4 for each item. Try to be 
as accurate as possible. There are no 
right or wrong answers. (Source: Adapted 
from Table 6 (p. 1073) from Scheier, M. F., 
Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. (1994). 
Distinguishing optimism from neuroticism 
(and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self- 
esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orienta- 
tion Test. Journal of Personality and Social 
Psychology, 67, 1063-1078. Copyright © 1994 
by The American Psychological Association. 
Adapted with permission. 


464 Chapter 13 Personality 


test standardization A technique 
used to validate questions in personal- 
ity tests by studying the responses of 
people with known diagnoses. 


FIGURE 2 A MMPI-2 profile of a person 
who suffers from obsessional anxiety, 


social withdrawal, and delusional thinking. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


other behaviors. For instance, MMPI-2 scores have been shown to be good predictors 
of whether college students will marry within 10 years of graduating and whether 
they will get an advanced degree. Police departments use the test to measure whether 
police officers are likely to use their weapons. Psychologists in Russia administer a 
modified form of the MMPI to their astronauts and Olympic athletes (Butcher, 2005; 
Sellbom & Ben-Porath, 2006; Sellbom, Fischler, & Ben-Porath, 2007). 

The test consists of a series of 567 items to which a person responds “true,” 
“false,” or “cannot say.” The questions cover a variety of issues ranging from mood 
(“I feel useless at times”) to opinions (“People should try to understand their dreams”) 
to physical and psychological health (“I am bothered by an upset stomach several 
times a week” and “I have strange and peculiar thoughts”). 

There are no right or wrong answers. Instead, interpretation of the results rests 
on the pattern of responses. The test yields scores on 10 separate scales, plus three 
scales meant to measure the validity of the respondent’s answers. For example, there 
is a “lie scale” that indicates when people are falsifying their responses in order to 
present themselves more favorably (through items such as, “I can’t remember ever 
having a bad night’s sleep”) (Butcher, 2005; Stein & Graham, 2005; Bacchiochi, 2006). 

How did the authors of the MMPI-2 determine what specific patterns of responses 
indicate? The procedure they used is typical of personality test construction—a pro- 
cess known as test standardization. To create the test, the test authors asked groups 
of psychiatric patients with a specific diagnosis, such as depression or schizophrenia, 
to complete a large number of items. They then determined which items best dif- 
ferentiated members of those groups from a comparison group of normal participants 
and included those specific items in the final version of the test. By systematically 
carrying out this procedure on groups with different diagnoses, the test authors were 
able to devise a number of subscales that identified different forms of abnormal 
behavior (see Figure 2). 


Clinical scales 


Hypochondriasis: 
Interest in bodily symptoms 


Depression: 
Hopeless, pessimistic attitude 


Hysteria: 
Uses physical ailments to avoid problems 


Psychopathic deviate: 
Antisocial behavior, disregards others 


Masculinity-femininity: 
Interests related to gender 


Paranoia: 
Defensiveness, suspiciousness, jealousy 


Psychasthenia: obsessiveness, 
compulsiveness, suspiciousness 


Schizophrenia: 
Loss of touch with reality, bizarre delusions 


Hypomania: 
Impulsiveness, overactivity 


Social introversion-extraversion: 
Insecure social interactions 





30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 


Score 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 42 Assessing Personality: Determining What Makes Us Distinctive 465 


When the MMPI-2 is used for the purpose for which it was devised—identifi- 
cation of personality disorders—it does a good job. However, like other personality 
tests, it presents an opportunity for abuse. For instance, employers who use it as 
a screening tool for job applicants may interpret the results improperly by relying 
too heavily on the results of individual scales instead of taking into account the 
overall patterns of results, which require skilled interpretation. Furthermore, critics 
point out that the individual scales overlap, which makes their interpretation dif- 
ficult. In sum, although the MMPI-2 remains the most widely used personality test 
and has been translated into more than 100 different languages, it must be used 
with caution (Valsiner, Diriwachter, & Sauck, 2005; Forbey & Ben-Porath, 2007; 
Ben-Porath & Archer, 2008). 


Projective Methods 


If you were shown the shape presented in Figure 3 and asked what it represented 
to you, you might not think that your impressions would mean very much. But to 
a psychodynamic theoretician, your responses to such an ambiguous figure would 
provide valuable clues to the state of your unconscious and ultimately to your gen- 
eral personality characteristics. 

The shape in the figure is representative of inkblots used in projective personal- 
ity tests, in which a person is shown an ambiguous stimulus and asked to describe 
it or tell a story about it. The responses are considered to be “projections” of the 
individual’s personality. 








FIGURE 3 This inkblot is similar to the type used in the Rorschach personality test. What do 
you see in it? (Source: Alloy, Jacobson, & Acocella, 1999.) 






y Alert 


In projective tests such as the 
Rorschach, researchers pres- 
ent an ambiguous stimulus 
and ask a person to de- 
scribe or tell a story about 
it. They then use the responses to 
make inferences about personality. 


projective personality test A test in 
which a person is shown an ambigu- 
ous stimulus and asked to describe it 
or tell a story about it. 


466 Chapter 13 Personality 





ScienceCartoonsPlus.com 


“Ronsatacn | WHAT'S 1 Become oF You?” 


Rorschach test A test that involves 
showing a series of symmetrical visual 
stimuli to people who then are asked 
what the figures represent to them. 


Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) 

A test consisting of a series of pictures 
about which a person is asked to write 
a story. 


behavioral assessment Direct mea- 
sures of an individual’s behavior used 
to describe personality characteristics. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


The best-known projective test is the Rorschach test. Devised by Swiss psychia- 
trist Hermann Rorschach (1924), the test involves showing a series of symmetrical 
stimuli similar to the one in Figure 3 to people who are then asked what the figures 
represent to them. Their responses are recorded, and people are classified by their 
personality type through a complex set of clinical judgments on the part of the exam- 
iner. For instance, respondents who see a bear in one inkblot are thought to have a 
strong degree of emotional control, according to the scoring guidelines Rorschach 
developed (Weiner, 2004b; Silverstein, 2007). 

The Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) is another well-known projective test. 
The TAT consists of a series of pictures about which a person is asked to write a 
story. The stories are then used to draw inferences about the writer’s personality 
characteristics (Weiner, 2004a; Langan-Fox & Grant, 2006). 

Tests with stimuli as ambiguous as those used in the Rorschach and TAT require 
particular skill and care in their interpretation—too much skill and care in many 
critics’ estimation. The Rorschach in particular has been criticized for requiring too 
much inference on the part of the examiner, and attempts to standardize scoring have 
frequently failed. Furthermore, many critics complain that the Rorschach does not 
provide much valid information about underlying personality traits. Despite such 
problems, both the Rorschach and the TAT are widely used, especially in clinical 
settings, and their proponents suggest that their reliability and validity are great 
enough to provide useful inferences about personality (Wood et al., 2003; Garb et al., 
2005; Society for Personality Assessment, 2005). 


Behavioral Assessment 


If you were a psychologist subscribing to a learning approach to personality, you 
would be likely to object to the indirect nature of projective tests. Instead, you 
would be more apt to use behavioral assessment—direct measures of an indi- 
vidual’s behavior designed to describe characteristics indicative of personality. As 
with observational research, behavioral assessment may be carried out naturalisti- 
cally by observing people in their own settings: in the workplace, at home, or in 
school. In other cases, behavioral assessment occurs in the laboratory under con- 
trolled conditions in which a psychologist sets up a situation and observes an 
individual’s behavior (Ramsay, Reynolds, & Kamphaus, 2002; Gladwell, 2004; 
Miller & Leffard, 2007). 

Regardless of the setting in which behavior is observed, an effort is made to 
ensure that behavioral assessment is carried out objectively and quantifies behavior 
as much as possible. For example, an observer may record the number of social 
contacts a person initiates, the number of questions asked, or the number of aggres- 
sive acts. Another method is to measure the duration of events: the duration of a 
child’s temper tantrum, the length of a conversation, the amount of time spent work- 
ing, or the time spent in cooperative behavior. 

Behavioral assessment is particularly appropriate for observing—and eventually 
remedying—specific behavioral difficulties, such as shyness in children. It provides 
a means of assessing the specific nature and incidence of a problem and subse- 
quently allows psychologists to determine whether intervention techniques have 
been successful. 

Behavioral assessment techniques based on learning theories of personality have 
also made important contributions to the treatment of certain kinds of psychological 
difficulties. In addition, they are also used to make hiring and personnel decision in 
the workplace. (Also see PsychWork.) 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 42 Assessing Personality: Determining What Makes Us Distinctive 467 


At some point, everyone goes looking for a job, and in many 
cases the first stop in the process will be with a human 
resources officer. John Murray, Human Resources Administra- 
tion Manager of Employment and Compensation Services at 
Indiana University—-Purdue University, is one of those people 
job applicants meet with initially. 

“We try to influence hiring departments to consider diverse 
applicants during the recruitment process,” Murray explained. 
“We train on and stress the value of behavioral-based inter- 


PsychWork 


HUMAN RESOURCES MANAGER 





Name: John P. Murray 
Profession: Human Resources Manager 


Education: BS in Business Administration; Indiana 
University-Purdue University, Indianapolis, 
Indiana 


views, and we try to help hiring managers see patterns of 
behavior that may not be seen if they only focus on apparent 
unrelated facts.” 

The interviewing process depends on the position being advertised, according to 
Murray. “Interviewing does vary depending on the position but also varies based on 
organizational needs that may be situational, such as make-up of current staff, antic- 
ipated changes in organization, move toward automation, and so forth. 

“When I interview for openings that I have or committees that I serve on, I try to 
quickly get to whether the applicant has the skills to do the job so I can spend more 
time gaining information to assess motivation, communication styles, and fit,” he added. 

“For example, when hiring for an employment consultant, technical knowledge 
of hiring processes is less important to me than consulting skills,” Murray explained. 
“A technical expert in employment law might not be as effective as an individual 
who can demonstrate communication, analytical, conceptualization, conflict resolu- 
tion, and other soft skills that are more difficult to develop.” 


Many companies ranging from General Motors to Microsoft BECOMING AN 


employ personality tests to help determine who gets hired. For INFORMED CONSUMER 


example, potential Microsoft employees have been asked 


brainteasers like, “If you had to remove one of the 50 U.S. states, of P S yc h O | O g y 


which would it be?” (Hint: First define “remove.” If you mean 
the death of everyone in the state, suggest a low-population 
state. If you mean quitting the country, then go for an outlying 
state like Alaska or Hawaii.) Other employers ask questions 
that are even more vague (“Describe November”). With such questions, it’s not always 
clear that the tests are reliable or valid (McGinn, 2003). 

Before relying too heavily on the results of such personality testing in the role of 
potential employee, employer, or consumer of testing services, you should keep several 
points in mind: 


e Understand what the test claims to measure. Standard personality measures are 
accompanied by information that discusses how the test was developed, to 
whom it is most applicable, and how the results should be interpreted. Read any 
explanations of the test; they will help you understand the results. 

¢ Do not base a decision only on the results of any one test. Test results should be 
interpreted in the context of other information, such as academic records, social 
interests, and home and community activities. 

e Remember that test results are not always accurate. The results may be in error; the 
test may be unreliable or invalid. For example, you may have had a “bad day” 
when you took the test, or the person scoring and interpreting the test may have 
made a mistake. You should not place too much significance on the results of a 
single administration of any test. 


Assessing Personality Assessments 








www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


468 Chapter 13 Personality 


Insum, itis important to keep in mind the complexity of human behavior—particularly 
your own. No single test can provide an understanding of the intricacies of someone’s 
personality without considering a good deal more information than can be provided ina 
single testing session (Gladwell, 2004; Paul, 2004; Hogan, Davies, & Hogan, 2007). 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 3. Tests such as the MMPI-2, in which a small sample of 
behavior is assessed to determine larger patterns, are 
examples of 
a. Cross-sectional tests 
b. Projective tests 
c. Achievement tests 
d. Self-report tests 

4. A person shown a picture and asked to make up a story 

What are the major types of personality measures? about it would be taking a per- 

e Self-report measures ask people about a sample range of sonality test. 
their behaviors. These reports are used to infer the pres- 
ence of particular personality characteristics. (p. 463) 

e Projective personality tests (such as the Rorschach and RETHINK 
the TAT) present an ambiguous stimulus; the test admin- 
istrator infers information about the test-taker from his 
or her responses. (p. 465) 

e Behavioral assessment is based on the principles of 
learning theory. It employs direct measurement of an in- 
dividual’s behavior to determine characteristics related 
to personality. (p. 466) 


How can we most accurately assess personality? 

e Psychological tests such as the MMPI-2 are standard as- 
sessment tools that measure behavior objectively. They 
must be reliable (measuring what they are trying to mea- 
sure consistently) and valid (measuring what they are 
supposed to measure). (p. 461) 





1. Should personality tests be used for personnel decisions? 
Should they be used for other social purposes, such as 
identifying individuals at risk for certain types of person- 
ality disorders? 

2. From the perspective of a politician: Imagine that you had to 
vote on a law that would require institutions and organi- 
zations to perform race norming procedures on standard- 
ized performance tests. Would you support such a law? 
Why or why not? In addition to race, should norming pro- 
cedures take other factors into account? Which ones and 

1, __________is the consistency of a personality test; why? 

is the ability of a test to actually measure what 
it is designed to measure. 

2, _______are standards used to compare scores of 
different people taking the same test. 


EVALUATE 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 
aatpalord *p ‘p'e ‘swou ‘z /Ayrprea ‘AqTIqeTer T 


KEY TERMS 


psychological tests p. 461 test standardization p. 464 Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) p. 466 
self-report measures p. 463 projective personality test p. 465 behavioral assessment p. 466 
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Rorschach test p. 466 


Inventory-2 (MMPI-2) p. 463 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Psychology on the Web 


1. Sigmund Freud is one of the towering figures in psychology. His influence extends far 
beyond his psychoanalytic work. Find information about Freud on the web. Pick one 
aspect of his work or influence (for example, on therapy, medicine, literature, film, or 
culture and society) and summarize in writing what you have found, including your 
attitude toward your findings. 

2. Find a website that links to personality tests and take one or two tests. Remember to 
take them with skepticism. For each test, summarize in writing the aspects of person- 
ality that were tested, the theoretical approach the test appeared to be based on, and 
your assessment of the trustworthiness of the results. 


È © | log U e We have discussed the different ways in which psycholo- 


gists have interpreted the development and structure of 
personality. The perspectives we examined ranged from Freud's analysis of personality 
based primarily on internal, unconscious factors to the externally based view champi- 
oned by learning theorists of personality as a learned set of traits and actions. We also 
noted that there are many ways to interpret personality; by no means does a consensus 
exist on what the key traits are that are central to personality. 

Return to the prologue and consider the case of convicted swindler Bernie Madoff. 
Use your understanding of personality to consider the following questions. 





1. How typical is it for people to have different sides to their personalities as Madoff 
does? 

2. How do you think Madoff would score on a test of extraversion? On a test of 
conscientiousness? 

3. How could you explain Madoff's personality from a biological approach? From a 
learning approach? 

4. Might a personality test have indicated Madoff's criminal intentions? Why or why not? 





469 








Health Psychology: Stress, 
Coping, and Well-Being 

















www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Key Concepts for Chapter 14 


MODULE 43 








How is health psychology a union between Stress and Coping 


PEE ; Stress: Reacting to Threat and Challenge 
medicine and psychology? @ What is stress, ee ; oh 
erg Ost O ress 


how does it affect us, and how can we best Coping with Stress 


Neuroscience in Your Life: 


ely it? 
cope with it? Stress and Social Support 


Becoming an Informed Consumer of 
Psychology: Effective Coping Strategies 


MODULE 44 






How do psychological factors affect health-related Psychological Aspects of Illness 


A and Well-Being 
problems such as coronary heart disease, cancer, = -he As, Bs, and Ds of Coronary 


and smoking? Heart Disease 
Psychological Aspects of Cancer 
Smoking 


Neuroscience in Your Life: Smoking— 
Why It's So Difficult to Quit 

Exploring Diversity: Hucksters of Death: 
Promoting Smoking Throughout the 
World 


MODULE 45 





How do our interactions with physicians Promoting Health and Wellness 


, . Following Medical Advi 
affect our health and compliance with a a naviG= 


Well-Being and Happiness 


medical treatment? @ How does a sense of Applying Psychology in the 21st Century: 


Catching Happiness: The Contagion 


well-being develop? of Emotions and Health 


471 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Prologue Never a Moment’s Rest 


Louisa Denby’s day began badly: She slept through her alarm 
and had to skip breakfast to catchthe bus to campus. Then, when 
she went to the library to catch up onthe reading she had to do 
before taking a test the next day, the one article she needed 

was missing. The librarian told her that replacing it would take 

24 hours. Feeling frustrated, she walked to the computer lab to 
print out the paper she had completed at home the night before. 


\ 


The computer wouldn't read her disk. She searched for 
someone to help her, but she was unable to find anyone who 
knew any more about computers than she did. 

It was only 9:42 a.m., and Louisa had a wracking headache. 
Apart from that pain, she was conscious of only one feeling: 
stress (Feldman, 2010). 





It’s not hard to understand why Louisa Denby was experiencing 
stress. For people like her—and that probably includes most of 
us—the intensity of juggling multiple roles leads to feelings of 
never having sufficient time and, in some cases, takes a toll on 
both physical and psychological well-being. 

Stress and how we cope with it have long been central topics 
of interest for psychologists. However, in recent years the focus 
has broadened as psychology has come to view stress in the 
broader context of one of psychology’s newer subfields: health 
psychology. Health psychology investigates the psychological 
factors related to wellness and illness, including the prevention, 
diagnosis, and treatment of medical problems. Health psycholo- 
gists investigate the effects of psychological factors such as stress 
on illness. They examine the psychological principles underlying 
treatments for disease and illness. They also study prevention: 
how healthier behavior can help people avoid and reduce health 
problems such as stress and heart disease. 

Health psychologists take a decisive stand on the enduring 
mind-body issue that philosophers, and later psychologists, have 
debated since the time of the ancient Greeks. In their view, the 
mind and the body are clearly linked rather than representing 
two distinct systems (Sternberg, 2000; Dalal & Misra, 2006). 

Health psychologists recognize that good health and the 
ability to cope with illness are affected by psychological factors 
such as thoughts, emotions, and the ability to manage stress. 
They have paid particular attention to the immune system, the 
complex system of organs, glands, and cells that constitute our 
bodies’ natural line of defense in fighting disease. 

In fact, health psychologists are among the primary investi- 
gators in a growing field called psychoneuroimmunology, or 
PNI, the study of the relationship among psychological factors, 
the immune system, and the brain. PNI has led to discoveries 
such as the existence of an association between a person's emo- 
tional state and the success of the immune system in fighting 
disease (Dickerson et al., 2004; Kemeny, 2007; Byrne-Davis & 
Vedhara, 2008). 


472 


In sum, health psychologists view the mind and the body as 
two parts of a whole human being that cannot be considered 
independently. This more recent view marks a sharp departure 
from earlier thinking. Previously, disease was seen as a purely 
biological phenomenon, and psychological factors were of little 
interest to most health-care workers. In the early 20th century, 
the primary causes of death were short-term infections from 
which one either rapidly recovered—or died. Now, however, the 
major causes of death, such as heart disease, cancer, and diabetes, 
are chronic illnesses that pose significant psychological issues 
because they often cannot be cured and may linger for years 
(Bishop, 2005; Rotan & Ospina-Kammerer, 2007). 

Advances in health psychology have had an impact across a 
variety of disciplines and professions. For instance, health-care 
professionals such as physicians and nurses, social workers, 
dieticians, pharmacists, occupational therapists, and even clergy 
are increasingly likely to receive training in health psychology. 

In the three modules that follow, we discuss the ways in which 
psychological factors affect health. We first focus on the causes 
and consequences of stress as well as on the means of coping 
with it. Next, we explore the psychological aspects of several 
major health problems, including heart disease, cancer, and 
ailments resulting from smoking. Finally, we examine the ways 
in which patient-physician interactions influence our health and 
offer suggestions for increasing people’s compliance with 
recommendations about behavior that will improve their 
well-being. 


health psychology The branch of psychology that investigates the 
psychological factors related to wellness and illness, including the 
prevention, diagnosis, and treatment of medical problems. 


psychoneuroimmunology (PNI) The study of the relationship among 
psychological factors, the immune system, and the brain. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 43 


Anthony Lepre started feeling awful almost as soon as Tom Ridge [U.S. Homeland Security 
Secretary] put the nation on high alert for a terrorist attack. ... He awoke in the middle 
of the night short of breath, his heart pounding. And the sound of his telephone seemed 
a sure sign of bad news. By midweek, he was rushing off to Costco to stock up on fruit 
juice, bottled water, peanut butter, canned tuna, “and extra food for my cats Monster, 
Monkey and Spike.” He also picked up a first-aid kit, six rolls of duct tape, and a bulk 
package of plastic wrap to seal his windows. “The biggest problem was that I felt 
helpless,” he says, “completely powerless over the situation.” (Cowley, 2003, pp. 43-44) 


Stress: Reacting to Threat 
and Challenge 


Most of us need little introduction to the phenomenon of stress, people’s response 
to events that threaten or challenge them. Whether it is a paper or an exam deadline, 
a family problem, or even the ongoing threat of a terrorist attack, life is full of cir- 
cumstances and events known as stressors that produce threats to our well-being. 
Even pleasant events—such as planning a party or beginning a sought-after job—can 
produce stress, although negative events result in greater detrimental consequences 
than positive ones. 

All of us face stress in our lives. Some health psychologists believe that daily life 
actually involves a series of repeated sequences of perceiving a threat, considering 
ways to cope with it, and ultimately adapting to the threat with greater or lesser suc- 
cess. Although adaptation is often minor and occurs without our awareness, adapta- 
tion requires a major effort when stress is more severe or long lasting. Ultimately, our 
attempts to overcome stress may produce biological and psychological responses that 
result in health problems (Boyce & Ellis, 2005; Dolbier, Smith, & Steinhardt, 2007). 





THE NATURE OF STRESSORS: MY STRESS IS YOUR PLEASURE 


Stress is a very personal thing. Although certain kinds of events, such as the death 
of a loved one or participation in military combat, are universally stressful, other 
situations may or may not be stressful to a specific person. 

Consider, for instance, bungee jumping. Some people would find jumping off 
a bridge while attached to a slender rubber tether extremely stressful. However, 
there are individuals who see such an activity as challenging and fun filled. Whether 
bungee jumping is stressful depends in part, then, on a person’s perception of the 
activity. 

For people to consider an event stressful, they must perceive it as threatening or 
challenging and must lack all the resources to deal with it effectively. Consequently, 
the same event may at some times be stressful and at other times provoke no 
stressful reaction at all. A young man may experience stress when he is turned down 
for a date—if he attributes the refusal to his unattractiveness or unworthiness. But if 


How is health psychology a 
union between medicine and 
psychology? 


What is stress, how does it 
affect us, and how can we 
best cope with it? 


A person’s response to events 
that are threatening or challenging. 


y Alert 


Remember the distinction 
between stressors and 
stress, which can be tricky: 
stressors (like an exam) 
cause stress (the physiolog- 
ical and psychological reaction that 
comes from the exam). 





I . / 


Even positive events can produce 
significant stress. 


473 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


474 Chapter 14 Health Psychology: Stress, Coping, and Well-Being 


FIGURE 1 The closer people lived to the site of the World Trade 
Center terrorist attack, the greater the rate of posttraumatic stress 
disorder. (Source: Susser, Herman, & Aaron, 2002.) 


cataclysmic events Strong stressors that 
occur suddenly and typically affect many 
people at once (e.g., natural disasters). 


personal stressors Major life events, 
such as the death of a family member, 
that have immediate negative conse- 
quences that generally fade with time. 


posttraumatic stress disorder 

(PTSD) A phenomenon in which 
victims of major catastrophes or strong 
personal stressors feel long-lasting 
effects that may include re-experiencing 
the event in vivid flashbacks or dreams. 





he attributes it to some factor unrelated to his self-esteem, 
such as a previous commitment of the woman he asked, the 
experience of being refused may create no stress at all. Hence, 
a person’s interpretation of events plays an important role in 
the determination of what is stressful (Folkman & Moskowitz, 
2000; Giacobbi Jr., et al., 2004; Friborg et al., 2006). 


CATEGORIZING STRESSORS 


What kinds of events tend to be seen as stressful? There are 
three general types of stressors: cataclysmic events, personal 
stressors, and background stressors. 

Cataclysmic events are strong stressors that occur sud- 
denly and typically affect many people simultaneously. Disas- 
ters such as tornadoes and plane crashes as well as terrorist 
attacks are examples of cataclysmic events that can affect hun- 
dreds or thousands of people simultaneously. 

Although it might seem that cataclysmic events would 
produce potent, lingering stress, in many cases they do not. In 
fact, cataclysmic events involving natural disasters may pro- 
duce less stress in the long run than events that initially are 
not as devastating. One reason is that natural disasters have a 
clear resolution. Once they are over, people can look to the 
future knowing that the worst is behind them. Moreover, oth- 
ers who also experienced the disaster share the stress induced 
by cataclysmic events. Such sharing permits people to offer 
one another social support and a firsthand understanding of 
the difficulties others are going through (Hobfoll et al., 1996; 
Benight, 2004; Yesilyaprak, Kisac, & Sanlier, 2007). 

In contrast, terrorist attacks like the one on the World 
Trade Center in 2001 are cataclysmic events that produce con- 
siderable stress. Terrorist attacks are deliberate, and victims 
(and observers) know that future attacks are likely. Govern- 
ment warnings in the form of heightened terror alerts may 
further increase the stress (Murphy, Wismar, & Freeman, 2003; 
Laugharne, Janca, & Widiger, 2007). 

The second major category of stressor is the personal 
stressor. Personal stressors include major life events such as 
the death of a parent or spouse, the loss of one’s job, a major 
personal failure, or even something positive such as getting 
married. Typically, personal stressors produce an immediate 
major reaction that soon tapers off. For example, stress arising 
from the death of a loved one tends to be greatest just after the time of death, but 
people begin to feel less stress and are better able to cope with the loss after the 
passage of time. 

Some victims of major catastrophes and severe personal stressors experience 
posttraumatic stress disorder, or PTSD, in which a person has experienced a sig- 
nificantly stressful event that has long-lasting effects that may include re-experienc- 
ing the event in vivid flashbacks or dreams. An episode of PTSD may be triggered 
by an otherwise innocent stimulus, such as the sound of a honking horn, that leads 
a person to re-experience a past event that produced considerable stress. 

Symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder also include emotional numbing, sleep 
difficulties, interpersonal problems, alcohol and drug abuse, and in some cases suicide. 
For instance, the suicide rate for military veterans, many of whom participated in the 
Iraq and Afghanistan wars, is twice as high as for nonveterans (Pole, 2007; Kaplan et 
al., 2007; Magruder & Yeager, 2009). 


BROOKLYN 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Around 16% of soldiers returning from 
Iraq show symptoms of PTSD. Further- 
more, those who have experienced child 
abuse or rape, rescue workers facing over- 
whelming situations, and victims of sudden 
natural disasters or accidents that produce 
feelings of helplessness and shock may suf- 
fer from the same disorder (Hoge & Castro, 
2006; Friedman, 2006; Marmar, 2009). 

Terrorist attacks produce high inci- 
dences of PTSD. For example, 11% of people 
in New York City had some form of PTSD 
in the months after the September 11 terror- 
ist attacks. But the responses varied signifi- 
cantly with a resident’s proximity to the 
attacks, as illustrated in Figure 1; the closer 
someone lived to the World Trade Center, 
the greater the likelihood of PTSD (Susser, 
Herman, & Aaron, 2002; Lee, Isaac, & Janca, 
2007; Marshall et al., 2007). 

Background stressors, or more infor- 
mally, daily hassles, are the third major cat- 
egory of stressors. Exemplified by standing 
in a long line at a bank and getting stuck in 


Module 43 Stress and Coping 475 





a traffic jam, daily hassles are the minor irri- Everyone confronts daily hassles, or background stressors, at some point. At what 
7 
tations of life that we all face time and time Point do daily hassles become more than mere irritants? 


again. Another type of background stressor 

is a long-term, chronic problem, such as 

experiencing dissatisfaction with school or a job, being in an unhappy relationship, 
or living in crowded quarters without privacy (Weinstein et al., 2004; McIntyre, Korn, 
& Matsuo, 2008). 

By themselves, daily hassles do not require much coping or even a response on 
the individual’s part, although they certainly produce unpleasant emotions and 
moods. Yet, daily hassles add up—and ultimately they may take as great of a toll as 
a single, more stressful incident. In fact, the number of daily hassles people face is 
associated with psychological symptoms and health problems such as flu, sore throat, 
and backaches. 

The flip side of hassles is uplifts, the minor positive events that make us feel 
good—even if only temporarily. As indicated in Figure 2 on the next page, uplifts 
range from relating well to a companion to finding one’s surroundings pleasing. 
What is especially intriguing about uplifts is that they are associated with people’s 
psychological health in just the opposite way that hassles are: The greater the num- 
ber of uplifts we experience, the fewer the psychological symptoms we report later 
(Chamberlain & Zika, 1990; Ravindran et al., 2002; Jain, Mills, & Von Känel, 2007). 


The High Cost of Stress 


Stress can produce both biological and psychological consequences. Often the most 
immediate reaction to stress is biological. Exposure to stressors generates a rise in 
hormone secretions by the adrenal glands, an increase in heart rate and blood pres- 
sure, and changes in how well the skin conducts electrical impulses. On a short-term 
basis, these responses may be adaptive because they produce an “emergency reac- 
tion” in which the body prepares to defend itself through activation of the sympa- 
thetic nervous system. Those responses may allow more effective coping with the 
stressful situation (Akil & Morano, 1996; McEwen, 1998). 





background stressors (“daily 
hassles”) Everyday annoyances, such 
as being stuck in traffic, that cause 
minor irritations and may have 
long-term ill effects if they continue 
or are compounded by other stressful 
events. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


476 Chapter 14 Health Psychology: Stress, Coping, and Well-Being 


FIGURE 2 The most common everyday 
hassles and uplifts. How many of these 
are part of your life, and how do you 
cope with them? (Source: Hassles— 
Chamberlain & Zika, 1990; Uplifts—Kanner 
et al., 1981.) 






y Alert 


Remember the three catego- 
ries of stressors—cataclysmic 
events, personal stressors, 

and background stressors— 
and that they produce 
different levels of stress. 


psychophysiological disorders 
Medical problems influenced by an 
interaction of psychological, emo- 
tional, and physical difficulties. 


Hassles 


Not enough time 

Too many things to do 

Troubling thoughts about the future 
Too many interruptions 

Misplacing or losing things 

Health of a family member 

Social obligations 

Concerns about standards 
Concerns about getting ahead 


Too many responsibilities 











0 20 40 60 80 100 


Percentage of people experiencing 


Uplifts 


Relating well with spouse or lover 
Relating well with friends 
Completing a task 

Feeling healthy 

Getting enough sleep 

Eating out 

Meeting responsibilities 

Visiting, phoning, or writing someone 
Spending time with family 


Home (inside) pleasing to you 











0 20 40 60 80 100 


Percentage of people experiencing 


However, continued exposure to stress results in a decline in the body’s overall 
level of biological functioning because of the constant secretion of stress-related 
hormones. Over time, stressful reactions can promote deterioration of body tissues 
such as blood vessels and the heart. Ultimately, we become more susceptible to 
disease as our ability to fight off infection is lowered (Brydon et al., 2004; Dean- 
Borenstein, 2007; Ellins et al., 2008). 

Furthermore, an entire class of physical problems known as psychophysiological 
disorders often result from or are worsened by stress. Once referred to as psychosomatic 
disorders (a term dropped because people assumed that the disorders were somehow 
unreal), psychophysiological disorders are actual medical problems that are influenced 
by an interaction of psychological, emotional, and physical difficulties. The more common 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


psychophysiological disorders range from major problems, such as high blood pressure, 
to usually less serious conditions, such as headaches, backaches, skin rashes, indiges- 
tion, fatigue, and constipation. Stress has even been linked to the common cold (Cohen 
et al., 2003; Andrasik, 2006). 

On a psychological level, high levels of stress prevent people from adequately 
coping with life. Their view of the environment can become clouded (for example, 
a minor criticism made by a friend is blown out of proportion). Moreover, at the 
highest levels of stress, emotional responses may be so extreme that people are 
unable to act at all. People under a lot of stress also become less able to deal with 
new stressors. 

In short, stress affects us in multiple ways. It may increase the risk that we will 
become ill, it may directly cause illness, it may make us less able to recover from a 
disease, and it may reduce our ability to cope with future stress. (See Figure 3 to get 
a measure of your own level of stress.) 


Module 43 Stress and Coping 


477 


—feahihaalaaalides! LLL OL OOOO 


How Stressful Is Your Life? 


Test your level of stress by answering these questions, and adding the score from each box. Questions apply to the last month only. 


A key below will help you determine the extent of your stress. 


























1. How often have you been upset because of something 8. How often have you felt that you were on top of things? 
that happened unexpectedly? | ]4=never, 3=almost never, 2=sometimes, 1=fairly often, 
0=never, 1=almost never, 2=sometimes, 0=very often 
3=fairly often, 4=very often 9. How often have you been angered because of things that 
2. How often have you felt that you were unable to control were outside your control? 
the important things in your life? 0=never, 1=almost never, 2=sometimes, 3=fairly often, 
[ O=never, 1=almost never, 2=sometimes, 4=very often 
3=fairly often, 4=very often 10. How often have you felt difficulties were piling up so high 
3. How often have you felt nervous and “stressed”? that you could not overcome them? 
O=never, 1=almost never, 2=sometimes, O=never, 1=almost never, 2=sometimes, 3=fairly often, 
i 3=fairly often, 4=very often 4=very often 
4. How often have you felt confident about your ability to 
handle your personal problems? How You Measure Up 
Bea 3=almost never, 2=sometimes, Stress levels vary among individuals—compare your total 
1=fairly often, O=very often score to the averages below: 
5. How often have you felt that things were going your AGE GENDER 
way? E Oh eenenn nea tay 14.2 Men 124 
a 4=never, 3=almost never, 2=sometimes, 30 AA eins ene 13.0 Women 13.7 
1=fairly often, O=very often ARS Ae een: 12.6 
6. How often have you been able to control irritations in IDA ooo ooto potona 11.9 
your life? 6S8 OVEN o ten 12.0 
4=never, 3=almost never, 2=sometimes, 
[] 1 T F ree ee aaa MARAL SUAUS 
d WW iToleSWV(xe leaea sn unranactanmannananhrmmeacancnhannnasnaga Hanan 12.6 
7. How often have you found that you could not cope with Married or living with a partner ..............cceeee eee e ees 12.4 
all the things that you had to do? Sine[l3@? MAVEN WECl, onccooansonvooeeocadnnssnonosooecoanene 14.1 
a O=never, 1=almost never, 2=sometimes, 3=fairly Divorced a a A A Soeur ners re rae er 14.7 
often, 4=very often Separate ak AT A AA E A E T: 16.6 


FIGURE 3 To get a sense of the level of stress in your life, complete this questionnaire. 
(Source: Cohen, Kamarck, & Mermelstein, 1983.) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


478 Chapter 14 Health Psychology: Stress, Coping, and Well-Being 


general adaptation syndrome 

(GAS) A theory developed by Selye 
that suggests that a person’s response 
to a stressor consists of three stages: 
alarm and mobilization, resistance, 
and exhaustion. 


THE GENERAL ADAPTATION SYNDROME MODEL: 
THE COURSE OF STRESS 


The effects of long-term stress are illustrated in a series of stages proposed by Hans 
Selye (pronounced “sell-yay”), a pioneering stress theorist (Selye, 1976, 1993). This 
model, the general adaptation syndrome (GAS), suggests that the physiological 
response to stress follows the same set pattern regardless of the cause of stress. 

As shown in Figure 4, the GAS has three phases. The first stage—alarm and 
mobilization—occurs when people become aware of the presence of a stressor. On a 
biological level, the sympathetic nervous system becomes energized, which helps a 
person cope initially with the stressor. 

However, if the stressor persists, people move into the second response stage: 
resistance. During this stage, the body is actively fighting the stressor on a biological 
level. During resistance, people use a variety of means to cope with the stressor— 
sometimes successfully but at a cost of some degree of physical or psychological 
well-being. For example, a student who faces the stress of failing several courses 
might spend long hours studying seeking to cope with the stress. 

If resistance is inadequate, people enter the last stage of the GAS: exhaustion. 
During the exhaustion stage, a person’s ability to fight the stressor declines to 
the point where negative consequences of stress appear: physical illness and 
psychological symptoms in the form of an inability to concentrate, heightened 
irritability, or, in severe cases, disorientation and a loss of touch with reality. In 
a sense, people wear out, and their physiological resources to fight the stressor 
are used up. 

How do people move out of the third stage after they have entered it? In some 
cases, exhaustion allows people to escape a stressor. For example, people who become 








Stressor 


|. Alarm and mobilization 








2. Resistance 3. Exhaustion 
Meeting and Coping with stress and Negative consequences of 
resisting stressor. resistance to stressor. stress (such as illness) occur 


when coping is inadequate. 





1 
Li I 
I 1 
l. ! 2 ! 3 
Alarm H Resistance l Exhaustion 
I Li 


FIGURE 4 According to the general adaptation syndrome (GAS) model there are three major 
stages to stress responses: alarm and mobilization; resistance; and exhaustion. The graph 
below the illustration shows the degree of effort expended to cope with stressors at each of 
the three stages. (Source: Selye, 1976.) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





ill from overwork may be excused from their duties for a time, which gives them a 
temporary respite from their responsibilities. At least for a time, then, the immediate 
stress is reduced. 

Although the GAS has had a substantial impact on our understanding of stress, 
Selye’s theory has not gone unchallenged. For example, the theory suggests that the 
biological reaction is similar regardless of the stressor, but some health psychologists 
disagree. They believe that people’s biological responses are specific to the way they 
appraise a stressful event. If a stressor is seen as unpleasant but not unusual, then 
the biological response may be different than if the stressor is seen as unpleasant, 
out of the ordinary, and unanticipated. This perspective has led to an increased focus 
on psychoneuroimmunology (Taylor et al., 2000; Gaab, Rohleder, Nater, & Ehlert, 
2005; Irwin, 2008). 


PSYCHONEUROIMMUNOLOGY AND STRESS 


Contemporary health psychologists specializing in PNI have taken a broader approach 
to stress. Focusing on the outcomes of stress, they have identified three main conse- 
quences of it (see Figure 5). 

First, stress has direct physiological results, including an increase in blood pres- 
sure, an increase in hormonal activity, and an overall decline in the functioning of 
the immune system. Second, stress leads people to engage in behaviors that are 
harmful to their health, including increased nicotine, drug, and alcohol use; poor 
eating habits; and decreased sleep. Finally, stress produces indirect consequences that 
result in declines in health: a reduction in the likelihood of obtaining health care and 
decreased compliance with medical advice when it is sought (Sapolsky, 2003; Bro- 
man, 2005; Lindblad, Lindahl, & Theorell, 2006). 

Why is stress so damaging to the immune system? One reason is that stress may 
overstimulate it. Rather than fighting invading bacteria, viruses, and other foreign 
invaders, it may begin to attack the body itself and damage healthy tissue. When 
that happens, it can lead to disorders such as arthritis and allergic reactions. 


Module 43 Stress and Coping 479 


FIGURE 5 Three major types of con- 
sequences result from stress: direct 
physiological effects, harmful behaviors, 
and indirect health-related behaviors. 
(Source: Adapted from Baum, 1994.) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


480 Chapter 14 Health Psychology: Stress, Coping, and Well-Being 





The ability to fight off disease is related 
to psychological factors. Here a cell from 
the body’s immune system engulfs and 
destroys disease-producing bacteria. 


coping The efforts to control, reduce, 
or learn to tolerate the threats that lead 
to stress. 


Stress can also decrease the immune system response, permitting germs that 
produce colds to reproduce more easily or allowing cancer cells to spread more 
rapidly. In normal circumstances, our bodies produce lymphocytes, specialized 
white blood cells that fight disease at an extraordinary rate—some 10 million 
every few seconds. It is possible that stress can alter this level of production 
(Cohen, Hamrick, N., & Rodriguez, 2002; Segerstrom & Miller, 2004; Dougall & 
Baum, 2004). 


Coping with Stress 


Stress is a normal part of life—and not necessarily a completely bad part. For exam- 
ple, without stress, we might not be sufficiently motivated to complete the activities 
we need to accomplish. However, it is also clear that too much stress can take a toll 
on physical and psychological health. How do people deal with stress? Is there a 
way to reduce its negative effects? 

Efforts to control, reduce, or learn to tolerate the threats that lead to stress are 
known as coping. We habitually use certain coping responses to deal with stress. 
Most of the time, we’re not aware of these responses—just as we may be unaware 
of the minor stressors of life until they build up to harmful levels (Wrzesniewski & 
Chylinska, 2007). 

We also have other, more direct and potentially more positive ways of coping 
with stress, which fall into two main categories (Folkman & Moskowitz, 2000, 2004; 
Baker & Berenbaum, 2007): 





e Emotion-focused coping. In emotion-focused coping, people try to manage their 
emotions in the face of stress by seeking to change the way they feel about or 
perceive a problem. Examples of emotion-focused coping include strategies 
such as accepting sympathy from others and looking at the bright side of a 
situation. 

e Problem-focused coping. Problem-focused coping attempts to modify the 
stressful problem or source of stress. Problem-focused strategies lead to 
changes in behavior or to the development of a plan of action to deal with 
stress. Starting a study group to improve poor classroom performance is an 
example of problem-focused coping. In addition, one might take a time-out 
from stress by creating positive events. For example, taking a day off from 
caring for a relative with a serious, chronic illness to go a health club or spa 
can bring significant relief from stress. 


People often employ several types of coping strategies simultaneously. Further- 
more, they use emotion-focused strategies more frequently when they perceive cir- 
cumstances as being unchangeable and problem-focused strategies more often in 
situations they see as relatively modifiable (Stanton et al., 2000; Penley, Tomaka, & 
Wiebe, 2002). 

Some forms of coping are less successful. One of the least effective forms of cop- 
ing is avoidant coping. In avoidant coping, a person may use wishful thinking to 
reduce stress or use more direct escape routes, such as drug use, alcohol use, and 
overeating. An example of wishful thinking to avoid a test would be to say to oneself, 
“Maybe it will snow so hard tomorrow that the test will be canceled.” Alternatively, 
a person might get drunk to avoid a problem. Either way, avoidant coping usually 
results in a postponement of dealing with a stressful situation, and this often makes 
the problem even worse (Roesch et al., 2005; Hutchinson, Baldwin, & Oh, 2006; Glass 
et al., 2009). 

Another way of dealing with stress occurs unconsciously through the use of 
defense mechanisms. As we discussed in Chapter 13, Personality, defense mecha- 
nisms are unconscious strategies that people use to reduce anxiety by concealing 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


the source from themselves and others. Defense mechanisms permit people to 
avoid stress by acting as if the stress were not even there. For example, one study 
examined California college students who lived in dormitories close to a geologi- 
cal fault (Lehman & Taylor, 1988). Those who lived in dorms that were known to 
be unlikely to withstand an earthquake were significantly more likely to doubt 
experts’ predictions of an impending earthquake than were those who lived in 
safer structures. 

Another defense mechanism used to cope with stress is emotional insulation in 
which a person stops experiencing any emotions at all and thereby remains unaf- 
fected and unmoved by both positive and negative experiences. The problem with 
defense mechanisms, of course, is that they merely hide the problem and do not deal 
with reality. 


LEARNED HELPLESSNESS 


Have you ever faced an intolerable situation that you just couldn’t resolve, and you 
finally simply gave up and accepted things the way they were? This example illus- 
trates one of the possible consequences of being in an environment in which control 
over a situation is not possible—a state that produces learned helplessness. Learned 
helplessness occurs when people conclude that unpleasant or aversive stimuli cannot 
be controlled—a view of the world that becomes so ingrained that they cease trying 
to remedy the aversive circumstances even if they actually can exert some influence 
on the situation (Seligman, 1975, 2007; Aujoulat, Luminet, & Deccache, 2007). 

Victims of learned helplessness have concluded that there is no link between 
the responses they make and the outcomes that occur. People experience more 
physical symptoms and depression when they perceive that they have little or no 
control than they do when they feel a sense of control over a situation (Chou, 2005; 
Bjornstad, 2006). 


COPING STYLES: THE HARDY PERSONALITY 


Most of us characteristically cope with stress by employing a coping style that repre- 
sents our general tendency to deal with stress in a specific way. For example, you 
may know people who habitually react to even the smallest amount of stress with 
hysteria and others who calmly confront even the greatest stress in an unflappable 
manner. These kinds of people clearly have very different cop- 
ing styles (Taylor, 2003; Kato & Pedersen, 2005). 

Among those who cope with stress most successfully are 
people who are equipped with hardiness, a personality char- 
acteristic associated with a lower rate of stress-related illness. 
Hardiness consists of three components (Baumgartner, 2002; 
Maddi, 2007): 


e Commitment. Commitment is a tendency to throw 
ourselves into whatever we are doing with a sense that 
our activities are important and meaningful. 

e Challenge. Hardy people believe that change rather than 
stability is the standard condition of life. To them, the 
anticipation of change serves as an incentive rather than 
a threat to their security. 

e Control. Hardiness is marked by a sense of control—the 
perception that people can influence the events in their 
lives. 


Hardy individuals approach stress optimistically and take 





Module 43 Stress and Coping 481 


learned helplessness A state in which 
people conclude that unpleasant or 
aversive stimuli cannot be controlled— 
a view of the world that becomes so 
ingrained that they cease trying to 
remedy the aversive circumstances 
even if they actually can exert some 
influence on the situation. 


hardiness A personality characteristic 
that is associated with a lower rate of 
stress-related illness and consists of 
three components: commitment, 
challenge, and control. 


© Edward Koren/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


: é : “Today, we examined our life style, we evaluated our diet and 
direct action to learn about and deal with stressors; they our exercise program, and we also assessed our behavioral 


thereby change stressful events into less threatening ones. As pattern. Then we needed a drink.” 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


482 Chapter 14 Health Psychology: Stress, Coping, and Well-Being 


social support A mutual network of 
caring, interested others. 


Stress and Social Support 





FIGURE 6 Participants in a study were threatened by being told they would be receiving a 
shock. When they were provided social support, areas of the brain that become activated 
during stress showed reduced activation. Specifically, social support in this study was having 
either a stranger or the participant's spouse hold his or her hand. In the figure, the color 
green highlights brain areas that showed reductions in activity when a spouse held the 
participant's hand, and the color blue highlights reductions in activity when either a spouse 
or a stranger held the participant's hand. (Source: Coan, Schaefer, & Davidson, 2006, Figure 3.) 





a consequence, hardiness acts as a defense against stress-related illness (Andrew et 
al., 2008; Bartone et al., 2008; Vogt et al., 2008). 

For those who confront the most profound difficulties, such as the death of a 
loved one or a permanent injury such as paralysis after an accident, a key ingredient 
in their psychological recovery is their degree of resilience. Resilience is the ability to 
withstand, overcome, and actually thrive after profound adversity (Bonanno, 2004; 
Norlander, Von Schedvin, & Archer, 2005; Jackson, 2006). 

Resilient people are generally easygoing, good-natured, and have good social 
skills. They are usually independent, and they have a sense of control over their own 
destiny—even if fate has dealt them a devastating blow. In short, they work with 
what they have and make the best of whatever situation they find themselves in 
(Spencer et al., 2003; Deshields et al., 2006; Friborg et al., 2005). 


SOCIAL SUPPORT: TURNING TO OTHERS 


Our relationships with others also help us cope with stress. Researchers have found 
that social support, the knowledge that we are part of a mutual network of caring, 
interested others, enables us to experience lower levels of stress and better cope with 
the stress we do undergo (Cohen, 2004; Martin & Brantley, 2004; Bolger & Amarel, 
2007). 

The social and emotional support people provide each other helps in dealing 
with stress in several ways. For instance, such support demonstrates that a person 
is an important and valued member of a social network. Similarly, other people can 
provide information and advice about appropriate ways of dealing with stress (Day 
& Livingstone, 2003; Lindorff, 2005). 

Finally, people who are part of a social support network can provide actual 
goods and services to help others in stressful situations. For instance, they can 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


supply temporary living quarters to a person whose house has burned down, or 
they can offer study help to a student who is experiencing stress because of poor 
academic performance (Natvig, Albrektsen, & Ovamstrom, 2003; Takizawa, Kondo, 
& Sakihara, 2007). 

Findings that attendance at religious services (as well as spirituality in general) 
provides health-related benefits also illustrate the importance of social support. For 
example, healthy people who regularly attend religious services live longer than 
those who do not attend regularly (Miller & Thoresen, 2003; Powell, Shahabi, & 
Thoresen, 2003; Gilbert, 2007). 

Recent research is also beginning to identify how social support affects brain 
processing. For instance, one experiment found that activation of the areas of the brain 
reflecting stress was reduced when social support—simply being able to hold the 
hand of another person—was available (see Neuroscience in Your Life, Figure 6; Coan, 
Schaefer, & Davidson, 2006). 


How can we deal with the stress in our lives? Although there is BECOMING AN 


Module 43 Stress and Coping 


no universal solution because effective coping depends on the INFORMED CONSUMER 


nature of the stressor and the degree to which it can be 


controlled, here are some general guidelines (Aspinwall & of Psychology 


Taylor, 1997; Folkman & Moskowitz, 2000): 


e Turn a threat into a challenge. When a stressful situation 
might be controllable, the best coping strategy is to treat 
the situation as a challenge and focus on ways to control it. For instance, if you 
experience stress because your car is always breaking down, you might take a 
course in auto mechanics and learn to deal directly with the car’s problems. 

e Make a threatening situation less threatening. When a stressful situation seems to be 
uncontrollable, you need to take a different approach. It is possible to change 
your appraisal of the situation, view it in a different light, and modify your 
attitude toward it. Research supports the old truism, “Look for the silver lining in 
every cloud” (Smith & Lazarus, 2001; Cheng & Cheung, 2005). 

e Change your goals. If you are faced with an uncontrollable situation, a reasonable 
strategy is to adopt new goals that are practical in view of the particular situa- 
tion. For example, a dancer who has been in an automobile accident and has lost 
full use of her legs may no longer aspire to a career in dance but might modify 
her goals and try to become a choreographer. 

e Take physical action. Changing your physiological reaction to stress can help with 
coping. For example, biofeedback (in which a person learns to control internal 
physiological processes through conscious thought) can alter basic physiological 
processes and allow people to reduce blood pressure, heart rate, and other 
consequences of heightened stress. Exercise can also be effective in reducing 
stress (Langreth, 2000; Spencer et al., 2003; Hamer, Taylor, & Steptoe, 2006). 

e Prepare for stress before it happens. A final strategy for coping with stress is proactive 
coping, anticipating and preparing for stress before it is encountered. For example, 
if you're expecting to go through a one-week period in which you must take a 
number of major tests, you can try to arrange your schedule so you have more 
time to study (Aspinwall & Taylor, 1997; Bode et al., 2007). 


Effective Coping Strategies 


483 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


484 Chapter 14 Health Psychology: Stress, Coping, and Well-Being 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP EVALUATE 

How is health psychology a union between medicine and 1. _____ is defined as a response to challenging or 

psychology? threatening events. 

e The field of health psychology considers how psychol- 2. Match each portion of the GAS with its definition. 

ogy can be applied to the prevention, diagnosis, and 1. Alarm and a. Ability to adapt to stress diminishes; 
treatment of medical problems. (p. 472) mobilization symptoms appear. 

What is stress, how does it affect us, and how can we best cope a Fahauekon Ba eny on Oley mpeniedonet vous 

3. Resistance system. 


with it? 

e Stress is a response to threatening or challenging 
environmental conditions. People encounter stressors— 
the circumstances that produce stress—of both a positive 
and a negative nature. (p. 473) 

e The way an environmental circumstance is interpreted 
affects whether it will be considered stressful. Still, there 
are general classes of events that provoke stress: 
cataclysmic events, personal stressors, and background 
stressors (daily hassles). (p. 474) 

e Stress produces immediate physiological reactions. In 
the short term those reactions may be adaptive, but in 
the long term they may have negative consequences, RETHINK 
including the development of psychophysiological 
disorders. (p. 476) 

e The consequences of stress can be explained in part by 
Selye’s general adaptation syndrome (GAS), which sug- 
gests that there are three stages in stress responses: alarm 
and mobilization, resistance, and exhaustion. (p. 477) 

e Coping with stress can take a number of forms, 


c. Various strategies are used to cope 
with a stressor. 
3. Stressors that affect a single person and produce an imme- 
diate major reaction are known as 
a. Personal stressors 
b. Psychic stressors 
c. Cataclysmic stressors 
d. Daily stressors 
4. People with the personality characteristic of 
seem to be better able to successfully combat stressors. 


1. Why are cataclysmic stressors less stressful in the long run 
than other types of stressors? Does the reason relate to the 
coping phenomenon known as social support? How? 

2. From the perspective of a social worker: How would you help 
people deal with and avoid stress in their everyday lives? 
How might you encourage people to create social support 


including the unconscious use of defense mechanisms MEH OHS! 

and the use of emotion-focused or problem-focused Answers to Evaluate Questions 

coping strategies. (p. 480) ssaurprey p ee (9-¢ ec daa oS T 

e Stress can be reduced by developing a sense of control 

over one’s circumstances. In some cases, however, 

people develop a state of learned helplessness. (p. 481) 
health psychology p. 472 personal stressors p. 474 psychophysiological learned helplessness p. 481 
psychoneuroimmunology posttraumatic stress disorders p. 477 hardiness p. 481 

(PNI) p.472 disorder (PTSD) p. 474 general adaptation social support p. 482 

stress p. 473 background stressors (“daily syndrome (GAS) p. 478 


cataclysmic events p. 474 hassles”) p. 475 coping p. 480 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 44 


I feel that it is absolutely necessary to be my own best advocate, and the best place to 
learn how to do that is in a group of other well-educated patients and their caregivers. 
We know what life post-diagnosis is like, and we help each other in ways that no docs, 
nurses, clergy, well-meaning friends and family possibly can. We laugh, we cry, we 
bitch, and we push and pull each other! We mourn the losses, celebrate small and large 
victories, and we educate ourselves and others. But most importantly—we embrace 
each other and our lives. (Anonymous blogpost, 2010) 


As recently as three decades ago, most psychologists and health-care providers 
would have scoffed at the notion that a discussion group could improve a cancer 
patient’s chances of survival. Today, however, such methods have gained increasing 
acceptance. 

Growing evidence suggests that psychological factors have a substantial impact 
both on major health problems that were once seen in purely physiological terms 
and on our everyday sense of health, well-being, and happiness. We’ll consider the 
psychological components of three major health problems—heart disease, cancer, and 
smoking—and then consider the nature of people’s well-being and happiness. 


The As, Bs, and Ds of Coronary 
Heart Disease 








Tim knew it wasn’t going to be his day when he got stuck in traffic behind a slow- 
moving farm truck. How could the driver dawdle like that? Didn’t he have anything of 
any importance to do? Things didn’t get any better when Tim arrived on campus and 
discovered the library didn’t have the books he needed. He could almost feel the 
tension rising. 


“I need that material to finish my paper,” he thought to himself. 


He knew that meant he wouldn’t be able to get his paper done early, and that meant he 
wouldn't have the time he wanted to revise the paper. He wanted it to be a first-class 
paper. This time Tim wanted to get a better grade than his roommate, Luis. Although 
Luis didn’t know it, Tim felt they were in competition and that Luis was always trying 
to better him whether academically or just playing cards. 


“In fact,” Tim mused to himself, “I feel like I’m in competition with everyone, no 
matter what I’m doing.” 


Have you, like Tim, ever seethed impatiently at being caught behind a slow- 
moving vehicle, felt anger and frustration at not finding material you needed at the 
library, or experienced a sense of competitiveness with your classmates? 

Many of us experience these sorts of feelings at one time or another, but for some 
people they represent a pervasive, characteristic set of personality traits known as 
the Type A behavior pattern. The Type A behavior pattern is a cluster of behaviors 
involving hostility, competitiveness, time urgency, and feeling driven. In contrast, the 


How do psychological factors 


affect health-related prob- 


lems such as coronary heart 
disease, cancer, and smoking? 


A cluster of 
behaviors involving hostility, competi- 


tiveness, time urgency, and feeling 
driven. 


485 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


486 Chapter 14 Health Psychology: Stress, Coping, and Well-Being 


Type B behavior pattern A cluster of 
behaviors characterized by a patient, 
cooperative, noncompetitive, and 
nonaggressive manner. 


y Alert 


It’s important to distinguish 
among Type A (hostility, 
competitiveness), Type B 
(patience, cooperative- 
ness), and Type D 
(distressed) behaviors. 


Type B behavior pattern is characterized by a patient, cooperative, noncompetitive, 
and nonaggressive manner. It’s important to keep in mind that Type A and Type B 
represent the ends of a continuum, and most people fall somewhere in between the 
two endpoints. Few people are purely a Type A or a Type B. 

The importance of the Type A behavior pattern lies in its links to coronary heart 
disease. Men who display the Type A pattern develop coronary heart disease twice 
as often and suffer significantly more fatal heart attacks than those classified as hav- 
ing the Type B pattern. Moreover, the Type A pattern predicts who is going to develop 
heart disease at least as well as—and independently of—any other single factor, 
including age, blood pressure, smoking habits, and cholesterol levels in the body 
(Rosenman et al., 1994; Wielgosz & Nolan, 2000; Beresnevaité, Taylor, & Bagby, 2007). 

Hostility is the key component of the Type A behavior pattern that is related to 
heart disease. Although competition, time urgency, and feelings of being driven may 
produce stress and potentially other health and emotional problems, they aren’t 
linked to coronary heart disease the way that hostility is (Williams et al., 2000; Boyle 
et al., 2005; Ohira et al., 2007). 

Why is hostility so toxic? The key reason is that hostility produces excessive 
physiological arousal in stressful situations. That arousal, in turn, results in increased 
production of the hormones epinephrine and norepinephrine as well as increases in 
heart rate and blood pressure. Such an exaggerated physiological response ultimately 
produces an increased incidence of coronary heart disease (Eaker et al., 2004; Dema- 
ree & Everhart, 2004; Myrtek, 2007). 

It’s important to keep in mind that not everyone who displays Type A behaviors 
is destined to have coronary heart disease. For one thing, a firm association between 
Type A behaviors and coronary heart disease has not been established for women; 
most findings pertain to males partly because until recently most research was done 
on men. In addition, other types of negative emotions besides the hostility found in 
Type A behavior appear to be related to heart attacks. For example, psychologist 
Johan Denollet has found evidence that what he calls Type D—for “distressed”— 
behavior is linked to coronary heart disease. In this view, insecurity, anxiety, and the 
negative outlook Type Ds display puts them at risk for repeated heart attacks (Denol- 
let, 2005; Schiffer et al., 2005; Spindler et al., 2009). 


Psychological Aspects of Cancer 


Hardly any disease is feared more than cancer. Most people think of cancer in terms 
of lingering pain, and being diagnosed with the disease is typically viewed as receiv- 
ing a death sentence. 

Although a diagnosis of cancer is not as grim as it once was—several kinds of 
cancer have a high cure rate if detected early enough—cancer remains the second 
leading cause of death after coronary heart disease. The precise trigger for the disease 
is not well understood, but the process by which cancer spreads is straightforward. 
Certain cells in the body become altered and multiply rapidly in an uncontrolled 
fashion. As those cells grow, they form tumors; if left unchecked, the tumors suck 
nutrients from healthy cells and body tissue and ultimately destroy the body’s abil- 
ity to function properly. 

Although the processes involved in the spread of cancer are basically physiolog- 
ical, some research suggests that the emotional responses of cancer patients to their 
disease may affect its course. For example, some findings show that a “fighting 
spirit” leads to better coping. On the other hand, there is little evidence that long- 
term survival rates are better than for patients with a less positive attitude (Watson 
et al., 1999; Rom, Miller, & Peluso, 2009). 

Despite conflicting evidence, health psychologists believe that patients’ emotions 
may at least partially determine the course of their disease. In the case of cancer, it 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 44 Psychological Aspects of Illness and Well-Being 487 


is possible that positive emotional responses may help generate specialized “killer” 
cells that help control the size and spread of cancerous tumors. Conversely, negative 
emotions may suppress the ability of those cells to fight tumors (Schedlowski & 
Tewes, 1999; Noy, 2006). 

Is a particular personality type linked to cancer? Some researchers suggest that 
cancer patients are less emotionally reactive, suppress anger, and lack outlets for 
emotional release. However, the data are too tentative and inconsistent to suggest 
firm conclusions about a link between personality characteristics and cancer. Cer- 
tainly no conclusive evidence suggests that people who develop cancer would not 
have done so if their personality had been of a different sort or if their attitudes had 
been more positive (Smith, 1988; Zevon & Corn, 1990; Holland & Lewis, 2001). 

What is increasingly clear, however, is that certain types of psychological therapy 
have the potential for improving quality of life and even extending the lives of can- 
cer patients. For example, the results of one study showed that women with breast 
cancer who received psychological treatment lived at least a year and a half longer 
and experienced less anxiety and pain than women who did not participate in ther- 
apy. Research on patients with other health problems, such as heart disease, also has 
found that therapy can be both psychologically and medically beneficial (Spiegel, 
1996; Frasure-Smith, Lesperance, & Talajic, 2000; Butler et al., 2009). 





Smoking 


Would you walk into a convenience store and buy an item with a label warning you 
that its use could kill you? Although most people would probably answer no, mil- 
lions make such a purchase everyday: a pack of cigarettes. Furthermore, they do this 
despite clear, well-publicized evidence that smoking is linked to cancer, heart attacks, 
strokes, bronchitis, emphysema, and a host of other serious illnesses. Smoking is the 
greatest preventable cause of death in the United States; one in five U.S. deaths is 
caused by smoking. Worldwide, close to 5 million people die each year from the 
effects of smoking (Danaei et al., 2005). 


WHY PEOPLE SMOKE OZ. 


Why do people smoke despite all the evidence showing that it is bad for their 
health? It is not that they are somehow unaware of the link between smoking CLEAN AIR CALABASAS 
and disease; surveys show that most smokers agree with the statement, “Cigarette MP eee 
smoking frequently causes disease and death.” And almost three-quarters of the 
48 million smokers in the United States say they would like to quit. Still, 700,000 
people a year take up the habit (Wetter et al., 1998; Price, 2008). 

Heredity seems to determine, in part, whether people will become smokers, 
how much they will smoke, and how easily they can quit. Genetics also influences 
how susceptible people are to the harmful effects of smoking. For instance, there 
is an almost 50% higher rate of lung cancer in African-American smokers than in 
white smokers. This difference may be due to genetically produced variations in 
the efficiency with which enzymes are able to reduce the effects of the cancer- 
causing chemicals in tobacco smoke (Pomerlau, 1995; Li et al., 2003; Li et al., 2008). 

However, although genetics plays a role in smoking, most research suggests 
that environmental factors are the primary cause of the habit. Smoking at first 
may be seen as “cool” or sophisticated, as a rebellious act, or as facilitating calm 
performance in stressful situations. Greater exposure to smoking in media such 
as film also leads to a higher risk of becoming an established smoker. In addition, 
smoking a cigarette is sometimes viewed as a “rite of passage” for adolescents Although smoking is prohibited in an 
undertaken at the urging of friends and viewed as a sign of growing up (Sargent increasing number of places, it remains a 
et al., 2007; Wills et al., 2008; Heatherton & Sargent, 2009). substantial social problem. 





488 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 14 Health Psychology: Stress, Coping, and Well-Being 


Smoking— 


Why It’s So Difficult to Quit 





FIGURE 1 Even with the use of treatments such as nicotine replacement therapies, relapse 
for exsmokers is common. One potential reason smokers relapse is that media, such as 
cigarette ads, can trigger cravings in exsmokers even when they are no longer addicted to 
nicotine. These MRI scans show the brain of a person after quitting smoking. Some areas 
of the brain, shown in red, relate to habit learning, action planning, and craving. These 
areas show greater activation when viewing smoking-related images after quitting as 
compared to before quitting smoking. In addition, some areas of the brain, shown in blue, 
relate to memory formation and show reduced activity while viewing smoking-related 
images after quitting. Differences in how an exsmoker’s brain responds to these images 
may explain why the desire to smoke persists long after quitting smoking. (Source: Janes 

et al., 2009, Figure 1.) 





Ultimately, smoking becomes a habit. And it’s an easy habit to pick up: Smok- 
ing even a single cigarette leads to a loss of autonomy, when a smoker finds that 
not smoking requires an effort or involves discomfort. Subsequently, people begin 
to label themselves smokers, and smoking becomes part of their self-concept. More- 
over, they become dependent physiologically as a result of smoking because nico- 
tine, a primary ingredient of tobacco, is highly addictive. A complex relationship 
develops among smoking, nicotine levels, and a smoker’s emotions in which a cer- 
tain nicotine level becomes associated with a positive emotional state. As a result, 
people smoke in an effort to regulate both emotional states and nicotine levels in the 
blood (Amos, Wiltshire, & Haw, 2006; Kassel et al., 2007; Ursprung, Sanouri, & 
DiFranza, 2009). 


QUITTING SMOKING 


Because smoking has both psychological and biological components, few habits are 
as difficult to break. Long-term successful treatment typically occurs in just 15% of 
those who try to stop smoking; once smoking becomes a habit, it is as hard to stop 
as an addiction to cocaine or heroin. In fact, some of the biochemical reactions to 
nicotine are similar to those to cocaine, amphetamines, and morphine. Furthermore, 
changes in brain chemistry brought about by smoking may make smokers more 
resistant to antismoking messages see also Figure 1; (Vanasse, Niyonsenga, & 
Courteau, 2004; Foulds et al., 2006; Dani & Montague, 2007). 

Many people try to quit smoking but fail. The average smoker tries to quit 8 
to 10 times before being successful, and even then many relapse. Even long-time 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 44 Psychological Aspects of Illness and Well-Being 


100 


=== 8th Grade 
=== |0th Grade 
=== |2th Grade 


80 


60 


Percent 


40 


20 





’76’78'80 8284868890 929496 98000204 ’06’08 
Year 
FIGURE 2 Although smoking among teenagers is lower than it was 20 years ago, a 
significant number still report smoking regularly. What factors might account for the 
continued high use of tobacco by teenagers despite the increase in antismoking advertising? 
(Source: Johnston et al., 2009.) 


quitters can fall off the wagon: About 10% relapse after more than year of avoiding 
cigarettes (Grady & Altman, 2008). 

Among the most effective tools for ending the smoking habit are drugs that 
replace the nicotine found in cigarettes. Whether in the form of gum, patches, nasal 
sprays, or inhalers, these products provide a dose of nicotine that reduces depen- 
dence on cigarettes. Another approach is exemplified by the drugs Zyban and Chant- 
rix; rather than replacing nicotine, they reduce the pleasure from smoking and 
suppress withdrawal symptoms that smokers experience when they try to stop (Gar- 
wood & Potts, 2007; Shiffman, 2007; Brody, 2008). 

Behavioral strategies, which view smoking as a learned habit and concentrate on 
changing the smoking response, can also be effective. Initial “cure” rates of 60% have 
been reported, and one year after treatment more than half of those who quit have 
not resumed smoking. Individual or group counseling also increases the rate of suc- 
cess in breaking the habit. The best treatment seems to be a combination of nicotine 
replacement and counseling. What doesn’t work? Going it alone: Only 5% of smok- 
ers who quit cold-turkey on their own are successful (Noble, 1999; Rock, 1999; Wood- 
ruff, Conway, & Edwards, 2007). 

In the long term, the most effective means of reducing smoking may be changes 
in societal norms and attitudes toward the habit. For instance, many cities and towns 
have made smoking in public places illegal; legislation based on strong popular 
sentiment that bans smoking in places such as college classrooms and buildings is 
being passed with increasing frequency. In addition, smokers are more likely to quit 
when their friends are quitting, so the social support of others quitting is helpful 
(Hamilton, Biener, & Brennan, 2007; Christakis & Fowler, 2008). 

The long-term effect of the barrage of information regarding the negative 
consequences of smoking on people’s health has been substantial; overall, smoking 
has declined over the last two decades, particularly among males. Still, more than 
one-fourth of students enrolled in high school are active smokers by the time they 
graduate, and there is evidence that the decline in smoking is leveling off. Among 
these students, around 10% become active smokers as early as the eighth grade 
(see Figure 2; Johnston, O’Malley, & Bachman, 2009; Fichtenberg & Glantz, 2006). 


489 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


490 Chapter 14 Health Psychology: Stress, Coping, and Well-Being 





we ji 
9 


E” Throughout the World 


a | 





In some countries, children as young as 
6 smoke regularly. 


I) Hucksters of Death: Promoting Smoking 


Ex p | O ri Nn g A Jeep decorated with the Camel logo pulls up to a high school in 
DIVERSITY 


Buenos Aires. A woman begins handing out free cigarettes to 15- and 
16-year-olds during their lunch recess. At a video arcade in Taipei, 
free American cigarettes are strewn atop each game. At a disco filled 
with high school students, free packs of Salems are on each table. 
(Ecenbarger, 1993, p. 50) 


Because the number of smokers has steadily declined in the United States, cigarette 
manufacturers have turned their sights to other parts of the world, where they see a 
fertile market for their product. Although they must often sell cigarettes more cheaply 
than they do in the United States, the huge number of potential smokers still makes it 
financially worthwhile for the tobacco companies. The United States is now the world’s 
largest exporter of cigarettes (Bartecchi, MacKenzie, & Schrier, 1995; Brown, 2001). 

Clearly, the push into worldwide markets has been successful. In some Latin 
American cities, as many as 50% of teenagers smoke. Children as young as age 7 
smoke in Hong Kong; 30% of children smoked their first whole cigarette before the 
age of 10 in India, Ghana, Jamaica, and Poland. The World Health Organization 
predicts that smoking will prematurely kill some 200 million of the world’s children 
and that ultimately 10% of the world’s population will die as a result of smoking. Of 
everyone alive today, 500 million will eventually die from tobacco use (Mackay & 
Eriksen, 2002). 

One reason for the increase in smoking in developing countries is that their 
governments make little effort to discourage it. In fact, many governments are in the 
tobacco business themselves and rely on revenues from tobacco. For example, the 
world’s largest manufacturer of cigarettes is the China National Tobacco Corporation, 
which is owned by the Chinese government (Marsh, 2008). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 44 Psychological Aspects of Illness and Well-Being 491 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 3. Acancer patient’s attitude and emotions may affect that 
person’s system and thus help or hinder the 
patient’s fight against the disease. 

4. Smoking is used to regulate both nicotine levels and emo- 


tional states in smokers. True or false? 


How do psychological factors affect health-related problems 
such as coronary heart disease, cancer, and smoking? 

e Hostility, a key component of the Type A behavior pat- 
tern, is linked to coronary heart disease. The Type A 
behavior pattern is a cluster of behaviors involving 
hostility, competitiveness, time urgency, and feeling 
driven. (p. 485) 

e People’s attitudes and emotional responses may affect 


RETHINK 


1. Is there a danger of “blaming the victim” when we argue 


1. Type 


the course of cancer through links to the immune sys- 
tem. (p. 486) 

Smoking, the leading preventable cause of health prob- 
lems, has proved to be difficult to quit, even though 
most smokers are aware of the dangerous consequences 
of the behavior. (p. 487) 


EVALUATE 


behavior is characterized by cooperative- 
ness and by being easygoing; Type behavior is 


characterized by hostility and competitiveness. 


2. The Type A behavior pattern is known to directly cause 


heart attacks. True or false? 


KEY TERMS 


Type A behavior pattern p. 485 


that the course of cancer can be improved if a person with 
the disease holds positive attitudes or beliefs, particularly 
when we consider people with cancer who are not recov- 
ering? Explain your answer. 


. From the perspective of a health-care provider: What type of 


advice would you give to your patients about the connec- 
tions between personality and disease? For example, 
would you encourage Type A people to become “less Type 
A” in order to decrease their risk of heart disease? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


and} 'p ‘ouUNUTUIT g 
‘ATJDAITP 4 asned ATLIessadau JOU Saop Mq asvastp Ray ATRUOIOD 
JO douaproUT JaYySIY L 0} POPPI St oTAvYyaq V ƏdÁ] ‘asTey "Zz ‘V ‘GT 


Type B behavior pattern p. 486 


“\ www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


How do our interactions with 
physicians affect our health 
and compliance with medical 
treatment? 


How does a sense of well- 
being develop? 


A negative emotional and 
cognitive reaction that results from the 
restriction of one’s freedom. 


492 


When Stuart Grinspoon first noticed the small lump in his arm, he assumed it was just 
a bruise from the touch football game he had played the previous week. But as he 
thought about it more, he considered more serious possibilities and decided that he’d 
better get it checked out at the university health service. But the visit was less than 
satisfactory. A shy person, Stuart felt embarrassed talking about his medical condition. 
Even worse, after answering a string of questions, he couldn’t even understand the phy- 
sician’s diagnosis and was too embarrassed to ask for clarification. 


Many of us share Stuart Grinspoon’s attitudes toward health care. We approach 
physicians the same way we approach auto mechanics. When something goes wrong 
with the car, we want the mechanic to figure out the problem and then fix it. In the 
same way, when something isn’t working right with our bodies, we want a diagno- 
sis of the problem and then a (we hope, quick) repair. 

Yet such an approach ignores the fact that—unlike auto repair—good health care 
requires taking psychological factors into account. Health psychologists have sought 
to determine the factors involved in the promotion of good health and, more broadly, 
a sense of well-being and happiness. Let’s take a closer look at two areas they have 
tackled: producing compliance with health-related advice and identifying the deter- 
minants of well-being and happiness. 





Following Medical Advice 


We're not very good at taking medical advice. Consider these figures: 


e As many as 85% of patients do not fully comply with a physician’s 
recommendations. 

e Between 14-21% of patients don’t ever fill their drug prescriptions. 

e Some 10% of adolescent pregnancies result from noncompliance with birth 
control practices. 

e Sixty percent of all patients cannot identify their own medicines. 

e From 30-50% of all patients ignore instructions or make errors in taking 
medication (Christensen & Johnson, 2002; Health Pages, 2003; Colland et al., 
2004). 


Noncompliance with medical advice can take many forms. For example, patients 
may fail to show up for scheduled appointments, not follow diets or not give up 
smoking, or discontinue medication during treatment. In some cases, they fail to take 
prescribed medicine at all. 

Patients also may practice creative nonadherence in which they adjust a treatment 
prescribed by a physician by relying on their own medical judgment and experience. 
In many cases patients’ lack of medical knowledge may be harmful (Taylor, 1995; 
Hamani et al., 2007). 

Noncompliance is sometimes a result of psychological reactance. Reactance is a 
negative emotional and cognitive reaction that results from the restriction of one’s 
freedom. People who experience reactance feel hostility and anger. Because of such 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 45 


emotions, they may seek to restore their sense of freedom but in a self-destructive 
manner by refusing to accept medical advice and perhaps acting in a way that worsens 
their medical condition. For instance, a man who is placed on a strict diet may experi- 
ence reactance and tend to eat even more than he did before his diet was restricted 
(Fogarty & Young, 2000; Dillard & Shen, 2004; Woller, Buboltz, & Loveland, 2007). 


COMMUNICATING EFFECTIVELY 
WITH HEALTH-CARE PROVIDERS 


I was lying on a gurney, trying to prepare myself for a six-hour breast-reconstruction 
surgery. A few months earlier, I’d had a mastectomy for breast cancer. Because I’m 
small-boned, my doctor told me I needed to have a muscle sliced from my back and 
moved to my chest to create a proper foundation for an implant. I knew the operation 
would slow me down—bad news for someone who swims, runs, and chases three 
young kids. But as the surgeon diagrammed incision points on my chest with a felt-tip 
pen, my husband asked a question: “Is it really necessary to transfer this back muscle?” 
(Halpert, 2003, p. 63) 


The surgeon’s answer shocked the patient: No, it wasn’t necessary. And if she didn’t 
have the procedure, her recovery time would be cut in half. The surgeon had simply 
assumed, without asking the patient, that she would prefer the more complicated 
procedure because it would be preferable cosmetically. But after a hurried consulta- 
tion with her husband, the patient opted for the less invasive procedure. 

Lack of communication between medical care providers and patients can be a 
major obstacle to good medical care. Such communication failures occur for several 
reasons. One is that physicians make assumptions about what patients prefer, or they 
push a specific treatment that they prefer without consulting patients. Furthermore, 
the relatively high prestige of physicians may intimidate patients. Patients may also 
be reluctant to volunteer information that might cast them in a bad light, and physi- 
cians may have difficulties encouraging their patients to provide information. In 
many cases, physicians dominate an interview with questions of a technical nature, 
whereas patients attempt to communicate a personal sense of their illness and the 
impact it is having on their lives, as illustrated in Figure 1 (Ihler, 2003; Schillinger et 
al., 2004; Wain, Grammer, & Stasinos, 2006). 

Furthermore, the view many patients hold that physicians are “all knowing” can 
result in serious communication problems. Many patients do not understand their 
treatments yet fail to ask their physicians for clear explanations of a prescribed course 
of action. About half of all patients are unable to report accurately how long they 
are to continue taking a medication prescribed for them, and about a quarter do not 
even know the purpose of the drug. In fact, some patients are not even sure as they 
are about to be rolled into the operating room, why they are having surgery (Svarstad, 
1976; Atkinson, 1997; Halpert, 2003)! 

Sometimes patient—physician communication difficulties occur 
because the material that must be communicated is too technical for 
patients, who may lack fundamental knowledge about the body and 
basic medical practices. In response to this problem, some health-care 
providers routinely use baby talking (calling patients “honey” or telling 
them to go “night-night”) and assume that patients are unable to 
understand even simple information (Whitbourne & Wills, 1993; Mika 
et al., 2007). 

The amount of physician—patient communication also is related 
to the sex of a physician and patient. Overall, female primary care 
physicians provide more patient-centered communications than do 
male primary care physicians. Furthermore, patients often prefer 


same-sex physicians (Roter, Hall, & Aoki, 2002; Kiss, 2004; Schnatz et “There s no easy way I can tell you this, so I’m 


Promoting Health and Wellness 





al., 2007; Bertakis, 2009; Bertakis, Franks, & Epstein, 2009). sending you to someone who can.” 


493 


© Peter C. Vey/The New Yorker Collection/www. 


cartoonbank.com. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


494 Chapter 14 Health Psychology: Stress, Coping, and Well-Being 


FIGURE 1 Effective communication 
between patient and physician is 
important but often proves frustrating 
for both. (Source: From Daniel Goleman, 
“Doctor and Patient; Physicians May Bungle 
Key Part of Treatment: The Medical Interview.’ 
From The New York Times, January 21, 1988, 
p. B16. © 1988 The New York Times. All rights 
reserved. Used by permission and protected 
by the Copyright Laws of the United States. 
The printing, copying, redistribution, or 
retransmission of the Material without 
express written permission is prohibited. 
www.nytimes.com.) 


A Patient Talks to Her Physician 


The following excerpt from a case study used at the Harvard Medical School is an 
example of poor interviewing technique on the part of the physician. 


Patient: | can hardly drink water. 

Doctor: Um hum. 

Patient: | Remember when it started? . . . It was pains in my head. It must have been then. 

Doctor: Um-hum. 

Patient: | don’t know what it is. The doctor looked at it . . . said something about 
glands. 

Doctor: Ok. Um-hum, aside from this, how have you been feeling? 

Patient: Terrible. 

Doctor: Yeah. 

Patient: Tired... there's pains .. . | don’t know what it is. 

Doctor: Ok.... Fever or chills? 

Patient: No. 

Doctor: Ok. ... Have you been sick to your stomach or anything? 

Patient: (Sniffles, crying) | don’t know what's going on. | get up in the morning tired. 
The only time | feel good . .. maybe like around suppertime . . . and 
everything (crying) and still the same thing. 

Doctor: | Um-hum. You're getting the nausea before you eat or after? (Goleman, 1988, p. B16) 


Although the frequent “um-hums” suggest that the physician is listening to the patient, 
in fact he does not encourage the patient to disclose more pertinent details. Even 
more, late in the interview, the physician ignores the patient’s emotional distress and 
coldly continues through the list of questions. 


Cultural values and expectations also contribute to communication barriers 
between patients and their physicians. Providing medical advice to a patient whose 
native language is not English may be problematic. Furthermore, medical practices 
differ between cultures, and medical practitioners need to be familiar with a patient’s 
culture in order to produce compliance with medical recommendations (Whaley, 
2000; Ho et al., 2004; Culhane-Pera, Borkan, & Patten, 2007). 

What can patients do to improve communication with health-care providers? 
Here are some tips provided by physician Holly Atkinson (Atkinson, 2003): 


e Make a list of health-related concerns before you visit a health-care provider. 

e Before a visit, write down the names and dosages of every drug you are 
currently taking. 

e Determine if your provider will communicate with you via e-mail and under 
what circumstances. 

e If you find yourself intimidated, take along an advocate—a friend or relative— 
who can help you communicate more effectively. 

e Take notes during the visit. 


INCREASING COMPLIANCE WITH MEDICAL ADVICE 


Although compliance with medical advice does not guarantee that a patient’s medi- 
cal problems will go away, it does optimize the possibility that the patient’s condition 
will improve. What, then, can health-care providers do to produce greater compli- 
ance on the part of their patients? One strategy is to provide clear instructions to 
patients regarding drug regimens. Maintaining good, warm relations with patients 
also leads to increased compliance (Cheney, 1996; Arbuthnott & Sharpe, 2009). 

In addition, honesty helps. Patients generally prefer to be well informed—even 
if the news is bad; their degree of satisfaction with their medical care is linked to 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 45 Promoting Health and Wellness 





how well and how accurately physicians are able to convey the nature of their med- 
ical problems and treatments (Haley, Clair, & Saulsberry, 1992; Zuger, 2005). 

The way in which a message is framed also can result in more positive responses 
to health-related information. Positively framed messages suggest that a change in 
behavior will lead to a gain and thus emphasize the benefits of carrying out a health- 
related behavior. For instance, suggesting that skin cancer is curable if it is detected 
early and that you can reduce your chances of getting the disease by using a sun- 
screen places information in a positive frame. In contrast, negatively framed messages 
highlight what you can lose by not performing a behavior. For instance, a physician 
might say that if you don’t use sunscreen, you’re more likely to get skin cancer, which 
can kill you if it’s not detected early. 

What type of message is more effective? It depends on the type of health behavior 
the health-care provider is trying to bring about. Positively framed messages are best 
for motivating preventive behavior. However, negatively framed messages are most 
effective in producing behavior that will lead to the detection of a disease (McCaul, 
Johnson, & Rothman, 2002; Apanovich, McCarthy, & Salovey, 2003; Lee & Aaker, 2004). 





Well-Being and Happiness 


What makes for a good life? 

This is a question that philosophers and theologians have pondered for centuries. 
Now health psychologists are turning their spotlight on the question by investigating 
subjective well-being, people’s evaluations of their lives in terms of both their 
thoughts and their emotions. Considered another way, subjective well-being is the 
measure of how happy people are (Diener, Lucas, & Oishi, 2002; Dolan & White, 
2007; Tsaousis, Nikolaou, & Serdaris, 2007; Kesebir & Diener, 2008). 


WHAT ARE THE CHARACTERISTICS OF HAPPY PEOPLE? 


Research on the subject of well-being shows that happy people share several character- 
istics (Myers, 2000; Diener & Seligman, 2002; Otake, Shimai, & Tanaka-Matsumi, 2006): 


e Happy people have high self-esteem. Particularly in Western cultures, which 
emphasize the importance of individuality, people who are happy like 


Positively framed messages suggest that 
a change in behavior will lead to a 
health-related gain. 


subjective well-being People’s own 
evaluation of their lives in terms 

of both their thoughts and their 
emotions. 


495 


mia) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


the 21st Century 





Catching Happiness: 
The Contagion of Emotions 
and Health 


The next time you get the flu, there will 
almost certainly be someone you can 
blame for your pain. There’s the inconsid- 
erate co-worker who decided to drag 
himself to the office and spent the day 
sniffling, sneezing and shivering in the 
cubicle next to yours. Or your child’s best 
friend, the one who showed up for a 
playdate with a runny nose and a short 
supply of tissues. Then there’s the guy at 
the gym who spent more time sneezing 
than sweating on the treadmill before you 
used it. (Park, 2008, p. 74) 


It’s often reasonable to blame someone else 
if you get sick. Contagious diseases, such 
as colds or flu, tend to work their way 
through social networks moving from one 
person to another. 

Similarly, if you’ve ever used Facebook, 
you already know how social networks op- 
erate: You have a circle of friends, and each 
of them has a circle of friends, and the 
many connections among all these people 
form a network. Moreover, if you’ve ever 
shared a web link with your Facebook 
friends, you've seen just how quickly infor- 
mation can spread through this network. 

But what else might spread from person 
to person? Can we “infect” others with our 
emotions or our behaviors? 

New research suggests that in fact emo- 
tions are contagious and that the effect can 
even spread beyond our immediate social 
circles to people we don’t even know. Re- 
searchers Nicholas Christakis and James 
Fowler (2009) studied emotions in a very 
large group of interrelated people. They 
found that friends of happy people were 


496 





Happiness can spread beyond our immediate social circles to people we don’t even know. 


15% more likely to be happy themselves, 
and that the friends of friends of happy 
people were 10% more likely to be happy— 
even if they didn’t even know the happy 
person directly. The influence of the happy 
person could even be found in unknown 
people who were three degrees of separa- 
tion away—their friend’s friend’s friend. 
Christakis and Fowler also found evi- 
dence that behaviors can spread across so- 
cial networks in a similar way. People with 
obese friends are more likely to be over- 
weight themselves, and people who are 
trying to quit smoking are more successful 


if their friends also quit. They even noticed 
that people who are smiling in their Face- 
book photos tend to have friends who do 
the same. The social contagiousness seems 
to be stronger in mutual friendships than it 
is in one-sided ones (Christakis & Fowler, 
2007, 2008). 

What this means is that people who ac- 
quire healthy behaviors or outlooks for 
themselves might “spread the wealth” to 
their friends and beyond. Efforts to map 
and use social networks in order to help 
people live healthier lives are already un- 
derway (Park, 2008). 





e Why do you think happiness and other emotions might be contagious? 
e What could explain the finding that obese people are more likely to have friends 


who are obese? 








themselves. They see themselves as more intelligent and better able to get 
along with others than the average person. In fact, they often hold positive 
illusions or moderately inflated views of themselves as good, competent, and 
desirable (Taylor et al., 2000; Boyd-Wilson, McClure, & Walkey, 2004). 

e Happy people have a firm sense of control. They feel more in control of events in 
their lives, unlike those who feel they are the pawns of others and who 


experience learned helplessness. 


e Happy individuals are optimistic. Their optimism permits them to persevere at tasks 
and ultimately to achieve more. In addition, their health is better (Peterson, 2000). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 45 Promoting Health and Wellness 497 


e Men and women generally are made happy by the same sorts of activities—but not 
always. Most of the time, adult men and women achieve the same level of 
happiness from the same things, such as hanging out with friends. But there 
are some differences: For example, women get less pleasure from being with 
their parents than men. The explanation? For women, time spent with their 
parents more closely resembles work, such as helping them cook or pay the 
bills. For men, it’s more likely to involve recreational activities, such as 
watching a football game with their fathers. The result is that men report 
being slightly happier than women (Kreuger, 2007). 

e Happy people like to be around other people. They tend to be extroverted and have 
a supportive network of close relationships. (Also see Applying Psychology in 
the 21st Century.) 


Perhaps most important, most people are at least moderately happy most of 
the time. In both national and international surveys, people living in a wide variety 
of circumstances report being happy. Furthermore, life-altering events that one 
might expect would produce long-term spikes in happiness, such as winning the 
lottery, probably won’t make you much happier than you already are, as we discuss 
next. 


DOES MONEY BUY HAPPINESS? 


If you were to win the lottery, would you be happier? 

Probably not. At least that’s the implication of health psychologists’ research 
on subjective well-being. That research shows that although winning the lottery 
brings an initial surge in happiness, winners’ level of happiness a year later seems 
to return to what it was before they won. The converse phenomenon occurs for 
people who have had serious injuries in accidents: Despite an initial decline in 
happiness, in most cases victims return to their prior levels of happiness after the 
passage of time (Diener & Biswas-Diener, 2002; Nissle & Bschor, 2002; Spinella & 
Lester, 2006). 


Why is the level of subjective well-being so stable? One explanation is that peo- y Alert 
ple have a general set point for happiness, a marker that establishes the tone for one’s Remember the concept that 
life. Although specific events may temporarily elevate or depress one’s mood (a sur- individuals have a set point 
prise promotion or a job loss, for example), ultimately people return to their general (a general, consistent level) 
level of happiness. relating to subjective well- 
Although it is not certain how people’s happiness set points are initially estab- being. 


lished, some evidence suggests that the set point is determined at least in part by 
genetic factors. Specifically, identical twins who grow up in widely different circum- 
stances turn out to have very similar levels of happiness (Kahneman, Diener, & 
Schwarz, 1998; Diener, Lucas, & Scollon, 2006; Weiss, Bates, & Luciano, 2008). 

Most people’s well-being set point is relatively high. For example, some 30% of 
people in the United States rate themselves as “very happy,” and only one in ten 
rates himself or herself as “not too happy.” Most people declare themselves to be 
“pretty happy.” Such feelings are graphically confirmed by people who are asked to 
place themselves on the measure of happiness illustrated in Figure 2. The scale 
clearly illustrates that most people view their lives quite positively. 

Similar results are found when people are asked to compare themselves with 
others. For example, when asked, “Who of the following people do you think is the 
happiest?” survey respondents answered “Oprah Winfrey” (23%), “Bill Gates” (7%), 
“the Pope” (12%), and “yourself” (49%), with 6% saying they didn’t know (Black & 
McCafferty, 1998). 

Few differences exist between members of different demographic groups. Men 
and women report being equally happy, and African Americans are only slightly less 
likely than European Americans to rate themselves as “very happy.” Furthermore, 
happiness is hardly unique to U.S. culture. Even countries that are not economically 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


498 Chapter 14 Health Psychology: Stress, Coping, and Well-Being 


FIGURE 2 Most people in the United Faces Scale:““Which face comes closest to expressing how you feel about your life as a whole?” 
States rate themselves as happy, while 


only a small minority indicate they are 
“not too happy.’ (Source: Myers, 2000, p. 57, 
drawn from Social Indicators of Well-Being: 


Americans’ Perceptions of Life Quality (pp. 207 20% 46% 27% 4% 2% 1% 0% 
and 306), by F. M. Andrews and S. B. Withey, 

1976. New York, Plenum. Copyright 1976 by 

Plenum.) 





prosperous have, on the whole, happy residents (Diener & Clifton, 2002; Suh, 2002; 
Suhail & Chaudhry, 2004). 

The bottom line: Money does not seem to buy happiness. Despite the ups and 
downs of life, most people tend to be reasonably happy, and they adapt to the trials 
and tribulations—and joys and delights—of life by returning to a steady-state level 
of happiness. That habitual level of happiness can have profound—perhaps life- 
prolonging—implications (Diener & Seligman, 2004; Hecht, 2007). 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 2. If you want people to floss more to prevent gum disease, 
the best approach is to 
a. Use a negatively framed message 
b. Use a positively framed message 
c. Have a dentist deliver an encouraging message on the 
pleasures of flossing 
d. Provide people with free dental floss 
3. Winning the lottery is likely to 
a. Produce an immediate and long-term increase in the 
level of well-being 
b. Produce an immediate but not lingering increase in the 
level of well-being 
How does a sense of well-being develop? . Produce a decline in well-being over the long run 
e Subjective well-being, the measure of how happy people d. Lead to an increase in greed over the long run 
are, is highest in people with high self-esteem, a sense of 
control, optimism, and a supportive network of close re- 
lationships. (p. 495) 


How do our interactions with physicians affect our health and 
compliance with medical treatment? 

e Although patients would often like physicians to base a 
diagnosis only on a physical examination, communicat- 
ing one’s problem to the physician is equally important. 
(p. 493) 

e Patients may find it difficult to communicate openly 
with their physicians because of physicians’ high social 
prestige and the technical nature of medical information. 
(p. 494) 


A 


RETHINK 


1. Do you think stress plays a role in making communication 
between physicians and patients difficult? Why? 
EVALUATE 2. From the perspective of a health-care provider: How would 
you try to better communicate with your patients? How 
might your techniques vary depending on the patient’s 
background, gender, age, and culture? 


1. Health psychologists are most likely to focus on which of 
the following problems with health care? 
a. Incompetent health-care providers 
b. Rising health-care costs Answers to Evaluate Questions 
c. Ineffective communication between physician and Oe 4% OI 
patient 
d. Scarcity of medical research funding 


KEY TERMS 


reactance p. 492 subjective well-being p. 495 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Psychology on the Web 


1. Find three or more websites that deal with stress reduction. Gather at least five 
techniques for reducing stress and summarize them. Write a critique and evaluation 
of those techniques using the information you have learned about stress. Which ones 
seem to have a sound basis in psychological theory and/or research? 

2. Are you closer to a Type A or a Type B personality? Find two websites offering tests 
that claim to provide the answer. Summarize the quality of the tests from a scientific 
point of view and compare the results you received from each one. 


E © | | OQ U e In this set of modules, we have explored the intersection 


of psychology and biology. We have seen how the 
emotional and psychological experience of stress can lead to physical symptoms of 
illness, how personality factors may be related to major health problems, and how 
psychological factors can interfere with effective communication between physician and 
patient. We have also looked at the other side of the coin noting that some relatively 
simple strategies can help us control stress, affect illness, and improve our interactions 
with physicians. 

Turn back to the prologue of this set of modules about Louisa Denby and her hectic 
schedule. Use your understanding of health psychology and stress to consider these 
questions. 





1. Based on the description of Denby’s day, which are personal stressors and which are 
background stressors? What might happen to “elevate” the stress level of a back- 
ground stressor to a more serious level? 

2. Are there likely to be any uplifts in her day? 

3. How does the GAS apply to Denby’s situation? How might events in her life move her 
along the three stages of the model? 

4. What steps would you advise Denby to take to keep her level of stress under control? 
How might others in her life be involved in such an effort? 





499 

















www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Key Concepts for Chapter 15 


MODULE 46 










How can we distinguish normal from Normal Versus Abnormal: 


Making the Distinction 


abnormal behavior? @ What are the major Seni l 
efining Abnormality 


Perspectives on Abnormality: 
From Superstition to Science 


perspectives on psychological disorders used 


by mental health professionals? @ What are Classifying Abnormal Behavior: 


x ; ; A The ABCs of DSM 
the major categories of psychological disorders? 


MODULE 47 


The Major Psychological Disorders 
Anxiety Disorders 


Neuroscience in Your Life: One Step 
Closer to Understanding OCD 


Somatoform Disorders 
Dissociative Disorders 
Mood Disorders 
Schizophrenia 


Neuroscience in Your Life: Brain 
Changes with Schizophrenia 


Personality Disorders 
Childhood Disorders 
Other Disorders 


MODULE 48 





How prevalent are psychological disorders? Psychological Disorders 


in Perspective 


@ What indicators signal a need for the help the coed nd cutii ere: 


of a mental health practitioner? Psychological. Disorders 


Applying Psychology in the 21st Century: 
Students Stressed and Depressed 
Exploring Diversity: DSM and Culture— 
and the Culture of DSM 

Becoming an Informed Consumer 

of Psychology: Deciding When 

You Need Help 


501 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Prologue Lily 





To have coffee with Lily (a pseudonym), you wouldn't get much 
sense of how she has suffered. She is 50 but could pass for 30.... 
She was friendly but not terribly expressive, and she carried an 
aura of self-protection... 

As a teenager, Lily felt little self-confidence. “Junior high and 
high school just sucks, right?” she’said, laughing. “But | had a 
propensity to take it a little more seriously.” With the help of 
therapy, she made it through high school and college, but in her 
late 20s, she became dissatisfied with her job selling specialty 
equipment. One October day, as she headed out for a 


\ 


\ 


Lily suffered from borderline personality disorder, a psychological 
disorder that afflicts an estimated 6% of the population. As we'll 
discuss later in the chapter, it is characterized by difficulties in 
forming a clear sense of self-identity, relationship problems, 
mood swings, and high rates of self-injury. 

Lily’s difficulties raise many questions. What triggered her 
disorder? Were genetic factors involved, or were stressors in her 
life responsible? Could the disorder have been prevented? And, 
more generally, how do we distinguish normal from abnormal 
behavior, and how can Lily’s behavior be categorized and 


502 


mountain-biking trip, she looked at the sky and had the feeling 
that something was wrong. Bleakness massed around her 
quickly, much faster than it had when she was younger. Soon, 
nothing gave Lily much joy. 

She recalled a talk show in which girls had discussed cutting 
themselves as a release, a way to relieve depression. “I was so 
numb,’ she said. “I just wanted to feel something—anything.” So 
she took a knife from the kitchen and cut deeply into her left arm. 
(Cloud, 2009, p. 45) 





classified in such a way as to pinpoint the specific nature of her 
problem? 

We address the issues raised by Lily’s case in this chapter. We 
begin by discussing the difference between normal and abnor- 
mal behavior, which can be surprisingly indistinct. We then turn 
to a consideration of the most significant kinds of psychological 
disorders. Finally, we'll consider ways of evaluating behavior— 
one’s own and that of others—to determine whether seeking 
help from a mental health professional is warranted. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 46 


Universally that person’s acumen is esteemed very little perceptive concerning what- 
soever matters are being held as most profitable by mortals with sapience endowed to 
be studied who is ignorant of that which the most in doctrine erudite and certainly 
by reason of that in them high mind’s ornament deserving of veneration constantly 
maintain when by general consent they affirm that other circumstances being equal 
by no exterior splendour is the prosperity of a nation. . . 


It would be easy to conclude that these words are the musings of a madman. To 
most people, the passage does not seem to make any sense at all. But literary schol- 
ars would disagree. Actually, this passage is from James Joyce’s classic Ulysses, hailed 
as one of the major works of 20th-century literature (Joyce, 1934, p. 377). 

As this example illustrates, casually examining a person’s writing is insufficient 
to determine the degree to which that person is “normal.” But even when we con- 
sider more extensive samples of a person’s behavior, we will find that there may be 
only a fine line between behavior that is considered normal and behavior that is 
considered abnormal. 


Defining Abnormality 


Because of the difficulty in distinguishing normal from abnormal behavior, psychol- 
ogists have struggled to devise a precise, scientific definition of “abnormal behavior.” 
For instance, consider the following definitions, each of which has advantages and 
disadvantages: 





e Abnormality as deviation from the average. To employ this statistically based 
approach, we simply observe what behaviors are rare or occur infrequently 
in a specific society or culture and label those deviations from the norm 
“abnormal.” 

The difficulty with this definition is that some statistically rare behav- 
iors clearly do not lend themselves to classification as abnormal. If most 
people prefer to have cornflakes for breakfast but you prefer raisin bran, 
this deviation hardly makes your behavior abnormal. Similarly, such a 
concept of abnormality would unreasonably label a person who has an 
unusually high IQ as abnormal simply because a high IQ is statistically 
rare. In short, a definition of abnormality that rests on deviation from the 
average is insufficient. 


e Abnormality as deviation from the ideal. An alternative approach considers 
abnormality in relation to the standard toward which most people are striv- 
ing—the ideal. This sort of definition considers behavior abnormal if it devi- 
ates enough from some kind of ideal or cultural standard. However, society 
has few standards on which people universally agree. (For example, we would 
be hard pressed to find agreement on whether the New Testament, the Koran, 
the Talmud, or the Book of Mormon provides the most reasonable standards.) 


How can we distinguish 
normal from abnormal 
behavior? 


What are the major perspec- 
tives on psychological dis- 
orders used by mental 
health professionals? 


What are the major categories 
of psychological disorders? 


503 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


504 Chapter 15 Psychological Disorders 





Andrea Yates 


y Alert 
Remember the different defi- 
nitions of abnormality (devi- 
ation from the average, 
deviation from the ideal, a 
sense of personal discom- 
fort, inability to function effectively, 
and abnormality as a legal concept). 


abnormal behavior Behavior that 
causes people to experience distress 
and prevents them from functioning 
in their daily lives. 


Furthermore, standards that do arise change over time and vary across 
cultures. Thus, the deviation-from-the-ideal approach is also inadequate. 

e Abnormality as a sense of personal discomfort. A more useful definition concen- 
trates on the psychological consequences of the behavior for the individual. 
In this approach, behavior is considered abnormal if it produces a sense of 
personal distress, anxiety, or guilt in an individual—or if it is harmful to 
others in some way. 

Even a definition that relies on personal discomfort has drawbacks, 
though, because in some especially severe forms of mental disturbance, 
people report feeling wonderful even though their behavior seems bizarre to 
others. In such cases, a personal state of well-being exists, yet most people 
would consider the behavior abnormal. For example, most of us would think 
that a woman who says she is hearing uplifting messages from Martians 
would be displaying abnormal behavior even though she may say that the 
messages make her feel happy. 


e Abnormality as the inability to function effectively. Most people are able to feed 
themselves, hold a job, get along with others, and in general live as produc- 
tive members of society. Yet there are those who are unable to adjust to the 
demands of society or function effectively. 

According to this view of abnormality, people who are unable to function 
effectively and to adapt to the demands of society are considered abnormal. 
For example, an unemployed, homeless woman living on the street may be 
considered unable to function effectively. Therefore, her behavior can be 
viewed as abnormal even if she has chosen to live this way. Her inability to 
adapt to the requirements of society is what makes her “abnormal,” according 
to this approach. 


e Abnormality as a legal concept. According to the jury that first heard her case, 
Andrea Yates, a woman who drowned her five children in a bathtub, was 
sane. She was sentenced to life in prison for her act. 

Although you might question this view (and a later appeals jury over- 
turned the conviction), the initial verdict reflected the way in which the 
law defines abnormal behavior. To the judicial system, the distinction be- 
tween normal and abnormal behavior rests on the definition of insanity, 
which is a legal but not a psychological term. The definition of insanity 
varies from one jurisdiction to another. In some states, insanity simply 
means that defendants cannot understand the difference between right and 
wrong at the time they commit a criminal act. Other states consider whether 
defendants are substantially incapable of understanding the criminality of 
their behavior or unable to control themselves. And in some jurisdictions, 
pleas of insanity are not allowed at all. (Weiner & Wettstein, 1993; Frost & 
Bonnie, 2001; Sokolove, 2003) 


Clearly, none of the previous definitions is broad enough to cover all instances 
of abnormal behavior. Consequently, the distinction between normal and abnormal 
behavior often remains ambiguous even to trained professionals. Furthermore, to a 
large extent, cultural expectations for “normal” behavior in a particular society influ- 
ence the understanding of “abnormal behavior” (Scheff, 1998; Sanderson, 2007). 
Given the difficulties in precisely defining the construct, psychologists typically 
define abnormal behavior broadly as behavior that causes people to experience dis- 
tress and prevents them from functioning in their daily lives (Nolen-Hoeksema, 
2007). Because of the imprecision of this definition, it’s best to view abnormal behav- 
ior and normal behavior as marking two ends of a continuum rather than as absolute 
states. Behavior should be evaluated in terms of gradations that range from fully 
normal functioning to extremely abnormal behavior. Behavior typically falls some- 
where between those extremes. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 46 Normal Versus Abnormal: Making the Distinction 505 


Perspectives on Abnormality: 
From Superstition to Science 


Throughout much of human history, people linked abnormal behavior to supersti- 
tion and witchcraft. Individuals who displayed abnormal behavior were accused 
of being possessed by the devil or some sort of demonic god. Authorities felt 
justified in “treating” abnormal behavior by attempting to drive out the source of 
the problem. This typically involved whipping, immersion in hot water, starvation, 
or other forms of torture in which the cure was often worse than the affliction 
(Berrios, 1996). 

Contemporary approaches take a more enlightened view. Today, six major per- 
spectives are used to understand psychological disorders. These perspectives suggest 
not only different causes of abnormal behavior but different treatment approaches as 
well. Furthermore, some perspectives are more applicable to specific disorders than 
are others. Figure 1 summarizes the perspectives and the way in which they can be 
applied to the experience of Lily, the person discussed in the chapter prologue. 





MEDICAL PERSPECTIVE 


When people display the symptoms of tuberculosis, medical professionals can generally 
find tubercular bacteria in their body tissue. Similarly, the medical perspective suggests 
that when an individual displays symptoms of abnormal behavior, the fundamental 
cause will be found through a physical examination of the individual, which may reveal 
a hormonal imbalance, a chemical deficiency, or a brain injury. Indeed, when we speak 
of mental “illness,” “symptoms” of abnormal behavior, and mental “hospitals,” we are 
using terminology associated with the medical perspective. 

Because many abnormal behaviors have been linked to biological causes, the 
medical perspective is a reasonable approach, yet serious criticisms have been leveled 
against it. For one thing, no biological cause has been identified for many forms of 
abnormal behavior. In addition, some critics have argued that the use of the term 
illness implies that people who display abnormal behavior have no responsibility for 
their actions (Szasz, 1994, 2006, Laing & Szasz, 2004). 


Perspectives on Psychological Disorders 


dy Alert 


Use Figure 1 to review the six 
major perspectives on 
abnormality and consider 
how they relate to the 
major perspectives on 
the field of psychology that we 
discussed in Chapter 1. 


medical perspective The perspective 
that suggests that when an individual 
displays symptoms of abnormal 
behavior, the root cause will be found 
in a physical examination of the 
individual, which may reveal a 
hormonal imbalance, a chemical 
deficiency, or a brain injury. 





Perspective Description Possible Application of Perspective to Lily’s Case 

Medical Assumes that physiological causes are at the root Examine Lily for medical problems, such as brain tumor, 
of psychological disorders chemical imbalance in the brain, or disease 

Psychoanalytic Argues that psychological disorders stem from Seek out information about Lily’s past, considering possible 
childhood conflicts childhood conflicts 

Behavioral Assumes that abnormal behaviors are learned Concentrate on rewards and punishments for Lily's behavior, 
responses and identify environmental stimuli that reinforce her behavior 

Cognitive Assumes that cognitions (people’s thoughts and Focus on Lily’s perceptions of self and her environment 
beliefs) are central to psychological disorders 

Humanistic Emphasizes people's responsibility for their own Consider Lily's behavior in terms of her choices and efforts 
behavior and the need to self-actualize to reach her potential 

Sociocultural Assumes that behavior is shaped by family, Focus on how societal demands contributed to Lily’s 
society, and culture disorder 


FIGURE 1 In considering Lily’s case discussed in the prologue, we can employ each of the 
different perspectives on abnormal behavior. Note, however, that because of the nature of 
her psychological disorder, some of the perspectives are more applicable than others. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


506 Chapter 15 Psychological Disorders 


psychoanalytic perspective 

The perspective that suggests that 
abnormal behavior stems from child- 
hood conflicts over opposing wishes 
regarding sex and aggression. 


behavioral perspective The perspec- 
tive that looks at the behavior itself as 
the problem. 


cognitive perspective The perspective 
that suggests that people’s thoughts 
and beliefs are a central component of 
abnormal behavior. 


Still, recent advances in our understanding of the biological bases of behavior 
underscore the importance of considering physiological factors in abnormal behavior. 
For instance, some of the more severe forms of psychological disturbance, such as 
major depression and schizophrenia, are influenced by genetic factors and malfunc- 
tions in neurotransmitter signals (Plomin & McGuffin, 2003; Iversen & Iversen, 2007; 
Howes & Kapur, 2009). 


PSYCHOANALYTIC PERSPECTIVE 


Whereas the medical perspective suggests that biological causes are at the root of 
abnormal behavior, the psychoanalytic perspective holds that abnormal behavior 
stems from childhood conflicts over opposing wishes regarding sex and aggression. 
According to Freud, children pass through a series of stages in which sexual and 
aggressive impulses take different forms and produce conflicts that require resolu- 
tion. If these childhood conflicts are not dealt with successfully, they remain unre- 
solved in the unconscious and eventually bring about abnormal behavior during 
adulthood. 

To uncover the roots of people’s disordered behavior, the psychoanalytic per- 
spective scrutinizes their early life history. However, because there is no conclusive 
way to link people’s childhood experiences with the abnormal behaviors they display 
as adults, we can never be sure that the causes suggested by psychoanalytic theory 
are accurate. Moreover, psychoanalytic theory paints a picture of people as having 
relatively little control over their behavior because much of it is guided by uncon- 
scious impulses. In the eyes of some critics, this perspective suggests that people 
have little responsibility for their own behavior. 

On the other hand, the contributions of psychoanalytic theory have been sig- 
nificant. More than any other approach to abnormal behavior, this perspective high- 
lights the fact that people can have a rich, involved inner life and that prior 
experiences can have a profound effect on current psychological functioning (Born- 
stein, 2003; Elliott, 2002; Rangell, 2007). 


BEHAVIORAL PERSPECTIVE 


Both the medical and psychoanalytic perspectives look at abnormal behaviors as 
symptoms of an underlying problem. In contrast, the behavioral perspective views 
the behavior itself as the problem. Using the basic principles of learning, behavioral 
theorists see both normal and abnormal behaviors as responses to various stimuli— 
responses that have been learned through past experience and are guided in the 
present by stimuli in the individual’s environment. To explain why abnormal behavior 
occurs, we must analyze how an individual has learned it and observe the circum- 
stances in which it is displayed. 

The emphasis on observable behavior represents both the greatest strength and 
the greatest weakness of the behavioral approach to abnormal behavior. This perspec- 
tive provides the most precise and objective approach for examining behavioral 
symptoms of specific disorders, such as attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder 
(ADHD), which we will discuss in a later module. At the same time, though, critics 
charge that the perspective ignores the rich inner world of thoughts, attitudes, and 
emotions that may contribute to abnormal behavior. 


COGNITIVE PERSPECTIVE 


The medical, psychoanalytic, and behavioral perspectives view people’s behavior as 
the result of factors largely beyond their control. To many critics of these views, 
however, people’s thoughts cannot be ignored. 

In response to such concerns, some psychologists employ a cognitive perspective. 
Rather than considering only external behavior, as in traditional behavioral approaches, 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 46 Normal Versus Abnormal: Making the Distinction 507 


the cognitive approach assumes that cognitions (people’s thoughts and beliefs) are 
central to a person’s abnormal behavior. A primary goal of treatment using the cogni- 
tive perspective is to explicitly teach new, more adaptive ways of thinking. 

For instance, suppose that you develop the erroneous belief that “doing well on 
this exam is crucial to my entire future” whenever you take an exam. Through ther- 
apy, you might learn to hold the more realistic and less anxiety-producing thought, 
“my entire future is not dependent on this one exam.” By changing cognitions in this 
way, psychologists working within a cognitive framework help people free them- 
selves from thoughts and behaviors that are potentially maladaptive (Clark, 2004; 
Everly & Lating, 2007). 

The cognitive perspective is not without critics. For example, it is possible that 
maladaptive cognitions are the symptoms or consequences of disorders rather than 
their cause. Furthermore, there are circumstances in which negative beliefs may not 
be irrational at all but simply reflect the unpleasant environments in which people 
live—after all, there are times when a single exam may be extremely important. Still, 
cognitive theorists would argue that one can find a more adaptive way of framing 
beliefs even in the most negative circumstances. 


HUMANISTIC PERSPECTIVE 


Psychologists who subscribe to the humanistic perspective emphasize the responsi- 
bility people have for their own behavior even when their behavior is considered 
abnormal. The humanistic perspective—growing out of the work of Carl Rogers and 
Abraham Maslow—concentrates on what is uniquely human—that is, it views people 
as basically rational, oriented toward a social world, and motivated to seek self- 
actualization (Rogers, 1995). 

Humanistic approaches focus on the relationship of the individual to society; it 
considers the ways in which people view themselves in relation to others and see 
their place in the world. The humanistic perspective views people as having an 
awareness of life and of themselves that leads them to search for meaning and self- 
worth. Rather than assuming that individuals require a “cure,” the humanistic per- 
spective suggests that they can, by and large, set their own limits of what is 
acceptable behavior. As long as they are not hurting others and do not feel personal 
distress, people should be free to choose the behaviors in which they engage. 

Although the humanistic perspective has been criticized for its reliance on unsci- 
entific, unverifiable information and its vague, almost philosophical formulations, it 
offers a distinctive view of abnormal behavior. It stresses the unique aspects of being 
human and provides a number of important suggestions for helping those with 
psychological problems. 


SOCIOCULTURAL PERSPECTIVE 


The sociocultural perspective assumes that people’s behavior—both normal and 
abnormal—is shaped by the kind of family group, society, and culture in which they 
live. According to this view, the nature of one’s relationships with others may sup- 
port abnormal behaviors and even cause them. Consequently the kinds of stresses 
and conflicts people experience in their daily interactions with others can promote 
and maintain abnormal behavior. 

This perspective finds statistical support for the position that sociocultural factors 
shape abnormal behavior given the fact that some kinds of abnormal behavior are far 
more prevalent among certain social classes than they are in others. For instance, 
diagnoses of schizophrenia tend to be higher among members of lower socioeconomic 
groups than among members of more affluent groups. Proportionally more African- 
American individuals are hospitalized involuntarily for psychological disorders than 
are whites. Furthermore, poor economic times seem to be linked to general declines 
in psychological functioning, and social problems such as homelessness are associated 


humanistic perspective The perspec- 
tive that emphasizes the responsibility 
people have for their own behavior, 

even when such behavior is abnormal. 


sociocultural perspective The per- 
spective that assumes that people’s 
behavior—both normal and abnormal— 
is shaped by the kind of family group, 
society, and culture in which they live. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


508 Chapter 15 Psychological Disorders 


© Arnie Levin/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 





with psychological disorders (López & Guarnaccia, 2005; 
Conger et al., 2002; Nasir & Hand, 2006; Greenberg & 
Rosenheck, 2008). 

On the other hand, alternative explanations abound for 
the association between abnormal behavior and social fac- 
tors. For example, people from lower socioeconomic levels 
may be less likely than those from higher levels to seek 
help, gradually reaching a point where their symptoms 
become severe and warrant a serious diagnosis. Further- 
more, sociocultural explanations provide relatively little 
specific guidance for the treatment of individuals showing 
mental disturbance because the focus is on broader societal 
factors (Paniagua, 2000). 


Classifying Abnormal 
Behavior: The ABCs 
of DSM 





“First off, you're not a nut. You're a legume.” 


Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of 
Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, 
Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) Asystem, 
devised by the American Psychiatric 
Association, used by most profession- 
als to diagnose and classify abnormal 
behavior. 


Crazy. Whacked. Mental. Loony. Insane. Neurotic. Psycho. 
Strange. Demented. Odd. Possessed. 

Society has long placed labels on people who display abnormal behavior. Unfor- 
tunately, most of the time these labels have reflected intolerance and have been used 
with little thought as to what each label signifies. 

Providing appropriate and specific names and classifications for abnormal behav- 
ior has presented a major challenge to psychologists. It is not hard to understand 
why, given the difficulties discussed earlier in simply distinguishing normal from 
abnormal behavior. Yet psychologists and other careproviders need to classify abnor- 
mal behavior in order to diagnose it and ultimately treat it. 


DSM-IV-TR: DETERMINING DIAGNOSTIC DISTINCTIONS 


Over the years, mental health professionals have developed many different classifica- 
tion systems that vary in terms of their utility and the degree to which they have 
been accepted. However, one standard system, devised by the American Psychiatric 
Association, has emerged in the United States. Most professionals today use this 
classification system, known as the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental 
Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) to diagnose and classify 
abnormal behavior. 

DSM-IV-TR presents comprehensive and relatively precise definitions for more 
than 200 disorders divided into 17 major categories. It also includes five types of 
information, known as axes, which have to be considered in assessing a patient: 


e Axis I: Clinical Disorders. Disorders that produce distress and impair 
functioning. 

e Axis II: Personality Disorders and Mental Retardation. Enduring, rigid 
behavior patterns. 

e Axis III: General Medical Conditions. Physical disorders that may be related 
to psychological disorders. 

e Axis IV: Psychosocial and Environmental Problems. Problems in a person’s 
life such as stressors or life events that may affect the diagnosis, treatment, 
and outcome of psychological disorders. 

e Axis V: Global Assessment of Functioning. Overall level of mental, social, 
occupational, and leisure functioning. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Categories of Disorders 


Anxiety (problems in which anxiety impedes 
daily functioning) 


Somatoform (psychological difficulties displayed 
through physical problems) 


Dissociative (the splitting apart of crucial parts 
of personality that are usually integrated) 


Module 46 Normal Versus Abnormal: Making the Distinction 


Examples 


Generalized anxiety disorder, panic disorder, 
phobic disorder, obsessive-compulsive 
disorder, posttraumatic stress disorder 


Hypochondriasis, conversion disorder 


Dissociative identity disorder (multiple 
personality), dissociative amnesia, dissociative 


509 


fugue 


TEXT REVISION 


Mood (emotions of depression or euphoria that 
are so strong they intrude on everyday living) 


Major depression, bipolar disorder 


Schizophrenia and psychotic disorders 
(declines in functioning, thought and language 
disturbances, perception disorders, emotional 
disturbances, and withdrawal from others) 


Disorganized, paranoid, catatonic, 
undifferentiated, residual 


Personality (problems that create little personal 
distress but that lead to an inability to function 
as a normal member of society) 


Antisocial (sociopathic) personality disorder, 
narcissistic personality disorder 


Sexual (problems related to sexual arousal from 
unusual objects or problems related to 
functioning) 


Paraphilia, sexual dysfunction 
AMERICAN PSYCHIATRIC ASSOCIATION 





Substance-related (problems related to drug 
dependence and abuse) 


Alcohol, cocaine, hallucinogens, marijuana 


Dementia, amnesia, and other cognitive 
disorders 


FIGURE 2 This list of disorders represents the major categories from the DSM-IV-TR. It is only 
a partial list of the more than 200 disorders included there. 


By following the criteria presented in the DSM-IV-TR classification system, diag- 
nosticians can identify the specific problem an individual is experiencing. (Figure 2 
provides a brief outline of the major diagnostic categories.) 

DSM-IV-TR is primarily descriptive and avoids suggesting an underlying cause 
for an individual’s behavior and problems. For instance, the term neurotic—a label that 
is commonly used by people in their everyday descriptions of abnormal behavior—is 
not listed as a DSM-IV-TR category. Because the term neurosis refers to problems 
associated with a specific cause based in Freud’s theory of personality, it is not 
included in DSM-IV-TR. 

DSM-IV-TR has the advantage, then, of providing a descriptive system that 
does not specify the cause of or reason for a problem. Rather, it paints a picture 
of the behavior that is being displayed. Why should this approach be important? 
For one thing, it allows communication between mental health professionals of 
diverse backgrounds and theoretical approaches. In addition, precise classifica- 
tion enables researchers to explore the causes of a problem. Without reliable 
descriptions of abnormal behavior, researchers would be hard pressed to find 
ways to investigate the disorder. Finally, DSM-IV-TR provides a kind of concep- 
tual shorthand through which professionals can describe the behaviors that tend 
to occur together in an individual (Widiger & Clark, 2000; First, Frances, & 
Pincus, 2002). 


510 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 15 Psychological Disorders 


y Alert 
It is important to understand 
the advantages and weak- 
nesses of the DSM 
classification system. 


CONNING THE CLASSIFIERS: THE SHORTCOMINGS OF DSM-IV-TR 


When clinical psychologist David Rosenhan and eight colleagues sought admis- 
sion to separate mental hospitals across the United States in the 1970s, each stated 
that he or she was hearing voices—“unclear voices” that said “empty,” “hollow,” 
and “thud’”—and each was immediately admitted to the hospital. However, the 
truth was that they actually were conducting a study, and none of them was really 
hearing voices. Aside from these misrepresentations, everything else they did and 
said represented their true behavior, including the responses they gave during 
extensive admission interviews and their answers to the battery of tests they were 
asked to complete. In fact, as soon as they were admitted, they said they no longer 
heard any voices. In short, each of the pseudo-patients acted in a “normal” way 
(Rosenhan, 1973). 

We might assume that Rosenhan and his colleagues would have been quickly 
discovered as the impostors they were, but this was not the case. Instead, each of 
them was diagnosed as severely abnormal on the basis of observed behavior. Men- 
tal health professionals labeled most as suffering from schizophrenia and kept them 
in the hospital 3-52 days, with the average stay of 19 days. Even when they were 
discharged, most of the “patients” left with the label schizophrenia—in remission, 
implying that the abnormal behavior had only temporarily subsided and could 
recur at any time. Most disturbing, no one on the hospital staff identified any of 
the pseudo-patients as impostors—although some of the actual patients figured out 
the ruse. 

The results of Rosenhan’s classic study illustrate that placing labels on individu- 
als powerfully influences the way mental health workers perceive and interpret their 
actions. It also points out that determining who is psychologically disordered is not 
always a clear-cut or accurate process. 

Although DSM-IV-TR was developed to provide more accurate and consistent 
diagnoses of psychological disorders, it has not been entirely successful. For instance, 
critics charge that it relies too much on the medical perspective. Because it was drawn 
up by psychiatrists—who are physicians—some condemn it for viewing psychologi- 
cal disorders primarily in terms of the symptoms of an underlying physiological 
disorder. It also does not fully take into account the advances in behavioral neuro- 
science that have identified the genetic underpinnings of some psychological disorders. 
Moreover, critics suggest that DSM-IV-TR compartmentalizes people into inflexible, 
all-or-none categories rather than considering the degree to which a person displays 
psychologically disordered behavior (Schmidt, Kotov, & Joiner, 2004; Samuel & Widi- 
ger, 2006). 

Other concerns with DSM-IV-TR are more subtle but equally important. For 
instance, some critics argue that labeling an individual as abnormal provides a de- 
humanizing, lifelong stigma. (Think, for example, of political contenders whose candi- 
dacies have been terminated by the disclosure that they received treatment for severe 
psychological disorders.) Furthermore, after an initial diagnosis has been made, men- 
tal health professionals, who may concentrate on the initial diagnostic category, could 
overlook other diagnostic possibilities (Szasz, 1994; Duffy et al., 2002; Quinn, Kahng, 
& Crocker, 2004). 

Still, despite the drawbacks inherent in any labeling system, DSM-IV-TR has had 
an important influence on the way in which mental health professionals view psy- 
chological disorders. It has increased both the reliability and the validity of diagnos- 
tic categorization. In addition, it offers a logical way to organize examination of the 
major types of mental disturbance. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 46 Normal Versus Abnormal: Making the Distinction 511 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 


How can we distinguish normal from abnormal behavior? 


Definitions of abnormality include deviation from the 
average, deviation from the ideal, a sense of personal 
discomfort, the inability to function effectively, and legal 
conceptions. (p. 503) 

Although no single definition is adequate, abnormal 
behavior can be considered to be behavior that causes 
people to experience distress and prevents them from 
functioning in their daily lives. Most psychologists 
believe that abnormal and normal behavior should be 
considered in terms of a continuum. (p. 504) 


What are the major perspectives on psychological disorders 
used by mental health professionals? 


The medical perspective views abnormality as a 
symptom of an underlying disease. (p. 505) 
Psychoanalytic perspectives suggest that abnormal 
behavior stems from childhood conflicts in the 
unconscious. (p. 506) 

Behavioral approaches view abnormal behavior not as a 
symptom of an underlying problem but as the problem 
itself. (p. 506) 

The cognitive approach suggests that abnormal behavior 
is the result of faulty cognitions (thoughts and beliefs). In 
this view, abnormal behavior can be remedied by 
changing one’s flawed thoughts and beliefs. (p. 506) 
Humanistic approaches emphasize the responsibility 
people have for their own behavior even when such 
behavior is seen as abnormal. (p. 507) 

Sociocultural approaches view abnormal behavior in 
terms of difficulties arising from family and other social 
relationships. (p. 507) 


What are the major categories of psychological disorders? 


The most widely used system for classifying psychological 
disorders is DSM-IV-TR—Diagnostic and Statistical 
Manual of Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision. 
(p. 508) 


EVALUATE 


1. One problem in defining abnormal behavior is that 


a. 
b. 


Statistically rare behavior may not be abnormal. 
Not all abnormalities are accompanied by feelings of 
discomfort. 


. Cultural standards are too general to use as a measur- 


ing tool. 
. All of the above. 


2. If abnormality is defined as behavior that causes personal 


discomfort or harms others, which of the following people 

is most likely to need treatment? 

a. An executive is afraid to accept a promotion because it 
would require moving from his ground-floor office to 
the top floor of a tall office building. 

b. A woman decides to quit her job and chooses to live on 
the street in order to live a “simpler life.” 

c. Aman believes that friendly spacemen visit his house 
every Thursday. 

d. A photographer lives with 19 cats in a small apartment 
lovingly caring for them. 


. Virginia’s mother thinks that her daughter’s behavior is 


clearly abnormal because, despite being offered admission 
to medical school, Virginia decides to become a waitress. 
What approach is Virginia’s mother using to define abnor- 
mal behavior? 


. Which of the following is a strong argument against the 


medical perspective on abnormality? 

a. Physiological abnormalities are almost always impossi- 
ble to identify. 

b. There is no conclusive way to link past experience and 
behavior. 

c. The medical perspective rests too heavily on the effects 
of nutrition. 

d. Assigning behavior to a physical problem takes respon- 
sibility away from the individual for changing his or 
her behavior. 


. Cheryl is painfully shy. According to the behavioral 


perspective, the best way to deal with her “abnormal” 

behavior is to 

a. Treat the underlying physical problem. 

b. Use the principles of learning theory to modify her shy 
behavior. 

c. Express a great deal of caring. 

d. Uncover her negative past experiences through 
hypnosis. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


512 Chapter 15 Psychological Disorders 


RETHINK the medical perspective, the psychoanalytic perspective, 
the behavioral perspective, the cognitive perspective, the 
humanistic perspective, and the sociocultural perspective? 
Based on the potential causes of the shoplifting, would 
you fire the employee? Why or why not? 


1. Do you agree or disagree that DSM should be updated 
every several years? Why? What makes abnormal 
behavior so variable? 

2. From the perspective of an employer: Imagine that a well-paid 
employee was arrested for shoplifting a sweater that costs Answers to Evaluate Questions 
$15. What sort of explanation for this behavior would the q's ‘pp Tepr ay} Woy uonerap ‘g e'z ‘p'r 
proponents of each perspective on abnormality provide: 


KEY TERMS 


abnormal behavior p. 504 cognitive perspective p. 506 Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of 
medical perspective p. 505 humanistic perspective p. 507 Mental Disorders, Fourth Edition, 
psychoanalytic perspective p. 506 sociocultural perspective p. 507 Text Revision (DSM-IV-TR) p. 509 


behavioral perspective p. 506 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 47 


Sally experienced her first panic attack out of the blue, 3 weeks after completing her 
senior year in college. She had just finished a job interview and was meeting some 
friends for dinner. In the restaurant, she began to feel dizzy. Within a few seconds, her 
heart was pounding, and she was feeling breathless, as though she might pass out. Her 
friends noticed that she did not look well and offered to drive her home. Sally sug- 
gested they stop at the hospital emergency room instead. Although she felt better by the 
time they arrived at the hospital, and tests indicated nothing wrong, Sally experienced a 
similar episode a week later while at a movie... . 


Her attacks became more and more frequent. Before long, she was having several 
attacks per week. In addition, she constantly worried about having attacks. She began 
to avoid exercise and other activities that produced physical sensations. She also noticed 
the attacks were worse when she was alone. She began to avoid driving, shopping in 
large stores, and eating in all restaurants. Some weeks she avoided leaving the house 
completely. (Antony, Brown, & Barlow, 1992, p. 79) 


Sally suffered from panic disorder, one of the specific psychological disorders we'll 
consider in this module. Keep in mind that although we'll be discussing these dis- 
orders objectively, each represents a very human set of difficulties that influence and 
in some cases considerably disrupt people’s lives. 


Anxiety Disorders 


All of us at one time or another experience anxiety, a feeling of apprehension or ten- 
sion, in reaction to stressful situations. There is nothing “wrong” with such anxiety. 
It is a normal reaction to stress that often helps rather than hinders our daily function- 
ing. Without some anxiety, for instance, most of us probably would not have much 
motivation to study hard, undergo physical exams, or spend long hours at our jobs. 

But some people experience anxiety in situations in which there is no external 
reason or cause for such distress. When anxiety occurs without external justification 
and begins to affect people’s daily functioning, mental health professionals consider 
it a psychological problem known as anxiety disorder. We’ll discuss the four major 
types of anxiety disorders: phobic disorder, panic disorder, generalized anxiety dis- 
order, and obsessive-compulsive disorder. 





PHOBIC DISORDER 


It’s not easy moving through the world when you're terrified of electricity. “Donna,” 45, 
a writer, knows that better than most. Get her in the vicinity of an appliance or a light 
switch or—all but unthinkable—a thunderstorm, and she is overcome by a terror so 
blinding she can think of nothing but fleeing. That, of course, is not always possible, so 
over time, Donna has come up with other answers. When she opens the refrigerator 
door, rubber-sole shoes are a must. If a light bulb blows, she will tolerate the dark until 
someone else changes it for her. Clothes shopping is done only when necessary, lest 
static on garments send her running from the store. And swimming at night is absolutely 
out of the question, lest underwater lights electrocute her. (Kluger, 2001, p. 51) 


What are the major psycho- 
logical disorders? 


The occurrence of 
anxiety without an obvious external 
cause that affects daily functioning. 


513 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


514 Chapter 15 Psychological Disorders 


Phobic Disorder 
Agoraphobia 
Specific phobias 
Animal type 

Natural environment 
type 

Situational type 


Blood injection-injury 
type 


Social phobia 


Intense, irrational fears of 
specific objects or situations. 


Anxiety disorder that 
takes the form of panic attacks lasting 
from a few seconds to several hours. 


Description Example 

Fear of places, such as unfamiliar or Person becomes housebound 

crowded spaces, where help might not be because any place other than coe 

available in case of emergency the person's home arouses y A 
extreme anxiety symptoms. = (TN = 


Fear of specific objects, ee 


places, or situations ls 





Specific animals Person has extreme fear of 








or insects dogs, cats, or spiders. 
Events or situations in the natural Person has extreme fear of storms, heights, 
environment or water. 
Public transportation, tunnels, Person becomes extremely 
bridges, elevators, flying, driving claustrophobic in elevators. (29 

"7 
Blood, injury, injections Person panics when y 

viewing a child’s scraped 
nee. 

Fear of being judged or Person avoids all social situations and 
embarrassed by others becomes a recluse for fear of encountering 


others’ judgment. 


FIGURE 1 Phobic disorders differ from generalized anxiety and panic disorders because a 
specific stimulus can be identified. Listed here are a number of types of phobias and their 
triggers. (Source: Adapted from Nolen-Hoeksema, 2007). 


Donna suffers from a phobia, an intense, irrational fear of a specific object or situ- 
ation. For example, claustrophobia is a fear of enclosed places, acrophobia is a fear 
of high places, xenophobia is a fear of strangers, social phobia is the fear of being 
judged or embarrassed by others, and—as in Donna’s case—electrophobia is a fear 
of electricity. 

The objective danger posed by an anxiety-producing stimulus (which can be just 
about anything, as you can see in Figure 1) is typically small or nonexistent. How- 
ever, to someone suffering from the phobia, the danger is great, and a full-blown 
panic attack may follow exposure to the stimulus. Phobic disorders differ from gen- 
eralized anxiety disorders and panic disorders in that there is a specific, identifiable 
stimulus that sets off the anxiety reaction. 

Phobias may have only a minor impact on people’s lives if those who suffer from 
them can avoid the stimuli that trigger fear. For example, a fear of heights may have 
little impact on people’s everyday lives (although it may prevent them from living 
in a high floor in an apartment)—unless they are firefighters or window-washers. On 
the other hand, a social phobia, or a fear of strangers, presents a more serious problem. 
In one extreme case, a Washington woman left her home just three times in 30 years— 
once to visit her family, once for an operation, and once to purchase ice cream for a 
dying companion (Adler, 1984; Kimbrel, 2007). 


PANIC DISORDER 


In another type of anxiety disorder, panic disorder, panic attacks occur that last from 
a few seconds to several hours. Unlike phobias, which are stimulated by specific 
objects or situations, panic disorders do not have any identifiable stimuli. Instead, 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 47 The Major Psychological Disorders 515 


during an attack such as those Sally experienced in the case IN 
described earlier, anxiety suddenly—and often without ' 
warning—rises to a peak, and an individual feels a sense of 
impending, unavoidable doom. Although the physical symp- 
toms differ from person to person, they may include heart 
palpitations, shortness of breath, unusual amounts of sweat- 
ing, faintness and dizziness, gastric sensations, and some- 
times a sense of imminent death. After such an attack, it is 
no wonder that people tend to feel exhausted (Rachman & 
deSilva, 2004; Laederach-Hofmann & Messerli-Buergy, 2007). 

Panic attacks seemingly come out of nowhere and are 
unconnected to any specific stimulus. Because they don’t 
know what triggers their feelings of panic, victims of panic 
attacks may become fearful of going places. In fact, some 
people with panic disorder develop a complication called 
agoraphobia, the fear of being in a situation in which escape 
is difficult and in which help for a possible panic attack 
would not be available. In extreme cases, people with agora- 
phobia never leave their homes (Marcaurelle, Bélanger, & 
Marchand, 2005; Herrán, Carrera, & Sierra-Biddle, 2006; 
Wittchen et al., 2008). 

In addition to the physical symptoms, panic disorder affects how the brain pro- 
cesses information. For instance, people with panic disorder have reduced reactions 
in the anterior cingulate cortex to stimuli (such as viewing a fearful face) that nor- 
mally produce a strong reaction in those without the disorder. It may be that recur- 
ring high levels of emotional arousal that patients with panic disorder experience 
desensitizes them to emotional stimuli (Pillay et al., 2006; Pillay et al., 2007). 





Acrophobia, the fear of heights, is not an uncommon phobia. What 
sort of behavior-modification approaches might be used to deal 
with acrophobia? 


GENERALIZED ANXIETY DISORDER 


People with generalized anxiety disorder experience long-term, persistent anxiety generalized anxiety disorder 
and uncontrollable worry. Sometimes their concerns are about identifiable issues The experience of long-term, 
involving family, money, work, or health. In other cases, though, people with the persistent anxiety and worry. 
disorder feel that something dreadful is about to happen but can’t identify the reason 
and thus experience “free-floating” anxiety. 
Because of persistent anxiety, people with generalized anxiety disorder cannot 
concentrate or set their worry and fears aside; their lives become centered on their 
worry. Furthermore, their anxiety is often accompanied by physiological symptoms 
such as muscle tension, headaches, dizziness, heart palpitations, or insomnia 
(Starcevic et al., 2007). Figure 2 shows the most common symptoms of generalized 
anxiety disorder. 


Obsessive-Compulsive Disorder. In obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), people —_ obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) 
are plagued by unwanted thoughts, called obsessions, or feel that they must carry A disorder characterized by obsessions 
out behaviors, termed compulsions, which they feel driven to perform. or compulsions. 

An obsession is a persistent, unwanted thought or idea that keeps recurring. 
For example, a student may be unable to stop thinking that she has neglected to 
put her name on a test and may think about it constantly for the two weeks it 
takes to get the paper back. A man may go on vacation and wonder the whole 
time whether he locked his house. A woman may hear the same tune running 
through her head over and over. In each case, the thought or idea is unwanted 
and difficult to put out of mind. Of course, many people suffer from mild obses- 
sions from time to time, but usually such thoughts persist only for a short period. 
For people with serious obsessions, however, the thoughts persist for days or 
months and may consist of bizarre, troubling images (Lee & Kwon, 2003, Lee et 
al., 2005; Rassin & Muris, 2007). 


obsession A persistent, unwanted 
thought or idea that keeps recurring. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


516 Chapter 15 Psychological Disorders 


FIGURE 2 Frequency of symptoms in 
cases of generalized anxiety disorder. 
(Source: Adapted from Beck & Emery, 1985, 
pp. 87-88.) 


compulsion An irresistible urge to 
repeatedly carry out some act that 
seems strange and unreasonable. 


Symptom 

Actual fainting 
Feeling of choking 
Diarrhea 

Nausea 

Hands trembling 
Difficulty breathing 
Fear of dying 
Sweating all over 
Speech blocked 
Wobbly 

Heart racing 

Hands sweating 
Terrified 

Weakness all over 
Confusion 

Unable to control thoughts 
Jumpy 

Fear of losing control 
Frightened 

Tense 

Difficulty concentrating 


Unable to relax 











0 10 20 30 40 50 60 70 80 90 100 


Percentage of cases in which symptom occurs 


As part of an obsessive-compulsive disorder, people may also experience 
compulsions, irresistible urges to repeatedly carry out some act that seems strange 
and unreasonable even to them. Whatever the compulsive behavior is, people 
experience extreme anxiety if they cannot carry it out even if it is something they 
want to stop. The acts may be relatively trivial, such as repeatedly checking the 
stove to make sure all the burners are turned off, or more unusual, such as wash- 
ing one’s hands so much that they bleed (Frost & Steketee, 2002; Clark, 2007; 
Moretz & McKay, 2009). 

For example, consider this passage from the autobiography of a person with 
obsessive-compulsive disorder: 


I thought my parents would die if I didn’t do everything in exactly the right way. 
When I took my glasses off at night I’d have to place them on the dresser at a particu- 
lar angle. Sometimes I’d turn on the light and get out of bed seven times until I felt 
comfortable with the angle. If the angle wasn’t right, I felt that my parents would die. 
The feeling ate up my insides. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 47 The Major Psychological Disorders 517 


If I didn’t grab the molding on the wall just the right way as I entered or 
exited my room; if I didn’t hang a shirt in the closet perfectly; if I didn’t read a 
paragraph a certain way; if my hands and nails weren’t perfectly clean, I thought 
my incorrect behavior would kill my parents (Summers, 2000, p. 42). 


Although carrying out compulsive rituals may lead to some immediate 
reduction of anxiety, in the long run the anxiety returns. In fact, people with 
severe cases lead lives filled with unrelenting tension (Goodman, Rudorfer, & 
Maser, 2000; Penzel, 2000). 


© Robert Mankoff/The New Yorker 
Collection/www.cartoonbank.com 





THE CAUSES OF ANXIETY DISORDERS “No, I dont need an alarm clock— 


anxiety is my alarm clock.” 
We've considered the four major types of anxiety disorders, but there are others as well. 
For instance, posttraumatic stress disorder (in which a person re-experiences a stressful 
event in vivid flashbacks or dreams and which we discussed when we considered 
stress) is classified as an anxiety disorder. 

The variety of anxiety disorders means that no single explanation fits all cases. 
Genetic factors clearly are part of the picture. For example, if one member of a pair 
of identical twins has panic disorder, there is a 30% chance that the other twin will 
have it also. Furthermore, a person’s characteristic level of anxiety is related to a 
specific gene involved in the production of the neurotransmitter serotonin. This is 
consistent with findings indicating that certain chemical deficiencies in the brain 
appear to produce some kinds of anxiety disorder (Holmes et al., 2003; Beidel & 
Turner, 2007; Chamberlain et al., 2008). 

Some researchers believe that an overactive autonomic nervous system may be 
at the root of panic attacks. Specifically, they suggest that poor regulation of the 
brain’s locus ceruleus may lead to panic attacks, which cause the limbic system to 
become overstimulated. In turn, the overstimulated limbic system produces chronic 
anxiety, which ultimately leads the locus ceruleus to generate still more panic attacks 
(Pine et al., 2000; Balaban, 2002; Davies et al., 2008). 

There are also biological causes at work in OCD. For example, researchers have 
found differences in the brains of those with the disorder compared to those without 
it (see Figure 3; Christian et al., 2008). 


One Step 


Closer to Understanding OCD 





FIGURE 3 People with obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) have structural differences in their 
brains. These images show increased levels of grey matter (i.e, more connections or more 
neurons) in the thalamus (a) and in the left frontal cortex (b) in people with OCD as compared to 
people without it. These findings help us to understand the potential causes of OCD and may 
lead to the development of better treatments for the disorder. (Source: Christian et al., 2008, Figure 1.) 





(a) (b) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


518 Chapter 15 Psychological Disorders 


somatoform disorders Psychological 
difficulties that take on a physical 
(somatic) form, but for which there is 
no medical cause. 


hypochondriasis A disorder in which 
people have a constant fear of illness 
and a preoccupation with their health. 


conversion disorder A major somato- 
form disorder that involves an actual 
physical disturbance, such as the 
inability to use a sensory organ or the 
complete or partial inability to move 
an arm or leg. 


Psychologists who employ the behavioral perspective have taken a different 
approach that emphasizes environmental factors. They consider anxiety to be a 
learned response to stress. For instance, suppose a dog bites a young girl. When the 
girl next sees a dog, she is frightened and runs away—a behavior that relieves her 
anxiety and thereby reinforces her avoidance behavior. After repeated encounters 
with dogs in which she is reinforced for her avoidance behavior, she may develop a 
full-fledged phobia regarding dogs. 

Finally, the cognitive perspective suggests that anxiety disorders grow out of 
inappropriate and inaccurate thoughts and beliefs about circumstances in a person’s 
world. For example, people with anxiety disorders may view a friendly puppy as a 
ferocious and savage pit bull, or they may see an air disaster looming every moment 
they are in the vicinity of an airplane. According to the cognitive perspective, people’s 
maladaptive thoughts about the world are at the root of an anxiety disorder (Frost & 
Steketee, 2002; Wang & Clark, 2002; Ouimet, Gawronski, & Dozois, 2009). 


Somatoform Disorders 


Somatoform disorder are psychological difficulties that take on a physical (somatic) 
form but for which there is no medical cause. Even though an individual with a 
somatoform disorder reports physical symptoms, no biological cause exists, or if 
there is a medical problem, the person’s reaction is greatly exaggerated. 

One type of somatoform disorder is hypochondriasis in which people have a 
constant fear of illness and a preoccupation with their health. These individuals believe 
that everyday aches and pains are symptoms of a dread disease. The “symptoms” are 
not faked; rather, they are misinterpreted as evidence of some serious illness—often in 
the face of inarguable medical evidence to the contrary (Noyes et al., 2003; Fallon & 
Feinstein, 2001; Abramowitz, Olatunji, & Deacon, 2007; Olatunji, 2008). 

Another somatoform disorder is conversion disorder. Unlike hypochondriasis, 
in which there is no physical problem, conversion disorders involve an actual 
physical disturbance, such as the inability to see or hear or to move an arm or leg. 
The cause of such a physical disturbance is purely psychological; there is no bio- 
logical reason for the problem. Some of Freud’s classic cases involved conversion 
disorders. For instance, one of Freud’s patients suddenly became unable to use 
her arm without any apparent physiological cause. Later, just as abruptly, the 
problem disappeared. 

Conversion disorders often begin suddenly. People wake up one morning blind 
or deaf, or they experience numbness that is restricted to a certain part of the body. 
A hand, for example, may become entirely numb, while an area above the wrist, 
controlled by the same nerves, remains sensitive to touch—something that is physi- 
ologically implausible. Mental health professionals refer to such a condition as “glove 
anesthesia” because the numb area is the part of the hand covered by a glove and 
not a region related to pathways of the nervous system (see Figure 4). 

Surprisingly, people who experience conversion disorders frequently remain 
unconcerned about symptoms that most of us would expect to be highly anxiety 
producing. For instance, a person in good health who wakes up blind may react in 
a bland, matter-of-fact way. Considering how most of us would feel if we 
woke up unable to see, this unemotional reaction (called la belle indifference, a French 
phrase meaning “a beautiful indifference”) hardly seems appropriate (Brasic, 2002). 





Dissociative Disorders 


The classic movie The Three Faces of Eve (about a woman with three wildly differ- 
ent personalities) and the book Sybil (about a girl who allegedly had 16 person- 
alities) represent a highly dramatic, rare, and controversial class of disorders: 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 47 The Major Psychological Disorders 519 


Head band 


Neck band 


Chest band 
Arm band 


Waist band 


Wrist band 


Glove 


Thigh band 
Knee band 





Sock Ankle band 





FIGURE 4 Conversion disorders sometimes produce numbness in specific and isolated areas 
of the body (indicated by the shaded areas in the figure). For instance, in glove anesthesia, 
the area of the body covered by a glove feels numb. However, the condition is biologically 
implausible because of the nerves involved, which suggests that the problem results from a 
psychological disorder rather than from actual nerve damage. 


dissociative disorders. Dissociative disorders are characterized by the separation dissociative disorders Psychological 
(or dissociation) of different facets of a person’s personality that are normally dysfunctions characterized by the 
integrated and work together. By dissociating key parts of who they are, people separation of different facets of a 
are able to keep disturbing memories or perceptions from reaching conscious person's personality that are normally 
awareness and thereby reduce their anxiety (Maldonado & Spiegel, 2003; integrated. 
Houghtalen & Talbot, 2007). 
Several dissociative disorders exist, although all of them are rare. A person with 
a dissociative identity disorder (DID) (once called multiple personality disorder) dis- dissociative identity disorder (DID) 
plays characteristics of two or more distinct personalities, identities, or personality A disorder in which a person displays 
fragments. Individual personalities often have a unique set of likes and dislikes and characteristics of two or more distinct 
their own reactions to situations. Some people with multiple personalities even carry personalities. 
several pairs of glasses because their vision changes with each personality. Moreover, 
each individual personality can be well adjusted when considered on its own (Ella- 
son & Ross, 2004; Stickley & Nickeas, 2006). 
The diagnosis of dissociative identity disorder is controversial. It was rarely 
diagnosed before 1980, when it was added as a category in the third edition of 
DSM for the first time. At that point, the number of cases increased significantly. 
Some clinicians suggest the increase was due to more precise identification of the 
disorder, while others suggest the increase was due to an overreadiness to use the 
classification. In addition, widespread publicity about cases of DID may have influ- 
enced patients to report symptoms of more common personality disorders in ways 
that made it more likely they would receive a diagnosis of DID. There are also 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


520 Chapter 15 Psychological Disorders 


dissociative amnesia A disorder in 
which a significant, selective memory 
loss occurs. 


dissociative fugue A form of amnesia 
in which the individual leaves home 
and sometimes assumes a new identity. 


mood disorder A disturbance in 
emotional experience that is strong 
enough to intrude on everyday living. 


significant cross-cultural differences in the incidence of DID (Kihlstrom, 2005a; Xiao 
et al., 2006). 

Dissociative amnesia is another dissociative disorder in which a significant, selec- 
tive memory loss occurs. Dissociative amnesia is unlike simple amnesia, which involves 
an actual loss of information from memory and typically results from a physiological 
cause. In contrast, in cases of dissociative amnesia, the “forgotten” material is still pres- 
ent in memory—it simply cannot be recalled. The term repressed memories is sometimes 
used to describe the lost memories of people with dissociative amnesia. 

In the most severe form of dissociative amnesia, individuals cannot recall their 
names, are unable to recognize parents and other relatives, and do not know their 
addresses. In other respects, though, they may appear quite normal. Apart from an 
inability to remember certain facts about themselves, they may be able to recall skills 
and abilities that they developed earlier. For instance, even though a chef may not 
remember where he grew up and received training, he may still be able to prepare 
gourmet meals. 

In some cases of dissociative amnesia, the memory loss is profound. For example, 
in one dramatic case, Raymond Power Jr., an attorney, husband, father of two, and 
Boy Scout leader, left home to go to work one morning. Two days later he was home- 
less, living a new life a thousand miles away, and had no memory of who he was 
or how he got there. He was found six months later but still had no recollection of 
his previous life, including any knowledge of his wife of 30 years or even that he 
had children (Foderaro, 2006). 

A more unusual form of amnesia is a condition known as dissociative fugue. In 
this state, people take sudden, impulsive trips and sometimes assume a new identity. 
After a period of time—days, months, or sometimes even years—they suddenly real- 
ize that they are in a strange place and completely forget the time they have spent 
wandering. Their last memories are those from the time just before they entered the 
fugue state (Hennig-Fast et al, 2008). 

The common thread among dissociative disorders is that they allow people to 
escape from some anxiety-producing situation. Either the person produces a new 
personality to deal with stress, or the individual forgets or leaves behind the situation 
that caused the stress as he or she journeys to some new—and perhaps less anxiety- 
ridden—environment (Putnam, 2000; R. J. Brown, 2006). 


Mood Disorders 


From the time I woke up in the morning until the time I went to bed at night, I 





was unbearably miserable and seemingly incapable of any kind of joy or enthusiasm. 
Everything—every thought, word, movement—was an effort. Everything that once 
was sparkling now was flat. I seemed to myself to be dull, boring, inadequate, thick 
brained, unlit, unresponsive, chill skinned, bloodless, and sparrow drab. I doubted, 
completely, my ability to do anything well. It seemed as though my mind had 
slowed down and burned out to the point of being virtually useless. (Jamison, 
1995a, p. 110) 


We all experience mood swings. Sometimes we are happy, perhaps even euphoric; 
at other times we feel upset, saddened, or depressed. Such changes in mood are 
a normal part of everyday life. In some people, however, moods are so pro- 
nounced and lingering—like the feelings described above by writer (and psy- 
chiatrist) Kay Jamison—that they interfere with the ability to function effectively. 
In extreme cases, a mood may become life threatening; in other cases, it may cause 
the person to lose touch with reality. Situations such as these represent mood 
disorders, disturbances in emotional experience that are strong enough to intrude 
on everyday living. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 47 The Major Psychological Disorders 521 


MAJOR DEPRESSION 


President Abraham Lincoln. Queen Victoria. Newscaster Mike Wallace. 

The common link among these people? Each suffered from periodic attacks of 
major depression, a severe form of depression that interferes with concentration, 
decision making, and sociability. Major depression is one of the more common forms 
of mood disorders. Some 15 million people in the United States suffer from major 
depression, and at any one time, 6-10% of the U.S. population is clinically depressed. 
Almost one in five people in the United States experiences major depression at some 
point in life, and 15% of college students have received a diagnosis of depression. 
The cost of depression is more than $80 billion a year in lost productivity (Winik, 
2006; Scelfo, 2007; Simon et al., 2008). 

Women are twice as likely to experience major depression as men, with one- 
fourth of all females apt to encounter it at some point during their lives. Furthermore, 
although no one is sure why, the rate of depression is going up throughout the world. 
Results of in-depth interviews conducted in the United States, Puerto Rico, Taiwan, 
Lebanon, Canada, Italy, Germany, and France indicate that the incidence of depres- 
sion has increased significantly over previous rates in every area. In fact, in some 
countries, the likelihood that individuals will have major depression at some point 
in their lives is three times higher than it was for earlier generations. In addition, 
people are developing major depression at increasingly younger ages (Miller, 2003; 
Staley, Sancora, & Tagman, 2006; Kendler et al., 2006a). 

When psychologists speak of major depression, they do not mean the sadness 
that comes from experiencing one of life’s disappointments that we all have 
experienced. Some depression is normal after the breakup of a long-term relation- 
ship, the death of a loved one, or the loss of a job. It is normal even after less 
serious problems, such as doing badly on a test or having a romantic partner 
forget one’s birthday. 

People who suffer from major depression experience similar feelings, but the 
severity tends to be considerably greater. They may feel useless, worthless, and 
lonely, and they may think the future is hopeless and no one can help them. They 
may lose their appetite and have no energy. Moreover, they may experience such 
feelings for months or even years. They may cry uncontrollably, have sleep distur- 
bances, and be at risk for suicide. The depth and duration of such behavior are the 
hallmarks of major depression. (Figure 5 provides a self-assessment of depression.) 


Se I A 


A Test for Depression 
To complete the questionnaire, count the number of statements with which you agree: 


| feel downhearted, blue, and sad. 

| don't enjoy the things that | used to. 

| feel that others would be better off if | were dead. 
| feel that I am not useful or needed. 

| notice that | am losing weight. 

| have trouble sleeping through the night. 

I am restless and can't keep still. 

My mind isn't as clear as it used to be. 

| get tired for no reason. 

| feel hopeless about the future. 


SND oS) fo Ca gS SS 


— 


Scoring: If you agree with at least five of the statements, including either item 1 or 2, and if you 
have had these symptoms for at least two weeks, help from a professional is strongly recom- 
mended. If you answer yes to number 3, you should get help immediately. 


major depression A severe form 

of depression that interferes with 
concentration, decision making, and 
sociability. 







udy Alert 

Major depression differs from 
the normal depression that 
occasionally occurs during 
most people's lives; major 
depression is more in- 
tense, lasts longer, and may 
have no clear trigger. 


FIGURE 5 This is a version of a test 
distributed by mental health organi- 
zations during the annual National 
Depression Screening Day, a nationwide 
event that seeks to identify people who 
are suffering from depression that is 
severe enough to warrant psychological 
intervention. (Source: National Depression 
Screening Day, 2003.) 


522 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 15 Psychological Disorders 


MANIA AND BIPOLAR DISORDER 


While depression leads to the depths of despair, mania leads to emotional heights. 


mania An extended state of intense, Mania is an extended state of intense, wild elation. People experiencing mania feel 
wild elation. intense happiness, power, invulnerability, and energy. Believing they will succeed 


at anything they attempt, they may become involved in wild schemes. Consider, 
for example, the following description of an individual who experienced a manic 
episode: 


Mr. O'Reilly took a leave of absence from his civil service job. He purchased a large 
number of cuckoo clocks and then an expensive car, which he planned to use as a 
mobile showroom for his wares, anticipating that he would make a great deal of money. 
He proceeded to “tear around town” buying and selling clocks and other merchandise, 
and when he was not out, he was continuously on the phone making “deals.” .. . He 
was $3,000 in debt and had driven his family to exhaustion with his excessive activity 
and talkativeness. He said, however, that he felt “on top of the world.” (Spitzer et al., 
1983, p. 115) 


Typically, people sequentially experience periods of mania and depression. This alter- 


bipolar disorder A disorder in which nation of mania and depression is called bipolar disorder (a condition previously 
a person alternates between periods of known as manic-depressive disorder). The swings between highs and lows may occur 
euphoric feelings of mania and periods a few days apart or may alternate over a period of years. In addition, in bipolar dis- 
of depression. order, periods of depression are usually longer than periods of mania. 


30 


25 


20 


Compositions 





Ironically, some of society’s most creative individuals may have suffered from 
bipolar disorder. The imagination, drive, excitement, and energy that they display 
during manic stages allow them to make unusually creative contributions. For 
instance, historical analysis of the composer Robert Schumann’s music shows that he 
was most prolific during periods of mania. In contrast, his output dropped off 
drastically during periods of depression (see Figure 6). On the other hand, the high 
output associated with mania does not necessarily lead to higher quality: Some of 
Schumann’s greatest works were created outside his periods of mania (Ludwig, 1996; 
Szegedy Maszak, 2003). 





Moderate mania Moderate mania Died in asylum 
throughout 1840 |X throughout 1849 (self-starvation) 
1856 





Severe depression; 


suicide attempt 
1833 











1829 1831 1833 1835 1837 1839 184] 1843 1845 1847 1849 1851 1853 1855 
Year 


FIGURE 6 The number of pieces written by composer Robert Schumann in a given year is 
related to his periods of depression and mania (Slater & Meyer, 1959; reprinted in Jamison, 
1993). Why do you think mania might be associated with creative productivity in some people? 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 47 The Major Psychological Disorders 


Despite the creative fires that may be lit by mania, persons who experience this 
disorder often show a recklessness that produces emotional and sometimes physical 
self-injury. They may alienate people with their talkativeness, inflated self-esteem, 
and indifference to the needs of others. 


CAUSES OF MOOD DISORDERS 


Because they represent a major mental health problem, mood disorders—and, in 
particular, depression—have received a good deal of study. Several approaches have 
been used to explain the disorders. 

Some mood disorders clearly have genetic and biochemical roots. In fact, most 
evidence suggests that bipolar disorders are caused primarily by biological factors. 
For instance, bipolar disorder (and some forms of major depression) clearly runs in 
some families. Furthermore, researchers have found that several neurotransmitters 
play a role in depression. For example, alterations in the functioning of serotonin and 
norepinephrine in the brain are related to the disorder (Plomin & McGuffin, 2003; 
Kato, 2007; Popa et al., 2008). 

Other explanations for depression have also included a focus on psychological 
causes. For instance, proponents of psychoanalytic approaches see depression as the 
result of feelings of loss (real or potential) or of anger directed at oneself. One psy- 
choanalytic approach, for instance, suggests that depression is produced by the loss 
or threatened loss of a parent early in life (Vanheule et al., 2006). 

Behavioral theories of depression argue that the stresses of life produce a reduc- 
tion in positive reinforcers. As a result, people begin to withdraw, which only reduces 
positive reinforcers further. In addition, people receive attention for their depressive 
behavior, which further reinforces the depression (Lewinsohn & Essau, 2002; 
Lewinsohn et al., 2003). 

Some explanations for mood disorders attribute them to cognitive factors. For 
example, psychologist Martin Seligman suggests that depression is largely a response 
to learned helplessness. Learned helplessness is a learned expectation that events in 
one’s life are uncontrollable and that one cannot escape from the situation. As a 
consequence, people simply give up fighting aversive events and submit to them, 
which thereby produces depression. Other theorists go a step further and suggest 
that depression results from hopelessness, a combination of learned helplessness and 
an expectation that negative outcomes in one’s life are inevitable (Kwon & Lau- 
renceau, 2002; Maier & Watkins, 2000; Bjornstad, 2006). 

Clinical psychologist Aaron Beck has proposed that faulty cognitions underlie 
people’s depressed feelings. Specifically, his cognitive theory of depression suggests 
that depressed individuals typically view themselves as life’s losers and blame them- 
selves whenever anything goes wrong. By focusing on the negative side of situations, 
they feel inept and unable to act constructively to change their environment. In sum, 
their negative cognitions lead to feelings of depression (Newman et al., 2002). 

Brain imaging studies suggest that people with depression experience a general 
blunting of emotional reactions. For example, one study found that the brains of 
people with depression showed significantly less activation when they viewed pho- 
tos of human faces displaying strong emotions than did those without the disorder 
(Gotlib et al., 2004). 

Other explanations of depression derive from evolutionary psychology, which 
considers how our genetic inheritance from our ancestors influences our behavior. In 
the evolutionary view, depression is an adaptive response to unattainable goals. 
When people fruitlessly pursue an ever-elusive goal, depression begins, ending pur- 
suit of the goal. Ultimately, when the depression lifts, people can turn to other, more 
reasonable goals. In this view, depression serves a positive function and in the long 
run increases the chances of survival for particular individuals, who can then pass 
the behavior to their offspring. Such reasoning, of course, is highly speculative 
(Nesse, 2000, Siegert & Ward, 2002; Pfeffer, 2006). 


523 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


524 Chapter 15 Psychological Disorders 


schizophrenia A class of disorders in 
which severe distortion of reality occurs. 


The various theories of depression have not provided a complete answer to an 
elusive question that has dogged researchers: Why does depression occur in approx- 
imately twice as many women as men—a pattern that is similar across a variety of 
cultures? 

One explanation suggests that the stress women experience may be greater than 
the stress men experience at certain points in their lives—such as when a woman 
must simultaneously earn a living and be the primary caregiver for her children. In 
addition, women have a higher risk for physical and sexual abuse, typically earn 
lower wages than men, report greater unhappiness with their marriages, and gener- 
ally experience chronic negative circumstances. Furthermore, women and men may 
respond to stress with different coping mechanisms. For instance, men may abuse 
drugs, while women respond with depression (Holden, 2005; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2007; 
Hyde, Mezulis, & Abramson, 2008). 

Biological factors may also explain some women’s depression. For example, the 
rate of female depression begins to rise during puberty, so some psychologists believe 
that hormones make women more vulnerable to the disorder. In addition, 25-50% of 
women who take oral contraceptives report symptoms of depression, and depression 
that occurs after the birth of a child is linked to hormonal changes. Finally, structural 
differences in men’s and women’s brains that we discussed in the neuroscience and 
behavior modules may be related to gender differences in depression (Holden, 2005; 
Graham, Bancroft, & Doll, 2007; Solomon & Herman, 2009). 

Ultimately, it is clear that researchers have discovered no definitive solutions to 
the puzzle of depression, and there are many alternative explanations. Most likely, a 
complex interaction of several factors causes mood disorders. 





Schizophrenia 


Things that relate, the town of Antelope, Oregon, Jonestown, Charlie Manson, the 
Hillside Strangler, the Zodiac Killer, Watergate, King’s trial in L.A., and many more. In 
the last 7 years alone, over 23 Star Wars scientists committed suicide for no apparent 
reason. The AIDS cover-up, the conference in South America in 87 had over 1,000 
doctors claim that insects can transmit it. To be able to read one’s thoughts and place 
thoughts in one’s mind without the person knowing it’s being done. Realization is a 
reality of bioelectromagnetic control, which is thought transfer and emotional control, 
recording individual brainwave frequencies of thought, sensation, and emotions. (Nolen- 
Heoksema, 2007, pp. 385-386). 


This excerpt illustrates the efforts of a person with schizophrenia, one of the more 
severe forms of mental disturbance, to communicate. People with schizophrenia 
account for by far the largest percentage of those hospitalized for psychological dis- 
orders. They are also in many respects the least likely to recover from their difficulties. 

Schizophrenia refers to a class of disorders in which severe distortion of reality 
occurs. Thinking, perception, and emotion may deteriorate; the individual may with- 
draw from social interaction; and the person may display bizarre behavior. Although 
there are several types of schizophrenia (see Figure 7), the distinctions between them 
are not always clear. Moreover, the symptoms displayed by persons with schizophre- 
nia may vary considerably over time, and people with schizophrenia show significant 
differences in the pattern of their symptoms even when they are labeled with the 
same diagnostic category. Nonetheless, a number of characteristics reliably distin- 
guish schizophrenia from other disorders. They include the following: 


e Decline from a previous level of functioning. An individual can no longer carry 
out activities he or she was once able to do. 

e Disturbances of thought and speech. People with schizophrenia use logic and 
language in a peculiar way. Their thinking often does not make sense, and 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 47 The Major Psychological Disorders 525 


Types of Schizophrenia 





Type Symptoms 
Disorganized (hebephrenic) Inappropriate laughter and giggling, silliness, 
schizophrenia incoherent speech, infantile behavior, strange and 


sometimes obscene behavior 


Paranoid schizophrenia Delusions and hallucinations of persecution or 
of greatness, loss of judgment, erratic and 
unpredictable behavior 


Catatonic schizophrenia Major disturbances in movement; in some phases, 
loss of all motion, with patient frozen into a single 
position, remaining that way for hours and 
sometimes even days; in other phases, hyperactivity 
and wild, sometimes violent, movement 


Undifferentiated Variable mixture of major symptoms of 

schizophrenia schizophrenia; classification used for patients who 
cannot be typed into any of the more specific 
categories 

Residual schizophrenia Minor signs of schizophrenia after a more serious 
episode 


FIGURE 7 The distinctions among the different types of schizophrenia are not always clear 
cut, and symptoms may vary considerably over time. 


their logic is frequently faulty, which is referred to as a formal thought disorder. 
They also do not follow conventional linguistic rules (Penn et al., 1997). 
Consider, for example, the following response to the question “Why do you 
think people believe in God?” 


Uh, let’s, I don’t know why, let’s see, balloon travel. He holds it up for you, the 
balloon. He don’t let you fall out, your little legs sticking down through the clouds. 
He’s down to the smokestack, looking through the smoke trying to get the balloon 
gassed up you know. Way they’re flying on top that way, legs sticking out. I don’t 
know, looking down on the ground, heck, that’d make you so dizzy you just stay and 
sleep you know, hold down and sleep there. I used to be sleep outdoors, you know, 
sleep outdoors instead of going home. (Chapman & Chapman, 1973, p. 3) 


As this selection illustrates, although the basic grammatical structure may be 
intact, the substance of thinking characteristic of schizophrenia is often illogi- 
cal, garbled, and lacking in meaningful content (Holden, 2003; Heinrichs, 
2005). 


e Delusions. People with schizophrenia often have delusions, firmly held, 
unshakable beliefs with no basis in reality. Among the common delusions 
people with schizophrenia experience are the beliefs that they are being 
controlled by someone else, they are being persecuted by others, and their 
thoughts are being broadcast so that others know what they are thinking 
(Coltheart, Langdon, & McKay, 2007; Startup, Bucci, & Langdon, 2009). 

e Hallucinations and perceptual disorders. People with schizophrenia do not 
perceive the world as most other people do. They also may have hallucinations, 
the experience of perceiving things that do not actually exist. Furthermore, 
they may see, hear, or smell things differently from others (see Figure 8); they 
do not even have a sense of their bodies in the way that others do and have 
difficulty determining where their bodies stop and the rest of the world begins 
(Copolov et al., 2003; Botvinick, 2004; Thomas et al., 2007). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


526 Chapter 15 Psychological Disorders 





Alert 


In Type | schizophrenia, posi- 
tive symptoms (in which 

hallucinations, delusions, 

and emotional extremes 
are present) are dominant; 
in Type Il schizophrenia, negative 
symptoms (characterized by an 
absence or loss of normal functioning) 
are dominant. 










FIGURE 8 This unusual art was created by an individual suffering from severe mental 
disturbance. 


e Emotional disturbances. People with schizophrenia sometimes show a lack of 
emotion in which even the most dramatic events produce little or no emo- 
tional response. Conversely, they may display emotion that is inappropriate to 
a situation. For example, a person with schizophrenia may laugh uproariously 
at a funeral or react with anger when being helped by someone. 

e Withdrawal. People with schizophrenia tend to have little interest in others. 
They tend not to socialize or hold real conversations with others, although 
they may talk at another person. In the most extreme cases, they do not even 
acknowledge the presence of other people and appear to be in their own 
isolated world. 


Usually, the onset of schizophrenia occurs in early adulthood, and the symptoms 
follow one of two primary courses. In process schizophrenia, the symptoms develop 
slowly and subtly. There may be a gradual withdrawal from the world, excessive 
daydreaming, and a blunting of emotion until eventually the disorder reaches the 
point where others cannot overlook it. In other cases, known as reactive schizophrenia, 
the onset of symptoms is sudden and conspicuous. The treatment outlook for reactive 
schizophrenia is relatively favorable, but process schizophrenia has proved more 
difficult to treat. 

DSM-IV-TR classifies the symptoms of schizophrenia into two types. Positive-symp- 
tom schizophrenia is indicated by the presence of disordered behavior such as hallucina- 
tions, delusions, and emotional extremes. In contrast, negative-symptom schizophrenia 
shows an absence or loss of normal functioning, such as social withdrawal or blunted 
emotions. Schizophrenia researchers sometimes speak of Type I schizophrenia, in which 
positive symptoms are dominant, and Type II schizophrenia, in which negative symptoms 
are more prominent (Buchanan et al., 2007; Levine & Rabinowitz, 2007). 

The distinction between Type I and Type II schizophrenia is important because 
it suggests that two different processes might trigger schizophrenia. Furthermore, it 
has implications for predicting treatment outcomes. 


SOLVING THE PUZZLE OF SCHIZOPHRENIA: BIOLOGICAL CAUSES 


Although schizophrenic behavior clearly departs radically from normal behavior, its 
causes are less apparent. It does appear, however, that schizophrenia has both bio- 
logical and environmental origins (Sawa & Snyder, 2002). 

Let’s first consider the evidence pointing to a biological cause. Because schizo- 
phrenia is more common in some families than in others, genetic factors seem to 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 47 The Major Psychological Disorders 


Risk of Developing Schizophrenia, Based on 
Genetic Relatedness to a Person with Schizophrenia 


Genetic Risk of Developing 
Relationship Relatedness, % Schizophrenia, % 
Identical twin 100 48 
Child of two schizophrenic parents 100 46 
Fraternal twin 50 17 
Offspring of one schizophrenic parent 50 17 
Sibling 50 9) 
Nephew or niece 25 4 
Spouse 0 2 
Unrelated person 0 1 


FIGURE 9 The closer the genetic links between two people, the greater the likelihood that if 
one experiences schizophrenia, so will the other sometime during his or her lifetime. However, 
genetics is not the full story; if it were, the risk of identical twins having schizophrenia would 
be 100% and not the 48% shown in this figure. (Source: Gottesman, 1991.) 


be involved in producing at least a susceptibility to or readiness for developing 
schizophrenia. For example, the closer the genetic link between a person with 
schizophrenia and another individual, the greater the likelihood that the other per- 
son will experience the disorder (see Figure 9; Brzustowicz et al., 2000; Plomin & 
McGuffin, 2003; Gottesman & Hanson, 2005). 

However, if genetics alone were responsible for schizophrenia, the chance of both 
of two identical twins having schizophrenia would be 100% instead of just under 
50% because identical twins have the same genetic makeup. Moreover, attempts to 
find a link between schizophrenia and a particular gene have been only partly suc- 
cessful. Apparently, genetic factors alone do not produce schizophrenia (Franzek & 
Beckmann, 1996; Lenzenweger & Dworkin, 1998). 

One intriguing biological hypothesis to explain schizophrenia is that the brains 
of people with the disorder may harbor either a biochemical imbalance or a structural 
abnormality. For example, the dopamine hypothesis suggests that schizophrenia occurs 
when there is excess activity in the areas of the brain that use dopamine as a neu- 
rotransmitter. This hypothesis came to light after the discovery that drugs that block 
dopamine action in brain pathways can be highly effective in reducing the symptoms 
of schizophrenia. Other research suggests that glutamate, another neurotransmitter, 
may be a major contributor to the disorder (Ohara, 2007; Stone, Morrison, & Pilowsky, 
2007; Howes & Kapur, 2009). 

Some biological explanations propose that structural abnormalities exist in the 
brains of people with schizophrenia perhaps as a result of exposure to a virus during 
prenatal development. For example, some research shows abnormalities in the neural 
circuits of the cortex and limbic systems of individuals with schizophrenia. Consis- 
tent with such research, people with schizophrenia and those without the disorder 
show different brain functioning (see Figure 10; Bartzokis et al., 2003; Reichenberg & 
Harvey, 2007; Reichenberg et al., 2009). 

Further evidence for the importance of biological factors shows that when people 
with schizophrenia hear voices during hallucinations, the parts of the brain respon- 
sible for hearing and language processing become active. When they have visual 
hallucinations, the parts of the brain involved in movement and color are active. At 
the same time, people with schizophrenia often have unusually low activity in the 
brain’s frontal lobes—the parts of the brain involved with emotional regulation, 
insight, and the evaluation of sensory stimuli (Stern & Silbersweig, 2001). 


527 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


528 Chapter 15 Psychological Disorders 


Brain 


Changes with Schizophrenia 





FIGURE 10 Changes in the brain have been found in people with schizophrenia. In an MRI 
reconstruction of the brain of a person with schizophrenia (a), the hippocampi (yellow) are 
shrunken, and the ventricles (gray) are enlarged and fluid-filled. In contrast, an MRI recon- 
struction of the brain of a person without the disorder (b) is structurally different with larger 
hippocampi and smaller ventricles. (Source: N.C. Andreasen, University of lowa.) 





(a) (b) 


ENVIRONMENTAL PERSPECTIVES ON SCHIZOPHRENIA 


Although biological factors provide important pieces of the puzzle of schizophrenia, 
we still need to consider past and current experiences in the environments of people 
who develop the disturbance. For instance, psychoanalytic approaches suggest that 
schizophrenia is a form of regression to earlier experiences and stages of life. Freud 
believed that people with schizophrenia lack egos that are strong enough to cope 
with their unacceptable impulses. They regress to the oral stage—a time when the 
id and ego are not yet separated. Therefore, individuals with schizophrenia essen- 
tially lack an ego and act out impulses without concern for reality. 

Although this reasoning is theoretically plausible, little evidence supports psy- 
choanalytic explanations. Somewhat more convincing theories look toward the emo- 
tional and communication patterns of the families of people with schizophrenia. For 
instance, some researchers suggest that schizophrenia results from high levels of 
expressed emotion. Expressed emotion is an interaction style characterized by family 
members’ criticism, hostility, and emotional intrusiveness. Other researchers suggest 
that faulty communication patterns lie at the heart of schizophrenia (Miklowitz & 
Thompson, 2003; Lobban, Barrowclough, & Jones, 2006). 

Psychologists who take a cognitive perspective on schizophrenia suggest that the 
problems in thinking that people with the disorder experience point to a cognitive 
cause. Some suggest that schizophrenia results from overattention to stimuli in the 
environment. Rather than being able to screen out unimportant or inconsequential 
stimuli and focus on the most important things in the environment, people with 
schizophrenia may be excessively receptive to virtually everything in their environ- 
ment. As a consequence, their information-processing capabilities become overloaded 
and eventually break down. Other cognitive experts argue that schizophrenia results 
from underattention to certain stimuli. According to this explanation, people with 
schizophrenia fail to focus sufficiently on important stimuli and pay attention to 
other, less important information in their surroundings (Cadenhead & Braff, 1995). 

Although it is plausible that overattention and underattention are related to dif- 
ferent forms of schizophrenia, these phenomena do not explain the origins of such 
information-processing disorders. Consequently, cognitive approaches—like other 
environmental explanations—do not provide a full explanation of the disorder. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 47 The Major Psychological Disorders 529 


THE MULTIPLE CAUSES OF SCHIZOPHRENIA 


The predominant approach used to explain the onset of schizophrenia today, the 
predisposition model of schizophrenia, incorporates a number of biological and environ- 
mental factors. This model suggests that individuals may inherit a predisposition or 
an inborn sensitivity to schizophrenia that makes them particularly vulnerable to 
stressful factors in the environment, such as social rejection or dysfunctional family 
communication patterns. The stressors may vary, but if they are strong enough and 
are coupled with a genetic predisposition, the result will be the onset of schizophre- 
nia. Similarly, a strong genetic predisposition may lead to the onset of schizophrenia 
even when the environmental stressors are relatively weak. 

In short, the models used today associate schizophrenia with several kinds of 
biological and environmental factors. It is increasingly clear, then, that no single fac- 
tor but a combination of interrelated variables produces schizophrenia (Meltzer, 2000; 
McDonald & Murray, 2004; Opler et al., 2008). 


Personality Disorders 





I had always wanted lots of things; as a child I can remember wanting a bullet that a 
friend of mine had brought in to show the class. I took it and put it into my school 
bag and when my friend noticed it was missing, I was the one who stayed after 
school with him and searched the room, and I was the one who sat with him and 
bitched about the other kids and how one of them took his bullet. I even went home 
with him to help him break the news to his uncle, who had brought it home from 
the war for him. 


But that was petty compared with the stuff I did later. I wanted a Ph.D. very badly, 
but I didn’t want to work very hard—just enough to get by. I never did the experiments 
I reported; hell, I was smart enough to make up the results. I knew enough about 
statistics to make anything look plausible. I got my master’s degree without even 
spending one hour in a laboratory. I mean, the professors believed anything. I’d stay 
out all night drinking and being with my friends, and the next day I’d get in just before 
them and tell ‘em I’d been in the lab all night. They'd actually feel sorry for me. (Duke 
& Nowicki, 1979, pp. 309-310) 


This excerpt provides a graphic first-person account of a person with a personality 
disorder. A personality disorder is characterized by a set of inflexible, maladaptive 
behavior patterns that keep a person from functioning appropriately in society. 
Personality disorders differ from the other problems we have discussed because 
those affected by them often have little sense of personal distress associated with 
the psychological maladjustment. In fact, people with personality disorders fre- 
quently lead seemingly normal lives. However, just below the surface lies a set of 
inflexible, maladaptive personality traits that do not permit these individuals to 
function as members of society (Davis & Millon, 1999; Clarkin & Lenzenweger, 
2004; Friedman, Oltmanns, & Turkheimer, 2007). 

The best-known type of personality disorder, illustrated by the case above, is the 
antisocial personality disorder (sometimes referred to as a sociopathic personality). 
Individuals with this disturbance show no regard for the moral and ethical rules of 
society or the rights of others. Although they can appear quite intelligent and likable 
(at least at first), upon closer examination they turn out to be manipulative and 
deceptive. Moreover, they lack any guilt or anxiety about their wrongdoing. When 
those with antisocial personality disorder behave in a way that injures someone else, 
they understand intellectually that they have caused harm but feel no remorse (Lyk- 
ken, 1995; Goodwin & Hamilton, 2003; Hilarski, 2007). 

People with antisocial personality disorder are often impulsive and lack the abil- 
ity to withstand frustration. They can be extremely manipulative. They also may have 


y Alert 


Remember that the multiple 
causes of schizophrenia 
include biological and 
environmental factors. 


personality disorder A disorder 
characterized by a set of inflexible, 
maladaptive behavior patterns that 
keep a person from functioning 
appropriately in society. 


antisocial personality disorder 

A disorder in which individuals show 
no regard for the moral and ethical 
rules of society or the rights of others. 






y Alert 


Unlike most psychological 
disorders, personality dis- 
orders produce little or 

no personal distress. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


530 Chapter 15 Psychological Disorders 


borderline personality disorder 

A disorder in which individuals have 
difficulty developing a secure sense 
of who they are. 


narcissistic personality disorder 
A personality disturbance charac- 
terized by an exaggerated sense of 
self-importance. 


attention-deficit hyperactivity 
disorder (ADHD) A disorder marked 
by inattention, impulsiveness, a low 
tolerance for frustration, and a great 
deal of inappropriate activity. 


excellent social skills; they can be charming, engaging, and highly persuasive. Some 
of the best con artists have antisocial personalities. 

What causes such an unusual constellation of problem behaviors? A variety of 
factors have been suggested ranging from an inability to experience emotions appro- 
priately to problems in family relationships. For example, in many cases of antisocial 
behavior, the individual has come from a home in which a parent has died or left or 
one in which there is a lack of affection, a lack of consistency in discipline, or outright 
rejection. Other explanations concentrate on sociocultural factors because an unusu- 
ally high proportion of people with antisocial personalities come from lower socio- 
economic groups. Still, no one has been able to pinpoint the specific causes of 
antisocial personalities, and it is likely that some combination of factors is respon- 
sible (Nigg & Goldsmith, 1994; Rosenstein & Horowitz, 1996; Costa & Widiger, 2002). 

People with borderline personality disorder have difficulty developing a secure 
sense of who they are. As a consequence, they tend to rely on relationships with 
others to define their identity. The problem with this strategy is that rejections are 
devastating. Furthermore, people with this disorder distrust others and have diffi- 
culty controlling their anger. Their emotional volatility leads to impulsive and self- 
destructive behavior. Individuals with borderline personality disorder often feel 
empty and alone, and they have difficulty cooperating with others. They may form 
intense, sudden, one-sided relationships in which they demand the attention of 
another person and then feel angry when they don’t receive it. One reason for this 
behavior is that they may have a background in which others discounted or criti- 
cized their emotional reactions, and they may not have learned to regulate their 
emotions effectively (Links, Eynan, & Heisel, 2007; King-Casas et al., 2008; Hopwood 
et al., 2009). 

Another example of a personality disturbance is the narcissistic personality 
disorder, which is characterized by an exaggerated sense of self-importance. Those 
with the disorder expect special treatment from others while at the same time disre- 
garding others’ feelings. In some ways, in fact, the main attribute of the narcissistic 
personality is an inability to experience empathy for other people. 

There are several other categories of personality disorder that range in severity 
from individuals who may simply be regarded by others as eccentric, obnoxious, or 
difficult to people who act in a manner that is criminal and dangerous to others. 
Although they are not out of touch with reality like people with schizophrenia, peo- 
ple with personality disorders lead lives that put them on the fringes of society 
(Millon, Davis, & Millon, 2000; Trull & Widiger, 2003). 


Childhood Disorders 


We typically view childhood as a time of innocence and relative freedom from stress. 
In reality, though, almost 20% of children and 40% of adolescents experience sig- 
nificant emotional or behavioral disorders (Romano et al., 2001; Broidy, Nagin, & 
Tremblay, 2003; Nolen-Hoeksema, 2007). 

For example, although major depression is more prevalent in adults, around 2.5% 
of children and more than 8% of adolescents suffer from the disorder. In fact, by the 
time they reach age 20, between 15-20% of children and adolescents will experience 
an episode of major depression (Garber & Horowitz, 2002). 

Children do not always display depression the same way adults do. Rather than 
showing profound sadness or hopelessness, childhood depression may produce the 
expression of exaggerated fears, clinginess, or avoidance of everyday activities. In 
older children, the symptoms may be sulking, school problems, and even acts of 
delinquency (Wenar, 1994; Seroczynski, Jacquez, & Cole, 2003; Koplewicz, 2002). 

A considerably more common childhood disorder is attention-deficit hyperactivity 
disorder, or ADHD, a disorder marked by inattention, impulsiveness, a low tolerance 
for frustration, and generally a great deal of inappropriate activity. Although all children 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 47 The Major Psychological Disorders 531 


show such behavior some of the time, it is so common in children diagnosed with 
ADHD that it interferes with their everyday functioning (Barkley, 2005; Swanson, Harris, 
& Graham, 2003; Smith, Barkley, & Shapiro, 2006). 

ADHD is surprisingly widespread with estimates ranging between 3-5% of the 
school-age population—or some 3.5 million children under the age of 18 in the 
United States. Children diagnosed with the disorder are often exhausting to parents 
and teachers, and even their peers find them difficult to deal with. 

The cause of ADHD is not known, although most experts feel that it is produced 
by dysfunctions in the nervous system. For example, one theory suggests that 
unusually low levels of arousal in the central nervous system cause ADHD. To 
compensate, children with ADHD seek out stimulation to increase arousal. Still, 
such theories are speculative. Furthermore, because many children occasionally 
show behaviors characteristic of ADHD, it often is misdiagnosed or in some cases 
overdiagnosed. Only the frequency and persistence of the symptoms of ADHD 
allow for a correct diagnosis, which only a trained professional can do (Barkley, 
2000; Sciutto & Eisenberg, 2007). 

Autism, a severe developmental disability that impairs children’s ability to com- 
municate and relate to others, is another childhood disorder that usually appears in 
the first three years and typically continues throughout life. Children with autism 
have difficulties in both verbal and nonverbal communication, and they may avoid 
social contact. About one in 110 children are now thought to have the disorder, and 
its prevalence has risen significantly in the last decade. Whether the increase is the 
result of an actual rise in the incidence of autism or is due to better reporting is a 
question of intense debate among researchers (Rice, 2009). 


Other Disorders 


It’s important to keep in mind that the various forms of psychological disorders 
described in DSM-IV-TR cover much more ground than we have been able to discuss 
in this module. Some relate to topics previously considered in other chapters. For 
example, psychoactive substance use disorder relates to problems that arise from the use 
and abuse of drugs. Furthermore, alcohol use disorders are among the most serious 
and widespread problems. Both psychoactive substance use disorder and alcohol use 
disorder co-occur with many other psychological disorders such as mood disorders, 
posttraumatic stress disorder, and schizophrenia, which complicates treatment con- 
siderably (Salgado, Quinlan, & Zlotnick, 2007). 

Another widespread problem is eating disorders. They include such disorders as 
anorexia nervosa and bulimia, which we considered in the chapter on motivation and 
emotion, as well as binge-eating disorder, characterized by binge eating without behav- 
iors designed to prevent weight gain. Finally, sexual disorders, in which one’s sexual 
activity is unsatisfactory, are another important class of problems. They include sex- 
ual desire disorders, sexual arousal disorders, and paraphilias, atypical sexual activities 
that may include nonhuman objects or nonconsenting partners. 

Another important class of disorders is organic mental disorders, some of which 
we touched on previously. These are problems that have a purely biological basis, 
such as Alzheimer’s disease and some types of mental retardation. Remember, there 
are other disorders that we have not mentioned at all, and each of the classes we 
have discussed can be divided into several subcategories (Kopelman & Fleminger, 
2002; Pratt et al., 2003; Reijonen et al., 2003). 





> 
4 
a 


ay ` 4 





In the most severe cases of autism, 
children display self-injurious behavior 
and must wear protective head gear. 


autism A severe developmental 
disability that impairs children’s 
ability to communicate and relate 
to others. 


532 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 15 Psychological Disorders 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 


What are the major psychological disorders? 


Anxiety disorders are present when a person experiences 
so much anxiety that it affects daily functioning. Specific 
types of anxiety disorders include phobic disorder, panic 
disorder, generalized anxiety disorder, and obsessive- 
compulsive disorder. (p. 513) 

Somatoform disorders are psychological difficulties that 
take on a physical (somatic) form but for which there is 
no medical cause. Examples are hypochondriasis and 
conversion disorders. (p. 518) 

Dissociative disorders are marked by the separation, or 
dissociation, of different facets of a person’s personality 
that are usually integrated. Major kinds of dissociative 
disorders include dissociative identity disorder, dissocia- 
tive amnesia, and dissociative fugue. (p. 518) 

Mood disorders are characterized by emotional states of 
depression or euphoria so strong that they intrude on 
everyday living. They include major depression and 
bipolar disorder. (p. 520) 

Schizophrenia is one of the more severe forms of mental 
illness. Symptoms of schizophrenia include declines in 
functioning, thought and language disturbances, percep- 
tual disorders, emotional disturbance, and withdrawal 
from others. (p. 524) 

Strong evidence links schizophrenia to genetic, bio- 
chemical, and environmental factors. According to the 
predisposition model, an interaction among various 
factors produces the disorder. (p. 529) 

People with personality disorders experience little or no 
personal distress, but they do suffer from an inability to 
function as normal members of society. These disorders 
include antisocial personality disorder, borderline per- 
sonality disorder, and narcissistic personality disorder. 
(p. 529) 

Childhood disorders include major depression, attention- 
deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), and autism. 

(p. 530) 


EVALUATE 


1. Kathy is terrified of elevators. She could be suffering from 


a(n) 

a. Obsessive-compulsive disorder 
b. Phobic disorder 

c. Panic disorder 

d. Generalized anxiety disorder 


2. Carmen described an incident in which her anxiety sud- 
denly rose to a peak, and she felt a sense of impending 
doom. Carmen experienced a(n) : 

3. Troubling thoughts that persist for weeks or months are 
known as 
a. Obsessions 
b. Compulsions 
c. Rituals 
d. Panic attacks 

4. An overpowering urge to carry out a strange ritual is 
called a(n) —___. 

5. The separation of the personality, which provides escape 
from stressful situations, is the key factor in 
disorders. 

6. States of extreme euphoria and energy paired with severe 
depression characterize _____ disorder. 

T _— schizophrenia is characterized by symptoms 
that are sudden and of easily identifiable onset; 

schizophrenia develops gradually over a per- 
son’s life span. 

8. The states that schizophrenia may 
be caused by an excess of certain neurotransmitters in the 
brain. 








RETHINK 


1. What cultural factors might contribute to the rate of anxi- 
ety disorders found in a culture? How might the experi- 
ence of anxiety differ among people of different cultures? 

2. From the perspective of a social worker: Personality disorders 
are often not apparent to others, and many people with 
these problems seem to live basically normal lives and are 
not a threat to others. Because these people often appear 
from the outside to function well in society, why should 
they be considered psychologically disordered? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 
stsoyjoddy əurwedop ‘g ‘ssaooid SAPLI *Z 
‘rejodiq *g ‘aayeossip ‘g tuorsmdwo ‘p e'g Spee oued ‘z ‘qr 


KEY TERMS 


anxiety disorder p. 513 
phobias p. 514 
panic disorder p. 514 
generalized anxiety 
disorder p. 515 
obsessive-compulsive 
disorder (OCD) p. 515 
obsession p. 515 
compulsion p. 516 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


somatoform 

disorders p. 518 
hypochondriasis p. 518 
conversion disorder p. 518 
dissociative 

disorders p. 519 
dissociative identity 

disorder (DID) p. 519 
dissociative amnesia p. 520 


Module 47 The Major Psychological Disorders 533 


dissociative fugue p. 520 
mood disorder p. 520 
major depression p. 521 
mania p. 522 
bipolar disorder p. 522 
schizophrenia p. 524 
personality disorder p. 529 
antisocial personality 
disorder p. 529 


borderline personality 
disorder p. 530 
natcissistic personality 
disorder p. 530 
attention-deficit 
hyperactivity disorder 
(ADHD) p. 530 
autism p. 531 


How prevalent are psycho- 
logical disorders? 


What indicators signal a need 
for the help of a mental 
health practitioner? 


> Study Alert 


Remember that the incidence 
of various types of psycho- 
logical disorders in the 
general population is 
surprisingly high. 


534 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


How common are the kinds of psychological disorders we’ve been discussing? Here’s 
one answer: Every second person you meet in the United States is likely to suffer at 
some point during his or her life from a psychological disorder. 

That’s the conclusion drawn from a massive study on the prevalence of psycho- 
logical disorders. In that study, researchers conducted face-to-face interviews with 
more than 8,000 men and women between the ages of 15 and 54. The sample was 
designed to be representative of the population of the United States. According to 
results of the study, 48% of those interviewed had experienced a disorder at some 
point in their lives. In addition, 30% experienced a disorder in any particular year, 
and the number of people who experienced simultaneous multiple disorders (known 
as comorbidity) was significant (Welkowitz et al., 2000; Merikangas et al., 2007; Kessler 
& Wang, 2008). 

The most common disorder reported in the study was depression; 17% of those 
surveyed reported at least one major episode. Ten percent had suffered from depres- 
sion during the current year. The next most common disorder was alcohol depen- 
dence, which occurred at a lifetime incidence rate of 14%. In addition, 7% of those 
interviewed had experienced alcohol dependence in the last year. Other frequently 
occurring psychological disorders were drug dependence, disorders involving panic 
(such as an overwhelming fear of talking to strangers and terror of heights), and 
posttraumatic stress disorder. 

Although some researchers think the estimates of severe disorders may be too 
high (Narrow et al., 2002), the national findings are consistent with studies of college 
students and their psychological difficulties. For example, in one study of the prob- 
lems of students who visited a college counseling center, more than 40% of students 
reported being depressed (see Figure 1). These figures include only students who 
sought help from the counseling center and not those who did not seek treatment. 
Consequently, the figures are not representative of the entire college population 
(Benton et al., 2003; also see Applying Psychology in the 21st Century on page 537). 

The significant level of psychological disorders is a problem not only in the 
United States; according to the World Health Organization, mental health difficulties 
are also a global concern. Throughout the world, psychological disorders are wide- 
spread. Furthermore, there are economic disparities in treatment; more affluent peo- 
ple with mild disorders receive more and better treatment than poor people who 
have more severe disorders. In fact, psychological disorders make up 14% of global 
illness, and 90% of people in developing countries receive no care at all for their 
disorders (see Figure 2 on page 536; The WHO World Mental Health Survey Con- 
sortium, 2004; Jacob et al., 2007; Wang et al., 2007). 

Also, keep in mind that the incidence of specific disorders varies significantly in 
other cultures. For instance, cross-cultural surveys show that the incidence of major 
depression varies significantly from one culture to another. The probability of having 
at least one episode of depression is only 1.5% in Taiwan and 2.9% in Korea com- 
pared with 11.6% in New Zealand and 16.4% in France. Such notable differences 
underscore the importance of considering the cultural context of psychological dis- 
orders (Weissman et al., 1997; Tseng, 2003). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 48 Psychological Disorders in Perspective 535 


Stress anxiety 
Situational 
Relationship 
Family issues 
Developmental 
Depression 
Academic skills 


Medication use 






Educational/vocational 
Physical problems 
Abuse 

Grief 

Suicidal 

Personality disorders 
Substance abuse 

Eating disorders 
Chronic mental disorder 
Sexual assault 


Legal 








o 
Ll | | 


20 40 60 80 


Percentage of students reporting problem 


The Social and Cultural Context 
of Psychological Disorders 


In considering the nature of the psychological disorders described in DSM-IV-TR, it’s 
important to keep in mind that the specific disorders reflect Western cultures at the 
turn of the 21st century. The classification system provides a snapshot of how its 
authors viewed mental disorder when it was published. In fact, the development of 
the most recent version of the DSM was a source of great debate, which in part 
reflects issues that divide society. 

For example, two disorders caused particular controversy during the revision 
process. One, known as self-defeating personality disorder, was ultimately removed from 
the appendix, where it had appeared in the previous revision. The term self-defeating 
personality disorder had been applied to cases in which people who were treated 
unpleasantly or demeaningly in relationships neither left nor took other action. It 
was typically used to describe people who remained in abusive relationships. 

Although some clinicians argued that it was a valid category they observed in 
clinical practice, the disorder lacked enough research evidence to support its designation 





FIGURE 1 The problems reported by 
students visiting a college counseling 
center. Would you have predicted this 
pattern of psychological difficulties? 
(Source: Benton et al., 2003.) 


y Alert 


It is important to understand 
that the DSM is a living doc- 
ument that presents a 
view of disorders that re- 
flects the culture and his- 
torical context of its authors. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


536 Chapter 15 Psychological Disorders 


Developed Countries 











0 5 10 15 20 25 30 


I United States [| Netherlands I Spain E Japan 
E France I Belgium E Germany |) Italy 


Less-Developed Countries 








0 


5 10 15 20 25 30 


E Lebanon E Ukraine I Colombia I China-Shanghai 

|] China—Beijing I Mexico Mi Nigeria 
FIGURE 2 According to a global survey conducted by the World Health Organization, the 
prevalence of psychological disorders is widespread. These figures show the prevalence of any 
psychological disorder within the last 12 months. (Source: The WHO World Mental Health Survey 
Consortium, 2004, Table 3.) 


as a disorder in DSM. Furthermore, some critics complained that use of the label 
had the effect of condemning targets of abuse for their plight—a blame-the-victim 
phenomenon—and as a result, the category was removed from the manual. 

A second and even more controversial category was premenstrual dysphoric disor- 
der, which is characterized by severe, incapacitating mood changes or depression 
related to a woman’s menstrual cycle. Some critics argued that the classification sim- 
ply labels normal female behavior as a disorder. Former U.S. Surgeon General Antonia 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Students Stressed 
and Depressed 


The years spent in college are often thought 
to be a carefree time filled with growth, in- 
tellectual stimulation, and socializing. But 
a recent survey shows that the opposite 
may be true: 85% of college students re- 
ported feeling stressed in their daily lives 
and cited such stress factors as grades, 
school work, finances, family issues, and 
relationships (Associated Press, 2009). 

More troubling is the fact that many stu- 
dents reported mood problems. Forty-two 
percent said they had felt down, depressed, 
or hopeless for at least several days during 
the prior two weeks. More than half re- 
ported feeling tired or having sleep prob- 
lems. More than a third said they felt bad 
about themselves or that they felt like a fail- 
ure, and 38% reported having little interest 
or pleasure in doing things. Eleven percent 
said they had recently had thoughts that 
they would be better off dead or that they 
wanted to hurt themselves in some way. 

In all, 9% of college students were 
judged to be at risk of at least moderate de- 
pression. Having a parent who had lost a 
job during the school year seemed to be an 
important risk factor: It doubled the 
chances that a student would be at risk for 
at least mild depression and would report 
thoughts of being better off dead. 

Another poll found that 55% of under- 
graduate students had thoughts of suicide 
at least one time, and 18% had seriously 
considered it. In addition, the severity of 
mental health issues among students at- 
tending college has been on the rise. One 
reason is that more people with relatively 
severe psychological disorders are now at- 





In spite of the high percentage of college students reporting mood disorders, few take 


advantage of available mental health resources. 


tending college as a result of better treat- 
ment options (Jayson, 2008). 

Despite their stress and depression, sur- 
prisingly few students report taking advan- 
tage of the mental health resources that are 
available to them. Of those who showed at 
least moderate symptoms of depression, 
only a third sought counseling, while fewer 
than half of those who had suicidal thoughts 
had done so. Many hadn’t even familiarized 
themselves with the mental health resources 
that were available on campus. 





Part of the problem is that students are 
reluctant to use counseling services per- 
haps because they tend to believe that their 
problems just aren’t serious enough to war- 
rant doing so. The survey found that while 
84% of students reported having a source of 
support that they could rely on if they were 
feeling bad or had suicidal thoughts, most 
looked to friends or family. Only 20% said 
that they would consider campus counsel- 
ing resources (Laven, 2008; Associated 
Press, 2009; Benac & Tompson, 2009). 





e Do you think college students might be at particular risk for developing a psycho- 


logical disorder? Why? 


e Why might having a parent lose a job during the school year lead to an increased 


risk for depression? 








Novello suggested that what “in women is called PMS [premenstrual syndrome, a 
similar classification] in men is called healthy aggression and initiative” (Cotton, 
1993, p. 270). Advocates for including the disorder prevailed, however, and “premen- 
strual dysphoric disorder” appears in the appendix of DSM-IV-TR (Hartung & Widiger, 
1998; Pearlstein & Steiner, 2008). 

Such controversies underline the fact that our understanding of abnormal behav- 
ior reflects the society and culture in which we live. Future revisions of DSM may 
include a different catalog of disorders. Even now, other cultures might include a list 
of disorders that are very different from the list that appears in the current DSM, as 
we discuss next. 


537 


538 


16 
P 
A 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 15 Psychological Disorders 


7 
F 
7 7 


3 





Exploring 


DSM and Culture— 
and the Culture of DSM 


In most people’s estimation, a person who hears voices of the 

DIVERSITY recently deceased is probably a victim of a psychological 
disturbance. Yet some Plains Indians routinely hear the voices 
of the dead calling to them from the afterlife. 

This is only one example of the role of culture in labeling 
behavior as “abnormal.” In fact, among all the major adult 
disorders included in the DSM categorization, only four are found 

across all cultures of the world: schizophrenia, bipolar disorder, major depression, and 
anxiety disorders. The rest are prevalent primarily in North America and Western Europe 
(Kleinman, 1996; Cohen, Slomkowski, & Robins, 1999; López & Guarnaccia, 2000). 

For instance, take anorexia nervosa, the disorder in which people develop inaccurate 
views of their body appearance, become obsessed with their weight, and refuse to eat, 
sometimes starving to death in the process. This disorder typically occurs only in 
cultures that hold the societal standard that slender female bodies are the most 
desirable. In most of the world, where such a standard does not exist, anorexia nervosa 
is rare. Until recently, there was little anorexia nervosa in Asia with some exceptions, 
such as the upper and upper-middle classes of Japan and Hong Kong, where Western 
influence is greatest (Watters, 2010). 

Similarly, dissociative identity (multiple personality) disorder makes sense as a 
problem only in societies in which a sense of self is fairly concrete. In India, the self is 
based more on external factors that are relatively independent of the person. There, 
when an individual displays symptoms of what people in a Western society would 
call dissociative identity disorder, Indians assume that that person is possessed either 
by demons (which they view as a malady) or by gods (which does not require 
treatment). 

Furthermore, even though disorders such as schizophrenia are found throughout 
the world, cultural factors influence the specific symptoms of the disorder. Hence, 
catatonic schizophrenia in which unmoving patients appear to be frozen in the same 
position (sometimes for days), is rare in North America and Western Europe. In 
contrast, in India, 80% of those with schizophrenia are catatonic. 

Other cultures have disorders that do not appear in the West. For example, in 
Malaysia, a behavior called amok is characterized by a wild outburst in which a usually 
quiet and withdrawn person kills or severely injures another. Koro is a condition found 
in Southeast Asian males who develop an intense panic that the penis is about to 
withdraw into the abdomen. Some West African men develop a disorder when they 
first attend college that they call “brain fag”; it includes feelings of heaviness or heat in 
the head as well as depression and anxiety. Finally, ataque de nervios is a disorder found 
most often among Latinos from the Caribbean. It is characterized by trembling, crying, 
uncontrollable screams, and incidents of verbal or physical aggression (Cohen, 
Slomkowski, & Robins, 1999; Lopez & Guarnaccia, 2000; Adams, 2007). 

Explanations for psychological disorders also differ among cultures. For example, 
in China, psychological disorders are commonly viewed as a weakness of the heart, a 
concept that derives from thousands of years of traditional Chinese medicine. Many 
terms used to describe emotions and symptoms of psychological disorders make direct 
reference to the heart—but the association isn’t simply a metaphorical one. Chinese 
people are more likely than people in Western cultures to express their emotional 
anguish in terms of physical symptoms such as heart pain, “heart panic,” or “heart 
vexed.” They may also see their emotional pain as merely a side effect of some 
underlying physical cause or even focus more on the effects that their symptoms are 
having on their relationships with friends and family members (Miller, 2006a; Lee, 
Kleinman, & Kleinman, 2007; Watters, 2010). 

Insum, we should not assume that the DSM provides the final word on psychological 
disorders. The disorders it includes are very much a creation and function of Western 
cultures at a particular moment in time, and its categories should not be seen as 
universally applicable (Tseng, 2003). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 48 Psychological Disorders in Perspective 


After you've considered the range and variety of psychological BECOMING AN 


disturbances that can afflict people, you may begin to feel that INFORMED CONSUMER 


you suffer from one (or more) of the problems we have 


discussed. In fact, this perception has a name: medical student's of Psyc h O | og y 


disease. Although in this case it might more aptly be labeled Deciding When You Need Help 


“psychology student’s disease,” the basic symptoms are the 
same: feeling that you suffer from the same sorts of problems 
you are studying. 

Most often, of course, your concerns will be unwarranted. As we have discussed, 
the differences between normal and abnormal behavior are often so fuzzy that it is easy 
to jump to the conclusion that you might have the same symptoms that are involved in 
serious forms of mental disturbance. 

Before coming to such a conclusion, though, keep in mind that from time to time we 
all experience a wide range of emotions, and it is not unusual to feel deeply unhappy, 
fantasize about bizarre situations, or feel anxiety about life’s circumstances. It is the 
persistence, depth, and consistency of such behavior that set normal reactions apart 
from abnormal ones. If you have not previously had serious doubts about the normality 
of your behavior, it is unlikely that reading about others’ psychological disorders will 
prompt you to re-evaluate your earlier conclusion. 

On the other hand, many people do have problems that merit concern, and in such 
cases, it is important to consider the possibility that professional help is warranted. The 
following list of symptoms can serve as a guideline to help you determine whether 
outside intervention might be useful (Engler & Goleman, 1992): 


e Long-term feelings of distress that interfere with your sense of well-being, 
competence, and ability to function effectively in daily activities 

e Occasions in which you experience overwhelmingly high stress accompanied by 
feelings of inability to cope with the situation 

e Prolonged depression or feelings of hopelessness, especially when they do not 
have any clear cause (such as the death of someone close) 

e Withdrawal from other people 

e Thoughts of inflicting harm on oneself or suicide 

e A chronic physical problem for which no physical cause can be determined 

e A fear or phobia that prevents you from engaging in everyday activities 

e Feelings that other people are out to get you or are talking about and plotting 
against you 

e Inability to interact effectively with others, preventing the development of friend- 
ships and loving relationships 


This list offers a rough set of guidelines for determining when the normal problems of 
everyday living have escalated beyond your ability to deal with them by yourself. In 
such situations, the least reasonable approach would be to pore over the psychological 
disorders we have discussed in an attempt at self-diagnosis. A more reasonable strategy 
is to consider seeking professional help. 


539 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


540 Chapter 15 Psychological Disorders 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 3. Match the disorder with the culture in which it is most 
How prevalent are psychological disorders? eee ee 
e About half the people in the United States are likely to 5 A b. Noe 
experience a psychological disorder at some point in a brain fag oY A Sree 


their lives; 30% experience a disorder in any specific 


year. (p. 534) 4. catatonic schizophrenia d. West Africa 


What indicators signal a need for the help of a mental health 





a RETHINK 
practitioner? 

e The signals that indicate a need for professional help 1. Why is inclusion in the DSM-IV-TR of “borderline” 
include long-term feelings of psychological distress, disorders such as self-defeating personality disorder and 
feelings of inability to cope with stress, withdrawal from premenstrual dysphoric disorder so controversial and 
other people, thoughts of inflicting harm on oneself or political? What disadvantages does inclusion bring? Does 
suicide, prolonged feelings of hopelessness, chronic inclusion bring any benefits? 
physical problems with no apparent causes, phobias and 2. From the perspective of a college counselor: What indicators 
compulsions, paranoia, and an inability to interact with might be most important in determining whether a college 
others. (p. 539) student is experiencing a psychological disorder? Do you 

believe that all students who show signs of a psychologi- 
EVALUATE cal disorder should seek professional help? How might 
your responses change if the student were from a different 
1. The latest version of DSM is considered to be the culture (e.g., an African society)? 
conclusive guideline on defining psychological disorders. 
True or false? Answers to Evaluate Questions 
2 , characterized by e-p ‘p-g 0-Z ‘q-T ‘g ‘əp10os1p ooydsAp [enysusutaid ‘z 
severe, incapacitating mood changes or depression related ‘Ajo1D0s aprarp yey} sansst Suydayar red u ÁsIƏA0IJU00 yvar8 JO 


to a woman’s menstrual cycle, was eventually added to EENES P SEMA GTI OBS SE SEH E O MUGEN PCMH ea) 


the appendix of DSM-IV-TR despite controversy 
surrounding its inclusion. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Psychology on the Web 


1. On the web, research the insanity defense as it is used in U.S. courts of law by 
consulting at least two sources. Summarize your findings and evaluate them against 
the perspectives on psychological disorders. Are there differences between legal and 
psychological interpretations of “sanity”? If so, what are they? Do you think such 
differences are appropriate? 

2. Find information on the web about the controversy surrounding dissociative (or 
multiple) personality disorder. Summarize both sides of the controversy. Using your 
knowledge of psychology, state your opinion on the matter. 


E D | | OQ U e We've discussed some of the many types of psychologi- 


cal disorders to which people are prone, noted the 
difficulty psychologists and physicians have in clearly differentiating normal from 
abnormal behavior, and looked at some of the approaches mental health professionals 
have taken to explain and treat psychological disorders. We considered today’s most 
commonly used classification scheme, categorized in the DSM-IV-TR, and examined some 
of the more prevalent forms of psychological disorders. To gain a perspective on the 
topic of psychological disorders, we discussed the surprisingly broad incidence of 
psychological disorders in U.S. society and the cultural nature of such disorders. 

Turn back to the prologue that described the case of Lily. Using the knowledge 
you've gained about psychological disorders, consider the following questions. 





1. Lily was diagnosed as suffering from borderline personality disorder. What elements 
of her behavior seem to fit the description of the disorder? 

2. More generally, what are the symptoms of borderline personality disorder? 

3. Which perspective (i.e, medical, psychoanalytic, behavioral, cognitive, humanistic, or 
sociocultural) provides the most useful explanation for Lily's case, in your opinion? 
Why? 

4. What advantages might there be in using multiple perspectives to address and 
explain Lily’s case? 





541 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Key Concepts for Chapter 16 


MODULE 49 








What are the goals of psychologically and Psychotherapy: Psychodynamic, 
Behavioral, and Cognitive 
Approaches to Treatment 


Psychodynamic Approaches to Therapy 


biologically based treatment approaches? 


@ What are the psychodynamic, behavioral, 
Behavioral Approaches to Therapy 


and cognitive approaches to treatment? Neuroséience in Your tite: How 
Behavioral Therapy Changes Your Brain 


Cognitive Approaches to Therapy 


MODULE 50 





What are the humanistic approaches to Psychotherapy: Humanistic, 

. Interpersonal, and Group 
treatment? @ What is interpersonal therapy? Approaches to Treatment 
Humanistic Therapy 


@ How does group therapy differ from individual 
PsychWork: Case Manager and 


types of therapy? @ How effective is psychotherapy, Substance Abuse Counselor 
nterpersonal Therapy 


and which kind of psychotherapy works best in a AR are ee 
given situation? and Self-Help Groups 
Evaluating Psychotherapy: 
Does Therapy Work? 


Exploring Diversity: Racial and Ethnic 


Factors in Treatment: Should 
Therapists Be Color Blind? 









MODULE 51 





How are drug, electroconvulsive, and Biomedical Therapy: Biological 


: . ; Approaches to Treatment 
psychosurgical techniques used today in the a ae 


treatment of psychological disorders? Electroconvulsive Therapy (ECT) 
Psychosurgery 

Biomedical Therapies in Perspective 
Community Psychology: 

Focus on Prevention 


Applying Psychology in the 21st Century: 
Beating the Odds: Preventing 
Psychological Disorders Before They Start 





Becoming an Informed 
Consumer of Psychology: 
Choosing the Right Therapist 


543 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Prologue Taking on Tourette’s 





Marg MackKrell was just three when her parents noticed the first 
signs of what turned out to be Tourette syndrome. The blond 
toddler began sniffing her fingers repeatedly, and over the next 
six years, her uncontrolled tics came to include clicking, whirring 
and scrunching her nose. Her-condition was manageable until 
last year, when, at the age of 9, she began to suffer about 60 
episodes a day of repeated head jerks.that left her sore and spent 
by nighttime. \ i 

So when MacKrell’s parents learned about an old but little- 
used therapy called habit-reversal training (HRT), they decided 

\ 


\ 
\ 





The therapy that helped young Marg Mackrell start to enjoy a 
more normal life again is just one of many that have revolution- 
ized the treatment of psychological disorders. Although treat- 
ment can take dozens of forms that range from one-meeting 
informal counseling sessions to long-term drug therapy, all the 
approaches have a common objective: the relief of psychological 
disorders with the ultimate aim of enabling individuals to achieve 
richer, more meaningful, and more fulfilling lives. 

Despite their diversity, approaches to treating psychological 
disorders fall into two main categories: psychologically based 
and biologically based therapies. Psychologically based therapy, 
or psychotherapy, is treatment in which a trained professional— 
a therapist—uses psychological techniques to help someone 
overcome psychological difficulties and disorders, resolve 
problems in living, or bring about personal growth. In psycho- 
therapy, the goal is to produce psychological change in a person 
(called a “client” or “patient”) through discussions and interac- 
tions with the therapist. In contrast, biomedical therapy relies 
on drugs and medical procedures to improve psychological 
functioning. 


544 


to try it. Last November, Marg started learning new ways to 
pre-empt her most severe tics at the Child and Family Study 
Center at Duke University Medical Center in Durham, N.C. When 
she felt a head jerk coming on, she was taught to drop her head 
and stare at the second hand on her watch for a minute. “Soon 
the head jerking was down by 90 percent,” says Marg’s mother, 
Diane MacKrell. “I couldn't believe it.’ (Skipp & Campo-Flores, 
2007, p. 53) 


Ahead 





As we describe the various approaches to therapy, keep in 
mind that although the distinctions may seem clear cut, the 
classifications and procedures overlap a good deal. In fact, many 
therapists today take an eclectic approach to therapy and use a 
variety of methods with an individual patient. Assuming that 
both psychological and biological processes often produce 
psychological disorders, eclectic therapists may draw from 
several perspectives simultaneously to address both the psycho- 
logical and the biological aspects of a person's problems (Goin, 
2005; Berman, Jobes, & Silverman, 2006). 


psychotherapy Treatment in which a trained professional—a 
therapist—uses psychological techniques to help a person overcome 
psychological difficulties and disorders, resolve problems in living, or 
bring about personal growth. 


biomedical therapy Therapy that relies on drugs and other medical 
procedures to improve psychological functioning. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 49 


Therapists use some 400 different varieties of psychotherapy, approaches to therapy 
that focus on psychological factors. Although diverse in many respects, all psycho- 
logical approaches see treatment as a way of solving psychological problems by 
modifying people’s behavior and helping them gain a better understanding of them- 
selves and their past, present, and future. 

In light of the variety of psychological approaches, it is not surprising that the 
people who provide therapy vary considerably in educational background and train- 
ing (see Figure 1). Many have doctoral degrees in psychology (that is, they have 
attended graduate school, learned clinical and research techniques, and held an 
internship). But therapy is also provided by people in fields allied with psychology, 
such as psychiatry and social work. 

Regardless of their specific training, almost all psychotherapists employ one of 
four major approaches to therapy: psychodynamic, behavioral, cognitive, and human- 
istic treatments. These approaches are based on the models of personality and psy- 
chological disorders developed by psychologists. Here we'll consider the 
psychodynamic, behavioral, and cognitive approaches in turn. In the next module, 
we'll explore the humanistic approach as well as interpersonal psychotherapy and 
group therapy, and we will evaluate the effectiveness of psychotherapy. 


Getting Help from the Right Person 


Clinical Psychologists 

Psychologists with a Ph.D. or Psy.D. who have also completed a postgraduate 
internship. They specialize in assessment and treatment of psychological difficulties, 
providing psychotherapy and, in some U.S. states, can prescribe drugs. 


Counseling Psychologists 
Psychologists with a Ph.D. or Ed.D. who typically treat day-to-day adjustment problems, 
often in a university mental health clinic. 


Psychiatrists 
M.D.s with postgraduate training in abnormal behavior. Because they can prescribe 
medication, they often treat the most severe disorders. 


Psychoanalysts 
Either M.D.s or psychologists who specialize in psychoanalysis, the treatment technique 
first developed by Freud. 


Licensed Professional Counselors or Clinical Mental Health Counselors 
Professionals with a master’s degree who provide therapy to individuals, couples, and 
families and who hold a national or state certification. 

Clinical or Psychiatric Social Workers 

Professionals with a master’s degree and specialized training who may provide therapy, 
usually regarding common family and personal problems. 


What are the goals of psycho- 
logically and biologically 
based treatment approaches? 


What are the psychodynamic, 
behavioral, and cognitive 
approaches to treatment? 


A variety of professionals 
provide therapy and counseling. Each 
could be expected to give helpful advice 
and direction. However, the nature of 
the problem a person is experiencing 
may make one or another therapy more 
appropriate. For example, a person who 
is suffering from a severe disturbance 
and who has lost touch with reality will 
typically require some sort of biologically 
based drug therapy. In that case, a 
psychiatrist—who is a physician—would 
be the professional of choice. In contrast, 
those suffering from milder disorders, 
such as difficulty adjusting to the death 
of a family member, have a broader 
choice that might include any of the 
professionals listed in the figure. 


545 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


546 Chapter 16 Treatment of Psychological Disorders 





























© Robert Mankoff/The New Yorker Collection/www. 


cartoonbank.com. 


“Look, call it denial if you like, but I think what goes on in 
my personal life is none of my own damn business.” 


psychodynamic therapy Therapy that 
seeks to bring unresolved past conflicts 
and unacceptable impulses from the 
unconscious into the conscious, where 
patients may deal with the problems 
more effectively. 


psychoanalysis Freudian psycho- 
therapy in which the goal is to release 
hidden unconscious thoughts and 
feelings in order to reduce their power 
in controlling behavior. 


y Alert 


To better understand how 
psychodynamic therapy 
works, review Freud's 

psychoanalytic theory 
discussed in the chapter 
on personality. 





Psychodynamic 
Approaches to 
Therapy 


Psychodynamic therapy seeks to bring unresolved past 
conflicts and unacceptable impulses from the unconscious 
into the conscious, where patients may deal with the prob- 
lems more effectively. Psychodynamic approaches are 
based on Freud’s psychoanalytic approach to personality, 
which holds that individuals employ defense mechanisms, 
psychological strategies to protect themselves from unac- 
ceptable unconscious impulses. 

The most common defense mechanism is repression, 
which pushes threatening conflicts and impulses back into the unconscious. However, 
since unacceptable conflicts and impulses can never be completely buried, some of the 
anxiety associated with them can produce abnormal behavior in the form of what 
Freud called neurotic symptoms. 

How do we rid ourselves of the anxiety produced by unconscious, unwanted 
impulses and drives? To Freud, the answer was to confront the conflicts and impulses 
by bringing them out of the unconscious part of the mind and into the conscious part. 
Freud assumed that this technique would reduce anxiety stemming from past conflicts 
and that the patient could then participate in his or her daily life more effectively. 

A psychodynamic therapist, then, faces the challenge of finding a way to assist 
patients’ attempts to explore and understand the unconscious. The technique that has 
evolved has a number of components, but basically it consists of guiding patients to 
consider and discuss their past experiences in explicit detail from the time of their first 
memories. This process assumes that patients will eventually stumble upon long-hidden 
crises, traumas, and conflicts that are producing anxiety in their adult lives. They will 
then be able to “work through”—understand and rectify—those difficulties. 





MANKOFF 


PSYCHOANALYSIS: FREUD’S THERAPY 


Classic Freudian psychodynamic therapy, called psychoanalysis, tends to be a lengthy 
and expensive affair. Psychoanalysis is Freudian psychotherapy in which the goal is 
to release hidden unconscious thoughts and feelings in order to reduce their power 
in controlling behavior. 

In psychoanalysis, patients may meet with a therapist with considerable frequency, 
sometimes as much as 50 minutes a day, four to five days a week, for several years. 
In their sessions, they often use a technique developed by Freud called free association. 
Psychoanalysts using this technique tell patients to say aloud whatever comes to mind, 
regardless of its apparent irrelevance or senselessness, and the analysts attempt to 
recognize and label the connections between what a patient says and the patient’s 
unconscious. Therapists also use dream interpretation, examining dreams to find clues 
to unconscious conflicts and problems. Moving beyond the surface description of a 
dream (called the manifest content), therapists seek its underlying meaning (the latent 
content), which thereby reveals the true unconscious meaning of the dream (Galatzer- 
Levy & Cohler, 1997; Auld, Hyman, & Rudzinski, 2005; Bodin, 2006). 

The processes of free association and dream interpretation do not always move 
forward easily. The same unconscious forces that initially produced repression may 
keep past difficulties out of the conscious mind, which produces resistance. Resistance 
is an inability or unwillingness to discuss or reveal particular memories, thoughts, 
or motivations. Patients can express resistance in many ways. For instance, they may 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 49 Psychotherapy: Psychodynamic, Behavioral, and Cognitive Approaches to Treatment 547 


be discussing a childhood memory and suddenly forget what they were saying, or 
they may change the subject completely. It is the therapist’s job to pick up instances 
of resistance and interpret their meaning as well as to ensure that patients return to 
the subject—which is likely to hold difficult or painful memories for the patients. 

Because of the close, almost intimate interaction between patient and psycho- 
analyst, the relationship between the two often becomes emotionally charged and 
takes on a complexity unlike most other relationships. Patients may eventually think 
of the analyst as a symbol of a significant other in their past, perhaps a parent or a 
lover, and apply some of their feelings for that person to the analyst—a phenomenon 
known as transference. Transference is the transfer to a psychoanalyst feelings of 
love or anger that had been originally directed to a patient’s parents or other author- 
ity figures (Van Beekum, 2005; Evans, 2007; Steiner, 2008). 

A therapist can use transference to help a patient recreate past relationships that 
were psychologically difficult. For instance, if a patient undergoing transference views 
her therapist as a symbol of her father—with whom she had a difficult relationship— 
the patient and therapist may “redo” an earlier interaction, this time including more 
positive aspects. Through this process, the patient may resolve conflicts regarding her 
real father—something that is beginning to happen in the following therapy session: 


Sandy: My father . . . never took any interest in any of us. . . . It was my 
mother—rest her soul—who loved us, not our father. He worked her to 
death. Lord, I miss her. . . . I must sound angry at my father. Don’t you 
think I have a right to be angry? 

Therapist: Do you think you have a right to be angry? 

Sandy: Of course, I do! Why are you questioning me? You don’t believe me, 
do you? 

Therapist: You want me to believe you. 

Sandy: I don’t care whether you believe me or not. . . . I know what you're 
thinking—you think I’m crazy—you must be laughing at me—I’ll probably 
be a case in your next book! You're just sitting there—smirking—making 
me feel like a bad person—thinking I’m wrong for being mad, that I have 
no right to be mad. 

Therapist: Just like your father. 

Sandy: Yes, you're just like my father—Oh my God! Just now—I—I—thought 
I was talking to him. (Sue, Sue, & Sue, 1990, pp. 514-515) 


CONTEMPORARY PSYCHODYNAMIC APPROACHES 


Few people have the time, money, or patience to participate in years 
of traditional psychoanalysis. Moreover, no conclusive evidence 
shows that psychoanalysis, as originally conceived by Freud in the 
19th century, works better than other, more recent forms of psycho- 
dynamic therapy. 

Today, psychodynamic therapy tends to be of shorter duration 
and usually lasts no longer than 3 months or 20 sessions. The thera- 
pist takes a more active role than Freud would have liked by control- 
ling the course of therapy and prodding and advising the patient with 
considerable directness. Finally, the therapist puts less emphasis on a 
patient’s past history and childhood and concentrates instead on an 
individual’s current relationships and specific complaints (Goode, 
2003; Charman, 2004; Wolitzky, 2006). 


EVALUATING PSYCHODYNAMIC THERAPY 








The close and intense relationship 
between therapist and patient may 
become highly complex. 


transference The transfer of feelings 
to a psychoanalyst of love or anger 
that had been originally directed to a 
patient’s parents or other authority 
figures. 


4 i C M A en 


© Michael Maslin/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


Even with its current modifications, psychodynamic therapy has its “And when did you first realize you weren't like 


critics. In its longer versions, it can be time consuming and expensive, 


other precipitation?” 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


548 Chapter 16 Treatment of Psychological Disorders 


especially in comparison with other forms of psychotherapy, such as behavioral and 
cognitive approaches. Furthermore, less articulate patients may not do as well as 
more articulate ones. 

Ultimately, the most important concern about psychodynamic treatment is 
whether it actually works, and there is no simple answer to this question. Psycho- 
dynamic treatment techniques have been controversial since Freud introduced them. 
Part of the problem is the difficulty in establishing whether patients have improved 
after psychodynamic therapy. Determining effectiveness depends on reports from the 
therapist or the patients themselves—reports that are obviously open to bias and 
subjective interpretation. 

Furthermore, critics have questioned the entire theoretical basis of psycho- 
dynamic theory; they maintain that constructs such as the unconscious have not been 
scientifically confirmed. Despite the criticism, though, the psychodynamic treatment 
approach has remained viable. For some people, it provides solutions to difficult 
psychological issues, provides effective treatment for psychological disturbance, and 
also permits the potential development of an unusual degree of insight into one’s 
life (Clay, 2000; Ablon & Jones, 2005; Bond, 2006). 


Behavioral Approaches 
to Therapy 


Perhaps, when you were a child, your parents rewarded you with an ice cream cone 
when you were especially good . . . or sent you to your room if you misbehaved. 
Sound principles back up such a child-rearing strategy: Good behavior is maintained 
by reinforcement, and unwanted behavior can be eliminated by punishment. 





behavioral treatment approaches These principles represent the basic underpinnings of behavioral treatment 
Treatment approaches that build on approaches. Building on the basic processes of learning, behavioral treatment 
the basic processes of learning, such as approaches make this fundamental assumption: Both abnormal behavior and normal 
reinforcement and extinction, and behavior are learned. People who act abnormally either have failed to learn the skills 
assume that normal and abnormal they need to cope with the problems of everyday living or have acquired faulty skills 
behavior are both learned. and patterns that are being maintained through some form of reinforcement. To 


modify abnormal behavior, then, proponents of behavioral 
approaches propose that people must learn new behavior to 
replace the faulty skills they have developed and unlearn their 
maladaptive behavior patterns (Krijn et al., 2004; Norton & 
Price, 2007). 

Behavioral psychologists do not need to delve into peo- 
ple’s pasts or their psyches. Rather than viewing abnormal 
behavior as a symptom of an underlying problem, they con- 
sider the abnormal behavior as the problem in need of mod- 
ification. The goal of therapy is to change people’s behavior 
to allow them to function more effectively, such as the treat- 
ment that was employed with Marg MacKrell, whose case was 

/ discussed in the chapter Prologue. In this view, then, there is 

Ai i no problem other than the maladaptive behavior itself; if you 

NN W can change that behavior, treatment is successful. 


r Keu 
Ni 


CLASSICAL CONDITIONING TREATMENTS 


Suppose you bite into your favorite candy bar and find that 
not only is it infested with ants, but you’ve also swallowed a 
Behavioral approaches to treatment would seek to modify the bunch of them. You immediately become sick to your stomach 
behavior of this couple rather than to focus on the underlying and throw up. Your long-term reaction? You never eat that 
causes of the behavior. kind of candy bar again, and it may be months before you eat 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 49 Psychotherapy: Psychodynamic, Behavioral, and Cognitive Approaches to Treatment 549 


any type of candy. You have learned through the basic process of classical condition- 
ing to avoid candy so that you will not get sick and throw up. 


Aversive Conditioning. This simple example illustrates how a person can be classi- 
cally conditioned to modify behavior. Behavior therapists use this principle when 
they employ aversive conditioning, a form of therapy that reduces the frequency of aversive conditioning A form of 
undesired behavior by pairing an aversive, unpleasant stimulus with undesired therapy that reduces the frequency of 
behavior. For example, behavior therapists might use aversive conditioning by pair- undesired behavior by pairing an 
ing alcohol with a drug that causes severe nausea and vomiting. After the two have aversive, unpleasant stimulus with 
been paired a few times, the person associates the alcohol alone with vomiting and undesired behavior. 
finds alcohol less appealing. 
Although aversion therapy works reasonably well in inhibiting substance-abuse 
problems such as alcoholism and certain kinds of sexual disorders, critics question 
its long-term effectiveness. Also, important ethical concerns surround aversion tech- 
niques that employ such potent stimuli as electric shock, which therapists use only 
in the most extreme cases, such as patient self-mutilation. Clearly, though, aversion 
therapy offers an important procedure for eliminating maladaptive responses for 
some period of time—a respite that provides, even if only temporarily, an opportu- 
nity to encourage more adaptive behavior patterns (Bordnick et al., 2004; Delgado, 
Labouliere, & Phelps, 2006). 


Systematic Desensitization. Another treatment that grew out of the classical condi- 

tioning is systematic desensitization. In systematic desensitization, gradual exposure systematic desensitization A behav- 

to an anxiety-producing stimulus is paired with relaxation to extinguish the response ioral technique in which gradual 

of anxiety (Pagoto, Kozak, & Spates, 2006; Choy, Fyer, & Lipsitz, 2007; Dowling, exposure to an anxiety-producing 

Jackson, & Thomas, 2008). stimulus is paired with relaxation to 
Suppose, for instance, you were extremely afraid of flying. The very thought extinguish the response of anxiety. 

of being in an airplane would make you begin to sweat and shake, and you 

couldn’t get yourself near enough to an airport to know how you'd react if you 

actually had to fly somewhere. Using systematic desensitization to treat your prob- 

lem, you would first be trained in relaxation techniques by a behavior therapist 

and learn to relax your body fully—a highly pleasant state, as you might imagine 

(see Figure 2). 


Step |. Pick a focus word or short phrase that's firmly rooted in your personal belief system. For 
example, a nonreligious individual might choose a neutral word like one or peace or love. 
A Christian person desiring to use a prayer could pick the opening words of Psalm 23. 
The Lord is my shepherd; a Jewish person could choose Shalom. 

Step 2. Sit quietly in a comfortable position. 

Step 3. Close your eyes. 

Step 4. Relax your muscles. 

Step 5. Breathe slowly and naturally, repeating your focus word or phrase silently as you exhale. 

Step 6. Throughout, assume a passive attitude. Don’t worry about how well you're doing. When 
other thoughts come to mind, simply say to yourself, “Oh, well,’ and gently return to the 
repetition. 

Step 7. Continue for 10 to 20 minutes. You may open your eyes to check the time, but do not 
use an alarm.When you finish, sit quietly for a minute or so, at first with your eyes closed 
and later with your eyes open. Then do not stand for one or two minutes. 





Step 8. Practice the technique once or twice a day. 


FIGURE 2 Following these basic steps will help you achieve a sense of calmness by 
employing the relaxation response. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


550 Chapter 16 Treatment of Psychological Disorders 


y Alert 


To help remember the 
concept of hierarchy of 
fears, think of something 
that you are afraid of and 
construct your own 
hierarchy of fears. 


exposure A behavioral treatment for 
anxiety in which people are confronted 
either suddenly or gradually with a 
stimulus that they fear. 


The next step would involve constructing a hierarchy of fears—a list in order of 
increasing severity of the things you associate with your fears. For instance, your 
hierarchy might resemble this one: 


. Watching a plane fly overhead 

. Going to an airport 

Buying a ticket 

. Stepping into the plane 

. Seeing the plane door close 

. Having the plane taxi down the runway 
. Taking off 

. Being in the air 


Once you had developed this hierarchy and learned relaxation techniques, you 
would learn to associate the two sets of responses. To do this, your therapist might 
ask you to put yourself into a relaxed state and then imagine yourself in the first 
situation identified in your hierarchy. Once you could consider that first step while 
remaining relaxed, you would move on to the next situation. Eventually you would 
move up the hierarchy in gradual stages until you could imagine yourself being in 
the air without experiencing anxiety. Ultimately, you would be asked to make a visit 
to an airport and later to take a flight. 


Exposure Treatments. Although systematic desensitization has proven to be a suc- 
cessful treatment, today it is often replaced with a less complicated form of therapy 
called exposure. Exposure is a behavioral treatment for anxiety in which people are 
confronted either suddenly or gradually with a stimulus that they fear. However, 
unlike systematic desensitization, relaxation training is omitted. Exposure allows the 
maladaptive response of anxiety or avoidance to extinguish, and research shows that 
this approach is generally as effective as systematic desensitization (Havermans et 
al., 2007; Hoffmann, 2007; Bush, 2008). 

In most cases, therapists use graded exposure in which patients are exposed to a 
feared stimulus in gradual steps. For example, a patient who is afraid of dogs might 
first view a video of dogs. Gradually the exposure escalates to seeing a live, leashed 
dog across the room and then actually petting and touching the dog (Berle, 2007; 
Means & Edinger, 2007). 

Exposure has proved to be an effective treatment for a number of problems, 
including phobias, anxiety disorders, and even impotence and fear of sexual contact. 
Through this technique, people can learn to enjoy the things they once feared (Choy, 
Fyer, & Lipsitz, 2007; Franklin, March, & Garcia, 2007; Powers & Emmelkamp, 2008). 


OPERANT CONDITIONING TECHNIQUES 


Some behavioral approaches make use of the operant conditioning principles that we 
discussed earlier in the book when considering the topic of learning. These approaches 
are based on the notion that we should reward people for carrying out desirable 
behavior and extinguish undesirable behavior by either ignoring it or punishing it. 

One example of the systematic application of operant conditioning principles is 
the token system, which rewards a person for desired behavior with a token such as 
a poker chip or some kind of play money. Although it is most frequently employed 
in institutional settings for individuals with relatively serious problems and some- 
times with children as a classroom management technique, the system resembles 
what parents do when they give children money for being well behaved—money 
that the children can later exchange for something they want. The desired behavior 
may range from simple things such as keeping one’s room neat to personal grooming 
and interacting with other people. In institutions, patients can exchange tokens for 
some object or activity, such as snacks, new clothes, or, in extreme cases, sleeping in 
one’s own bed rather than in a sleeping bag on the floor. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 49 Psychotherapy: Psychodynamic, Behavioral, and Cognitive Approaches to Treatment 551 


Contingency contracting, a variant of the token system, 
has proved quite effective in producing behavior modifica- 
tion. In contingency contracting, the therapist and client (or 
teacher and student or parent and child) draw up a written 
agreement. The contract states a series of behavioral goals the 
client hopes to achieve. It also specifies the positive conse- 
quences for the client if the client reaches goals—usually an 
explicit reward such as money or additional privileges. Con- 
tracts frequently state negative consequences if the client does 
not meet the goals. For example, clients who are trying to quit 
smoking might write out a check to a cause they have no 
interest in supporting (for instance, the National Rifle 
Association if they are strong supporters of gun control). If 
the client smokes on a given day, the therapist will mail 
the check. 





Behavior therapists also use observational learning, the pro- A “Fearless Peer” who models appropriate and effective behavior 
cess in which the behavior of other people is modeled, to sys- can help children overcome their fears. 


tematically teach people new skills and ways of handling their 

fears and anxieties. For example, modeling helps when therapists are teaching basic 
social skills, such as maintaining eye contact during conversation and acting assert- 
ively. Similarly, children with dog phobias have been able to overcome their fears by 
watching another child—called the “Fearless Peer”— repeatedly walk up to a dog, 
touch it, pet it, and finally play with it. Modeling, then, can play an effective role in 
resolving some kinds of behavior difficulties, especially if the model receives a 
reward for his or her behavior (Bandura, Grusec, & Menlove, 1967; Greer, Dudek- 
Singer, & Gautreaux, 2006; Egliston & Rapee, 2007). 


DIALECTICAL BEHAVIOR THERAPY 


In dialectical behavior therapy, the focus is on getting people to accept who they 
are regardless of whether it matches their ideal. Even if their childhood has been 
dysfunctional or they have ruined relationships with others, that’s in the past. What 
matters is who they wish to become (Manning, 2005; Lynch et al., 2007; Wagner, 
Rizvi, & Hamed, 2007). 

Like treatment approaches based on the principles of classical and operant con- 
ditioning, dialectical behavior therapy is an outgrowth of behavioral approaches, but 
it also includes components of other perspectives. Therapists using dialectical behav- 
ior therapy seek to have patients realize that they basically have two choices: Either 
they remain unhappy, or they change. Once patients agree that they wish to change, 
it is up to them to modify their behavior. Patients are taught that even if they expe- 
rience unhappiness, anger, or any other negative emotion, it doesn’t need to rule 
their behavior. It’s their behavior that counts—not their inner life. 

Dialectical behavior therapy teaches behavioral skills that help people behave 
more effectively and keep their emotions in check. Although it is a relatively new 
form of therapy, increasing evidence supports its effectiveness, particularly with cer- 
tain personality disorders (Swales & Heard, 2007; Katz, Fotti, & Postl, 2009; Soler et 
al., 2009). 


EVALUATING BEHAVIOR THERAPY 


Behavior therapy works especially well for eliminating anxiety disorders, treating 
phobias and compulsions, establishing control over impulses, and learning complex 
social skills to replace maladaptive behavior. More than any of the other therapeutic 
techniques, it provides methods that nonprofessionals can use to change their own 
behavior. Moreover, it is efficient because it focuses on solving carefully defined 
problems (Richard & Lauterbach, 2006; Barlow, 2007). 


dialectical behavior therapy A form 
of treatment in which the focus is on 
getting people to accept who they are 
regardless of whether it matches their 
ideal. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


552 Chapter 16 Treatment of Psychological Disorders 


cognitive treatment approaches Treat- 
ment approaches that teach people to 
think in more adaptive ways by 
changing their dysfunctional cogni- 
tions about the world and themselves. 


How Behavioral 


Therapy Changes Your Brain 





FIGURE 3 This figure highlights areas of the brain that show a reduced response to highly 
emotionally arousing pictures after behavioral therapy in patients with borderline personality 
disorder. The scans on the left (a) show areas of increased activity (in red, yellow, and orange) 
in those with borderline personality disorder before behavioral therapy as compared to 
participants who do not have this disorder. The scans on the right (b) show this same 
comparison after treatment. The scans on the right (b) contain fewer areas of activation, 
which suggests that after behavioral therapy, the brains of those with borderline personality 
disorder react more similarly to the brains of those who do not have the disorder. (Source: 
Schnell & Herpertz, 2007, Figure 3) 





(a) (b) 


Critics of behavior therapy believe that because it emphasizes changing external 
behavior, people do not necessarily gain insight into thoughts and expectations that 
may be fostering their maladaptive behavior. On the other hand, neuroscientific evi- 
dence shows that behavioral treatments can produce actual changes in brain func- 
tioning, which suggests that behavioral treatments can produce changes beyond 
external behavior. 

For example, one experiment looked at the neurological reactions of patients 
with borderline personality disorder who participated in a 12-week dialectical behav- 
ioral therapy program. Compared with a control group composed of people who did 
not have the disorder, the patients showed significant changes in their reactions to 
highly arousing emotion-evoking stimuli. Following therapy, the patients’ neuro- 
logical functioning was more similar to those without the disorder than it was prior 
to therapy (see Figure 3). 


Cognitive Approaches 
to Therapy 


If you assumed that illogical thoughts and beliefs lie at the heart of psychological 
disorders, wouldn’t the most direct treatment route be to teach people new, more 
adaptive modes of thinking? The answer is yes, according to psychologists who take 
a cognitive approach to treatment. 

Cognitive treatment approaches teach people to think in more adaptive ways 
by changing their dysfunctional cognitions about the world and themselves. Unlike 
behavior therapists, who focus on modifying external behavior, cognitive therapists 
attempt to change the way people think as well as their behavior. Because they often 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 49 Psychotherapy: Psychodynamic, Behavioral, and Cognitive Approaches to Treatment 





FIGURE 4 In the A-B-C model of rational-emotive behavior therapy, negative activating 
conditions (A) lead to the activation of an irrational belief system (B), which leads to 
emotional consequences (C). Those emotional consequences then feed back and support the 
belief system. At what steps in the model could change occur as a result of rational-emotive 
behavior therapy? 


use basic principles of learning, the methods they employ are sometimes referred to 
as the cognitive-behavioral approach (Beck & Rector, 2005; Butler et al., 2006; Fried- 
berg, 2006). 

Although cognitive treatment approaches take many forms, they all share the 
assumption that anxiety, depression, and negative emotions develop from maladap- 
tive thinking. Accordingly, cognitive treatments seek to change the thought patterns 
that lead to getting “stuck” in dysfunctional ways of thinking. Therapists systemati- 
cally teach clients to challenge their assumptions and adopt new approaches to old 
problems. 

Cognitive therapy is relatively short term and usually lasts a maximum of 20 
sessions. Therapy tends to be highly structured and focused on concrete problems. 
Therapists often begin by teaching the theory behind the approach and then continue 
to take an active role throughout the course of therapy by acting as a combination 
of teacher, coach, and partner. 

One good example of cognitive treatment, rational-emotive behavior therapy, 
attempts to restructure a person’s belief system into a more realistic, rational, and 
logical set of views. According to psychologist Albert Ellis (2002, 2004), many people 
lead unhappy lives and suffer from psychological disorders because they harbor 
irrational, unrealistic ideas such as these: 


e We need the love or approval of virtually every significant other person for 
everything we do. 

e We should be thoroughly competent, adequate, and successful in all possible 
respects in order to consider ourselves worthwhile. 

e It is horrible when things don’t turn out the way we want them to. 


Such irrational beliefs trigger negative emotions, which in turn support the irra- 
tional beliefs and lead to a self-defeating cycle. Ellis calls it the A-B-C model in which 
negative activating conditions (A) lead to the activation of an irrational belief system 
(B), which in turn leads to emotional consequences (C). For example, if a person 
experiences the breakup of a close relationship (A) and holds the irrational belief (B) 
that “TIl never be loved again,” this triggers negative emotions (C) that in turn feed 
back into support of the irrational belief (see Figure 4). 

Rational-emotive behavior therapy aims to help clients eliminate maladaptive 
thoughts and beliefs and adopt more effective thinking. To accomplish this goal, 
therapists take an active, directive role during therapy and openly challenge patterns 
of thought that appear to be dysfunctional. Consider this example: 


Martha: The basic problem is that I’m worried about my family. I’m worried 
about money. And I never seem to be able to relax. 

Therapist: Why are you worried about your family? . . .What’s to be concerned 
about? They have certain demands which you don’t want to adhere to. 


cognitive-behavioral approach 

A treatment approach that incorpo- 
rates basic principles of learning to 
change the way people think. 


rational-emotive behavior therapy 
A form of therapy that attempts to 
restructure a person’s belief system 
into a more realistic, rational, and 
logical set of views by challenging 
dysfunctional beliefs that maintain 
irrational behavior. 


553 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


554 Chapter 16 Treatment of Psychological Disorders 


Martha: I was brought up to think that I mustn’t be selfish. 

Therapist: Oh, we’ll have to knock that out of your head! 

Martha: My mother feels that I shouldn’t have left home—that my place is 
with them. There are nagging doubts about what I should— 

Therapist: Why are there doubts? Why should you? 

Martha: I think it’s a feeling I was brought up with that you always have to 
give of yourself. If you think of yourself, you’re wrong. 

Therapist: That’s a belief. Why do you have to keep believing that—at your 
age? You believed a lot of superstitions when you were younger. Why do 
you have to retain them? Your parents indoctrinated you with this non- 
sense, because that’s their belief. . . . Who needs that philosophy? All it’s 
gotten you, so far, is guilt. (Ellis, 1974, pp. 223-286) 


By poking holes in Martha’s reasoning, the therapist is attempting to help her 
adopt a more realistic view of herself and her circumstances (Ellis, 2002; Dryden & 
David, 2008). 

Another influential form of therapy that builds on a cognitive perspective is that 
of Aaron Beck (Beck, 1995, 2004). Like rational-emotive behavior therapy, Beck’s 
cognitive therapy aims to change people’s illogical thoughts about themselves and 
the world. 

However, cognitive therapy is considerably less confrontational and challenging 
than rational-emotive behavior therapy. Instead of the therapist actively arguing 
with clients about their dysfunctional cognitions, cognitive therapists more often 
play the role of teacher. Therapists urge clients to obtain information on their own 
that will lead them to discard their inaccurate thinking through a process of cogni- 
tive appraisal. In cognitive appraisal, clients are asked to evaluate situations, them- 
selves, and others in terms of their memories, values, beliefs, thoughts, and 
expectations. During the course of treatment, therapists help clients discover ways 
of thinking more appropriately about themselves and others (Rosen, 2000; Beck, 
Freeman, & Davis, 2004; Moorey, 2007). 


EVALUATING COGNITIVE APPROACHES TO THERAPY 


Cognitive approaches to therapy have proved successful in dealing with a broad 
range of disorders, including anxiety disorders, depression, substance abuse, and 
eating disorders. Furthermore, the willingness of cognitive therapists to incorporate 
additional treatment approaches (e.g., combining cognitive and behavioral tech- 
niques in cognitive behavioral therapy) has made this approach a particularly effec- 
tive form of treatment (Mitte, 2005; Ishikawa et al., 2007; Bhar et al., 2008). 

At the same time, critics have pointed out that the focus on helping people to 
think more rationally ignores the fact that life is in reality sometimes irrational. 
Changing one’s assumptions to make them more reasonable and logical thus may 
not always be helpful—even assuming it is possible to bring about true cognitive 
change. Still, the success of cognitive approaches has made it one of the most 
frequently employed therapies (Leahy, 2003; Beck & Rector, 2005). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 49 Psychotherapy: Psychodynamic, Behavioral, and Cognitive Approaches to Treatment 555 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 2. According to Freud, people use as 
a means of preventing unwanted impulses from intruding 
on conscious thought. 

3. In dream interpretation, a psychoanalyst must learn to 
distinguish between the ________ content of a dream, 
which is what appears on the surface, and the 
content, its underlying meaning. 

4. Which of the following treatments deals with phobias by 
gradual exposure to the item producing the fear? 





What are the goals of psychologically and biologically based 
treatment approaches? 

e Psychotherapy (psychologically based therapy) and bio- 
medical therapy (biologically based therapy) share the 
goal of resolving psychological problems by modifying 
people’s thoughts, feelings, expectations, evaluations, 
and ultimately behavior. (p. 544) 


What are the psychodynamic, behavioral, and cognitive a. Systematic desensitization 
approaches to treatment? b. Partial reinforcement 
e Psychoanalytic approaches seek to bring unresolved past c. Behavioral self-management 
conflicts and unacceptable impulses from the uncon- d. Aversion therapy 
scious into the conscious, where patients may deal with 
the problems more effectively. To do this, therapists use RETHINK 
techniques such as free association and dream interpreta- 
tion. (p. 546) 1. In what ways are psychoanalysis and cognitive therapy 
e Behavioral approaches to treatment view abnormal be- similar, and how do they differ? 
havior as the problem rather than viewing that behavior 2. From the perspective of a child-care provider: How might you 
as a symptom of some underlying cause. To bring about use systematic desensitization to help children overcome 
a “cure,” this view suggests that the outward behavior their fears? 


must be changed by using methods such as aversive con- 
ditioning, systematic desensitization, observational 
learning, token systems, contingency contracting, and di- 
alectical behavior therapy. (p. 548) 

e Cognitive approaches to treatment consider that the goal 
of therapy is to help a person restructure his or her faulty 
belief system into a more realistic, rational, and logical 
view of the world. Two examples of cognitive treatments 
are the rational-emotive behavior therapy and cognitive 
therapy. (p. 552) 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 
Vp ‘JuoszL] ‘Jsaftueul “Eg “SUISTURYDOUT esUeJep 7 ‘q-f ‘P-E “L-7 -ŢI 


EVALUATE 


1. Match the following mental health practitioners with the 
appropriate description. 
1. Psychiatrist a. PhD specializing in the treatment 


2. Clinical of psychological disorders 
psychologist b. Professional specializing in 
3. Counseling Freudian therapy techniques 


psychologist c. MD trained in abnormal behavior 
4. Psychoanalyst d. PhD specializing in the adjustment 
of day-to-day problems 


KEY TERMS 


psychotherapy p. 544 behavioral treatment exposure p. 550 cognitive-behavioral 
biomedical therapy p. 544 approaches p. 548 dialectical behavior approach p. 553 
psychodynamic aversive therapy p. 551 rational-emotive behavior 

therapy p. 546 conditioning p. 549 cognitive treatment therapy p. 553 
psychoanalysis p. 546 systematic approaches p. 552 


transference p. 547 desensitization p. 549 


MODULE 50 


What are the humanistic 
approaches to treatment? 


What is interpersonal therapy? 


How does group therapy 
differ from individual types of 
therapy? 


How effective is psycho- 
therapy, and which kind of 
psychotherapy works best 
in a given situation? 


Therapy in which 
the underlying rationale is that people 
have control of their behavior, can 
make choices about their lives, and are 
essentially responsible for solving their 
own problems. 





Humanistic therapy focuses on 
self-responsibility. 


556 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Humanistic Therapy 


As you know from your own experience, a student cannot master the material cov- 
ered in a course without some hard work, no matter how good the teacher and the 
textbook are. You must take the time to study, memorize the vocabulary, and learn 
the concepts. Nobody else can do it for you. If you choose to put in the effort, you'll 
succeed; if you don’t, you'll fail. The responsibility is primarily yours. 

Humanistic therapy draws on this philosophical perspective of self-responsibility 
in developing treatment techniques. The many different types of therapy that fit into 
this category have a similar rationale: We have control of our own behavior, we can 
make choices about the kinds of lives we want to live, and it is up to us to solve the 
difficulties we encounter in our daily lives. 

Humanistic therapists believe that people naturally are motivated to strive for self- 
actualization. As we discussed in the chapter on motivation, self-actualization is the term 
that clinical psychologist Abraham Maslow used to describe the state of self-fulfillment 
in which people realize their highest potentials in their own unique way. 

Instead of acting in the more directive manner of some psychodynamic and 
behavioral approaches, humanistic therapists view themselves as guides or facilita- 
tors. Therapists using humanistic techniques seek to help people understand them- 
selves and find ways to come closer to the ideal they hold for themselves. In this 
view, psychological disorders result from the inability to find meaning in life and 
from feelings of loneliness and a lack of connection to others (Cain, 2002). 

Humanistic approaches have produced many therapeutic techniques. Among the 
most important is person-centered therapy. 


PERSON-CENTERED THERAPY 
Consider the following therapy session excerpt: 


Alice: I was thinking about this business of standards. I somehow developed a 
sort of a knack, I guess, of—well—habit—of trying to make people feel at 
ease around me, or to make things go along smoothly. . . . 

Therapist: In other words, what you did was always in the direction of trying 
to keep things smooth and to make other people feel better and to smooth 
the situation. 

Alice: Yes. I think that’s what it was. Now the reason why I did it probably 
was—I mean, not that I was a good little Samaritan going around making 
other people happy, but that was probably the role that felt easiest for me 
to play. ... 

Therapist: You feel that for a long time you've been playing the role of kind of 
smoothing out the frictions or differences or what not... . 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 50 Psychotherapy: Humanistic, Interpersonal, and 


Alice: M-hm. 
Therapist: Rather than having any opinion or reaction of your own in the 
situation. Is that it? (Rogers, 1951, pp. 152-153) 


The therapist does not interpret or answer the questions the client has raised. 
Instead, the therapist clarifies or reflects back what the client has said (e.g., “In other 
words, what you did... .”; “You feel that. . . .”; “Is that it?”). This therapeutic tech- 
nique, known as nondirective counseling, is at the heart of person-centered therapy, 
which was first practiced by Carl Rogers in the mid-20th century (Rogers, 1951, 1980; 
Raskin & Rogers, 1989). 

Person-centered therapy (also called client-centered therapy) aims to enable people 
to reach their potential for self-actualization. By providing a warm and accepting 
environment, therapists hope to motivate clients to air their problems and feelings. 
In turn, this enables clients to make realistic and constructive choices and decisions 
about the things that bother them in their current lives (Bozarth, Zimring, & Tausch, 
2002; Kirschenbaum, 2004; Bohart, 2006). 

Instead of directing the choices clients make, therapists provide what Rogers 
calls unconditional positive regard—expressing acceptance and understanding, regard- 
less of the feelings and attitudes the client expresses. By doing this, therapists hope 
to create an atmosphere that enables clients to come to decisions that can improve 
their lives (Kirschenbaum & Jourdan, 2005; Vieira & Freire, 2006). 

Furnishing unconditional positive regard does not mean that therapists must 
approve of everything their clients say or do. Rather, therapists need to communicate 
that they are caring, nonjudgmental, and empathetic—that is, understanding of a cli- 
ent’s emotional experiences (Fearing & Clark, 2000). 

Person-centered therapy is rarely used today in its purest form. Contemporary 
approaches tend to be somewhat more directive with therapists nudging clients toward 
insights rather than merely reflecting back their statements. However, therapists still 
view clients’ insights as central to the therapeutic process (also see PsychWork). 


“At the Kansas City Community Center, we help people 
rebuild their lives.” 

For Vickie Dickerman, a case manager and substance 
abuse counselor, this statement is the foundation of her work. 


PsychWork 


CASE MANAGER AND 


Group Approaches to Treatment 557 


person-centered therapy Therapy 
in which the goal is to reach one’s 
potential for self-actualization. 


y Alert 
To better remember the 
concept of unconditional 
positive regard, try offering 
it to a friend during a 
conversation by showing 
your support, acceptance, and 
understanding no matter what 
thought or attitude is being offered. 


“Treatment is a collaboration between what the client 
wants from a program and what you, as the professional, see 
as essential in the treatment planning process. We also assist 
clients in obtaining their basic needs in order that they may 
become productive members of society,” she said. 

Dickerman, who has worked for 20 years in the field of 
substance abuse with criminal offenders, uses several 
approaches in helping people rebuild their lives. 

“I address ‘criminal thinking errors’ along with client- 


SUBSTANCE ABUSE 
COUNSELOR 





Name: Vicki Dickerman 

Position: Case Manager and Substance Abuse 
Counselor, Kansas City Community Center 
Education: BS in Addiction Studies and 
Psychology, Park University, Parkville, Missouri; 
Certified Co-Occurring Disorders Professional 


centered therapy,” she explained. “When clients engage in 

‘criminal thinking,’ they view themselves as victims, focus 

only their positive attributes, and avoid taking responsibility for their actions.” Dick- 
erman seeks to change such thinking and help clients rebuild their self-confidence in 
their ability to meet their own basic needs. 

“Expressing praise to offenders for something that would not appear to be an 
accomplishment to most of us is of importance since clients’ low self-esteem and lack 
of self-confidence are often the result of lack of reinforcement from family members 
and their peers,” Dickerman added. “Allowing setbacks should be considered a part 
of the process since individuals with criminal thinking errors experience difficulty 
with receiving praise and may attempt to sabotage their success.” 





558 


interpersonal therapy (IPT) Short- 
term therapy that focuses on the 
context of current social relationships. 


group therapy Therapy in which 
people meet in a group with a thera- 
pist to discuss problems. 


© Tom Cheney/The New Yorker Collection/www. 


cartoonbank.com. 





“So, would anyone in the group care to respond to 
what Clifford has just shared with us?” some, the therapist is quite directive; in others, the members of the group 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Chapter 16 Treatment of Psychological Disorders 


EVALUATING HUMANISTIC APPROACHES TO THERAPY 


The notion that psychological disorders result from restricted growth potential 
appeals philosophically to many people. Furthermore, when humanistic thera- 
pists acknowledge that the freedom we possess can lead to psychological difficul- 
ties, clients find an unusually supportive environment for therapy. In turn, this 
atmosphere can help clients discover solutions to difficult psychological problems 
(Cooper, 2007). 

However, humanistic treatments lack specificity, a problem that has troubled 
their critics. Humanistic approaches are not very precise and are probably the least 
scientifically and theoretically developed type of treatment. Moreover, this form of 
treatment works best for the same type of highly verbal client who profits most from 
psychoanalytic treatment. 


Interpersonal Therapy 


Interpersonal therapy (IPT) considers therapy in the context of social relationships. 
Although its roots stem from psychodynamic approaches, interpersonal therapy con- 
centrates more on the here and now with the goal of improving a client’s current 
relationships. It typically focuses on interpersonal issues such as conflicts with others, 
social skills issues, role transitions (such as divorce), or grief (Weissman, Markowitz, 
& Klerman, 2007). 

Interpersonal therapy is more active and directive than traditional psycho- 
dynamic approaches, and sessions are more structured. The approach makes no 
assumptions about the underlying causes of psychological disorders but focuses on 
the interpersonal context in which a disorder is developed and maintained. It also 
tends to be shorter than traditional psychodynamic approaches and typically lasts 
only 12-16 weeks. During those sessions, therapists make concrete suggestions on 
improving relations with others and offer recommendations and advice. 

Because interpersonal therapy is short and structured, researchers have been able 
to demonstrate its effectiveness more readily than longer-term types of therapy. Eval- 
uations of the approach have shown that interpersonal therapy is especially effective 
in dealing with depression, anxiety, addictions, and eating disorders (Salsman, 2006; 
Grigoriadis & Ravitz, 2007; Miller et al., 2008). 





Group Therapy, Family Therapy, 
and Self-Help Groups 


Although most treatment takes place between a single individual and a 
therapist, some forms of therapy involve groups of people seeking treat- 
ment. In group therapy, several unrelated people meet with a therapist 
to discuss some aspect of their psychological functioning. 

People typically discuss with the group their problems, which often 
center on a common difficulty, such as alcoholism or a lack of social 
skills. The other members of the group provide emotional support and 
dispense advice on ways they have coped effectively with similar prob- 
lems (Alonso, Alonso, & Piper, 2003; Scaturo, 2004; Rigby & Waite, 2007). 

Groups vary greatly in terms of the particular model they employ; 
there are psychoanalytic groups, humanistic groups, and groups corre- 
sponding to the other therapeutic approaches. Furthermore, groups also 
differ with regard to the degree of guidance the therapist provides. In 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 50 Psychotherapy: Humanistic, Interpersonal, and Group Approaches to Treatment 559 


set their own agenda and determine how the group will pro- 
ceed (Beck & Lewis, 2000; Stockton, Morran, & Krieger, 2004). 

Because several people are treated simultaneously in 
group therapy, it is a much more economical means of treat- 
ment than individual psychotherapy. On the other hand, crit- 
ics argue that group settings lack the individual attention 
inherent in one-to-one therapy and that especially shy and 
withdrawn individuals may not receive the attention they 
need in a group setting. 


FAMILY THERAPY 


One specialized form of group therapy is family therapy. As 
the name implies, family therapy involves two or more fam- 
ily members, one (or more) of whose problems led to treat- 
ment. But rather than focusing simply on the members of the 
family who present the initial problem, family therapists con- 
sider the family as a unit to which each member contributes. 
By meeting with the entire family simultaneously, family therapists try to understand 
how the family members interact with one another (Cooklin, 2000; Strong & Tomm, 
2007). 

Family therapists view the family as a “system” and assume that individuals in family therapy An approach that 
the family cannot improve without understanding the conflicts found in interactions focuses on the family and its dynamics. 
among family members. Thus, the therapist expects each member to contribute to 
the resolution of the problem being addressed. 

Many family therapists believe that family members fall into rigid roles or set 
patterns of behavior with one person acting as the scapegoat, another as a bully, and 
so forth. In their view, that system of roles perpetuates family disturbances. One goal 
of this type of therapy, then, is to get the family members to adopt new, more con- 
structive roles and patterns of behavior (Minuchin, 1999; Sprenkle & Moon, 1996; 
Sori, 2006). 





In group therapy, people with psychological difficulties meet with a 
therapist to discuss their problems. 


SELF-HELP THERAPY 


In many cases, group therapy does not involve a professional therapist. Instead, 
people with similar problems get together to discuss their shared feelings and expe- 
riences. For example, people who have recently experienced the death of a spouse 
might meet in a bereavement support group, or college students may get together to 
discuss their adjustment to college. 

One of the best-known self-help groups is Alcoholics Anonymous (AA), designed 
to help members deal with alcohol-related problems. AA prescribes 12 steps that 
alcoholics must pass through on their road to recovery; they begin with an admission 
that they are alcoholics and powerless over alcohol. AA provides more treatment for 
alcoholics than any other therapy; AA and other 12-step programs (such as Narcotics 
Anonymous) can be as successful in treating alcohol and other substance-abuse prob- 
lems as traditional types of therapy (Bogenschutz, Geppert, & George, 2006; Galanter, 
2007; Gossop, Stewart, & Marsden, 2008). 


Evaluating Psychotherapy: 
Does Therapy Work? 


Your best friend, Ben, comes to you because he just hasn’t been feeling right about 
things lately. He’s upset because he and his girlfriend aren’t getting along, but his 
difficulties go beyond that. He can’t concentrate on his studies, has a lot of trouble 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


560 Chapter 16 Treatment of Psychological Disorders 


getting to sleep, and—this is what really bothers him—has begun to think that peo- 
ple are ganging up on him, talking about him behind his back. It seems that no one 
really cares about or understands him or makes any effort to see why he’s become 
so miserable. 

Ben knows that he ought to get some kind of help, but he is not sure where to 
turn. He is fairly skeptical of psychologists and thinks that a lot of what they say 
is just mumbo-jumbo, but he’s willing to put his doubts aside and try anything to 
feel better. He also knows there are many different types of therapy, and he doesn’t 
have a clue about which would be best for him. He turns to you for advice because 
he knows you are taking a psychology course. He asks, “Which kind of therapy 
works best?” 


IS THERAPY EFFECTIVE? 


This question requires a complex response. In fact, identifying the single most appro- 


y Alert priate form of treatment is a controversial and still unresolved task for psychologists 

Pay special attention to the specializing in psychological disorders. In fact, even before considering whether one 

discussion of (1) whether form of therapy works better than another, we need to determine whether therapy 

therapy is effective in in any form effectively alleviates psychological disturbances. 

general and (2) what Until the 1950s, most people simply assumed that therapy was effective. But in 

specific types of therapy 1952 psychologist Hans Eysenck published an influential study challenging that 

are effective because it is a assumption. He claimed that people who received psychodynamic treatment and 

key issue for therapists. related therapies were no better off at the end of treatment than were people who 

were placed on a waiting list for treatment but never received it. Eysenck concluded 

spontaneous remission Recovery that people would go into spontaneous remission, recovery without treatment, if 
without treatment. they were simply left alone—certainly a cheaper and simpler process. 


Although other psychologists quickly challenged Eysenck’s conclusions, his 
review stimulated a continuing stream of better controlled, more carefully crafted 
studies on the effectiveness of psychotherapy. Today most psychologists agree: Ther- 
apy does work. Several comprehensive reviews indicate that therapy brings about 
greater improvement than no treatment at all, with the rate of spontaneous remis- 
sion being fairly low. In most cases, then, the symptoms of abnormal behavior do 
not go away by themselves if left untreated—although the issue continues to be 
hotly debated (Seligman, 1996; Westen, Novotny, & Thompson-Brenner, 2004; Lutz 
et al., 2006). 


WHICH KIND OF THERAPY WORKS BEST? 


Although most psychologists feel confident that psychotherapeutic treatment in gen- 
eral is more effective than no treatment at all, the question of whether any specific 
form of treatment is superior to any other has not been answered definitively (Nathan, 
Stuart, & Dolan, 2000; Westen, Novotny, & Thompson-Brenner, 2004; Abboud, 2005). 

For instance, one classic study comparing the effectiveness of various approaches 
found that although success rates vary somewhat by treatment form, most treatments 
show fairly equal success rates. As Figure 1 indicates, the rates ranged from about 
70-85% greater success for treated compared with untreated individuals. Behavioral 
and cognitive approaches tended to be slightly more successful, but that result may 
have been due to differences in the severity of the cases treated (Smith, Glass, & 
Miller, 1980; Orwin & Condray, 1984). 

Other research, which relies on meta-analysis in which data from a large number 
of studies are statistically combined, yields similar general conclusions. Furthermore, 
a large survey of 186,000 individuals found that respondents felt they had benefited 
substantially from psychotherapy. However, there was little difference in “consumer 
satisfaction” on the basis of the specific type of treatment they had received (Selig- 
man, 1995; Malouff, Thorsteinsson, & Schutte, 2007; Cuijpers et al., 2008). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 50 Psychotherapy: Humanistic, Interpersonal, and Group Approaches to Treatment 561 


FIGURE 1 Estimates of the effective- 
ness of different types of treatment, 

in comparison to control groups of 
untreated people. The percentile score 
shows how much more effective a 
specific type of treatment is for the 
average patient rather than is no 
treatment. For example, people given 
psychodynamic treatment score, on 
average, more positively on outcome 
measures than about three-quarters of 
untreated people. (Source: Adapted from 
Smith, Glass, & Miller, 1980). 


Psychodynamic 


Gestalt 


Person- or client-centered 


Systematic desensitization 


Behavior modification 


Cognitive behavioral 











50 60 70 80 90 


Success rate (percentage) 


In short, converging evidence allows us to draw several conclusions about the 
effectiveness of psychotherapy (Strupp & Binder, 1992; Seligman, 1996; Goldfried & 
Pachankis, 2007): 


e For most people, psychotherapy is effective. This conclusion holds over different 
lengths of treatment, specific kinds of psychological disorders, and various 
types of treatment. Thus, the question, “Does psychotherapy work?” appears 
to have been answered convincingly: It does (Seligman, 1996; Spiegel, 1999; 
Westen, Novotny, & Thompson-Brenner, 2004; Payne & Marcus, 2008). 

e On the other hand, psychotherapy doesn’t work for everyone. As many as 10% of 
people treated show no improvement or actually deteriorated (Boisvert & 
Faust, 2003; Pretzer & Beck, 2005; Coffman et al., 2007; Lilienfeld, 2007). 

e No single form of therapy works best for every problem, and certain specific types 
of treatment are better, although not invariably, for specific types of problems. For 
example, cognitive therapy works especially well for panic disorders, and 
exposure therapy relieves specific phobias effectively. However, there are 
exceptions to these generalizations, and often the differences in success rates 
for different types of treatment are not substantial (Miller & Magruder, 1999; 
Westen, Novotny, & Thompson-Brenner, 2004). 

e Most therapies share several basic similar elements. Despite the fact that the specific 
methods used in different therapies are very different from one another, there 
are several common themes that lead them to be effective. These elements 
include the opportunity for a client to develop a positive relationship with a 
therapist, an explanation or interpretation of a client’s symptoms, and confron- 
tation of negative emotions. The fact that these common elements exist in most 
therapies makes it difficult to compare one treatment against another (Nor- 
cross, 2002; Norcross, Beutler & Levant, 2006). 


Consequently, there is no single, definitive answer to the broad question, “Which 
therapy works best?” because of the complexity in sorting out the various factors 
that enter into successful therapy. Recently, however, clinicians and researchers have 
reframed the question by focusing on evidence-based psychotherapy practice. 
Evidence-based psychotherapy practice seeks to use research findings to determine the 
best practices for treating a specific disorder. To determine best practices, researchers 
use clinical interviews, client self-reports of improvement in quality of life, reductions 
in symptoms, observations of behavior, and other outcomes to compare different 
therapies. By using objective research findings, clinicians are increasingly able to 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


562 Chapter 16 Treatment of Psychological Disorders 


determine the most effective treatment for a specific disorder (APA Presidential Task 
Force, 2006; Brownlee, 2007; Kazdin, 2008). 

Because no single type of psychotherapy is invariably effective for every indi- 
vidual, some therapists use an eclectic approach to therapy. In an eclectic approach to 
therapy, therapists use a variety of techniques, thus integrating several perspectives, 
to treat a person’s problems. By employing more than one approach, therapists can 
choose the appropriate mix of evidence-based treatments to match the individual’s 
specific needs. Furthermore, therapists with certain personal characteristics may 
work better with particular individuals and types of treatments, and—as we consider 
next—even racial and ethnic factors may be related to the success of treatment 
(Cheston, 2000; Chambless et al., 2006; Hayes, 2008). 





Aas; k Ex p | O ri nN g Consider the following case report written by a school counselor 
oe ~e # 73 DIVERSITY about Jimmy Jones, a 12-year-old student who was referred to a 


" Pas. Bacial'and Ethnic Factorsin Treatment: counselor because of his lack of interest in schoolwork: 


Da Should Therapists Be Color Blind? Jimmy does not pay attention, daydreams often, and frequently falls 
asleep during class. There is a strong possibility that Jimmy is 
harboring repressed rage that needs to be ventilated and dealt with. 
His inability to directly express his anger had led him to adopt 

passive-aggressive means of expressing hostility, i.e., inattentiveness, daydreaming, 
falling asleep. It is recommended that Jimmy be seen for intensive counseling to 
discover the basis of the anger. (Sue & Sue, 1990, p. 44) 


The counselor was wrong, however. Rather than suffering from “repressed rage,” 
Jimmy lived in a poverty stricken and disorganized home. Because of overcrowding at 
his house, he did not get enough sleep and consequently was tired the next day. 
Frequently, he was also hungry. In short, the stresses arising from his environment and 
not any deep-seated psychological disturbances caused his problems. 

This incident underscores the importance of taking people’s environmental and 
cultural backgrounds into account during treatment for psychological disorders. In 
particular, members of racial and ethnic minority groups, especially those who are also 
poor, may behave in ways that help them deal with a society that discriminates against 
them. As a consequence, behavior that may signal psychological disorder in middle- 
class and upper-class whites may simply be adaptive in people from other racial and 
socioeconomic groups. For instance, characteristically suspicious and distrustful 
people may be displaying a survival strategy to protect themselves from psychological 
and physical injury rather than suffering from a psychological disturbance (Paniagua, 
2000; Tseng, 2003; Pottick et al., 2007). 

In fact, therapists must question some basic assumptions of psychotherapy when 
dealing with racial, ethnic, and cultural minority group members. For example, 
compared with the dominant culture, Asian and Latino cultures typically place much 
greater emphasis on the group, family, and society. When an Asian or Latino faces a 
critical decision, the family helps make it—a cultural practice suggesting that family 
members should also play a role in psychological treatment. Similarly, the traditional 
Chinese recommendation for dealing with depression or anxiety is to urge people who 
experience such problems to avoid thinking about whatever is upsetting them. Consider 
how this advice contrasts with treatment approaches that emphasize the value of insight 
(Ponterotto, Gretchen, & Chauhan, 2001; McCarthy, 2005; Leitner, 2007). 





Therapists’ interpretation of their clients’ Clearly, therapists cannot be “colorblind.” Instead, they must take into account the 
behavior is influenced by racial, ethnic, racial, ethnic, cultural, and social class backgrounds of their clients in determining the 
cultural, and social class backgrounds of nature of a psychological disorder and the course of treatment (Aponte & Wohl, 2000; 


the clients. Pedersen et al., 2002; Hayes, 2008). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 50 Psychotherapy: Humanistic, Interpersonal, and Group Approaches to Treatment 563 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 3. Behavioral b. “I want you all to take turns talking 
therapy about why you decided to come and 
4. Nondirective what you hope to gain from therapy.” 
counseling c. “I can understand why you wanted 
to wreck your friend’s car after she 
hurt your feelings. Now tell me more 
about the accident.” 
. “That’s not appropriate behavior. 
Let’s work on replacing it with some- 


thing else.” 


What are humanistic approaches to treatment? 

e Humanistic therapy is based on the premise that people 
have control of their behavior, that they can make 
choices about their lives, and that it is up to them to 
solve their own problems. Humanistic therapies, which 
take a nondirective approach, include person-centered a 
therapy. (p. 556) 


What is interpersonal therapy? 
e Interpersonal therapy focuses on interpersonal relation- 2. therapies assume that people should take re- 

ships and strives for immediate improvement during sponsibility for their lives and the decisions they make. 
short-term therapy. (p. 558) 3. One of the major criticisms of humanistic therapies is that 

a. They are too imprecise and unstructured. 

b. They treat only the symptom of the problem. 

c. The therapist dominates the patient-therapist interaction. 

d. They work well only on clients of lower socioeconomic 


How does group therapy differ from individual types 
of therapy? 
e In group therapy, several unrelated people meet with a 
therapist to discuss some aspect of their psychological 


mate status. 
eee erraio fier con nena commen poay 4. Ina controversial study, Eysenck found that some people 
[P+ go into , or recovery without treat- 





How effective is psychotherapy, and which kind of psycho- ment, if they are simply left alone instead of treated. 
therapy works best in a given situation? 
e Most research suggests that, in general, therapy is more 
effective than no therapy, although how much more 
effective is not known. (p. 559) 


e The more difficult question of which therapy works best 


RETHINK 


1. How can people be successfully treated in group therapy 
when individuals with the “same” problem are so differ- 


is harder to answer, but it is clear particular kinds of 
therapy are more appropriate for some problems than 
for others. (p. 560) 

e Because no single type of psychotherapy is invariably 
effective, eclectic approaches in which a therapist uses a 


ent? What advantages might group therapy offer over in- 
dividual therapy? 


. From the perspective of a social worker: How might the types 


of therapies you employ vary depending on a client’s cul- 
tural and socioeconomic background? 


variety of techniques and thus integrates several 


i q Answers to Evaluate Questions 
perspectives are sometimes used. (p. 561) 


UOISsTUaI snoougzuods ‘p ‘e'g ONSsTURUNY ‘g ‘e-p ‘P-E 0-7 q- “T 


EVALUATE 


1. Match each of the following treatment strategies with the 
statement you might expect to hear from a therapist using 
that strategy. 

1. Group therapy a. “In other words, you don’t get 
2. Unconditional along with your mother because she 
positive regard hates your girlfriend, is that right?” 


KEY TERMS 


humanistic therapy p. 556 
person-centered therapy p. 557 


interpersonal therapy (IPT) p. 558 
group therapy p. 558 


family therapy p. 559 
spontaneous remission p. 560 


How are drug, electroconvul- 
sive, and psychosurgical 
techniques used today in the 
treatment of psychological 
disorders? 


Control of psychological 
disorders through the use of drugs. 


Drugs that 
temporarily reduce psychotic symp- 
toms such as agitation, hallucinations, 
and delusions. 


564 


~ www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


If you get a kidney infection, your doctor gives you an antibiotic; with luck your 
kidney should be as good as new about a week later. If your appendix becomes 
inflamed, a surgeon removes it and your body functions normally once more. Could 
a comparable approach that focuses on the body’s physiology be effective for psy- 
chological disturbances? 

According to biological approaches to treatment, the answer is yes. Therapists 
routinely use biomedical therapies. This approach suggests that rather than focusing 
on a patient’s psychological conflicts, past traumas, or environmental factors that 
may produce abnormal behavior, focusing treatment directly on brain chemistry and 
other neurological factors may be more appropriate. To do this, therapists can pro- 
vide treatment with drugs, electric shock, or surgery. 


Drug Therapy 


Drug therapy, the control of psychological disorders through drugs, works by altering 
the operation of neurotransmitters and neurons in the brain. Some drugs operate by 
inhibiting neurotransmitters or receptor neurons, which reduces activity at particular 
synapses, the sites where nerve impulses travel from one neuron to another. Other 
drugs do just the opposite: They increase the activity of certain neurotransmitters or 
neurons, which allows particular neurons to fire more frequently (see Figure 1). 





ANTIPSYCHOTIC DRUGS 


Probably no greater change has occurred in mental hospitals than the successful 
introduction in the mid-1950s of antipsychotic drugs—drugs used to reduce severe 
symptoms of disturbance, such as loss of touch with reality and agitation. Previously, 
the typical mental hospital wasn’t very different from the stereotypical 19th-century 
insane asylum; it gave mainly custodial care to screaming, moaning, clawing patients 
who displayed bizarre behaviors. However, in just a matter of days after hospital 
staff members administered antipsychotic drugs, the wards became considerably 
calmer environments in which professionals could do more than just try to get 
patients through the day without causing serious harm to themselves or others. 

This dramatic change came about through the introduction of the drug chlor- 
promazine. Along with other similar drugs, chlorpromazine rapidly became the most 
popular and successful treatment for schizophrenia. Today drug therapy is the pre- 
ferred treatment for most cases of severely abnormal behavior and is used for most 
patients hospitalized with psychological disorders. The newest generation of anti- 
psychotics, referred to as atypical antipsychotics, have fewer side effects; they include 
rizperidone, olanzapine, and paliperidone (Lublin, Eberhard, & Levander, 2005; Savas, 
Yumru, & Kaya, 2007; Nasrallah et al., 2008). 

How do antipsychotic drugs work? Most block dopamine receptors at the brain’s 
synapses. Atypical antipsychotics affect both serotonin and dopamine levels in cer- 
tain parts of the brain, such as those related to planning and goal-directed activity 
(Sawa & Snyder, 2002; Advokat, 2005). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 51 Biomedical Therapy: Biological Approaches to Treatment 565 


Drug Treatments 





Class of Drug Effects of Drug Primary Action of Drug Examples 


Antipsychotic Drugs, Atypical Reduction in loss of touch with Block dopamine receptors Antipsychotic: Chlorpromazine 


Antipsychotic Drugs reality, agitation 


Antidepressant Drugs 


Tricyclic antidepressants Reduction in depression 


MAO inhibitors Reduction in depression 


down neurotransmitters 


Selective serotonin reuptake Reduction in depression 
inhibitors (SSRIs) 


Mood Stabilizers 
Lithium Mood stabilization 


Antianxiety Drugs Reduction in anxiety Increase activity of 


neurotransmitter GABA 


FIGURE 1 The major classes of drugs used to treat psychological disorders have different 
effects on the brain and nervous system. 


Despite the effectiveness of antipsychotic drugs, they do not produce a “cure” 
in the same way that, say, penicillin cures an infection. Most of the time, the symp- 
toms reappear when the drug is withdrawn. Furthermore, such drugs can have long- 
term side effects, such as dryness of the mouth and throat, dizziness, and sometimes 
tremors and loss of muscle control, which may continue after drug treatments are 
stopped (Voruganti et al., 2007). 


ANTIDEPRESSANT DRUGS 


As their name suggests, antidepressant drugs are a class of medications used in cases 
of severe depression to improve the patient’s mood and feeling of well-being. They 
are also sometimes used for other disorders, such as anxiety disorders and bulimia 
(Walsh et al., 2006; Hedges et al., 2007). 

Most antidepressant drugs work by changing the concentration of specific neu- 
rotransmitters in the brain. For example, tricyclic drug increase the availability of 
norepinephrine at the synapses of neurons, whereas MAO inhibitors prevent the 
enzyme monoamine oxidase (MAO) from breaking down neurotransmitters. Newer 
antidepressants—such as Lexapro—are selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs). 
SSRIs target the neurotransmitter serotonin and permit it to linger at the synapse. 
Some antidepressants produce a combination of effects. For instance, nefazodone 
(Serzone) blocks serotonin at some receptor sites but not others, while bupropion 
(Wellbutrin and Zyban) affects the norepinephrine and dopamine systems (see Figure 2; 
Lucki & O'Leary, 2004; Robinson, 2007; Dhillon, Yang, & Curran, 2008). 

Finally, there are some newer drugs on the horizon. For instance, scientists have 
found that the anesthetic ketamine blocks the neural receptor NMDA, which affects 
the neurotransmitter glutamate. Glutamate plays an important role in mood regula- 
tion and the ability to experience pleasure, and researchers believe that ketamine 
blockers may prove to be useful in the treatment of depression (Skolnick, Popik, & 
Trullas, 2009). 


Permit rise in neurotransmitters 
such as norepinepherine 


Prevent MAO from breaking 


Inhibit reuptake of serotonin 


Can alter transmission of 
impulses within neurons 


(Thorazine), clozapine (Clozaril), 
haloperidol (Haldol) 


Atypical Antipsychotic: 
rizperadine, olanzapine 


Trazodone (Desyrel), 
amitriptyline (Elavil), 
desipramine (Norpamin) 
Phenelzine (Nardil), 
tranylcypromine (Parnate) 


Fluoxetine (Prozac), Luvox, Paxil, 
Celexa, Zoloft, nefazodone 
(Serzone) 


Lithium (Lithonate), Depakote, 
Tegretol 


Benzodiazepines (Valium, 
Xanax) 


antidepressant drugs Medications 
that improve a severely depressed 
patient’s mood and feeling of well- 
being. 


dy Alert 
To help organize your study 
of different drugs used in 
therapy, review Figure 1, 
which classifies them 
according to the catego- 
ries of antipsychotic, atypical 
antipsychotic, antidepressant, 
mood-stabilizing, and antianxiety 
drugs. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


566 Chapter 16 Treatment of Psychological Disorders 


FIGURE 2 In (a), selective serotonin | | q 


reuptake inhibitors (SSRIs) reduce | New N Renie | Neuron N 
depression by permitting the neuro- | blocked Nefazodone / à 
transmitter serotonin to remain in the / oO / ® 
synapse. In (b), a newer antidepressant, © © 





selectively to block serotonin at some 


J ( 
sites but not others, which helps to i n= \ : rh =) a 
reduce the side effects of the drug. U 5 y 5 y ww 
S SSRI —® D a ® i; 
(Source: Based on Mischoulon, 2000.) ynapse @ > v o fai gl = g ® ynapse 


p a 


p” 


Nefazodone (Serzone), operates more Q 2 2 C. 4 © & ® © 
\ D / 








\ 


f Receptor neuron Receptor neuron | 


(a) 7 (b) 


The overall success rates of antidepressant drugs is good. Unlike antipsychotic 
drugs, antidepressants can produce lasting, long-term recovery from depression. In 
many cases, even after patients stop taking the drugs, their depression does not 
return. On the other hand, antidepressant drugs may produce side effects such as 
drowsiness and faintness, and there is evidence that SSRI antidepressants can increase 
the risk of suicide in children and adolescents (Gibbons et al., 2007; Leckman & King, 
2007; Olfson & Marcus, 2008). 

Consumers spend billions of dollars each year on antidepressant drugs. In par- 
ticular, the antidepressant Fluoxetine, sold under the trade name Prozac, has been 
highlighted on magazine covers and has been the topic of best-sellers. 

Does Prozac deserve its acclaim? In some respects, yes. It is effective and has 
relatively few side effects. Furthermore, many people who do not respond to other 
types of antidepressants do well on Prozac. On the other hand, 20-30% of users 
report experiencing nausea and diarrhea, and a smaller number report sexual dys- 
functions (Kramer, 1993; Brambilla et al., 2005; Fenter, 2006). 

Another substance that has received a great deal of publicity is St. John’s wort, 
an herb that some have called a “natural” antidepressant. Although it is widely used 
in Europe for the treatment of depression, the U.S. Food and Drug Administration 
considers it a dietary supplement, and therefore the substance is available here with- 
out a prescription. 

Prozac is a widely prescribed—but still Despite the popularity of St. John’s wort, definitive clinical tests have found that 

controversial—antidepressant. the herb is ineffective in the treatment of depression. However, because some research 
shows that the herb successfully reduces certain psychological symptoms, some pro- 
ponents argue that using it is reasonable. In any case, people should not use St. John’s 
wort to medicate themselves without consulting a mental health-care professional 
(Williams et al., 2000; Shelton et al., 2002; Thachil, Mohan & Bhugra, 2007). 





MOOD STABILIZERS 


mood stabilizers Drugs used to treat Mood stabilizers are used to treat mood disorders. For example, the drug lithium, a 
mood disorders that prevent manic form of mineral salts, has been used very successfully in patients with bipolar dis- 
episodes of bipolar disorder. orders. Although no one knows definitely why, lithium and other mood stabilizers 


such as divalproex sodium (Depakote) and carbamazepine (Tegretol) effectively reduce 
manic episodes. However, they do not effectively treat depressive phases of bipolar 
disorder, so antidepressants are usually prescribed during those phases (Abraham & 
Calabrese, 2007; L. Smith et al., 2007; Salvi et al., 2008). 

Lithium and similar drugs have a quality that sets them apart from other drug 
treatments: They can be a preventive treatment that blocks future episodes of manic 
depression. Often, people who have had episodes of bipolar disorder can take a daily 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 51 Biomedical Therapy: Biological Approaches to Treatment 567 


dose of lithium to prevent a recurrence of their symptoms. Most other drugs are 
useful only when symptoms of psychological disturbance occur. 


ANTIANXIETY DRUGS 


As the name implies, antianxiety drugs reduce the level of anxiety a person experi- 
ences and increase feelings of well-being. They are prescribed not only to reduce 
general tension in people who are experiencing temporary difficulties, but also to aid 
in the treatment of more serious anxiety disorders (Zito, 1993). 

Antianxiety drugs such as Xanax and Valium are among the medications physi- 
cians most frequently prescribe. In fact, more than half of all U.S. families have 
someone who has taken such a drug at one time or another. 

Although the popularity of antianxiety drugs suggests that they hold few risks, 
they can produce a number of potentially serious side effects. For instance, they can 
cause fatigue, and long-term use can lead to dependence. Moreover, when taken in 
combination with alcohol, some antianxiety drugs can be lethal. But a more impor- 
tant issue concerns their use to suppress anxiety. Almost every therapeutic approach 
to psychological disturbance views continuing anxiety as a signal of some other sort 
of problem. Thus, drugs that mask anxiety may simply be hiding other difficulties. 
Consequently, rather than confronting their underlying problems, people may be 
hiding from them through the use of antianxiety drugs. 





Electroconvulsive Therapy (ECT) 


First introduced in the 1930s, electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) is a procedure used 
in the treatment of severe depression. In the procedure, an electric current of 70-150 
volts is briefly administered to a patient’s head, which causes a loss of consciousness 
and often causes seizures. Typically, health-care professionals sedate patients and 
give them muscle relaxants before administering the current; such preparations help 
reduce the intensity of muscle contractions produced during ECT. The typical patient 
receives about 10 ECT treatments in the course of a month, but some patients con- 
tinue with maintenance treatments for months afterward (Greenberg & Kellner, 2005; 
Stevens & Harper, 2007). 

ECT is a controversial technique. Apart from the obvious distastefulness of a treat- 
ment that evokes images of electrocution, side effects occur frequently. For instance, 
after treatment patients often experience disorientation, confusion, and sometimes 
memory loss that may remain for months. Furthermore, ECT often does not produce 
long-term improvement; one study found that without follow-up medication, depres- 
sion returned in most patients who had undergone ECT treatments. Finally, even when 
ECT does work, we do not know why, and some critics believe it may cause permanent 
brain damage (Sackeim et al., 2001; Gardner & O’Connor, 2008; Kato, 2009). 

In light of the drawbacks to ECT, why do therapists use it at all? Basically, they 
use it because in many severe cases of depression, it offers the only quickly effective 
treatment. For instance, it may prevent depressed, suicidal individuals from commit- 
ting suicide, and it can act more quickly than antidepressive medications. 

The use of ECT has risen in the last decade with more than 100,000 people 
undergoing it each year. Still, ECT tends to be used only when other treatments have 
proved ineffective, and researchers continue to search for alternative treatments 
(Fink, 2000; Eranti & McLoughlin, 2003; Pandya, Pozuelo, & Malone, 2007). 

One new and promising alternative to ECT is transcranial magnetic stimulation 
(TMS). TMS creates a precise magnetic pulse in a specific area of the brain. By acti- 
vating particular neurons, TMS has been effective in relieving the symptoms of major 
depression in a number of controlled experiments. However, the therapy can produce 
side effects, such as seizures and convulsions, and it is still considered experimental 
(Lefaucheur et al., 2007; Leo & Latif, 2007; Kim, Pesiridou, & O’Reardon, 2009). 


antianxiety drugs Drugs that reduce 
the level of anxiety a person experi- 
ences essentially by reducing excitabil- 
ity and increasing feelings of well-being. 


electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) 

A procedure used in the treatment of 
severe depression in which an electric 
current of 70-150 volts is briefly 
administered to a patient's head. 


transcranial magnetic stimulation 
(TMS) A depression treatment in 
which a precise magnetic pulse is 
directed to a specific area of the brain. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


568 Chapter 16 Treatment of Psychological Disorders 


psychosurgery Brain surgery once 
used to reduce the symptoms of 
mental disorder but rarely used today. 





Psychosurgery 


If ECT strikes you as a questionable procedure, the use of psychosurgery—brain 
surgery in which the object is to reduce symptoms of mental disorder—probably 
appears even more dubious. A technique used only rarely today, psychosurgery was 
introduced as a “treatment of last resort” in the 1930s. 

The initial form of psychosurgery, a prefrontal lobotomy, consisted of surgically 
destroying or removing parts of a patient’s frontal lobes, which surgeons thought 
controlled emotionality. In the 1930s and 1940s, surgeons performed the procedure 
on thousands of patients often with little precision. For example, in one common 
technique, a surgeon literally would jab an ice pick under a patient’s eyeball and 
swivel it back and forth (El-Hai, 2005; Ogren & Sandlund, 2007). 

Psychosurgery often did improve a patient’s behavior—but not without drastic 
side effects. Along with remission of the symptoms of the mental disorder, patients 
sometimes experienced personality changes and became bland, colorless, and unemo- 
tional. In other cases, patients became aggressive and unable to control their impulses. 
In the worst cases, treatment resulted in the patient’s death. 

With the introduction of effective drug treatments—and the obvious ethical ques- 
tions regarding the appropriateness of forever altering someone’s personality— 
psychosurgery became nearly obsolete. However, it is still used in very rare cases 
when all other procedures have failed and the patient’s behavior presents a high risk 
to the patient and others. For example, surgeons sometimes use a more precise form 
of psychosurgery called a cingulotomy in rare cases of obsessive-compulsive disorder 
in which they destroy tissue in the anterior cignulate area of the brain. In another 
technique, gamma knife surgery, beams of radiation are used to destroy areas of the 
brain related to obsessive-compulsive disorder (Shah et al., 2008; Carey, 2009c; Lopes 
et al, 2009; Wilkinson, 2009). 

Occasionally, dying patients with severe, uncontrollable pain also receive psy- 
chosurgery. Still, even these cases raise important ethical issues, and psychosurgery 
remains a highly controversial treatment (Mashour, Walker, & Martuza, 2005; Steele 
et al., 2007). 


Biomedical Therapies 
in Perspective 


In some respects, no greater revolution has occurred in the field of mental health 
than biological approaches to treatment. As previously violent, uncontrollable patients 
have been calmed by the use of drugs, mental hospitals have been able to concentrate 
more on actually helping patients and less on custodial functions. Similarly, patients 
whose lives have been disrupted by depression or bipolar episodes have been able 
to function normally, and other forms of drug therapy have also shown remarkable 
results. 

The use of biomedical therapy for everyday problems is rising. For example, one 
survey of users of a college counseling service found that from 1989 to 2001, the 
proportion of students receiving treatment who were taking medication for psycho- 
logical disorders increased from 10% to 25% (Benton et al., 2003). 

Furthermore, new forms of biomedical therapy are promising. For example, the 
newest treatment possibility—which remains experimental at this point—is gene 
therapy. As we discussed when considering behavioral genetics, specific genes may 
be introduced to particular regions of the brain. These genes then have the potential 
to reverse or even prevent biochemical events that give rise to psychological disor- 
ders (Sapolsky, 2003; Lymberis et al., 2004; Tuszynski, 2007). 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 51 Biomedical Therapy: Biological Approaches to Treatment 569 























© Paul Noth/The New Yorker Collection/www. 


cartoonbank.com. 














“The drug has, however, proved more effective We 
than traditional psychoanalysis.” wo 


Despite their current usefulness and future promise, biomedical therapies do not 
represent a cure-all for psychological disorders. For one thing, critics charge that such 
therapies merely provide relief of the symptoms of mental disorder; as soon as the 
drugs are withdrawn, the symptoms return. Although it is considered a major step 
in the right direction, biomedical treatment may not solve the underlying problems 
that led a patient to therapy in the first place. Biomedical therapies also can produce 
side effects that range from minor to serious physical reactions to the development 
of new symptoms of abnormal behavior. Finally, an overreliance on biomedical ther- 
apies may lead therapists to overlook alternative forms of treatment that may be 
helpful. 

Still, biomedical therapies—sometimes alone and more often in conjunction with 
psychotherapy—have permitted millions of people to function more effectively. Fur- 
thermore, although biomedical therapy and psychotherapy appear distinct, research 
shows that biomedical therapies ultimately may not be as different from talk thera- 
pies as one might imagine, at least in terms of their consequences. 

Specifically, measures of brain functioning as a result of drug therapy compared 
with psychotherapy show little difference in outcomes. For example, one study com- 
pared the reactions of patients with major depression who received either an anti- 
depressant drug or psychotherapy. After six weeks of either therapy, activity in the 
portion of the brain related to the disorder—the basal ganglia—had changed in sim- 
ilar ways, and that area appeared to function more normally. Although such research 
is not definitive, it does suggest that at least for some disorders, psychotherapy may 
be just as effective as biomedical interventions—and vice versa. Research also makes 
it clear that no single treatment is effective universally and that each type of treat- 
ment has both advantages and disadvantages (Hollon, Thase, & Markowitz, 2002; 
DeRubeis, Hollon, & Shelton, 2003; Pinquart, Duberstein, & Lyness, 2006; Greenberg 
& Goldman, 2009). 






y Alert 

Remember that biomedical 
treatments have both bene- 
fits and drawbacks. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


570 Chapter 16 Treatment of Psychological Disorders 





While deinstitutionalization has had 
many successes, it has also contributed 
to the release of mental patients into 
the community with little or no support. 
As a result, many have become 
homeless. 


community psychology A branch of 
psychology that focuses on the 
prevention and minimization of 
psychological disorders in the 
community. 


deinstitutionalization The transfer of 
former mental patients from institu- 
tions to the community. 


Community Psychology: 
Focus on Prevention 


Each of the treatments we have reviewed has a common element: It is a “restorative” 
treatment aimed at alleviating psychological difficulties that already exist. However, 
an approach known as community psychology has a different aim: to prevent or 
minimize the incidence of psychological disorders. 

Community psychology came of age in the 1960s, when mental health profes- 
sionals developed plans for a nationwide network of community mental health cen- 
ters. The hope was that those centers would provide low-cost mental health services, 
including short-term therapy and community educational programs. In another 
development, the population of mental hospitals has plunged as drug treatments 
made physical restraint of patients unnecessary. 

This transfer of former mental patients out of institutions and into the commu- 
nity—a process known as deinstitutionalization—was encouraged by the growth of 
the community psychology movement (see Figure 3). Proponents of deinstitutional- 
ization wanted to ensure not only that deinstitutionalized patients received proper 
treatment, but also that their civil rights were maintained (Wolff, 2002; St. Dennis et 
al., 2006). 

Unfortunately, the promise of deinstitutionalization has not been met largely 
because insufficient resources are provided to deinstitutionalized patients. What 
started as a worthy attempt to move people out of mental institutions and into the 
community ended, in many cases, with former patients being dumped into the com- 
munity without any real support. Many became homeless—between 15-35% of all 
homeless adults are thought to have a major psychological disorder—and some 
became involved in illegal acts caused by their disorders. In short, many people who 





4000 


3000 


Outpatient 
facilities 


N 
So 
Q 
[=] 


Inpatient 
facilities 


Mental health care patient episodes 
per 100,000 population 


1000 


State mental 
hospitals 











1950 1960 1970 1980 1990 2000 
Year 


FIGURE 3 As deinstitutionalization has become more prevalent over the last half century, the 
number of patients being treated in state mental hospitals has declined significantly, while the 
number of outpatient facilities has increased. (Source: From Rodger Doyle, “Deinstitutionalization, 
Scientific American, December 2002, p. 38. Copyright 2002 Rodger Doyle. Reprinted with permission.) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 51 Biomedical Therapy: Biological Approaches to Treatment 





© Gahan Wilson/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


“Looking good!” 


need treatment do not get it, and in some cases care for people with psychological 
disorders has simply shifted from one type of treatment site to another (Shinn et al., 
2007; Dumont & Dumont, 2008; Price, 2009). 

On the other hand, the community psychology movement has had some positive 
outcomes. Its emphasis on prevention has led to new approaches to psychological 
disorders as we discuss in Applying Psychology in the 21st Century. Furthermore, tele- 
phone “hot lines” are now common. At any time of the day or night, people expe- 
riencing acute stress can call a trained, sympathetic listener who can provide 
immediate—although obviously limited—treatment (Reese, Conoley, & Brossart, 
2002; Paukert, Stagner, & Hope, 2004; Cauce, 2007). 

College and high school crisis centers are another innovation that grew out of the 
community psychology movement. Modeled after suicide prevention hot-line centers 
(services that enable potential suicide victims to call and speak to someone about their 
difficulties), crisis centers give callers an opportunity to discuss life crises with a sym- 
pathetic listener, who is often a volunteer. 


571 


Applying Psy gy in 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


the 21st Century 





Beating the Odds: Preventing 
Psychological Disorders 
Before They Start 


When we think about the possibility of 
contracting physical diseases such as can- 
cer, diabetes, or heart disease, we often 
think of preventive measures we can take: 
lose weight, eat nutritious foods, exercise 
regularly, don’t smoke. But when we think 
of psychological disorders such as depres- 
sion, anxiety disorders, or schizophrenia, 
we tend to assume that we are just passive 
victims and can do nothing to prevent 
them. But is that assumption really true? 

Rather than treating psychological dis- 
orders after the symptoms fully appear, 
some researchers now suggest that an al- 
ternative might be to provide appropriate 
intervention as soon as the initial signs of 
an impending problem emerge before a 
disorder fully develops. Taking advantage 
of opportunities to potentially stop or 
reduce the progression of psychological 
disorders would reduce the cost of mental 
health care as well as lessen the suffering 
of individual patients. Essentially, these 
researchers argue, it is cheaper and less 
painful to prevent a problem from occur- 
ring than it is to fix it after the fact (Cloud, 
2009; National Research Council and In- 
stitute of Medicine of the National Acad- 
emies, 2009). 

But can psychological disorders be pre- 
vented, particularly when they have a 
genetic component? The answer, according 
to a growing body of research, is yes. The 
expression of a genetic disposition toward 
mental illness often depends on environ- 
mental factors. Research is finding that 
young people who are at risk of develop- 
ing a disorder (because one of their parents 
suffers from the disorder themselves, for 
example) benefit from school- and home- 
based interventions that reduce the envi- 
ronmental stress factors that might trigger 
the disorder. 

Consider, for example, depression. 
Children of depressed parents are at 


572 





Early intervention can prevent psychological disorders from becoming more serious. 


increased risk of developing depression 
themselves. But when these children are 
active and engaged with others, and when 
they understand what depression is and 
that it can be treated, their increased risk of 
becoming depressed appears to diminish. 
Specifically, family-based interventions to 
help children of depressed parents develop 
resilience have been shown to be effective 
in reducing the onset of depression. In such 
programs, parents are taught communica- 
tion skills, and the children are taught cop- 
ing skills. In addition, the entire family is 
educated about depression and how it af- 
fects their relationships with one another. 
Interventions such as this can reduce 
children’s predepressive symptoms and 
provide better long-term outcomes (Beards- 


ETHINK 


lee et al., 2007; Riley et al. 2008; Garber et 
al., 2009). 

Other research suggests that early inter- 
ventions can reduce the onset of additional 
psychological disorders and related prob- 
lems, such as substance abuse, conduct dis- 
orders, antisocial behavior, aggression, and 
even schizophrenia. The key is to catch and 
stop these problems early in life among at- 
risk children. 

The potential benefits in terms of im- 
proved quality of life for those individuals 
as well as potential benefits to society of 
reducing the cost of care are great, generat- 
ing increasing interest in approaches to 
prevention of psychological disorders (Na- 
tional Research Council and Institute of 
Medicine of the National Academies, 2009). 





of disorders? Why or why not? 


disorder? Why or why not? 





e Because it is important to catch the symptoms of psychological disorders early on, 
would you favor routine in-school behavioral screening of children for symptoms 


e Do you think children of individuals with moderate or severe psychological disor- 
ders should receive genetic screening to determine if they carry genes related to the 








www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 51 Biomedical Therapy: Biological Approaches to Treatment 573 


If you decide to seek therapy, you're faced with a daunting task. 
Choosing a therapist is not a simple matter. One place to begin 
the process of identifying a therapist is at the “Help Center” of 
the American Psychological Association at http://www.apa 
.org/helpcenter. And, if you start therapy, several general 
guidelines can help you determine whether you’ve made the 
right choice: 

You and your therapist should agree on the goals for 
treatment. They should be clear, specific, and attainable. 


BECOMING AN 
INFORMED CONSUMER 


of Psychology 


Choosing the Right Therapist 


e You should feel comfortable with your therapist. You should not be intimidated by or 
in awe of a therapist. Rather, you should trust the therapist and feel free to 
discuss the most personal issues without fearing a negative reaction. In sum, the 


“personal chemistry” should be right. 


e Therapists should have appropriate training and credentials and should be licensed by 


appropriate state and local agencies. Check therapists’ membership in national and 
state professional associations. In addition, the cost of therapy, billing practices, 
and other business matters should be clear. It is not a breach of etiquette to put 
these matters on the table during an initial consultation. 

You should feel that you are making progress after therapy has begun, despite occasional 
setbacks. If you have no sense of improvement after repeated visits, you and your 
therapist should discuss this issue frankly. Although there is no set timetable, the 
most obvious changes resulting from therapy tend to occur relatively early in the 


course of treatment. For instance, half of patients in psychotherapy improve by 
the 8th session, and three-fourths by the 26th session. The average number of 
sessions with college students is just 5 (Crits-Cristoph, 1992; HMHL, 1994; 


Lazarus, 1997). 


Be aware that you will have to put in a great deal of effort in therapy. Although our 
culture promises quick cures for any problem, in reality, solving difficult problems is not 
easy. You must be committed to making therapy work and should know that it is you, 
not the therapist, who must do most of the work to resolve your problems. The effort 
has the potential to pay off handsomely—as you experience a more positive, fulfilling, 


and meaningful life. 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 


How are drug, electroconvulsive, and psychosurgical tech- 
niques used today in the treatment of psychological disorders? 
e Biomedical treatment approaches suggest that therapy 
should focus on the physiological causes of abnormal 
behavior rather than considering psychological factors. 
Drug therapy, the best example of biomedical treatments, 
has brought about dramatic reductions in the symptoms 
of mental disturbance. (p. 564) 
e Antipsychotic drugs such as chlorpromazine very effec- 
tively reduce psychotic symptoms. Antidepressant drugs 
such as Prozac reduce depression so successfully that 


they are used very widely. Antianxiety drugs, or minor 
tranquilizers, are among the most frequently prescribed 
medications of any sort. (p. 564) 

In electroconvulsive therapy (ECT), used in severe cases 
of depression, a patient receives a brief electric current of 
70 to 150 volts. (p. 567) 

Psychosurgery typically consists of surgically destroying 
or removing certain parts of a patient’s brain. (p. 568) 
The community psychology approach encouraged 
deinstitutionalization in which previously hospitalized 
mental patients were released into the community. 

(p. 570) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


574 Chapter 16 Treatment of Psychological Disorders 


EVALUATE RETHINK 

1. Antipsychotic drugs have provided effective, long-term, 1. One of the main criticisms of biological therapies is that 
and complete cures for schizophrenia. True or false? they treat the symptoms of mental disorder without un- 

2. One highly effective biomedical treatment for a psycho- covering and treating the underlying problems from 
logical disorder that is used mainly to arrest and prevent which people are suffering. Do you agree with this criti- 
manic-depressive episodes, is cism? Why? 
a. Chlorpromazine 2. From the perspective of a politician: How would you go 
b. Lithium about regulating the use of electroconvulsive therapy and 
c. Librium psychosurgery? Would you restrict their use or make ei- 
d. Valium ther one completely illegal? Why? 


3. Psychosurgery has grown in popularity as a method of 
treatment as surgical techniques have become more pre- ; 
p uUoyezyeuoynyysurop ‘p ‘4108991 
cise. True or false? yse] JO 4uəwzea]} L se ATUO pasn Mou st Á1ə3msoypÁsd ‘əsjez 'g fq ‘Z 
4. The trend toward releasing more patients from mental fuoyeorpour Aq paid jou mq paTjoryuod aq ued eruarydoztyps ‘as[ey ‘L 
hospitals and into the community is known as 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


KEY TERMS 


drug therapy p. 564 antianxiety drugs p. 567 transcranial magnetic community 
antipsychotic drugs p. 564 electroconvulsive stimulation (TMS) p. 567 psychology p. 570 
antidepressant drugs p. 565 therapy (ECT) p. 567 psychosurgery p. 568 deinstitutionalization p. 570 


mood stabilizers p. 566 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Psychology on the Web 


1. Investigate computer-assisted psychotherapy on the web. Locate (a) a computerized 
therapy program, such as ELIZA, which offers “therapy” over the Internet, and (b) a 
report on “cybertherapy,’ in which therapists use the web to interact with patients. 
Compare the two approaches; describe how each one works and relate it to the 
therapeutic approaches you have studied. 

2. Find more information on the web about deinstitutionalization. Try to find pro and 
con arguments about it and summarize the arguments, including your judgment of 
the effectiveness and advisability of deinstitutionalization as an approach to dealing 
with mental illness. 


E © | log U = We have examined how psychological professionals treat 
people with psychological disorders. We have considered 
a range of approaches that include both psychologically based and biologically based 
therapies. Clearly, the field has made substantial progress in recent years both in treating 
the symptoms of mental disorders and in understanding their underlying causes. 

Before we leave the topic of treatment of psychological disorders, turn back to the 
prologue in which Marg MacKrell’s Tourette syndrome was treated by a new therapy. On 
the basis of your understanding of the treatment of psychological disorders, consider the 
following questions. 





1. What approach to therapy seemed to be used successfully with Marg MacKrell? 

2. How might Marg MacKrell have been treated differently by a therapist using a 
psychodynamic approach? What might be some drawbacks to using this approach? 

3. How might a family therapist have approached Marg MacKrell's problem differently? 





A 


575 











CHAPTER 17 


Social Psychology 











www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Key Concepts for Chapter 17 


MODULE 52 







What are attitudes, and how are they formed, Attitudes and Social Cognition 


x š Persuasion: Changing Attitudes 
maintained, and changed? @ How often do Aad 
PsychWork: Advertising Agency Creator 


people form impressions of what others are Social Cognition: Understanding Others 


Exploring Diversity: Attributions in a 
Cultural Context—How Fundamental Is 
the Fundamental Attribution Error? 


like and the causes of their behavior? @ What 
are the biases that influence the ways in which 
people view others’ behavior? 


MODULE 53 










What are the major sources and tactics of Social Influence and Groups 

soa Conformity: Following What Others Do 

social influence? 3 H 
Compliance: Submitting to 


Direct Social Pressure 
Obedience: Following Direct Orders 






MODULE 54 






How do stereotypes, prejudice, and Prejudice and Discrimination 


4 weds r ; The Foundati f Prejudi 
discrimination differ? @ How can we reduce P wiuuak ches 
Neuroscience in Your Life: 


prejudice and discrimination? The Prejudiced Brain 


Measuring Prejudice and Discrimination: 
The Implicit Personality Test 





Reducing the Consequences 
of Prejudice and Discrimination 


MODULE 55 






Why are we attracted to certain people, and Positive and Negative 


Social Behavior 


; f f r 3 
what progression do social relationships follow? P ET 


@ What factors underlie aggression and prosocial Attaction and the Development 
of Relationships 


behavior? Applying Psychology in the 21st Century: 
Friends Online—Is Facebook 

the New Student Union? 
Aggression and Prosocial Behavior: 
Hurting and Helping Others 
Helping Others: The Brighter 

Side of Human Nature 
Neuroscience in Your Life: Moral 
Decisions and the Brain 

Becoming an Informed 

Consumer of Psychology: 

Dealing Effectively with Anger 





577 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Prologue A Gift of Life 





Ten weeks pregnant, Katie Purdom had just picked up daughter 
Victoria Leigh, 4, from preschool in Lebanon, Ky., on a misty 
afternoon... when a neighbor's dog ran in front of her SUV. She 
swerved to avoid it, Causing the car to flip over and land ina 
4-ft.-deep creek; her seat belt locked, Katie was pinned, as was 
Victoria, who was belted into her booster seat in back. “The water 
was coming in everywhere,’ says Katie, 31, a nonswimmer who 
works in the bakery at a local Wal-Mart. “Tori kept saying, ‘I’m 
cold, Momma, help!’ It was terrifying.” As she tried to reach back 
to hold her child's face above the rising water, Katie heard a man’s 


voice: It was Perry Bland, 52, who had spotted them while driving 
on the route he’s covered for 27 years. With calming words, he 
pulled out his plastic letter opener and started to saw away at the 
seat belts. Ten minutes later both mother and daughter were free, 
having escaped with only minor scratches. Now seven months 
along with a baby boy, Katie says, “| owe Perry our lives.’ Perry, 
who has become something of a local celebrity, is just thankful it 
all worked out. “I still get choked up,” he says. “I know how close 

it could have been.’ (Mascia & Servis, 2009, p. 109) 





What led Perry Bland to behave so heroically? Was it simply the 
circumstances, or was it something about the kind of person 
Bland was? What, in general, drives some people to help others— 
and conversely, why do other people show no concern for the 
welfare of others? More broadly, how can we improve social 
conditions so that people can live together in harmony? 

We can fully answer these questions only by taking into 
account findings from the field of social psychology, the branch 
of psychology that focuses on the aspects of human behavior 
that unite—and separate—us from one another. Social psychology 
is the scientific study of how people's thoughts, feelings, and 
actions are affected by others. Social psychologists consider the 
kinds and causes of the individual’s behavior in social situations. 
They examine how the nature of situations in which we find 
ourselves influences our behavior in important ways. 

The broad scope of social psychology is conveyed by the kinds 
of questions social psychologists ask, such as: How can we 
convince people to change their attitudes or adopt new ideas 
and values? In what ways do we come to understand what others 
are like? How are we influenced by what others do and think? 


578 


Why do some people display so much violence, aggression, and 
cruelty toward others that people throughout the world live in 
fear of annihilation at their hands? And why, in comparison, do 
some people place their own lives at risk to help others? In 
exploring these and other questions, we also discuss strategies 
for confronting and solving a variety of problems and issues that 
all of us face—ranging from achieving a better understanding of 
persuasive tactics to forming more accurate impressions of 
others. 

We begin with a look at how our attitudes shape our behavior 
and how we form judgments about others. We'll discuss how we 
are influenced by others, and we will consider prejudice and 
discrimination by focusing on their roots and the ways we can 
reduce them. After examining what social psychologists have 
learned about the ways people form friendships and relation- 
ships, we'll conclude with a look at the determinants of aggres- 
sion and helping—two opposing sides of human behavior. 


social psychology The scientific study of how people's thoughts, 
feelings, and actions are affected by others. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 52 


What do Rachael Ray and Tom Brady have in common? Both have appeared in 
advertisements designed to mold or change our attitudes. Such commercials are part 
of the barrage of messages we receive each day from sources as varied as politicians, 
sales staff in stores, and celebrities—all of which are meant to influence us. 


Persuasion: Changing Attitudes 


Persuasion is the process of changing attitudes, one of the central concepts of social 
psychology. Attitudes are evaluations of a particular person, behavior, belief, or con- 
cept. For example, you probably hold attitudes toward the U.S. president (a person), 
abortion (a behavior), affirmative action (a belief), or architecture (a concept) (Brock 
& Green, 2005; Hegarty & Massey, 2007; Simon & Hoyt, 2008). 

The ease with which we can change our attitudes depends on a number of fac- 
tors, including: 





e Message source. The characteristics of a person who delivers a persuasive message, 
known as an attitude communicator, have a major impact on the effectiveness of 
that message. Communicators who are physically and socially attractive produce 
greater attitude change than those who are less attractive. Moreover, the commu- 
nicator’s expertise and trustworthiness are related to the impact of a message— 
except in situations in which the audience believes the communicator has an 
ulterior motive (Ariyanto, Hornsey, & Gallois, 2006; McClure, Sutton, & Sibley, 
2007; Messner, Reinhard, & Sporer, 2008). 

e Characteristics of the message. It is not just who delivers a message but what the 
message is like that affects attitudes. Generally, two-sided messages—which 
include both the communicator’s position and the one he or she is arguing 
against—are more effective than one-sided messages, given the assumption that 
the arguments for the other side can be effectively refuted and the audience is 
knowledgeable about the topic. In addition, fear-producing messages (“If you 
don’t practice safer sex, you'll get AIDS”) are generally effective when they 
provide the audience with a means for reducing the fear. However, if the fear 
that is aroused is too strong, messages may evoke people’s defense mechanisms 
and be ignored (Perloff, 2003). 

e Characteristics of the target. Once a communicator has delivered a message, charac- 
teristics of the target of the message may determine whether the message will 
be accepted. For example, intelligent people are more resistant to persuasion 
than those who are less intelligent. Gender differences in persuasibility also 
seem to exist. In public settings, women are somewhat more easily persuaded 
than men, particularly when they have less knowledge about the message’s 
topic. However, they are as likely as men to change their private attitudes. In 
fact, the magnitude of the differences in resistance to persuasion between men 
and women is not large (Wood, 2000; Guadagno & Cialdini, 2002). 


What are attitudes, and how 
are they formed, maintained, 
and changed? 


How do people form impres- 
sions of what others are like 
and of the causes of their 
behavior? 


What are the biases that 
influence the ways in which 
people view others’ behavior? 


Evaluations of a particular 
person, behavior, belief, or concept. 





Do celebrities such as Patriot quarterback 
Tom Brady influence attitudes? Advertisers 
certainly believe this is true, investing 
millions of dollars in endorsement fees. 


579 


580 Chapter 17 Social Psychology 


a 


central route processing Message 
interpretation characterized by 
thoughtful consideration of the issues 
and arguments used to persuade. 


peripheral route processing Message 
interpretation characterized by 
consideration of the source and related 
general information rather than of the 
message itself. 


y Alert 


Central route processing in- 

volves the content of the 
message; peripheral route 
processing involves how 

the message is provided. 






www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


= Ea 
= 


FIGURE 1 Routes to persuasion. Targets who are highly involved, motivated, and attentive use 
central route processing when they consider a persuasive message, which leads to a more 
lasting attitude change. In contrast, uninvolved, unmotivated, and inattentive targets are more 
likely to use peripheral route processing, and attitude change is likely to be less enduring. Can 
you think of specific advertisements that try to produce central route processing? 


Peripheral route 


processing 


ROUTES TO PERSUASION 


Recipients’ receptiveness to persuasive messages relates to the type of information- 
processing they use. Social psychologists have discovered two primary information- 
processing routes to persuasion: central route and peripheral route processing. Central 
route processing occurs when the recipient thoughtfully considers the issues and 
arguments involved in persuasion. In central route processing, people are swayed in 
their judgments by the logic, merit, and strength of arguments. 

In contrast, peripheral route processing occurs when people are persuaded on 
the basis of factors unrelated to the nature or quality of the content of a persuasive 
message. Instead, factors that are irrelevant or extraneous to the issue, such as who 
is providing the message, how long the arguments are, or the emotional appeal of 
the arguments, influence them (Petty et al., 2005; Wegener et al., 2004; Warden, Wu, 
& Tsai, 2006). 

In general, people who are highly involved and motivated use central route 
processing to comprehend a message. However, if a person is uninvolved, unmoti- 


annia 


The Need for Cognition 
Which of the following statements apply to you? 


= 


. | really enjoy a task that involves coming up with new solutions to problems. 

. | would prefer a task that is intellectual, difficult, and important to one that is some- 
what important but does not require much thought. 

3. Learning new ways to think doesn’t excite me very much. 

4. The idea of relying on thought to make my way to the top does not appeal to me. 

5. | think only as hard as | have to. 

6 

7 

8 


N 


. | like tasks that require little thought once I've learned them. 
. | prefer to think about small, daily projects rather than long-term ones. 
. | would rather do something that requires little thought than something that is sure 
to challenge my thinking abilities. 
9. | find little satisfaction in deliberating hard and for long hours. 
10. | don't like to be responsible for a situation that requires a lot of thinking. 


Scoring: The more you agree with statements 1 and 2, and disagree with the rest, the greater the 
likelihood that you have a high need for cognition. 


FIGURE 2 This simple questionnaire will give you a general idea of the level of your need for 
cognition. (Source: Cacioppo, Berntson, & Crites, 1996.) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 52 Attitudes and Social Cognition 581 


vated, bored, or distracted, the nature of the message becomes less 
important, and peripheral factors become more critical (see Figure 1). 
Although both central route and peripheral route processing lead 
to attitude change, central route processing generally leads to 
stronger, more lasting attitude change. 

Are some people more likely than others to use central route 
processing rather than peripheral route processing? The answer is 
yes. People who have a high need for cognition, a person’s habitual 
level of thoughtfulness and cognitive activity, are more likely to 
employ central route processing. Consider the statements shown 
in Figure 2. People who agree with the first two statements and 
disagree with the rest have a relatively high need for cognition 
(Cacioppo et al., 1996; Dai & Wang, 2007). 

People who have a high need for cognition enjoy thinking, 














© Mick Stevens/The New Yorker Collection/www. 


cartoonbank.com. 


philosophizing, and reflecting on the world. Consequently, they “T'm neither a good cop nor a bad cop, Jerome. Like yourself, 
tend to reflect more on persuasive messages by using central route I’m a complex amalgam of positive and negative personality 
processing, and they are likely to be persuaded by complex, logical, traits that emerge or not, depending on circumstances. 


and detailed messages. In contrast, those who have a low need for 

cognition become impatient when forced to spend too much time thinking about an 
issue. Consequently, they usually use peripheral route processing and are persuaded 
by factors other than the quality and detail of messages (Dollinger, 2003; VanOver- 
walle & Siebler, 2005). (Also see the PsychWork box.) 


Over the years, businesses have used dozens of different ways 

to catch our attention and lure us into buying their products Psyc hWo r k 

or services. For Vlad Kolarov, owner of the FunnySells.com ADVERTISING AGENCY CREATOR 
advertising agency, humor is the best approach. 

“Humor is universal. It makes us feel better. Everybody 
likes a good laugh, even people who say they do not have a Position: Advertising Agency Creator, 
sense of humor,” he noted. “Companies that use humor in British Columbia, Canada 
advertising campaigns generally enjoy a boost in the sales of 
their product and service. The trick, however, is creating a 
memorable, humorous advertising campaign. Humor and 
comedy, as a genre, are the toughest to master. 

“When done correctly, companies can use it to create and maintain a brand iden- 
tity; introduce a new product, service, or a change in the existing one; increase the 
buzz-value of the brand or the company; and increase sales,” he added. 

Using humor as a promotional tool can be beneficial but tricky as well, according 
to Kolarov. 

“An advertising company needs to determine what the target audience is and 
determine the best way to present the advertised product or service. When things 
are not done correctly, they may have a reverse effect,” he explained. 

Is there anything that can’t be promoted with humor? 

“Td like to say no, but the fact is there are products I personally will choose not 
to advertise with humor. Then again, every rule has an exception. When done clev- 
erly and tastefully, even things we consider impossible can become successful. In this 
business, as in life, one has to keep an open mind,” Kolarov said. 





Name: Vlad Kolarov 


Education: BA in Law, Sofia University, Sofia, 
Bulgaria 


THE LINK BETWEEN ATTITUDES AND BEHAVIOR 


Not surprisingly, attitudes influence behavior. The strength of the link between par- 
ticular attitudes and behavior varies, of course, but generally people strive for con- 
sistency between their attitudes and their behavior. Furthermore, people hold fairly 
consistent attitudes. For instance, you would probably not hold the attitude that 


582 Chapter 17 Social Psychology 


cognitive dissonance The conflict that 
occurs when a person holds two 
contradictory attitudes or thoughts 
(referred to as cognitions). 





Changing perceived 
importance of the cognition 
(“The evidence is weak that 
smoking causes cancer.”) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


eating meat is immoral and still have a positive attitude toward hamburgers (Ajzen, 
2002; Conner et al., 2003; Levi, Chan, & Pence, 2006). 

Ironically, the consistency that leads attitudes to influence behavior sometimes 
works the other way around; in some cases our behavior shapes our attitudes. Con- 
sider, for instance, the following incident: 


You've just spent what you feel is the most boring hour of your life turning pegs for a 
psychology experiment. Just as you finally finish and are about to leave, the experi- 
menter asks you to do him a favor. He tells you that he needs a helper for future 
experimental sessions to introduce subsequent participants to the peg-turning task. Your 
specific job will be to tell them that turning the pegs is an interesting, fascinating 
experience. Each time you tell this tale to another participant, you'll be paid $1. 


If you agree to help the experimenter, you may be setting yourself up for a state of 
psychological tension called cognitive dissonance. According to social psychologist 
Leon Festinger (1957), cognitive dissonance occurs when a person holds two contra- 
dictory attitudes or thoughts (referred to as cognitions). 

If you participate in the situation just described, you are left with two contradic- 
tory thoughts: (1) I believe the task is boring, but (2) I said it was interesting with 
little justification ($1). These two thoughts should arouse dissonance. How can you 
reduce cognitive dissonance? You cannot deny having said that the task is interesting 
without breaking with reality. Relatively speaking, it is easier to change your attitude 
toward the task—and thus the theory predicts that participants will reduce disso- 
nance by adopting more positive attitudes toward the task (Cooper, Mirabile, & 
Scher, 2005; Cooper, 2007; Rydell, McConnell, & Mackie, 2008). 

A classic experiment (Festinger & Carlsmith, 1959) confirmed this prediction. The 
experiment followed essentially the same procedure outlined earlier in which a par- 
ticipant was offered $1 to describe a boring task as interesting. In addition, in a 
comparison condition, some participants were offered $20 to say that the task was 
interesting. The reasoning behind this condition was that $20 was so much money 
that participants in this condition had a good reason to be conveying incorrect infor- 
mation; dissonance would not be aroused, and less attitude change would be 
expected. The results supported this notion. More of the participants who were paid 
$1 changed their attitudes (becoming more positive toward the peg-turning task) 
than participants who were paid $20. 


| Two contradictory cognitions 


| 1. “I smoke.” 
2. “Smoking leads to cancer.” 


| 









FIGURE 3 Cognitive dissonance. The simultaneous presence of two contradictory cognitions 
(“I smoke” and “Smoking leads to cancer”) produces dissonance, which can be reduced 
through several methods. What are additional ways in which dissonance can be reduced? 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 52 Attitudes and Social Cognition 583 


We now know that dissonance explains many everyday events involving attitudes 
and behavior. For example, smokers who know that smoking leads to lung cancer 
hold contradictory cognitions: (1) I smoke, and (2) smoking leads to lung cancer. The 
theory predicts that these two thoughts will lead to a state of cognitive dissonance. 
More important, it predicts that—assuming they don’t change their behavior by 
quitting smoking—smokers will be motivated to reduce their dissonance by one of 
the following methods: (1) modifying one or both of the cognitions, (2) changing the 
perceived importance of one cognition, (3) adding cognitions, or (4) denying that 
the two cognitions are related to each other. Hence, a smoker may decide that he really 
doesn’t smoke all that much or that he’ll quit soon (modifying the cognition), that the 
evidence linking smoking to cancer is weak (changing the importance of a cognition), 
that the amount of exercise he gets compensates for the smoking (adding cognitions), 
or that there is no evidence linking smoking and cancer (denial). Whichever technique 
the smoker uses results in reduced dissonance (see Figure 3). 


Social Cognition: 
Understanding Others 


Regardless of Bill Clinton’s personal transgressions and impeachment trial, many Amer- 
icans genuinely liked him when he was president, and his popularity remained high 
throughout his term in office. Cases like this illustrate the power of our impressions 
and attest to the importance of determining how people develop an understanding of 
others. One of the dominant areas in social psychology during the last few years has 
focused on learning how we come to understand what others are like and how we 
explain the reasons underlying others’ behavior. 





UNDERSTANDING WHAT OTHERS ARE LIKE 


Consider for a moment the enormous amount of information about other people to 
which we are exposed. How can we decide what is important and what is not and 
make judgments about the characteristics of others? Social psychologists interested 
in this question study social cognition—the way people understand and make sense 
of others and themselves. Those psychologists have learned that individuals have 
highly developed schemas, sets of cognitions about people and social experiences. 
Those schemas organize information stored in memory; represent in our minds the 
way the social world operates; and give us a framework to recognize, categorize, and 
recall information relating to social stimuli such as people and groups (Brewer & 
Hewstone, 2003; Moskowitz, 2004; Smith & Semin, 2007). 

We typically hold schemas for specific types of people. Our schema for “teacher,” 
for instance, generally consists of a number of characteristics: knowledge of the sub- 
ject matter he or she is teaching, a desire to impart that knowledge, and an awareness 
of the student’s need to understand what is being said. Or we may hold a schema 
for “mother” that includes the characteristics of warmth, nurturance, and caring. 
Regardless of their accuracy, schemas are important because they organize the way 
in which we recall, recognize, and categorize information about others. Moreover, 
they help us predict what others are like on the basis of relatively little information 
because we tend to fit people into schemas even when we do not have much concrete 
evidence to go on (Bargh & Chartrand, 2000; Ruscher, Fiske, & Schnake, 2000). 


IMPRESSION FORMATION 


How do we decide that Sayreeta is a flirt, Jacob is obnoxious, or Hector is a really 
nice guy? The earliest work on social cognition examined impression formation, the 
process by which an individual organizes information about another person to form 
an overall impression of that person. In a classic study, for instance, students learned 


social cognition The cognitive pro- 
cesses by which people understand and 
make sense of others and themselves. 


schemas Sets of cognitions about 
people and social experiences. 


584 Chapter 17 Social Psychology 


central traits The major traits consid- 
ered in forming impressions of others. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


that they were about to hear a guest lecturer (Kelley, 1950). Researchers told one 
group of students that the lecturer was “a rather warm person, industrious, critical, 
practical, and determined,” and told a second group that he was “a rather cold per- 
son, industrious, critical, practical, and determined.” 

The simple substitution of “cold” for “warm” caused drastic differences in the way 
the students in each group perceived the lecturer even though he gave the same talk 
in the same style in each condition. Students who had been told he was “warm” rated 
him considerably more positively than students who had been told he was “cold.” 

The findings from this experiment led to additional research on impression for- 
mation that focused on the way in which people pay particular attention to certain 
unusually important traits—known as central traits—to help them form an overall 
impression of others. According to this work, the presence of a central trait alters the 
meaning of other traits. Hence, the description of the lecturer as “industrious” pre- 
sumably meant something different when it was associated with the central trait 
“warm” than it meant when it was associated with “cold” (Widmeyer & Loy, 1988; 
Glicksohn & Nahari, 2007). 

Other work on impression formation has used information-processing approaches 
to develop mathematically oriented models of how individual personality traits com- 
bine to create an overall impression. Generally, the results of this research suggest 
that in forming an overall judgment of a person, we use a psychological “average” 
of the individual traits we see just as we would find the mathematical average of 
several numbers (Mignon & Mollaret, 2002). 

We make such impressions remarkably quickly. In just a few seconds, using what 
have been called “thin slices of behavior,” we are able to make judgments of people 
that are accurate and that match those of people who make judgments based on 
longer samples of behavior (Carney, Colvin, & Hall, 2007; Pavitt, 2007; Holleran, 
Mehl, & Levitt, 2009). 

Of course, as we gain more experience with people and see them exhibiting 
behavior in a variety of situations, our impressions of them become more complex. 
However, because our knowledge of others usually has gaps, we still tend to fit 
individuals into personality schemas that represent particular “types” of people. For 
instance, we may hold a “gregarious person” schema made up of the traits of friend- 
liness, aggressiveness, and openness. The presence of just one or two of those traits 
may be sufficient to make us assign a person to a particular schema. 

However, our schemas are susceptible to error. For example, mood affects how 
we perceive others. Happy people form more favorable impressions and make more 
positive judgments than people who are in a bad mood (Forgas & Laham, 2005). 

Even when schemas are not entirely accurate, they serve an important function: 
They allow us to develop expectations about how others will behave. Those expecta- 
tions permit us to plan our interactions with others more easily and serve to simplify 
a complex social world. 


ATTRIBUTION PROCESSES: UNDERSTANDING 
THE CAUSES OF BEHAVIOR 


When Barbara Washington, a new employee at the Ablex Computer Company, com- 
pleted a major staffing project two weeks early, her boss, Yolanda, was delighted. At 
the next staff meeting, she announced how pleased she was with Barbara and explained 
that this was an example of the kind of performance she was looking for in her staff. 
The other staff members looked on resentfully, trying to figure out why Barbara had 
worked night and day to finish the project not just on time but two weeks early. She 
must be an awfully compulsive person, they decided. 


At one time or another, most of us have puzzled over the reasons behind someone’s 
behavior. Perhaps it was in a situation similar to the one above, or it may have been 
in more formal circumstances, such as being a judge on a student judiciary board in 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 52 Attitudes and Social Cognition 585 





Yes 





a cheating case. In contrast to theories of social cognition, which describe how people 
develop an overall impression of others’ personality traits, attribution theory seeks 
to explain how we decide, on the basis of samples of an individual’s behavior, what 
the specific causes of that person’s behavior are. 

The general process we use to determine the causes of behavior and other social 
occurrences proceeds in several steps illustrated in Figure 4. After first noticing that 
something unusual has happened—for example, tennis star Roger Federer has played 
a terrible set of tennis—we try to interpret the meaning of the event. This leads us 
to formulate an initial explanation (maybe Federer stayed up late the night before 
the match). Depending on the time available, the cognitive resources on hand (such 
as the attention we can give to the matter), and our motivation (determined in part 
by how important the event is), we may choose to accept our initial explanation or 
seek to modify it (Federer was sick, perhaps). If we have the time, cognitive resources, 
and motivation, the event triggers deliberate problem solving as we seek a fuller 
explanation. During the problem formulation and resolution stage, we may try out 
several possibilities before we reach a final explanation that seems satisfactory to us 
(Malle, 2004; Brown, 2006). 

In seeking an explanation for behavior, we must answer one central question: Is 
the cause situational or dispositional? Situational causes are those brought about by 
something in the environment. For instance, someone who knocks over a quart of 
milk and then cleans it up probably does the cleaning not because he or she is nec- 
essarily a neat person but because the situation requires it. In contrast, a person who 
spends hours shining the kitchen floor probably does so because he or she is a neat 


FIGURE 4 Determining why people 
behave the way they do. The general 
process we use to determine the causes 
of others’ behavior proceeds in several 
steps. The kind of explanation we come 
up with depends on the time available 
to us, our cognitive resources, and our 
degree of motivation to come up with 
an accurate explanation. If time, cogni- 
tive resources, and motivation are 
limited, we'll make use of our first 
impression, which may be inaccurate. 
(Source: Adapted from Krull & Anderson, 
1997, p. 2.) 


attribution theory The theory of 
personality that seeks to explain how 
we decide, on the basis of samples of 
an individual’s behavior, what the 
specific causes of that person’s 
behavior are. 


situational causes (of behavior) 
Perceived causes of behavior that 
are based on environmental factors. 


586 Chapter 17 Social Psychology 


dispositional causes (of behavior) Per- 
ceived causes of behavior that are based 
on internal traits or personality factors. 





y Alert 


The central question in 
making an attribution is 
whether the cause of be- 
havior is due to situational 
or dispositional factors. 


halo effect A phenomenon in which 
an initial understanding that a person 
has positive traits is used to infer other 
uniformly positive characteristics. 


assumed-similarity bias The ten- 
dency to think of people as being 
similar to oneself even when meeting 
them for the first time. 


The assumed similarity bias leads us to 
believe that others hold similar attitudes, 
opinions, and likes and dislikes. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


person. Hence, the behavior has a dispositional cause—that is, it is prompted by the 
person’s disposition (his or her internal traits or personality characteristics). 

In our example involving Barbara Washington, her fellow employees attributed 
her behavior to her disposition rather than to the situation. But from a logical stand- 
point, it is equally plausible that something about the situation caused the behavior. 
If asked, Barbara might attribute her accomplishment to situational factors and 
explain that she had so much other work to do she just had to get the project out of 
the way or the project was not all that difficult and was easy to complete ahead of 
schedule. To her, then, the reason for her behavior might not be dispositional at all; 
it could be situational. 


ATTRIBUTION BIASES: TO ERR IS HUMAN 


If we always processed information in the rational manner that attribution theory 
suggests, the world might run a lot more smoothly. Unfortunately, although attribu- 
tion theory generally makes accurate predictions, people do not always process infor- 
mation about others as logically as the theory seems to suggest. In fact, research 
reveals consistent biases in the ways people make attributions. Typical biases include 
the following: 


e The halo effect. Harry is intelligent, kind, and loving. Is he also conscientious? 
If you were to guess, your most likely response probably would be yes. Your 
guess reflects the halo effect, a phenomenon in which an initial understanding 
that a person has positive traits is used to infer other uniformly positive 
characteristics. The opposite would also hold true. Learning that Harry was 
unsociable and argumentative would probably lead you to assume that he was 
lazy as well. However, few people have either uniformly positive or uniformly 
negative traits, so the halo effect leads to misperceptions of others (Goffin, 
Jelley, & Wagner, 2003; Dennis, 2007). 

e Assumed-similarity bias. How similar to you—in terms of attitudes, opinions, 
likes, and dislikes—are your friends and acquaintances? Most people believe 
that their friends and acquaintances are fairly similar to themselves. But this 
feeling goes beyond just people we know to a general tendency—known as 
the assumed-similarity bias—to think of people as being similar to oneself 
even when meeting them for the first time. Given the range of people in the 
world, this assumption often reduces the accuracy of our judgments (Lemay, 
Clark, & Feeney, 2007; Lemay & Clark, 2008). 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 52 


e The self-serving bias. When their teams win, coaches usually feel that the 
success is due to their coaching. But when their teams lose, coaches may think 
it’s due to their players’ poor skills. Similarly, if you get an A on a test, you 
may think it’s due to your hard work, but if you get a poor grade, it’s due to 
the professor’s inadequacies. The reason is the self-serving bias, the tendency 
to attribute success to personal factors (skill, ability, or effort) and attribute 
failure to factors outside oneself (Krusemark, Campbell, & Clementz, 2008; 
Shepperd, Malone, & Sweeny, 2008). 

e The fundamental attribution error. One of the more common attribution biases 
is the tendency to overattribute others’ behavior to dispositional causes and 
the corresponding failure to recognize the importance of situational causes. 
Known as the fundamental attribution error, this tendency is prevalent in 
Western cultures. We tend to exaggerate the importance of personality 
characteristics (dispositional causes) in producing others’ behavior and 
minimize the influence of the environment (situational factors). For example, 
we are more likely to jump to the conclusion that someone who is often late 
to work is too lazy to take an earlier bus (a dispositional cause) than to 
assume that the lateness is due to situational factors, such as the bus always 
running behind schedule. 

Why is the fundamental attribution error so common? One reason pertains 
to the nature of information available to the people making an attribution. 
When we view another person’s behavior in a particular setting, the most 
conspicuous information is the person’s behavior. Because the individual's 
immediate surroundings remain relatively unchanged and less attention 
grabbing, we center our attention on the person whose behavior we're consid- 
ering. Consequently, we are more likely to make attributions based on personal 
dispositional factors and less likely to make attributions relating to the situation 
(Follett & Hess, 2002; Langdridge & Butt, 2004; Tal-Or & Papirman, 2007). 


Social psychologists’ awareness of attribution biases has led, in part, to the devel- 
opment of a new branch of economics called behavioral economics. Behavioral economics 
is concerned with how individuals’ biases and irrationally affect economic decisions. 
Rather than viewing people as rational, thoughtful decision makers who are impar- 
tially weighing choices to draw conclusions, behavioral economists focus on the irra- 
tionality of judgments (Ariely & Norton, 2009). 


culture in which we are raised clearly plays a role in the way we 
attribute others’ behavior. 

Take, for example, the fundamental attribution error: the 
tendency to overestimate the importance of personal, dispositional 
factors and underattribute situational factors in determining the 
causes of others’ behavior. The error is pervasive in Western 
cultures and not in Eastern societies. For instance, adults in India were more likely to use 
situational attributions than dispositional ones in explaining events. These findings are 
the opposite of those for the United States, and they contradict the fundamental attribution 
error (Miller, 1984, Lien et al., 2006). 

One reason for the difference may lie in the norms and values of Eastern society, 
which emphasize social responsibility and societal obligations to a greater extent than 
Western societies. In addition, the language spoken in a culture may lead to different 
sorts of attributions. For instance, a tardy person using English may say, “I am late”; 
this suggests a personal, dispositional cause (“I am a tardy person”). In contrast, 
speakers of Spanish who are late say, “The clock caused me to be late.” Clearly, the 
statement in Spanish implies that the cause is situational (Zebrowitz-McArthur, 1988; 
Macduff, 2006; Alon & Brett, 2007). 


Attribution biases do not affect all of us in the same way. The Ex p | O ri n g EE 4 s 


Attribution Biases in a Cultural 
Context: How Fundamental 
Is the Fundamental Attribution Error? 


Attitudes and Social Cognition 


587 


self-serving bias The tendency to 


attribute personal success to p 


ersonal 


factors (skill, ability, or effort) and to 
attribute failure to factors outside 


oneself. 


fundamental attribution error 

A tendency to overattribute others’ 
behavior to dispositional causes and 
minimize of the importance of 


situational causes. 


on 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


588 Chapter 17 Social Psychology 


Cultural differences in attributions affect subsequent behavior. For example, parents 
in Asia tend to attribute good academic performance to effort and hard work (situational 
factors). In contrast, parents in Western cultures tend to de-emphasize the role of effort 
and attribute school success to innate ability (a dispositional factor). As a result, Asian 
students in general may strive harder to achieve and ultimately outperform U.S. 
students in school (Stevenson, Lee, & Mu, 2000; Lien et al., 2006). 

The difference in thinking between people in Asian and Western cultures is a 
reflection of a broader difference in the way the world is perceived. Asian societies 
generally have a collectivistic orientation, a worldview that promotes the notion of 
interdependence. People with a collectivistic orientation generally see themselves as 
parts of a larger, interconnected social network and as responsible to others. In contrast, 
people in Western cultures are more likely to hold an individualist orientation that 
emphasizes personal identity and the uniqueness of the individual. They focus more 
on what sets them apart from others and what makes them special (Markus & Kitayama, 
2003; Wang, 2004; Markus, 2007). 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP e Attribution theory tries to explain how we understand 
the causes of behavior, particularly with respect to situa- 


What are attitudes, and how are they formed, maintained, and Yighal or dispositional factors. (p. 585) 


changed? 
e Social psychology is the scientific study of the ways in What are the biases that influence the ways in which people 
which people’s thoughts, feelings, and actions are af- view others’ behavior? 
fected by others and the nature and causes of individual e Even though logical processes are involved, attribution 


behavior in social situations. (p. 578) 

Attitudes are evaluations of a particular person, behav- 
ior, belief, or concept. (p. 579) 

Cognitive dissonance occurs when an individual simul- 
taneously holds two cognitions—attitudes or thoughts— 
that contradict each other. To resolve the contradiction, 
the person may modify one cognition, change its impor- 
tance, add a cognition, or deny a link between the two 
cognitions—thus bringing about a reduction in disso- 
nance. (p. 582) 


How do people form impressions of what others are like and 
of the causes of their behavior? 


Social cognition involves the way people understand 
and make sense of others and themselves. People de- 
velop schemas that organize information about people 
and social experiences in memory and allow them to in- 
terpret and categorize information about others. (p. 583) 
People form impressions of others in part through the 
use of central traits—personality characteristics that re- 
ceive unusually heavy emphasis when we form an im- 
pression. (p. 584) 

Information-processing approaches have found that we 
tend to average together sets of traits to form an overall 
impression. (p. 585) 


is prone to error. For instance, people are susceptible to 
the halo effect, assumed-similarity bias, self-serving bias, 
and fundamental attribution error (the tendency to over- 
attribute others’ behavior to dispositional causes and the 
corresponding failure to recognize the importance of sit- 
uational causes). (p. 586) 


EVALUATE 


1. 


2. 


An evaluation of a particular person, behavior, belief, or 
concept is called a(n) : 

One brand of peanut butter advertises its product by de- 
scribing its taste and nutritional value. It is hoping to per- 
suade customers through ________ route processing. In 
ads for a competing brand, a popular actor happily eats 
the product—but does not describe it. This approach 
hopes to persuade customers through _____ route 
processing. 


. Cognitive dissonance theory suggests that we commonly 


change our behavior to keep it consistent with our atti- 
tudes. True or false? 


. Sopan was happy to lend his textbook to a fellow student 


who seemed bright and friendly. He was surprised when 
his classmate did not return it. His assumption that the 
bright and friendly student would also be responsible 
reflects the _____ effect. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


RETHINK 


1. Joan sees Annette, a new coworker, act in a way that 
seems abrupt and curt. Joan concludes that Annette is 
unkind and unsociable. The next day Joan sees Annette 
acting kindly toward another worker. Is Joan likely to 


Module 52 Attitudes and Social Cognition 589 


. From the perspective of a marketing specialist: Suppose you 


were assigned to develop a full advertising campaign for a 
product, including television, radio, and print ads. How 
might theories of persuasion guide your strategy to suit 


change her impression of Annette? Why or why not? the different media? 
Finally, Joan sees several friends of hers laughing and 

joking with Annette, treating her in a very friendly fashion. 
Is Joan likely to change her impression of Annette? Why or 


why not? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


oey ‘p feoueUOSsSIP aATTUSOD doNpal 0} JOTARYaq INO Jou puer sapnyyyze 
Imo asueyp ÁeərdÁ} am ‘asyey Eg ‘Teraydised Jerzu z IPNI “LT 


KEY TERMS 


social psychology p. 578 
attitudes p. 579 
central route 
processing p. 580 
peripheral route 
processing p. 580 


situational causes 
(of behavior) p. 585 
dispositional causes 
(of behavior) p. 586 
halo effect p. 586 


cognitive 

dissonance p. 582 
social cognition p. 583 
schemas p. 583 
central traits p. 584 
attribution theory p. 585 


assumed-similarity 
bias p. 586 

self-serving bias p. 587 

fundamental attribution 
error p. 587 


MODULE 53 


What are the major sources 
and tactics of social influence? 


The process by which 
the actions of an individual or group 
affect the behavior of others. 


Two or more people who 
interact with one another, perceive 
themselves as part of a group, and 
are interdependent. 


dy Alert 


The distinction between 
the three types of social 
pressure—conformity, 
compliance, and 
obedience—depends on 
the nature and strength of the social 
pressure brought to bear on a person. 


A change in behavior or 
attitudes brought about by a desire to 
follow the beliefs or standards of other 
people. 


590 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


You have just transferred to a new college and are attending your first class. When 
the professor enters, your fellow classmates instantly rise, bow to the professor, and 
then stand quietly with their hands behind their backs. You’ve never encountered 
such behavior, and it makes no sense to you. Is it more likely that you will (1) jump 
up to join the rest of the class or (2) remain seated? 

On the basis of what research has told us about social influence, the process by 
which the actions of an individual or group affect the behavior of others, a person 
would almost always choose the first option. As you undoubtedly know from your 
own experience, pressures to conform can be painfully strong and can bring about 
changes in behavior that otherwise never would have occurred. 

Why can conformity pressures in groups be so strong? For one reason, groups 
and other people generally play a central role in our lives. As defined by social 
psychologists, groups consist of two or more people who (1) interact with one 
another; (2) perceive themselves as part of a group; and (3) are interdependent— 
that is, the events that affect one group member affect other members, and the 
behavior of members has significant consequences for the success of the group in 
meeting its goals. 

Groups develop and hold norms, expectations regarding behavior appropriate to 
the group. Furthermore, we understand that not adhering to group norms can result 
in retaliation from other group members, ranging from being ignored to being overtly 
derided or even being rejected or excluded by the group. Thus, people conform to 
meet the expectations of the group (Baumeister, Twenge, & Nuss, 2002; Jetten, Horn- 
sey, & Adarves-Yorno, 2006). 

Groups exert considerable social influence over individuals that ranges from the 
mundane, such as the decision to wear a certain kind of jeans, to the extreme, such 
as the cruelty of guards at the Abu Ghraib prison in Iraq. We’ll consider three types 
of social pressure: conformity, compliance, and obedience. 


Conformity: Following 
What Others Do 


Conformity is a change in behavior or attitudes brought about by a desire to follow 
the beliefs or standards of other people. Subtle or even unspoken social pressure 
results in conformity. 

The classic demonstration of pressure to conform comes from a series of studies 
carried out in the 1950s by Solomon Asch (Asch, 1951). In the experiments, the par- 
ticipants thought they were taking part in a test of perceptual skills with six other 
people. The experimenter showed the participants one card with three lines of vary- 
ing length and a second card that had a fourth line that matched one of the first three 
(see Figure 1). The task was seemingly straightforward: Each of the participants had 
to announce aloud which of the first three lines was identical in length to the “standard” 
line on the second card. Because the correct answer was always obvious, the task 
seemed easy to the participants. 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 53 Social Influence and Groups 591 


Indeed, because the participants all agreed on the first few trials, the procedure 
appeared to be simple. But then something odd began to happen. From the perspec- 
tive of the participant in the group who answered last on each trial, all the answers 
of the first six participants seemed to be wrong—in fact, unanimously wrong. And 
this pattern persisted. Over and over again, the first six participants provided answers 
that contradicted what the last participant believed to be correct. The last participant 
faced the dilemma of whether to follow his or her own perceptions or follow the | 2 3 
group by repeating the answer everyone else was giving. Standard line Comparison lines 

As you might have guessed, this experiment was more contrived than it appeared. FIGURE 1 Which of the three comparison 
The first six participants were actually confederates (paid employees of the experi- lines is the same length as the “standard” 
menter) who had been instructed to give unanimously erroneous answers in many 
of the trials. And the study had nothing to do with perceptual skills. Instead, the 
issue under investigation was conformity. 

Asch found that in about one-third of the trials, the participants conformed to 
the unanimous but erroneous group answer; about 75% of all participants conformed 
at least once. However, he found strong individual differences. Some participants 
conformed nearly all the time, whereas others never did. 


line? 


CONFORMITY CONCLUSIONS 


Since Asch’s pioneering work, literally hundreds of studies have examined confor- 
mity, and we now know a great deal about the phenomenon. Significant findings 
focus on: 


e The characteristics of the group. The more attractive a group appears to its mem- 
bers, the greater its ability to produce conformity. Furthermore, a person’s 
relative status, the social rank held within a group, is critical: The lower a status The social rank held within 
person’s status in the group, the greater groups’ power over that person’s a group. 
behavior (Hogg & Hains, 2001). 
e The situation in which the individual is responding. Conformity is considerably 
higher when people must respond publicly than it is when they can do so 
privately, as the founders of the United States noted when they authorized 
secret ballots in voting. 
e The kind of task. People working on ambiguous tasks and questions (those with 
no clear answer) are more susceptible to social pressure. When asked to give 
an opinion on something, such as what type of clothing is fashionable, a 
person will more likely yield to conformist pressures than he or she will if 
asked a question of fact. In addition, tasks at which an individual is less compe- 
tent than others in the group make conformity more likely. For example, a 
person who is an infrequent computer user may feel pressure to conform to 
an opinion about computer brands when in a group of experienced computer 
users. 
e Unanimity of the group. Groups that unanimously support a position show the 
most pronounced conformity pressures. But what about the case in which 
people with dissenting views have an ally in the group, known as a social social supporter A group member 
supporter, who agrees with them? Having just one person present who shares whose dissenting views make non- 
the minority point of view is sufficient to reduce conformity pressures (Prislin, conformity to the group easier. 
Brewer, & Wilson, 2002; Goodwin, Costa, & Adonu, 2004; Levine & Moreland, 
2006). 


GROUPTHINK: CAVING IN TO CONFORMITY 


Although we usually think of conformity in terms of our individual relations with 
others, in some instances conformity pressures in organizations can lead to disastrous 
effects with long-term consequences. For instance, consider NASA's determination 
that the falling foam that hit the space shuttle Columbia when it took off in 2003 would 


592 Chapter 17 Social Psychology 


Groupthink may explain the poor dec- 
ision making of NASA engineers that led 
to the destruction of the space shuttle 
Columbia. 


groupthink A type of thinking in 
which group members share such a 
strong motivation to achieve consensus 
that they lose the ability to critically 
evaluate alternative points of view. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





pose no significant danger when it was time for the Columbia to land. Despite some 
engineers’ misgivings, a consensus formed that the foam was not dangerous to the 
shuttle. Ultimately, that consensus proved wrong: The shuttle came apart as it 
attempted to land, which killed all the astronauts on board (Schwartz & Wald, 2003). 

In hindsight, NASA’s decision was clearly wrong. How could such a poor deci- 
sion have been made? 


A phenomenon known as groupthink may provide an explanation. Groupthink 
is a type of thinking in which group members share such a strong motivation to 
achieve consensus that they lose the ability to critically evaluate alternative points of 
view. Groupthink is most likely to occur when a popular or powerful leader is sur- 
rounded by people of lower status—which is obviously the case with any U.S. pres- 
ident and his or her advisers but is also true in a variety of other organizations (Janis, 
1997; Kowert, 2002; Baron, 2005; Henningsen, Henningsen, & Eden, 2006). 

Groupthink typically leads to poor decisions. Groups limit the list of possible solu- 
tions to just a few, and they spend relatively little time considering any alternatives 
once the leader seems to be leaning toward a particular solution. In addition, groups 
may fall prey to entrapment, a circumstance in which commitments to a failing point 
of view or course of action are increased to justify investments in time and energy that 
have already been made (Weiss & Weiss, 2003; Turner, Pratkanis, & Struckman, 2007). 

Ultimately, group members may completely ignore information that challenges 
a developing consensus. Because historical research suggests that many disastrous 
decisions reflect groupthink, it is important for groups to be on guard (Kowert, 2002; 
Chapman, 2006; Packer, 2009). 


CONFORMITY TO SOCIAL ROLES 


Another way in which conformity influences behavior is through social roles. Social 
roles are the behaviors that are associated with people in a given position. For exam- 
ple, the role of “student” comprises behaviors such as studying, listening to an 
instructor, and attending class. Like a theatrical role, social roles tell us what behav- 
ior is associated with a given position. 

In some cases, though, social roles influence us so profoundly that we engage in 
behavior in entirely atypical—and damaging—ways. This fact was brought home in 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 53 Social Influence and Groups 593 


an influential experiment conducted by Philip Zimbardo and colleagues. In the study, 
the researchers set up a mock prison complete with cells, solitary confinement cubi- 
cles, and a small recreation area. The researchers then advertised for participants who 
were willing to spend 2 weeks in a study of prison life. Once they identified the 
study participants, a flip of a coin designated who would be a prisoner and who 
would be a prison guard. Neither prisoners nor guards were told how to fulfill their 
roles (Zimbardo, Maslach, & Haney, 2000; Zimbardo, 1973, 2007). 

After just a few days in this mock prison, the students assigned to be guards 
became abusive to the prisoners by waking them at odd hours and subjecting them 
to arbitrary punishment. They withheld food from the prisoners and forced them 
into hard labor. On the other hand, the students assigned to the prisoner role soon 
became docile and submissive to the guards. They became extremely demoralized, 
and one slipped into a depression so severe he was released after just a few days. In 
fact, after only 6 days of captivity, the remaining prisoners’ reactions became so 
extreme that the study was ended. 

The experiment (which, it’s important to note, drew criticism on both method- 
ological and ethical grounds) provided a clear lesson: Conforming to a social role 
can have a powerful consequence on the behavior of even normal, well-adjusted 
people and induce them to change their behavior in sometimes undesirable ways. 
This phenomenon may explain how the situation in which U.S. Army guards at the 
Iraq Abu Ghraib prison found themselves could have led to their abusive behavior 
toward the prisoners (Zimbardo, 2007; Haney & Zimbardo, 2009). 


Compliance: Submitting 
to Direct Social Pressure 


When we refer to conformity, we usually mean a phenomenon in which the social 
pressure is subtle or indirect. But in some situations social pressure is much more 
obvious with direct, explicit pressure to endorse a particular point of view or behave 
in a certain way. Social psychologists call the type of behavior that occurs in response 
to direct social pressure compliance. 

Several specific techniques represent attempts to gain compliance. Those fre- 
quently employed include: 





e Foot-in-the-door technique. A salesperson comes to your door and asks you to 
accept a small sample. You agree, thinking you have nothing to lose. A little 
later a larger request comes; because you have already agreed to the first one, 
you have a hard time turning it down. 

The salesperson in this case is using a tried-and-true strategy that social 
psychologists call the foot-in-the-door technique. In the foot-in-the-door tech- 
nique, you ask a person to agree to a small request and later ask that person to 
comply with a more important one. It turns out that compliance with the more 
important request increases significantly when the person first agrees to the 
smaller favor. 

Researchers first demonstrated the foot-in-the-door phenomenon in a 
study in which a number of experimenters went door to door asking resi- 
dents to sign a petition in favor of safe driving (Freedman & Fraser, 1966). 
Almost everyone complied with that small, benign request. A few weeks later, 
different experimenters contacted the residents and made a much larger 
request for the residents to erect a huge sign on their front lawns that read, 
“Drive Carefully.” The results were clear: 55% of those who had signed the 
petition agreed to the request to put up a sign, whereas only 17% of the 
people in a control group who had not been asked to sign the petition agreed 
to put up a sign. 


compliance Behavior that occurs in 
response to direct social pressure. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


594 Chapter 17 Social Psychology 





Why does the foot-in-the-door technique work? For one reason, 
involvement with the small request leads to an interest in an issue; 
taking an action—any action—makes the individual more commit- 
ted to the issue, which thereby increasing the likelihood of future 
compliance. Another explanation revolves around people’s self- 
perceptions. By complying with the initial request, individuals may 
come to see themselves as people who provide help when asked. 
Then, when confronted with the larger request, they agree in order 
to maintain the kind of consistency in attitudes and behavior that 
we described earlier. Although we don’t know which of these two 
explanations is more accurate, it is clear that the foot-in-the-door 
strategy is effective (Burger & Caldwell, 2003; Bloom, McBride, 

& Pollak, 2006; Guéguen et al., 2008). 

e Door-in-the-face technique. A fund-raiser asks for a $500 contribution. 
You laughingly refuse and tell her that the amount is way out of 
your league. She then asks for a $10 contribution. What do you do? 
If you are like most people, you'll probably be a lot more compliant 
than you would be if she hadn’t asked for the huge contribution 
first. In this tactic, called the door-in-the-face technique, someone makes 


The persuasive techniques identified by social psychol- a large request, expects it to be refused, and follows it with a smaller 


ogists can be seen in practice at auto dealerships. 


one. This strategy, which is the opposite of the foot-in-the-door 
approach, has also proved to be effective (Pascual & Guéguen, 2005, 
2006; Turner et al., 2007; Ebster & Neumayr, 2008). 

In a field experiment that demonstrates the success of this approach, 
experimenters stopped college students on the street and asked them to 
agree to a substantial favor—acting as unpaid counselors for juvenile delin- 
quents 2 hours a week for 2 years (Cialdini et al., 1975). Not surprisingly, no 
one agreed to make such an enormous commitment. But when they were 
later asked the considerably smaller favor of taking a group of delinquents 
on a 2-hour trip to the zoo, half the people complied. In comparison, only 
17% of a control group of participants who had not first received the larger 
request agreed. 

The use of this technique is widespread. You may have tried it at some 
point yourself by perhaps by asking your parents for a large increase in your 
allowance and later settling for less. Similarly, television writers, by sometimes 
sprinkling their scripts with obscenities that they know network censors will 
cut out, hope to keep other key phrases intact (Cialdini & Sagarin, 2005). 
That’s-not-all technique. In this technique, a salesperson offers you a deal at an 
inflated price. But immediately after the initial offer, the salesperson offers an 
incentive, discount, or bonus to clinch the deal. 

Although it sounds transparent, this practice can be quite effective. In 
one study, the experimenters set up a booth and sold cupcakes for 75¢ each. 
In one condition, the experimenters directly told customers that the price 
was 75¢. In another condition, they told customers that the price was origi- 
nally $1 but had been reduced to 75¢. As we might predict, more people 
bought cupcakes at the “reduced” price—even though it was identical to the 
price in the other experimental condition (Burger, Reed, & DeCesare, 1999; 
Pratkanis, 2007). 

Not-so-free sample. If you ever receive a free sample, keep in mind that it 
comes with a psychological cost. Although they may not couch it in these 
terms, salespeople who provide samples to potential customers do so to 
instigate the norm of reciprocity. The norm of reciprocity is the well accepted 
societal standard dictating that we should treat other people as they treat us. 
Receiving a not-so-free sample, then, suggests the need for reciprocation—in the 
form of a purchase, of course (Cialdini, 2006; Park & Antonioni, 2007; Burger 
et al., 2009). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 53 Social Influence and Groups 595 


Companies seeking to sell their products to consumers often use the tech- 
niques identified by social psychologists for promoting compliance. But employers 
also use them to bring about compliance and raise employees’ productivity in the 
workplace. In fact, industrial-organizational (I/O) psychology, a close cousin to 
social psychology, considers issues such as worker motivation, satisfaction, safety, 
and productivity. I/O psychologists also focus on the operation and design of 
organizations; they ask questions such as how decision making can be improved 
in large organizations and how the fit between workers and their jobs can be 
maximized. 


Obedience: Following 
Direct Orders 


Compliance techniques are used to gently lead people toward agreement with a 
request. In some cases, however, requests aim to produce obedience, a change in 
behavior in response to the commands of others. Although obedience is considerably 
less common than conformity and compliance, it does occur in several specific kinds 
of relationships. For example, we may show obedience to our bosses, teachers, or 
parents merely because of the power they hold to reward or punish us. 

To acquire an understanding of obedience, consider for a moment how you 
might respond if a stranger said to you: 





I’ve devised a new way of improving memory. All I need is for you to teach people a 
list of words and then give them a test. The test procedure requires only that you give 
learners a shock each time they make a mistake on the test. To administer the shocks, 
you will use a “shock generator” that gives shocks ranging from 15 to 450 volts. You 
can see that the switches are labeled from “slight shock” through “danger: severe 
shock” at the top level, where there are three red Xs. But don’t worry; although the 
shocks may be painful, they will cause no permanent damage. 


Presented with this situation, you would be likely to think that neither you nor 
anyone else would go along with the stranger’s unusual request. Clearly, it lies out- 
side the bounds of what we consider good sense. 

Or does it? Suppose the stranger asking for your help was a psychologist con- 
ducting an experiment. Or suppose the request came from your teacher, your 
employer, or your military commander—all people in authority with a seemingly 
legitimate reason for the request. 

If you still believe it’s unlikely that you would comply—think again. The situa- 
tion presented above describes a classic experiment conducted by social psychologist 
Stanley Milgram in the 1960s. In the study, an experimenter told participants to give 
increasingly stronger shocks to another person as part of a study on learning (see 
Figure 2). In reality, the experiment had nothing to do with learning; the real issue 
under consideration was the degree to which participants would comply with the 
experimenter’s requests. In fact, the “learner” supposedly receiving the shocks was 
a confederate who never really received any punishment (Milgram, 2005). 

Most people who hear a description of Milgram’s experiment feel it is unlikely 
that any participant would give the maximum level of shock—or, for that matter, any 
shock at all. Even a group of psychiatrists to whom the situation was described 
predicted that fewer than 2% of the participants would fully comply and administer 
the strongest shocks. 

However, the actual results contradicted both experts’ and nonexperts’ predic- 
tions. Some 65% of the participants eventually used the highest setting on the shock 
generator—450 volts—to shock the learner. This obedience occurred even though 
the learner, who had mentioned at the start of the experiment that he had a heart 


industrial-organizational (I/O) 
psychology The branch of psychology 
focusing on work- and job-related 
issues, including worker motivation, 
satisfaction, safety, and productivity. 


obedience A change in behavior in 
response to the commands of others. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


596 Chapter 17 Social Psychology 





FIGURE 2 This fearsome “shock generator” led participants to believe they were administering 
electric shocks to another person, who was connected to the generator by electrodes that 
were attached to the skin. (Source: Copyright 1965 by Stanley Milgram. From the film Obedience, 
distributed by the New York University Film Library and Pennsylvania State University, PCR.) 


condition, demanded to be released, screaming, “Let me out of here! Let me out of 
here! My heart’s bothering me. Let me out of here!” Despite the learner’s pleas, most 
participants continued to administer the shocks. 

Why did so many individuals comply with the experimenter’s demands? The 
participants, who were extensively interviewed after the experiment, said they obeyed 
primarily because they believed that the experimenter would be responsible for any 
potential ill effects that befell the learner. The participants accepted the experimenter’s 
orders, then, because they thought that they personally could not be held accountable 
for their actions—they could always blame the experimenter (Blass, 1996, 2004). 

Although most participants in the Milgram experiment said later they felt the 
knowledge gained from the study outweighed the discomfort they may have felt, 
the experiment has been criticized for creating an extremely trying set of circum- 
stances for the participants and thereby raised serious ethical concerns. Undoubtedly, 
the same experiment could not be conducted today because of ethical considerations. 

Other critics have suggested that Milgram’s methods were ineffective in creating 
a situation that actually mirrored real-world obedience. For example, how often are 
people placed in a situation in which someone orders them to continue hurting a 
victim, while the victim’s protests are ignored (Blass, 2000, 2004)? 

Despite these concerns, Milgram’s research remains the strongest laboratory 
demonstration of obedience. And partial replications of Milgram’s work, conducted 
in an ethically defensible way, find similar results, which adds credence to the orig- 
inal work (Blass, 2009; Burger, 2009). 


y Alert Furthermore, we need only consider actual instances of obedience to authority 
Because of its graphic to witness some frightening real-life parallels. For instance, after World War II, the 
demonstration of obedience major defense that Nazi officers gave to excuse their participation in atrocities during 
to authority, the Milgram the war was that they were “only following orders.” Milgram’s experiment, which 
experiment is one of the was motivated in part by his desire to explain the behavior of everyday Germans 


most famous and influential during World War IL, forces us to ask ourselves this question: Would we be able to 
studies in social psychology. withstand the intense power of authority? 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


RECAP 


What are the major sources and tactics of social influence? 
e Social influence is the area of social psychology con- 


Module 53 Social Influence and Groups 597 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


4. The 





technique begins with an outrageous request that makes a 
subsequent, smaller request seem reasonable. 


5. is a ch in behavior that is due t thi 
cerned with situations in which the actions of an individ- T o paps N ea a AA 
ual or group affect the behavior of others. (p. 590) P f 

e Conformity refers to changes in behavior or attitudes 
that result from a desire to follow the beliefs or standards RETHINK 
of ohera (P 320) 1. Why do you think the Milgram experiment is so contro- 


e Compliance is behavior that results from direct social 


pressure. Among the ways of eliciting compliance are the 
foot-in-the-door, door-in-the-face, that’s-not-all, and not- 


so-free-sample techniques. (p. 593) 
e Obedience is a change in behavior in response to the 
commands of others. (p. 595) 


versial? What sorts of effects might the experiment have 
had on participants? Do you think the experiment would 
have had similar results if it had not been conducted in a 
laboratory setting but among members of a social group 
(such as a fraternity or sorority) with strong pressures to 
conform? 


2. From the perspective of a sales representative: Imagine that 
you have been trained to use the various compliance tech- 
EVALUATE - TOM ; 
niques described in this section. Because these compliance 
LA , or person who agrees with the techniques are so powerful, should the use of certain such 





dissenting viewpoint, is likely to reduce conformity. 
2. Who pioneered the study of conformity? 


techniques be forbidden? Should consumers be taught de- 
fenses against such techniques? Is the use of such tech- 


a. Skinner niques ethically and morally defensible? Why? 

b. Asch 3. From the perspective of an educator: Student obedience in the 
c. Milgram elementary and secondary classroom is a major issue for 
d. Fiala many teachers. How might you promote student obedi- 


3. Which of the following techniques asks a person to com- 
ply with a small initial request to enhance the likelihood 
that the person will later comply with a larger request? 
a. Door-in-the-face 
b. Foot-in-the-door 
c. That’s-not-all 
d. Not-so-free sample 


ence in the classroom? What are some of the potentially 
harmful ways that teachers could use their social influence 
to elicit student obedience? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


adusIpaqo *g /advJ-ay}-UL-IOOp “pM ‘q'e ‘qz ‘10}10ddns epos “Tf 


KEY TERMS 


social influence p. 590 
group p.590 
conformity p. 590 
status p. 591 


social supporter p. 591 
groupthink p. 592 
compliance p. 593 


industrial-organizational 
(I/O) psychology p. 595 
obedience p. 595 


How do stereotypes, prejudice, 
and discrimination differ? 


How can we reduce prejudice 
and discrimination? 


A set of generalized beliefs 
and expectations about a particular 
group and its members. 


A negative (or positive) 
evaluation of a particular group and its 
members. 


Behavior directed 
toward individuals on the basis of their 
membership in a particular group. 


> Study Alert 


Remember that prejudice 
relates to attitudes about a 
group and its members, 

while discrimination 
relates to behavior directed 
to a group and its members. 


598 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


What do you think when someone says, “He’s African American,” “She’s Chinese,” 
or “That’s a woman driver”? 

If you're like most people, you'll probably automatically form some sort of 
impression of what each person is like. Most likely your impression is based on 
a stereotype, a set of generalized beliefs and expectations about a specific group 
and its members. Stereotypes, which may be negative or positive, grow out of our 
tendency to categorize and organize the vast amount of information we encounter 
in our everyday lives. All stereotypes share the common feature of oversimplifying 
the world: We view individuals not in terms of their unique, personal character- 
istics, but also in terms of characteristics we attribute to all the members of a 
particular group. 

Stereotypes can lead to prejudice, a negative (or positive) evaluation of a group 
and its members. For instance, racial prejudice occurs when a member of a racial 
group is evaluated in terms of race and not because of his or her own characteristics 
or abilities. Although prejudice can be positive (I love the Irish”), social psychologists 
have focused on understanding the roots of negative prejudice ("I hate immigrants”). 

Common stereotypes and forms of prejudice involve race, religion, ethnicity, and 
gender. Over the years, various groups have been called “lazy” or “shrewd” or 
“cruel” with varying degrees of regularity by those who are not members of that 
group. Even today, despite major progress toward reducing legally sanctioned forms 
of prejudice, such as school segregation, stereotypes remain (Eberhardt et al., 2004; 
Pettigrew, 2004; Hunt, Seifert, & Armenta, 2006). 

Even people who on the surface appear to be unprejudiced may harbor hidden 
prejudice. For example, when white participants in experiments are shown faces on 
a computer screen so rapidly that they cannot consciously perceive the faces, they 
react more negatively to black than to white faces—an example of what has been 
called modern racism (Dovidio, Gaertner, & Pearson, 2005; Liu & Mills, 2006; Pearson, 
Dovidio, & Pratto, 2007). 

Although usually backed by little or no evidence, stereotypes can have harmful 
consequences. Acting on negative stereotypes results in discrimination—behavior 
directed toward individuals on the basis of their membership in a particular group. 
Discrimination can lead to exclusion from jobs, neighborhoods, and educational 
opportunities, and it may result in lower salaries and benefits for members of specific 
groups. Discrimination can also result in more favorable treatment to favored 
groups—for example, when an employer hires a job applicant of his or her own racial 
group because of the applicant’s race (Avery, McKay, & Wilson, 2008; Pager & Shep- 
herd, 2008). 

Stereotyping not only leads to overt discrimination, but also can cause members 
of stereotyped groups to behave in ways that reflect the stereotype through a phe- 
nomenon known as the self-fulfilling prophecy. Self-fulfilling prophecies are expecta- 
tions about the occurrence of a future event or behavior that act to increase the 
likelihood the event or behavior will occur. For example, if people think that mem- 
bers of a specific group lack ambition, they may treat them in a way that actually 
brings about a lack of ambition (Oskamp, 2000; Seibt & Förster, 2005; Madon, Willard, 
& Guyll, 2006). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 54 Prejudice and Discrimination 599 


The Foundations of Prejudice 


No one has ever been born disliking a specific racial, religious, or ethnic group. 
People learn to hate in much the same way that they learn the alphabet. 

According to observational learning approaches to stereotyping and prejudice, the 
behavior of parents, other adults, and peers shapes children’s feelings about mem- 
bers of various groups. For instance, bigoted parents may commend their children 
for expressing prejudiced attitudes. Likewise, young children learn prejudice by imi- 
tating the behavior of adult models. Such learning starts at an early age: Children as 
young as 6 months judge others according to their skin color, and by 3 years of age 
they begin to show preferences for members of their own race (Ponterotto, Utsey, & 
Pedersen, 2006; Dovidio & Gaertner, 2006; Bronson & Merryman, 2009). 

The mass media also provide information about stereotypes not just for children 
but for adults as well. Even today, some television shows and movies portray Italians 
as Mafia-like mobsters, Jews as greedy bankers, and African Americans as promiscu- 
ous or lazy. When such inaccurate portrayals are the primary source of information 
about minority groups, they can lead to the development and maintenance of unfa- 
vorable stereotypes (Coltraine & Messineo, 2000; Ward, 2004; Do, 2006). 

Other explanations of prejudice and discrimination focus on how being a mem- 
ber of a specific group helps to magnify one’s sense of self-esteem. According to 
social identity theory, we use group membership as a source of pride and self-worth. 
Social identity theory suggests that people tend to be ethnocentric, viewing the 
world from their own perspective and judging others in terms of their group mem- 
bership. Slogans such as “gay pride” and “Black is beautiful” illustrate that the 
groups to which we belong give us a sense of self-respect (Tajfel & Turner, 2004; 
Hogg, 2006). 

However, the use of group membership to provide social respect produces an 
unfortunate outcome. In an effort to maximize our sense of self-esteem, we may come 
to think that our own group (our ingroup) is better than groups to which we don’t 
belong (our outgroups). Consequently, we inflate the positive aspects of our ingroup— 
and, at the same time, devalue outgroups. Ultimately, we come to view members of 








Fy, 


Like father, like son: Social learning approaches to stereotyping and prejudice suggest that 
attitudes and behaviors toward members of minority groups are learned through the 
observation of parents and other individuals. How can this cycle be broken? 


600 Chapter 17 Social Psychology 


social neuroscience The subfield of 
social psychology that seeks to identify 
the neural basis of social behavior. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


The Prejudiced Brain 


FIGURE 1 Both white and black participants showed greater activity in the amygdala when 
viewing black faces as compared to viewing white faces in this composite fMRI scan. The 
researchers hypothesized that negative cultural messages about blacks lead to higher activation 
of the amygdala while viewing black faces due to its role in negative emotions. What other 
explanations might have produced the same findings? (Source: Lieberman et al., 2005, Figure 1b.) 





outgroups as inferior to members of our ingroup (Tajfel & Turner, 2004). The end 
result is prejudice toward members of groups of which we are not a part. 

Neither the observational learning approach nor the social identity approach 
provides a full explanation for stereotyping and prejudice. For instance, some psy- 
chologists argue that prejudice results when there is perceived competition for scarce 
societal resources. Thus, when competition exists for jobs or housing, members of 
majority groups may believe (however unjustly or inaccurately) that minority group 
members are hindering their efforts to attain their goals; this belief can lead to prej- 
udice. In addition, other explanations for prejudice emphasize human cognitive 
limitations that lead us to categorize people on the basis of visually conspicuous 
physical features such as race, sex, and ethnic group. Such categorization can lead 
to the development of stereotypes and, ultimately, to discriminatory behavior (Mullen 
& Rice, 2003; Weeks & Lupfer, 2004; Hugenberg & Sacco, 2008). 

The most recent approach to understanding prejudice comes from an increas- 
ingly important area in social psychology: social neuroscience. Social neuroscience 
seeks to identify the neural basis of social behavior. It looks at how we can illuminate 
our understanding of groups, interpersonal relations, and emotions by understand- 
ing their neuroscientific underpinnings (Cacioppo, Visser, & Pickett, 2005; Harmon- 
Jones & Winkielman, 2007). 

In one example of the value of social neuroscience approaches, researchers exam- 
ined activation of the amygdala, the structure in the brain that relates to emotion- 
evoking stimuli and situations, while viewing white and black faces. Because the 
amygdala is especially responsive to threatening, unusual, or highly arousing stimuli, 
the researchers hypothesized greater activation of the amygdala during exposure to 
black faces due to negative cultural associations with racial minorities (Lieberman et 
al., 2005; Lieberman, 2007). 

As you can see in Figure 1, the hypothesis was confirmed: The amygdala showed 
more activation when participants saw a black face than when they saw a white one. 
Because both blacks and whites were participants in the study, it is unlikely that the 
amygdala activation was simply the result of the novelty of viewing members of a 
racial minority. Instead, the findings suggest that culturally learned societal messages 
about race led to the brain activation. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 54 Prejudice and Discrimination 601 


Measuring Prejudice and 
Discrimination: The Implicit 
Personality Test 


Could you be prejudiced and not even know it? The answer, according to the 
researchers who developed the Implicit Association Test, is probably yes. People often 
fool themselves, and they are very careful about revealing their true attitudes about 
members of various groups, not only to others but to themselves. However, even 
though they may truly believe that they are unprejudiced, the reality is that they 
actually routinely differentiate between people on the basis of race, ethnicity, gender, 
and sexual orientation. 

The Implicit Association Test, or IAT, is an ingenious measure of prejudice that 
permits a more accurate assessment of people’s discrimination between members of 
different groups. It was developed, in part, as a reaction to the difficulty in finding 
a questionnaire that would reveal prejudice. Direct questions such as, “Would you 
prefer interacting with a member of Group X rather than Group Y?” typically iden- 
tify only the most blatant prejudices, because people try to censor their responses 
(Rudman & Ashmore, 2007; Schnabel, Asendorpf, & Greenwald, 2008; Greenwald 
et al., 2009). 

In contrast, the IAT makes use of the fact that people’s automatic reactions often 
provide the most valid indicator of what they actually believe. The test asks people 
a series of questions on a computerized survey that assess the degree to which peo- 
ple associate members of target groups (say, African-Americans versus Whites) with 
positive stimuli (such as a puppy) versus negative stimuli (such as a funeral). 
The test is based on the fact that growing up in a particular culture teaches us to 
unconsciously associate members of particular groups with positive or negative 
qualities, and we tend to absorb associations about those groups that reflect the 
culture without even being aware of it (Lane et al., 2007). (If you would like to 
try out a version of the IAT yourself, there is a demonstration website with a 
sample test at https://implicit.harvard.edu/implicit. You may well be surprised 
at the results.) 

The results of the IAT show that almost 90% of test-takers have an implicit pro- 
white bias, and more than two-thirds of non-Arab, non-Muslim volunteers display 
implicit biases against Arab Muslims. Moreover, more than 80% of heterosexuals 
display an implicit bias against gays and lesbians (Wittenbrink & Schwarz, 2007). 

So, of course, having an implicit bias does not mean that people will overtly 
discriminate, which is a criticism that has been made of the test. Yet it does mean 
that the cultural lessons to which we are exposed have a considerable unconscious 
influence on us. (Interested in how you would perform on the IAT? Go to this web- 
site to take the test: https: //implicit.harvard.edu/implicit). 





Reducing the Consequences of 
Prejudice and Discrimination 


How can we diminish the effects of prejudice and discrimination? Psychologists have 
developed several strategies that have proved effective. 





e Increasing contact between the target of stereotyping and the holder of the stereotype. 
Research consistently shows that increasing the amount of interaction between 
people can reduce negative stereotyping. But only certain kinds of contact are 


y Alert 
Remember that the IAT allows 
measurement of attitudes 
about which people might 
not be consciously aware as 
well as attitudes they wish 
to keep hidden from others. 


602 


Chapter 17 Social Psychology 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


likely to reduce prejudice and discrimination. Situations in which contact is 
relatively intimate, the individuals are of equal status, or participants must 
cooperate with one another or are dependent on one another are more likely 
to reduce stereotyping (Dovidio, Gaertner, & Kawakami, 2003; Tropp & 
Pettigrew, 2005; Pettigrew & Tropp, 2006). 

e Making values and norms against prejudice more conspicuous. Sometimes just 
reminding people about the values they already hold regarding equality and 
fair treatment of others is enough to reduce discrimination. Similarly, people 
who hear others making strong, vehement antiracist statements are subse- 
quently more likely to strongly condemn racism (Czopp & Monteith, 2006; 
Ponterotto, Utsey, & Pedersen, 2006; Tropp & Bianchi, 2006). 

e Providing information about the targets of stereotyping. Probably the most direct 
means of changing stereotypical and discriminatory attitudes is education: 
teaching people to be more aware of the positive characteristics of targets of 
stereotyping. For instance, when the meaning of puzzling behavior is 
explained to people who hold stereotypes, they may come to appreciate the 
actual significance of the behavior (Isbell & Tyler, 2003; Banks, 2006; Nagda, 
Tropp, & Paluck, 2006). 

e Reducing stereotype threat. Social psychologist Claude Steele suggests that many 
African Americans suffer from stereotype vulnerability, obstacles to performance 
that stem from their awareness of society’s stereotypes regarding minority 
group members. He argues that African-American students who receive 
instruction from teachers who may doubt their abilities and who set up special 
remedial programs to assist them may come to accept society’s stereotypes and 
believe that they are prone to fail (Aronson & Steele, 2005; Nussbaum & 
Steele, 2007). 


Such beliefs can have devastating effects. When confronted with an academic 
task, African-American students may fear that their performance will simply confirm 
society’s negative stereotypes. The immediate consequence of this fear is anxiety that 
hampers performance. But the long-term consequences may be even worse: Doubting 
their ability to perform successfully in academic environments, African Americans 
may decide that the risks of failure are so great it is not worth the effort even to 
attempt to do well. Ultimately, they may “disidentify” with academic success by 
minimizing the importance of academic endeavors (Steele, 1997; Stone, 2002). 

However, Steele’s analysis suggests that African Americans may be able to over- 
come their predicament. Specifically, schools can design intervention programs to 
train minority group members about their vulnerability to stereotypes and provide 
them with self-affirmation that reinforces their confidence in their abilities and 
thereby inoculates them against the fear and doubt triggered by negative stereotypes 
(Cohen et al., 2006; Wilson, 2006). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 54 Prejudice and Discrimination 603 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 


How do stereotypes, prejudice, and discrimination differ? 


Stereotypes are generalized beliefs and expectations 
about a specific group and its members. Stereotyping can 
lead to prejudice and self-fulfilling prophecies. (p. 598) 
Prejudice is the negative (or positive) evaluation of a par- 
ticular group and its members. (p. 598) 

Stereotyping and prejudice can lead to discrimination, 
behavior directed toward individuals on the basis of 
their membership in a particular group. (p. 598) 
According to observational learning approaches, chil- 
dren learn stereotyping and prejudice by observing the 
behavior of parents, other adults, and peers. Social iden- 
tity theory suggests that group membership is used as 
a source of pride and self-worth, and this may lead 
people to think of their own group as better than 
others. (p. 599) 

Social neuroscientific approaches to prejudice examine 
the functioning of the brain and nervous system to un- 
derstand the basis of prejudice. (p. 600) 


How can we reduce prejudice and discrimination? 


Among the ways of reducing prejudice and discrimina- 
tion are increasing contact, demonstrating positive val- 
ues against prejudice, and education. (p. 601) 


EVALUATE 


1. 


Any expectation—positive or negative—about an individ- 
ual solely on the basis of that person’s membership in a 
group can be a stereotype. True or false? 


KEY TERMS 


stereotype p. 598 
prejudice p. 598 


discrimination p. 598 
social neuroscience p. 600 


2. The negative (or positive) evaluation of a group and its 


members is called 

a. Stereotyping 

b. Prejudice 

c. Self-fulfilling prophecy 
d. Discrimination 


. Paul is a store manager who does not expect women to 


succeed in business. He therefore offers important, high- 
profile responsibilities only to men. If the female employ- 
ees fail to move up in the company, it could be an example 
ofa prophecy. 





RETHINK 


1. Do you think women can be victims of stereotype vulner- 


ability? In what topical areas might this occur? Can men 
be victims of stereotype vulnerability? Why? 


. From the perspective of a corrections officer: How might overt 


forms of prejudice and discrimination toward disadvan- 
taged groups (such as African Americans) be reduced in a 
state or federal prison? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


Suys e ‘qz ‘ony T 


Why are we attracted to 
certain people, and what 
progression do social 
relationships follow? 


What factors underlie aggres- 
sion and prosocial behavior? 


interpersonal attraction (or close 
relationship) Positive feelings for 
others; liking and loving. 


604 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Are people basically good or bad? 

Like philosophers and theologians, social psychologists have pondered the basic 
nature of humanity. Is it represented mainly by the violence and cruelty we see 
throughout the world, or does something special about human nature permit loving, 
considerate, unselfish, and even noble behavior as well? 

We turn to two routes that social psychologists have followed in seeking answers 
to these questions. We first consider what they have learned about the sources of our 
attraction to others, we end with a look at two opposite sides of human behavior: 
aggression and helping. 


Liking and Loving: Interpersonal 
Attraction and the Development 
of Relationships 


Nothing is more important in most people’s lives than their feelings for others. Con- 
sequently, it is not surprising that liking and loving have become a major focus of 
interest for social psychologists. Known more formally as the study of interpersonal 
attraction or close relationships, this area addresses the factors that lead to positive 
feelings for others. 





By far the greatest amount of research has focused on liking, probably because it is 
easier for investigators conducting short-term experiments to produce states of liking 
in strangers who have just met than to instigate and observe loving relationships 
over long periods. Consequently, research has given us a good deal of knowledge 
about the factors that initially attract two people to each other. The important factors 
social psychologists consider are the following: 


e Proximity. If you live in a dormitory or an apartment, consider the friends you 
made when you first moved in. Chances are that you became friendliest with 
those who lived geographically closest to you. In fact, this is one of the more 
firmly established findings in the literature on interpersonal attraction: Proxim- 
ity leads to liking (Burgoon et al., 2002; Smith & Weber, 2005). 

e Mere exposure. Repeated exposure to a person is often sufficient to produce 
attraction. Interestingly, repeated exposure to any stimulus—a person, picture, 
compact disc, or virtually anything—usually makes us like the stimulus more. 
Becoming familiar with a person can evoke positive feelings; we then transfer 
the positive feelings stemming from familiarity to the person him- or herself. 
There are exceptions, though. In cases of strongly negative initial interactions, 
repeated exposure is unlikely to cause us to like a person more. Instead, the 
more we are exposed to him or her, the more we may dislike the individual 
(Zajonc, 2001; Butler & Berry, 2004). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 55 Positive and Negative Social Behavior 605 


e Similarity. Folk wisdom tells us that birds of a feather flock 
together. However, it also maintains that opposites attract. Social 
psychologists have come up with a clear verdict regarding which 
of the two statements is correct: We tend to like those who are 
similar to us. Discovering that others have similar attitudes, 
values, or traits promotes our liking for them. Furthermore, the 
more similar others are, the more we like them. One reason 
similarity increases the likelihood of interpersonal attraction is 
that we assume people with similar attitudes will evaluate us 
positively. Because we experience a strong reciprocity-of-liking 
effect (a tendency to like those who like us), knowing that 
someone evaluates us positively promotes our attraction to that 
person. In addition, we assume that when we like someone else, 
that person likes us in return (Bates, 2002; Umphress, Smith- 
Crowe, & Brief, 2007; Montoya & Insko, 2008). 

e Physical attractiveness. For most people, the equation beautiful = 
good is quite true. As a result, physically attractive people are 
more popular than physically unattractive ones, if all other 
factors are equal. This finding, which contradicts the values that 


most people say they hold, is apparent even in childhood—with < | Í 


children of nursery school age rating their peers’ popularity on 
the basis of attractiveness—and continues into adulthood. 
Indeed, physical attractiveness may be the single most important 
element promoting initial liking in college dating situations, 
although its influence eventually decreases when people get to know each 
other better (Zebrowitz & Montepare, 2005; Little, Burt, & Perrett, 2006; Luo & 
Zhang, 2009). 


These factors alone, of course, do not account for liking. For example, in one 
experiment that examined the desired qualities in a friendship, the top-rated qualities 
in a same-sex friend included sense of humor, warmth and kindness, expressiveness 
and openness, an exciting personality, and similarity of interests and leisure activities 
(Sprecher & Regan, 2002). (For more on friendship, see Applying Psychology in the 21st 
Century for a discussion of online social networking.) 


HOW DO I LOVE THEE? LET ME COUNT THE WAYS 


Whereas our knowledge of what makes people like one another is extensive, our 
understanding of love is more limited in scope and recently acquired. For some time, 
many social psychologists believed that love was too difficult to observe and study 
in a controlled, scientific way. However, love is such a central issue in most people’s 
lives that eventually social psychologists could not resist its allure. 

As a first step, researchers tried to identify the characteristics that distinguish 
between mere liking and full-blown love. They discovered that love is not simply a 
greater quantity of liking but a qualitatively different psychological state. For instance, 
at least in its early stages, love includes relatively intense physiological arousal, an 
all-encompassing interest in another individual, fantasizing about the other, and 
relatively rapid swings of emotion. Similarly, love, unlike liking, includes elements 
of passion, closeness, fascination, exclusiveness, sexual desire, and intense caring. We 
idealize partners by exaggerating their good qualities and minimizing their imperfec- 
tions (Garza-Guerrero, 2000; Murray, Holmes, & Griffin, 2004). 

Other researchers have theorized that there are two main types of love: passion- 
ate love and companionate love. Passionate (or romantic) love represents a state of 
intense absorption in someone. It includes intense physiological arousal, psycho- 
logical interest, and caring for the needs of another. In contrast, companionate love 
is the strong affection we have for those with whom our lives are deeply involved. 
The love we feel for our parents, other family members, and even some close friends 

















© Richard Cline/The New Yorker Collection/www.cartoonbank.com. 


| ZAI 


fA. — 


“I’m attracted to you, but then I'm attracted to me, too.” 


reciprocity-of-liking effect A tendency 
to like those who like us. 


passionate (or romantic) love A state 
of intense absorption in someone that 
includes intense physiological arousal, 
psychological interest, and caring for 
the needs of another. 


companionate love The strong 
affection we have for those with whom 
our lives are deeply involved. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Friends Online: Is Facebook 
the New Student Union? 


If you're a typical college student, you 
probably use Facebook or some other so- 
cial networking website. In fact, estimates 
of usage rates of social networking sites on 
college campuses run as high as 90%, com- 
pared to 33% of older Internet users. When 
that many people are using a new technol- 
ogy for interpersonal communication, so- 
cial psychologists take note, and they have 
begun to examine how college students 
use social network sites (Lenhart, 2009). 
One finding is clear: College students use 
Facebook primarily to keep in touch with 
their friends. Few use it to keep in touch 
with their parents or to meet new people. 
What seems to interest college students most 
is the ability to maintain social connections 
across geographic boundaries efficiently. 
Specifically, social networking sites allow 
them to easily keep tabs on old friends from 
home while at the same time interacting 
with new friends made at college (Wiley & 
Sisson, 2006; Pempek, Yermolayeva, & Cal- 
vert, 2009; Subrahmanyam et al., 2008). 
Another way college students use social 
network sites is to explore their developing 
identities. Because users can control how 
they present themselves to the world on a 
social network profile, it is easy for college 
students to “try on” an identity by posting 
selected photos of themselves, revealing 
specific tastes and interests, or otherwise 
presenting themselves in new and different 


606 














ways. The feedback they get from others 
may help them decide which identities and 
forms of self-presentation suit them best. 
College students report that religion, polit- 
ical ideology, work, and tastes in media 
such as music and movies are their most 
important identity expressions (Pempek, 
Yermolayeva, & Calvert, 2009). 

But how do social network sites affect 
users’ nonvirtual social lives? Maintaining 
social connections that might otherwise 
have withered and died seems like a good 
thing, but it may be detrimental if students 
spend so much time maintaining online 
distant or superficial friendships that they 
sacrifice time spent on intimate, face-to- 
face interactions with close friends. 


New research suggests that social net- 
working sites provide a less intimidating 
social outlet for students who otherwise 
have trouble making and keeping friend- 
ships, such as those who are introverted or 
have low self-esteem. One study that 
tracked college students’ Facebook use 
over time found that those with low self- 
esteem benefitted the most in terms of 
building greater social networks through 
Facebook. Students reported that they 
found it less awkward to initiate contact 
with people through Facebook than 
through other means. They were also bet- 
ter able to learn about social events and 
other opportunities for face-to-face interac- 
tion through Facebook postings than by 
direct word of mouth, which thereby im- 
proved their offline social lives (Steinfeld, 
Ellison, & Lampe, 2008). 

In addition, intensity of Facebook use is 
positively related to students’ life satisfac- 
tion, social trust, and civic engagement. In 
short, research suggests that users of social 
network sites are not disengaged from the 
real world and that the benefits of social 
networking outweigh the costs to students’ 
social lives (Valenzuela, Park, & Kee, 2009). 








e Why might college students who use social networking sites more heavily have 
greater satisfaction in life than those who use it less? 

e What aspects of social networking sites allow students to explore their identities? 
What might be the drawbacks of such experimentation? 








falls into the category of companionate love (Masuda, 2003; Regan, 2006; Loving, 
Crockett, & Paxson, 2009). 

Psychologist Robert Sternberg makes an even finer differentiation between types 
of love. He proposes that love consists of three parts: 


e Decision/commitment, the initial thoughts that one loves someone and the 
longer-term feelings of commitment to maintain love. 

e Intimacy component, feelings of closeness and connectedness. 

e Passion component, the motivational drives relating to sex, physical closeness, 
and romance. 


According to Sternberg, these three components combine to produce the different 
types of love. He suggests that different combinations of the three components vary 
over the course of relationships. For example, in strong, loving relationships, the level 
of commitment peaks and then remains stable. Passion, on the other hand, peaks 
quickly and then declines and levels off relatively early in most relationships. In 
addition, relationships are happiest in which the strength of the various components 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 55 Positive and Negative Social Behavior 607 


Liking FIGURE 1 According to Sternberg, love 
(intimacy) has three main components: intimacy, 
passion, and decision/commitment. 
Different combinations of these compo- 
nents can create other types of love. 


Nonlove contains none of the three 
Companionate love components. 
(intimacy + decision/commitment) 


A Empty love 


i (decision/ 
(passion) Fatuous love commitment) 
(passion + decision/commitment) 


Romantic love 
(intimacy + passion) 


love 
(intimacy + passion + 








cision/commitment) 


Infatuation 


are similar between the two partners (Sternberg, Hojjat, & Barnes, 2001; Sternberg, 
2004a; Sternberg, 2006; see Figure 1). 

Is love a necessary ingredient in a good marriage? Yes, if you live in the United 
States. In contrast, it’s considerably less important in other cultures. Although mutual 
attraction and love are the two most important characteristics men and women in 
the United States desire in a mate, men in China rated good health as most important, 
and women there rated emotional stability and maturity as most important. Among 
the Zulu in South Africa, men rated emotional stability first and women rated 
dependable character first (Buss, Abbott, & Angleitner, 1990; see Figure 2). 


Rank Ordering of Desired Characteristics in a Mate 








United States China South Africa Zulu 
Females Males Females Males Females Males 

Mutual attraction—love 1 1 8 4 5 10 
Emotional stability and maturity 2 2 1 5 2 1 
Dependable character 3 3 y 6 1 3 
Pleasing disposition 4 4 16 13 3 4 
Education and intelligence 5 5 4 8 6 6 
Good health 9 6 3 1 4 5 
Good looks 13 7 15 1 16 14 
Sociability 8 8 9 12 8 11 
Desire for home and children 7 9 2 2 9 9 
Refinement, neatness 12 10 10 7 10 7 
Ambition and industriousness 6 11 5 10 7 8 
Similar education 10 12 12 15 12 12 
Good cook and housekeeper 16 13 11 9 5 2 
Favorable social status or rating 14 14 13 14 14 17 
Similar religious background 15 15 18 18 11 16 
Good financial prospect 11 16 14 16 13 18 
Chastity (no prior sexual intercourse) 18 17 6 3 18 ils) 
Similar political background 17 18 17 17 17 15 


FIGURE 2 Although love may be an important factor in choosing a marriage partner if you 
live in the United States, other cultures place less importance on it. (Source: Buss, Abbott, & 
Angleitner, 1990.) 


608 Chapter 17 Social Psychology 


aggression The intentional injury of, 
or harm to, another person. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Liking and loving clearly show a positive side of human social behavior. Now 
we turn to behaviors that are just as much a part of social behavior: aggression and 
helping behavior. 


Aggression and Prosocial 
Behavior: Hurting and 
Helping Others 


Drive-by shootings, carjackings, and abductions are just a few examples of the vio- 
lence that seems all too common today. Yet we also find examples of generous, unself- 
ish, thoughtful behavior that suggest a more optimistic view of humankind. Consider, 
for instance, people such as Mother Teresa, who ministered to the poor in India. Or 
contemplate the simple kindnesses of life: lending a valued compact disc, stopping 
to help a child who has fallen off a bicycle, or merely sharing a candy bar with a 
friend. Such instances of helping are no less characteristic of human behavior than 
the distasteful examples of aggression. 





HURTING OTHERS: AGGRESSION 


We need look no further than the daily paper or the nightly news to be bombarded 
with examples of aggression both on a societal level (war, invasion, assassination) 
and on an individual level (crime, child abuse, and the many petty cruelties humans 
are capable of inflicting on one another). Is such aggression an inevitable part of the 
human condition? Or is aggression primarily a product of particular circumstances 
that, if changed, could lead to its reduction? 

The difficulty of answering such knotty questions becomes apparent as soon as 
we consider how best to define the term aggression. Depending on the way we define 
the word, many examples of inflicted pain or injury may or may not qualify as 
aggression (see Figure 3). For instance, a rapist is clearly acting with aggression 
toward his victim. On the other hand, it is less certain that a physician carrying out 
an emergency medical procedure without an anesthetic, thereby causing incredible 
pain to the patient, should be considered aggressive. 

Most social psychologists define aggression in terms of the intent and the purpose 
behind the behavior. Aggression is intentional injury of or harm to another person. 
By this definition, the rapist is clearly acting aggressively, whereas the physician 
causing pain during a medical procedure is not (Berkowitz, 2001). 

We turn now to several approaches to aggressive behavior developed by social 
psychologists. 


INSTINCT APPROACHES: AGGRESSION AS A RELEASE 


If you have ever punched an adversary in the nose, you may have experienced a 
certain satisfaction despite your better judgment. Instinct theories, which note the 
prevalence of aggression not only in humans but in animals as well, propose that 
aggression is primarily the outcome of innate—or inborn—urges. 

Sigmund Freud was one of the first to suggest, as part of his theory of personal- 
ity, that aggression is a primary instinctual drive. Konrad Lorenz, an ethologist (a 
scientist who studies animal behavior), expanded Freud’s notions by arguing that 
humans, along with members of other species, have a fighting instinct, which in 
earlier times ensured protection of food supplies and weeded out the weaker of the 
species (Lorenz, 1966, 1974). Lorenz’s instinct approach led to the controversial notion 
that aggressive energy constantly builds up in an individual until the person finally 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 55 Positive and Negative Social Behavior 609 


—feahialibaalilaalie PDO 


Is This Aggression? 


To see for yourself the difficulties involved in defining aggression, consider each of the 
following acts and determine whether it represents aggressive behavior—according to 
your own definition of aggression. 























1. A spider eats a fly. Yes No 

2. Two wolves fight for the leadership of the pack. Yes No 

3. A soldier shoots an enemy at the front line. Yes No 

4. The warden of a prison executes a convicted criminal. Yes No 

5. A man viciously kicks a cat. Yes No 

6. Aman, while cleaning a window, knocks over a flower pot, which, in falling, injures a 
pedestrian. Yes No 

7. Mr. X, a notorious gossip, speaks disparagingly of many people of his acquaintance. 
Yes No 

8. A man mentally rehearses a murder he is about to commit. Yes 
No 

9. An angry son purposely fails to write to his mother, who is expecting a letter and will 
be hurt if none arrives. Yes No 





10. An enraged boy tries with all his might to inflict injury on his antagonist, a bigger 
boy, but is not successful in doing so. His efforts simply amuse the bigger boy. 
Yes No 

11. A senator does not protest the escalation of bombing to which she is normally 
opposed. Yes No 

12. A farmer beheads a chicken and prepares it for supper. Yes 
No 

13. A hunter kills an animal and mounts it as a trophy. Yes No 

14. A physician gives a flu shot to a screaming child. Yes No 

15. A boxer gives his opponent a bloody nose. Yes No 

16. A Girl Scout tries to assist an elderly woman but trips her by accident. Yes 
No = 

17. A bank robber is shot in the back while trying to escape. Yes No 

18. Atennis player smashes her racket after missing a volley. Yes No 

19. A person commits suicide. Yes No 

20. A cat kills a mouse, parades around with it, and then discards it. Yes 
No 


























discharges it in a process called catharsis. The longer the energy builds up, says 
Lorenz, the greater the amount of the aggression displayed when it is discharged. 

Probably the most controversial idea to come out of instinct theories of aggression 
is Lorenz’s proposal that society should provide acceptable ways of permitting cathar- 
sis. For example, he suggested that participation in aggressive sports and games 
would prevent the discharge of aggression in less socially desirable ways. However, 
little research has found evidence for the existence of a pent-up reservoir of aggression 
that needs to be released. In fact, some studies flatly contradict the notion of catharsis, 
which leads psychologists to look for other explanations for aggression (Bushman, 
Wang, & Anderson, 2005; Scheele & DuBois, 2006; Verona & Sullivan, 2008). 


FRUSTRATION-AGGRESSION APPROACHES: 
AGGRESSION AS A REACTION TO FRUSTRATION 


Suppose you've been working on a paper that is due for a class early the next morn- 
ing, and your computer printer runs out of ink just before you can print out the 
paper. You rush to the store to buy more ink only to find the sales clerk locking the 


FIGURE 3 What is aggression? It 
depends on how the word is defined 
and in what context it is used. (Source: 
Adapted from Benjamin, 1985, p. 41.) 


catharsis The process of discharging 
built-up aggressive energy. 


610 Chapter 17 Social Psychology 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


door for the day. Even though the clerk can see you gesturing and beg- 
ging him to open the door, he refuses, shrugs his shoulders, and points 
to a sign that indicates when the store will open the next day. At that 
moment, the feelings you experience toward the sales clerk probably 
place you on the verge of real aggression, and you are undoubtedly seeth- 
ing inside. 

Frustration-aggression theory tries to explain aggression in terms of 
events like this one. It suggests that frustration (the reaction to the thwart- 
ing or blocking of goals) produces anger, which leads to a readiness to 
act aggressively. Whether actual aggression occurs depends on the pres- 
ence of aggressive cues, stimuli that have been associated in the past with 
actual aggression or violence and that will trigger aggression again 
(Berkowitz, 2001). 

What kinds of stimuli act as aggressive cues? They can range from 
the most explicit, such as the presence of weapons, to more subtle cues, 
such as the mere mention of the name of an individual who behaved 
violently in the past. For example, angered participants in experiments 
behave significantly more aggressively when in the presence of a gun 
than in a comparable situation in which no guns are present. Similarly, 
frustrated participants who view a violent movie are more physically 
aggressive toward a confederate with the same name as the star of the 


~~ movie than they are toward a confederate with a different name. It 
Is road rage a result of frustration? According to appears, then, that frustration does lead to aggression—at least when 
frustration-aggression approaches, frustration is a aggressive cues are present (Berkowitz, 2001; Marcus-Newhall, Pederson, 


likely cause. 


y Alert 
Understand the distinction 
between the instinctual, 
frustration-aggression, and 
observational learning 
approaches to aggression. 


& Carlson, 2000). 


OBSERVATIONAL LEARNING APPROACHES: 
LEARNING TO HURT OTHERS 


Do we learn to be aggressive? The observational learning (sometimes called social 
learning) approach to aggression says that we do. Taking an almost opposite view 
from instinct theories, which focus on innate explanations of aggression, observa- 
tional learning theory emphasizes that social and environmental conditions can teach 
individuals to be aggressive. The theory sees aggression not as inevitable, but rather 
as a learned response that can be understood in terms of rewards and punishments. 

Observational learning theory pays particular attention not only to direct rewards 
and punishments that individuals themselves receive, but also to the rewards and 
punishments that models—individuals who provide a guide to appropriate behavior— 
receive for their aggressive behavior. According to observational learning theory, 
people observe the behavior of models and the subsequent consequences of that 
behavior. If the consequences are positive, the behavior is likely to be imitated when 
observers find themselves in a similar situation. 

Suppose, for instance, a girl hits her younger brother when he damages one of 
her new toys. Whereas instinct theory would suggest that the aggression had been 
pent up and was now being discharged, and frustration-aggression theory would 
examine the girl’s frustration at no longer being able to use her new toy, observational 
learning theory would look to previous situations in which the girl had viewed others 
being rewarded for their aggression. For example, perhaps she had watched a friend 
get to play with a toy after he painfully twisted it out of the hand of another child. 

Observational learning theory has received wide research support. For example, 
children of nursery school age who have watched an adult model behave aggressively 
and then receive reinforcement for it later display similar behavior themselves if they 
have been angered, insulted, or frustrated after exposure. Furthermore, a significant 
amount of research links watching television shows containing violence with subse- 
quent viewer aggression (Winerman, 2005; Greer, Dudek-Singer, & Gautreaux, 2006; 
Carnagey, Anderson, & Bartholow, 2007). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 55 Positive and Negative Social Behavior 611 


Helping Others: The Brighter 
Side of Human Nature 


Turning away from aggression, we move now to the opposite—and brighter—side 
of human nature: helping behavior. Helping behavior, or prosocial behavior as it is 
more formally known, has been considered under many different conditions. How- 
ever, the question that psychologists have looked at most closely relates to bystander 
intervention in emergency situations. What are the factors that lead someone to help 
a person in need? 

One critical factor is the number of others present. When more than one person 
witnesses an emergency situation, a sense of diffusion of responsibility can arise 
among the bystanders (as we discussed earlier in the book when we considered 
research methods). Diffusion of responsibility is the tendency for people to feel that 
responsibility for acting is shared, or diffused, among those present. The more people 
who are present in an emergency, the less personally responsible each individual 
feels—and therefore the less help he or she provides (Blair, Thompson, & Wuensch, 
2005 Barron & Yechiam, 2002; Gray, 2006). 

For example, think back to the classic case of Kitty Genovese that we described 
when discussing the topic of research early in the book. Genovese was stabbed 
multiple times, and—according to some accounts of the event—no one offered help, 
despite the fact that allegedly close to 40 people who lived in nearby apartments 
heard her screams for help. The lack of help has been attributed to diffusion of 
responsibility: The fact that there were so many potential helpers led each indi- 
vidual to feel diminished personal responsibility (Rogers & Eftimiades, 1995; Rosen- 
thal, 2008). 

Although most research on helping behavior supports the diffusion-of- 
responsibility explanation, other factors are clearly involved in helping behavior. 
According to a model of the helping process, the decision to give aid involves four 
basic steps (Latané & Darley, 1970; Garcia et al., 2002; see Figure 4): 





e Noticing a person, event, or situation that may require help. 

e Interpreting the event as one that requires help. Even if we notice an event, it may 
be sufficiently ambiguous for us to interpret it as a nonemergency situation. It 
is here that the presence of others first affects helping behavior. The presence 
of inactive others may indicate to us that a situation does not require help—a 
judgment we do not necessarily make if we are alone. 

e Assuming responsibility for helping. It is at this point that diffusion of responsi- 
bility is likely to occur if others are present. Moreover, a bystander’s particular 
expertise is likely to play a role in determining whether he or she helps. For 
instance, if people with training in medical aid or lifesaving techniques are 
present, untrained bystanders are less likely to intervene because they feel 
they have less expertise. 

e Deciding on and implementing the form of helping. After we assume responsibility 
for helping, we must decide how to provide assistance. Helping can range 
from very indirect forms of intervention, such as calling the police, to more 
direct forms, such as giving first aid or taking the victim to a hospital. Most 
social psychologists use a rewards—costs approach for helping to predict the 
nature of the assistance a bystander will choose to provide. The general notion 
is that the bystander’s perceived rewards for helping must outweigh the costs 
if helping is to occur, and most research tends to support this notion (Koper & 
Jaasma, 2001; Bartlett & DeSteno, 2006; Lin & Lin, 2007). 


After determining the nature of the assistance needed, the actual help must be 
implemented. A rewards-costs analysis suggests that we are most likely to use the 


prosocial behavior Helping behavior. 


diffusion of responsibility The 
tendency for people to feel that respon- 
sibility for acting is shared, or diffused, 
among those present. 





FIGURE 4 The basic steps of helping. 
(Source: Latané, Bibb; Darley, & John M., The 
Unresponsive Bystander: Why Doesn't He Help? 
Reproduced by permission of Pearson 
Education, Inc., p. 87-91. Copyright © 1971 
Appleton-Century-Crofts.) 


612 Chapter 17 Social Psychology 


altruism Helping behavior that is 
beneficial to others but clearly requires 
self-sacrifice. 






Alert 
The distinction between 
prosocial behavior and 
altruism is important. 
Prosocial behavior need 
not have a self-sacrificing 
component; altruism, by 
definition, contains an 
element of self-sacrifice. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Altruism is often the only bright side of a natural disaster. 


least costly form of implementation. However, this is not always the case: In some 
situations, people behave altruistically. Altruism is helping behavior that is beneficial 
to others but clearly requires self-sacrifice. For example, people who put themselves 
at mortal risk to help strangers escape from the burning World Trade Center towers 
during the 9/11 terrorist attack would be considered altruistic (Krueger, Hicks, & 
McGue, 2001; Batson & Powell, 2003; Manor & Gailliot, 2007). 

People who intervene in emergency situations tend to possess certain personal- 
ity characteristics that differentiate them from nonhelpers. For example, helpers are 
more self-assured, sympathetic, and emotionally understanding, and they have 
greater empathy (a personality trait in which someone observing another person expe- 
riences the emotions of that person) than are nonhelpers (Graziano et al., 2007; 
Walker & Frimer, 2007; Stocks, Lishner, & Decker, 2009). 

Still, most social psychologists agree that no single set of attributes differenti- 
ates helpers from nonhelpers. For the most part, temporary situational factors 
(such as the mood we’re in) determine whether we will intervene in a situation 
requiring aid (Eisenberg, Guthrie, & Cumberland, 2002; Dovidio et al., 2006; Sal- 
Iquist et al., 2009). 

More generally, what leads people to make moral decisions? Clearly, situational 
factors make a difference. For example, one study asked people to judge the moral- 
ity of plane crash survivors cannibalizing an injured boy to avoid starvation. Par- 
ticipants in the study were more likely to condemn the behavior if they were placed 
in an emotional state than if they were less emotional (Schnall et al., 2008). 

Other psychologists, using a neuroscience perspective, believe that there’s a kind 
of tug of war between emotion and rationale thinking in the brain. If the rational 
side wins out, we’re more likely to take a logical view of moral situations (if you’re 
at risk for starving, go ahead and eat the injured boy). On the other hand, if the 
emotional side prevails, we’re more likely to condemn the cannibalism, even if it 
means we may be harmed. In support of such reasoning, researchers have found that 
different areas of the brain are involved in honest and dishonest moral decisions 
(Miller, 2008; Greene & Paxton, 2009; see Figure 5). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 55 Positive and Negative Social Behavior 


Moral Decisions and the Brain 





FIGURE 5 Making moral decisions about whether to act honestly or dishonestly results in 


different areas of the brain being activated. In a study in which people were induced to behave 
dishonestly, participants showed increased activity in the bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal cortex 


(DLPFC) area of the brain when they made decisions to lie. (Source: Greene & Paxton, 2009, 
Figure 3A.) 





At one time or another, almost everyone feels angry. The anger may BECOMING AN 
result from a frustrating situation, or it may be due to another INFORMED CONSUMER 


individual’s behavior. The way we deal with anger may determine 


the difference between a promotion and a lost job or a broken of Psyc h O | ogy 


relationship and one that mends itself. 

Social psychologists who have studied the topic suggest several 
good strategies to deal with anger that maximize the potential for 
positive consequences (Ellis, 2000; Nelson & Finch, 2000). Among 
the most useful strategies are the following: 


e Look again at the anger-provoking situation from the perspective of others. By taking 
others’ points of view, you may be able to understand the situation better, and 
with increased understanding you may become more tolerant of the apparent 
shortcomings of others. 

e Minimize the importance of the situation. Does it really matter that someone is 


driving too slowly and that you'll be late to an appointment as a result? Reinter- 


pret the situation in a way that is less bothersome. 


e Fantasize about getting even—but don’t act on it. Fantasy provides a safety valve. In 


your fantasies, you can yell at that unfair professor all you want and suffer no 


consequences at all. However, don’t spend too much time brooding: Fantasize, 


but then move on. 

e Relax. By teaching yourself the relaxation techniques used in systematic 
desensitization (discussed in the module on treatment of psychological 
disorders), you can help reduce your reactions to anger. In turn, your anger 
may dissipate. 


No matter which of these strategies you try, above all, don’t ignore your anger. 
People who always try to suppress their anger may experience a variety of consequences, 
such as self-condemnation, frustration, and even physical illness (Burns, Quartana, & 


Bruehl, 2007; Quartana & Burns, 2007; Gardner & Moore, 2008). 


613 


Dealing Effectively with Anger 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


614 Chapter 17 Social Psychology 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 3. Based on research evidence, which of the following might 
be the best way to reduce the amount of fighting a young 
boy does? 

a. Take him to the gym and let him work out on the box- 
ing equipment. 

b. Make him repeatedly watch violent scenes from the 
film The Matrix Reloaded in the hope that it will provide 
catharsis. 

c. Reward him if he doesn’t fight during a certain period. 

d. Ignore it and let it die out naturally. 

4. If a person in a crowd does not help in an apparent emer- 
gency situation because many other people are present, 
that person is falling victim to the phenomenon of 


Why are we attracted to certain people, and what progression 
do social relationships follow? 
e The primary determinants of liking include proximity, 
exposure, similarity, and physical attractiveness. (p. 604) 
e Loving is distinguished from liking by the presence of 
intense physiological arousal, an all-encompassing inter- 
est in another, fantasies about the other, rapid swings of 
emotion, fascination, sexual desire, exclusiveness, and 
strong feelings of caring. (p. 604) 
e Love can be categorized as passionate or companionate. 
In addition, love has several components: intimacy, pas- 
sion, and decision/commitment. (p. 605) 





What factors underlie aggression and prosocial behavior? 
e Aggression is intentional injury of or harm to another 
person. (p. 608) RETHINK 

e Explanations of aggression include instinct approaches, 
frustration-aggression theory, and observational learn- 
ing. (p. 610) 

e Helping behavior in emergencies is determined in part 
by the phenomenon of diffusion of responsibility, which 
results in a lower likelihood of helping when more peo- 
ple are present. (p. 611) 

e Deciding to help is the outcome of a four-stage process 
that consists of noticing a possible need for help, 
interpreting the situation as requiring aid, assuming 
responsibility for taking action, and deciding on and 
implementing a form of assistance. (p. 611) 


1. Can love be studied scientifically? Is there an elusive qual- 
ity to love that makes it at least partially unknowable? 
How would you define “falling in love”? How would you 
study it? 

2. From the perspective of a criminal justice worker: How would 
proponents of the three main approaches to the study of 
aggression—instinct approaches, frustration-aggression 
approaches, and observational learning approaches—in- 
terpret the aggression of Eric Rudolph, who was convicted 
of exploding a bomb during the 1996 Summer Olympics 
in Atlanta and later of attacking several women’s clinics? 
Do you think any of these approaches fits the Rudolph 
case more closely than the others? 


EVALUATE Answers to Evaluate Questions 


; ee Á suodsax Jo uoTs E D oncom 
1. We tend to like people who are similar to us. True or false? A E E 


2. Which of the following sets are the three components of 
love proposed by Sternberg? 
a. Passion, closeness, sexuality 
b. Attraction, desire, complementarity 
c. Passion, intimacy, decision / commitment 
d. Commitment, caring, sexuality 


KEY TERMS 


interpersonal reciprocity-of-liking companionate love p. 605 diffusion of 
attraction (or close effect p. 605 aggression p. 608 responsibility p. 611 
relationship) p. 604 passionate (or romantic) catharsis p. 609 altruism p. 611 


love p. 605 prosocial behavior p. 611 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Psychology on the Web 


1. Find examples on the web of advertisements or other persuasive messages that use 
central route processing and peripheral route processing. What type of persuasion 
appears to be more prevalent on the web? For what type of persuasion does the web 
appear to be better suited? Is there a difference between web-based advertising and 
other forms of advertising? 

2. Is “hate crimes legislation” a good idea? Use the web to find at least two discussions 
of hate crimes legislation—one in favor and one opposed—and summarize in writing 
the main issues and arguments presented. Using your knowledge of prejudice and 
aggression, evaluate the arguments for and against hate crimes legislation. State your 
opinion about whether this type of legislation is advisable. 


E © | log U Q We have touched on some of the major ideas, research 


topics, and experimental findings of social psychology. 
We examined how people form, maintain, and change attitudes and how they form 
impressions of others and assign attributions to them. We also saw how groups, through 
conformity and tactics of compliance, can influence individuals’ actions and attitudes. 
Finally, we discussed interpersonal relationships, including both liking and loving, and 
looked at aggression and prosocial behavior, the two sides of a coin that represent the 
extremes of social behavior. 

Turn back to the prologue of this set of modules, which describes Perry Bland’s 
heroism. Use your understanding of social psychology to consider the following 
questions. 





1. What factors would a social psychologist consider in examining why Bland rushed to 
help two strangers who were trapped in their car? 

2. Given what social psychologists know about the factors that lead people to be 
helpful, do you believe that Bland’s helpfulness was caused by situational factors or 
that it had more to do with his personality? Why? 

3. Do you think Bland would have reacted in a similar manner if he had had more time 
to think about acting? Why or why not? 

4. What are some ways in which helping behavior can be encouraged? 





615 








APPENDIX 


Going by the Numbers: 
Statistics in Psychology 


re oo NT 
















ty f ey 
x ey i Ware 






_ 
x 
"4 
ae 
A A 
yo. 
A 
wy 


T i Care al Ys, ia 





ETA 


4 











www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Key Concepts for Appendix 


MODULE 56 







What measures can we use to summarize Descriptive Statistics 


The Mean: Finding the Average 
The Median: Finding the Middle 
The Mode: Finding What 

Is Most Frequent 


sets of data? 





Comparing the Three M's: Mean 
Versus Median Versus Mode 






MODULE 57 







How can we assess the variability of a set of Measures of Variability 
The Range: Highest Minus Lowest 


? 
data? The Standard Deviation: 
Differences from the Mean 





MODULE 58 





How do we generalize from data? @ Using Statistics to Answer 
Questions: Inferential Statistics 
and Correlation 

The Correlation Coefficient: Measuring 


Relationships 


How can we determine the nature of a 
relationship, and the significance of 


differences, between two sets of scores? 


A-3 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Prologue Selma Vorwerk 





As the boat moved nearer to shore, the outline of the Statue of 
Liberty was plainly visible in the distance. Closer and closer it 
came, sending a chill down the Selma Vorwerk’s spine. A symbol 
of America, the statue represented the hopes she carried from 
her native Europe in the early 1900s—hopes of liberty, of success, 
of a life free of economic and social strain. 

Yet as the boat sailed closer to Ellis Island, the first point of 
arrival in the United States, Vorwerk did not realize that her very 
presence—and that of the other thousands of immigrants 
seeking their fortune in a land of opportunity—was threatened. 
A strong political movement was growing in the country on 
which she was pinning her hopes. That movement sought, by 
using information collected by psychologists, to stem the flow 
of immigrants through “scientific” analysis of data. 


The groups’ major claim was that a flood of “mentally defi- 
cient” immigrants was poisoning the intellectual capacity of the 
United States. To supporters of that view, unless drastic measures 
were taken, it would not be too many years before Western 
civilization collapsed from a lack of intelligence. 

To support this assertion, Lathrop Stoddard, a member of 
the anti-immigration movement, reported the results of a study 
of intelligence in which tests were administered to a group of 
82 children and 400 adults. On the basis of those test results, he 
concluded that the average mental age of people in the United 
States was only 14 years—proof to him that unlimited immigra- 
tion had already produced a serious decline in the country’s 
intelligence. 





Fortunately for immigrants such as Selma Vorwerk, observers in 
favor of immigration pointed out the fallacy of using data from 
a relatively small sample—when a considerably larger set of 
intelligence test data was available. The U.S. Army had been 
collecting intelligence data for years and had the test scores of 
1.7 million men available. When those scores were analyzed, it 
immediately became apparent that the claim that the average 
mental age of American adults was 14 years was completely 
without merit. 

A debate reminiscent of this one rages today as some 
observers suggest that an unrestrained flow of immigrants—this 


time from Latin America and Asia—will seriously damage the 
United States. This time, though, the debate is based more on 
analyses of social and economic statistics: opponents of immigra- 
tion suggest that the social fabric of the country will be changed 
and that jobs are being taken away from longer-term residents 
because of the influx of immigrants. Equally vehement propo- 
nents of immigration suggest that the relevant statistics are 
being misinterpreted and that their analyses of the situation 
result in a very different conclusion. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


MODULE 56 


Statistics, the branch of mathematics concerned with collecting, organizing, analyz- 
ing, and drawing conclusions from numerical data, is a part of all of our lives. For 
instance, we are all familiar with the claims and counterclaims regarding the effects 
of smoking. The U.S. government requires cigarette manufacturers to include a warn- 
ing that smoking is dangerous to people’s health on every package of cigarettes and 
in their advertisements; the government’s data show clear statistical links between 
smoking and disease. At the same time, the tobacco industry has long minimized the 
negative effects of smoking. 

Statistics is also at the heart of a considerable number of debates in the field of 
psychology. How do we determine the nature and strength of the effects of heredity 
on behavior? What is the relationship between learning and schedules of reinforce- 
ment? How do we know if the “double standard” regarding male and female sexual 
practices has shifted over time? These questions and most others of interest to psy- 
chologists cannot be answered without using statistics. 

In this set of modules, we consider the basic approaches to statistical measure- 
ment. We first discuss approaches to summarizing data that allow us to describe sets 
of observations. Next, we consider techniques for deciding how different one set of 
scores is from another. Finally, we examine approaches to measuring the relationship 
between two sets of scores. 

Suppose as an instructor of college psychology you wanted to evaluate your 
class’s performance on its initial exam. Where might you begin? 

You would probably start by using descriptive statistics, the branch of statistics 
that provides a means of summarizing data and presenting it in a usable and con- 
venient form. For instance, you might first simply list the scores the pupils had 
received on the test: 


72 78 78 92 69 73 85 49 
86 86 72 59 58 85 89 
80 83 69 78 90 90 96 83 


Viewed this way, the scores are a jumble of numbers of which 
it is difficult to make any sense. However, there are several meth- 
ods by which you could begin to organize the scores in a more 
meaningful way. For example, you might sort them in order of 
highest score to lowest score, as in Figure 1. By indicating the 
number of people who obtained each score, you would have pro- 
duced what is called a frequency distribution, an arrangement of 
scores from a sample that indicates how often a particular score 
is present. 

Another way of summarizing the scores is to consider them 
visually. For example, you could construct the histogram, or bar 
graph, shown in Figure 2. In this histogram, the number of people 
obtaining a given score is represented pictorially. The scores are 





What measures can we use 
to summarize sets of data? 


The branch of mathematics 
concerned with collecting, organizing, 
analyzing, and drawing conclusions 


from numerical data. 


The branch of 


statistics that provides a means of 
summarizing data. 


An arrange- 


ment of scores from a sample that 


indicates how often a particular score 


is present. 


Bar graph. 





ordered along one dimension of the graph and the number of peo- “Meaningless statistics were up one-point-five percent 
ple obtaining each score along the other dimension. this month over last month.” 


© The New Yorker Collection 1977 Dana Fradon from 


cartoonbank.com. All Rights Reserved. 


A-6 


A Sample Frequency Distribution 


Test 

Score 
96 
92 
90 
89 
86 
85 
83 
80 
78 
73 
72. 
69 
52 
58 
49 


Number of Students 
Attaining That Score 


—--—- -NN—-WH-NNNH— NY -— = 


FIGURE 1 Example of a frequency 


distribution, which arranges scores from 


a sample and indicates how often a 
particular score is present. 


Number of students 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Appendix Going by the Numbers: Statistics in Psychology 


Arranging the scores from the highest to the lowest allows us to visually inspect 
the data. Most often, however, visual inspection is insufficient. For one thing, there 
may be so many scores in a sample that it is difficult to construct a meaningful visual 
representation. For another, our interpretations of patterns on a graph or table are 
often biased and inaccurate; more precise, mathematically based measures would 
seem to be preferable. In cases in which a precise means of summarizing the data is 
desirable, psychologists turn to measures of central tendency. Central tendency is an 
index of the central location within a distribution of scores. There are three major 
measures of central tendency: the mean, the median, and the mode. 





The Mean: Finding the Average 


The most familiar measure of central tendency is the mean. A mean is the technical term 
for an average, which is simply the sum of all the scores in a set, divided by the number 
of scores making up the set. For example, to calculate the mean of the sample we have 
been using, begin by adding each of the numbers (96 + 92 + 90 + 90 + 89 +... and 
so forth). When you have the total, divide that sum by the number of scores, which is 
23. This calculation, 1,800/23 = 78.26 produces a mean score, or average, for our sample. 

In general, the mean is an accurate reflection of the central score in a set of scores; 
as you can see from the histogram in Figure 2, our mean of 78.26 falls roughly in the 
center of the distribution of scores. Yet the mean does not always provide the best 
measure of central tendency. For one thing, the mean is very sensitive to extreme 
scores. As an example, imagine that we added two scores of 20 and 22 to our sample 
scores. The mean would now become 1,842/25, or 73.68—a drop of almost five 
points. Because of its sensitivity to extreme scores, then, the mean can sometimes 
present a deceptive picture of a set of scores, especially in cases where the mean is 
based on a relatively small number of scores. 





78 
3 
3 69 72 83 8586 90 
i 49 58 59 73 E 92 96 
0 a eae E | De (PL ee | | fi =p f= al | | | | bl l | ae | 
50 55 60 65 70 75 80 85 90 95 
Score 


FIGURE 2 In this histogram, the number of students obtaining each score is represented 
by a bar. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


The Mean: 
Finding the Middle 


A measure of central tendency that is less sensitive to extreme scores than the mean 
is the median. The median is the point in a distribution of scores that divides the 
distribution exactly in half. If we arrange all the scores in order from the highest to 
the lowest, the median lies in the middle of the distribution. 

For example, consider a distribution of five scores: 10, 8, 7, 4, and 3. The point 
that divides the distribution exactly in half is the score 7: Two scores in the distribu- 
tion lie above the 7 score, and two scores lie below it. If there are an even number 
of scores in a distribution—in which case there will be no score lying in the middle— 
the two middle scores are averaged. If our distribution consisted of scores of 10, 8, 
7,6, 4, and 3, then, we would average the two middle scores of 7 and 6 to form a 
median of 7 + 6 divided by 2, or 13/2 = 6.5. 

In our original sample test scores, there are 23 scores. The score that divides the 
distribution exactly in half will be the 12th score in the frequency distribution of 
scores because the 12th score has 11 scores above it and 11 below it. If you count 
down to the 11th score in the distribution depicted in Figure 1, you will see that the 
score is 80. Therefore, the median of the distribution is 80. 

One feature of the median as a measure of central tendency is that it is insensi- 
tive to extreme scores. For example, adding the scores of 20 and 22 to our distribution 
would change the median no more than would adding scores of 48 and 47 to the 
distribution. The reason is clear: The median divides a set of scores in half, and the 
magnitude of the scores is of no consequence in this process. 

The median is often used instead of the mean when extreme scores might be 
misleading. For example, government statistics on income are typically presented 
using the median as the measure of central tendency because the median corrects for 
the small number of extreme cases of very wealthy individuals, whose high incomes 
might otherwise inflate the mean income. 


The Mode: 
Finding What Is Most Frequent 


The final measure of central tendency is the mode. The mode is the most frequently 
occurring score in a set of scores. If you return to the distribution in Figure 1, you 
can see that three people scored 78, and the frequency of all the other scores is either 
2 or 1. The mode for the distribution, then, is 78. 

Some distributions, of course, may have more than one score occurring most 
frequently. For instance, we could imagine that if the distribution had a score of 86 
added to the two that are already there, there would be two most frequently occur- 
ring categories: 78 and now 86. In this instance, we would say there are two modes— 
a case known as a bimodal distribution. 

The mode is often used as a measure of preference or popularity. For instance, 
if teachers wanted to know who was the most popular child in their elementary 
school classrooms, they might develop a questionnaire that asked the students to 
choose someone with whom they would like to participate in some activity. After the 
choices were tallied, the mode probably would provide the best indication of which 
child was the most popular. 








Module 56 Descriptive Statistics A-7 


central tendency An index of the 
central location within a distribution 
of scores; the most representative score 
in a distribution of scores (the mean, 
median, and mode are measures of 
central tendency). 


mean The average of all scores, 
arrived at by adding scores together 
and dividing by the number of scores. 


median The point in a distribution of 
scores that divides the distribution 
exactly in half when the scores are 
listed in numerical order. 


mode The most frequently occurring 
score in a set of scores. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


A-8 Appendix Going by the Numbers: Statistics in Psychology 


normal distribution A distribution of 
scores that produces a symmetrical, 
bell-shaped curve in which the right 
half mirrors the left half and in which 
the mean, median, and mode all have 
the same value. 


FIGURE 3 In a normal distribution, the 
mean, median, and mode are identical, 
thus falling at the center of the 
distribution. 


FIGURE 4 In this skewed distribution, 
most scores are low. 


Comparing the Three M's: Mean 
Versus Median Versus Mode 


If a sample is sufficiently large, there is generally little difference between the mean, 
median, and mode. The reason is that with large samples, scores typically form what 
is called a normal distribution. A normal distribution is a distribution of scores that 
produces a symmetrical, bell-shaped curve, such as the one displayed in Figure 3 in 
which the right half mirrors the left half and in which the mean, median, and mode 
all have the same value. 

Most large distributions—those containing many scores—produce a normal 
curve. For instance, if you asked a large number of students how many hours a week 
they studied, you might expect to find that most studied within a similar range of 
hours, and there would be a few who studied many, many hours and a very few 
who did not study at all. There would be many scores hovering around the center 
of the distribution of scores, then, and only a few at the extremes—producing a 
normal distribution. Many phenomena of interest to psychologists produce a normal 
curve when graphed. For example, the distribution of IQ scores among the general 
population falls into a normal distribution. 

The mean, median, and mode fall at exactly the same point in a normal distribu- 
tion. This means that in a normal distribution of scores, the mean score will divide 
the distribution exactly in half (making it the median), and it will be the most fre- 
quently occurring score in the distribution (making it the mode). 

The mean, median, and mode differ, however, when distributions are not normal. 
In cases in which the distributions are skewed, or not symmetrical, there is a “hump” at 
one end (see Figures 4 and 5). For instance, if we gave a calculus exam to a group of 
students enrolled in an elementary algebra class, we would expect that most of the 
students would fail the test, leading to low scores being overrepresented in the distribu- 
tion as in Figure 4. On the other hand, if we gave the same students a test of elementary 


Mean 
Median 
Mode 











Mode 
Median 





Mean 








Low scores High scores 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Mode 
Median 


Mean 








Module 56 Descriptive Statistics A-9 


FIGURE 5 In this example of a skewed 
distribution, there tend to be more high 
scores than low scores. 





Low scores High scores 


addition problems, the scores would probably form a distribution in which high scores 
predominated as in Figure 5. Both distributions are skewed although in opposite direc- 
tions, and the mean, median, and mode are different from one another. 


RECAP 


What measures can we use to summarize sets of data? 

e Statistics is concerned with collecting, organizing, analyz- 
ing, and drawing conclusions from numerical data. (p. A-5) 

e Descriptive statistics provides a means of summarizing 
data and presenting it in a usable and convenient form. 
(p. A-5) 

e A frequency distribution arranges scores from a sample 
by indicating how often a particular score is presented. 
A histogram, or bar graph, presents the same data 
pictorially. (p. A-5) 

e Central tendency is the most representative score in a 


distribution of scores. The mean (or average) is generally 


the best measure of central tendency. The median is the 
point or score in a distribution that divides the distribu- 
tion in half so that half the scores are higher and half are 
lower. The third measure of central tendency is the 
mode, the most frequently occurring score in a distribu- 
tion of scores. (p. A-6) 


EVALUATE 


1. A frequency distribution of numbers could be displayed 
pictorially by constructing a bar graph, or 


KEY TERMS 


statistics p. A-5 
descriptive statistics p. A-5 
frequency distribution p. A-5 


histogram p. A-5 
central tendency p. A-6 
mean p. A-6 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


2. 


Match each item in the left-hand column with the corre- 
sponding item in the right-hand column. 

1. Mean = 10 Bl, Pp S 10 1213 le} 

2. Median = 11 lo 4,5, 10, 110), 15), 6 

3. Mode = 12 i AL, 12 12, 1D, 6 

The mean, median, and mode are measures of 


Professor Garcia explains to the class that most of the 

40 exam scores fell within a B range, but there were two 
extremely high scores. Should she report the median or 
the mean as a measure of central tendency? 

The mean, median, and mode will differ in a normal dis- 
tribution. True or false? 


RETHINK 


1. 


Government statistics on family income are presented in a 
variety of ways. What would be the most useful way of 
providing a summary of family incomes across the coun- 
try: the mean, median, or mode? Why might providing 
only the mean be misleading? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


Tenba aq [Im ayy ‘aspey G /SƏI00S IUƏIJXƏ 0} BATIISUS 00} ST UaUT 
əy} ‘uerpou oy} “pf ‘Áouəpuə LUD *E 19-¢ “L-Z ‘q-[ °Z ‘wIeISOYSTY ‘TL 


median p. A-7 
mode p. A-7 
normal distribution p. A-8 


ULE 


Measures of Variability 


Key Concept 


How can we assess the vari- 
ability of a set of data? 


variability The spread, or dispersion, 
of scores in a distribution. 


range The difference between the 
highest score and the lowest score in a 
distribution. 


A-10 





Although measures of central tendency provide information about where the center 
of a distribution lies, often this information is insufficient. For example, suppose a 
psychologist was interested in determining the nature of people’s eye movements 
while they were reading in order to perfect a new method to teach reading. It would 
not be enough to know how most people moved their eyes (information that a mea- 
sure of central tendency would provide); it would also be important to know how 
much individual people’s eye movements differed or varied from one another. 

A second important characteristic of a set of scores provides this information: 
variability. Variability is a term that refers to the spread, or dispersion, of scores in 
a distribution. Figure 1 contains two distributions of scores that have identical means 
but differ in variability. Measures of variability provide a way to describe the spread 
of scores in a distribution. 


Mean=50 


Mean=50 











(a) (b) 


FIGURE 1 Although the mean is iden- 
tical in these two distributions, the variability, or spread of scores, is very different. Specifically, 
the variability is considerably greater in (a) than in (b). 


The Range: 
Highest Minus Lowest 


The simplest measure of variability is the range. The range is the difference between 
the highest score in a distribution and the lowest score. In the following set of scores 
the distribution has a range of 47 (96 — 49 = 47). 





96 92 90 90 89 86 
86 85 85 83 83 80 
78 78 78 73 72 72. 
69 69 59 58 49 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 57 Measures of Variability A-11 


The fact that a range is simple to calculate is about its only virtue. The problem 
with this particular measure of variability is that it is based entirely on extreme 
scores, and a single score that is very different from the others in a distribution can 
distort the picture of the distribution as a whole. For example, the addition of a score 
of 20 to the test score distribution we are considering would almost double the range 
even though the variability of the remaining scores in the distribution would not 
have changed at all. 


The Standard Deviation: 
Differences from the Mean 


The most frequently used method of characterizing the variability of a distribution 
of scores is the standard deviation. The standard deviation bears a conceptual rela- 
tionship to a mean. Recall that the mean is the average score in a distribution of 
scores. A standard deviation is an index of the average deviation of a set of scores 
from the center of the distribution. 

Consider, for instance, the distributions in Figure 1. The distribution on the left 
is widely dispersed; on the average an individual score in the distribution can be 
thought of as deviating quite a bit from the center of the distribution. Certainly the 
scores in the distribution on the left are going to deviate more from the center of the 
distribution than those in the distribution on the right. 

In contrast, in the distribution on the right, the scores are closely packed together 
and there is little deviation of a typical score from the center of the distribution. On 
the basis of this analysis, then, it would be expected that a good measure of vari- 
ability would yield a larger value for the distribution on the left than it would for 
the one on the right—and, in fact, a standard deviation would do exactly this by 
indicating how far away a typical score lies from the center of the distribution. 

In a normal distribution, 68% of the scores fall within one standard deviation of 
the mean (34% on either side of it), 95% of the scores fall within two standard devi- 
ations, and 99.7% fall within three standard deviations. In the general population, IQ 
scores of intelligence fall into a normal distribution, and they have a mean of 100 
and a standard deviation of 15. Consequently, an IQ score of 100 does not deviate 
from the mean, whereas an IQ score that is three standard deviations above the mean 
(or 145) is very unusual (higher than 99% of all IQ scores). 

The calculation of the standard deviation follows the logic of calculating the dif- 
ference of individual scores from the mean of the distribution (see Figure 2). Not 
only does the standard deviation provide an excellent indicator of the variability of 
a set of scores, it provides a means for converting initial scores on standardized tests 
such as the SAT (the college admissions exam) into the scales used to report results. 
In this way, it is possible to make a score of 585 on the verbal section of the SAT 
exam, for example, equivalent from one year to the next even though the specific test 
items differ from year to year. 





standard deviation An index of the 
average deviation of a set of scores 
from the center of the distribution. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


A-12 Appendix Going by the Numbers: Statistics in Psychology 


Calculating a Standard Deviation 


1. The calculation of a standard deviation begins with the calculation of the mean of 
distribution. In the following distribution of scores on a psychology student’s weekly 
quizzes, the mean is 84.5: 82, 88, 71, 86, 96, 84. (As you recall, the mean is the sum 
of the scores divided by the number of scores in the distribution, or 507 +6=84.5.) 

2. The next step is to produce a deviation score for each score in the distribution. A 
deviation score is simply an original score minus the mean of all the scores in a 
distribution. This has been done in the second column below: 


Deviation Score 





Original Score Deviation Score Squared 
82 Z229 6.25 
88 35 1225 
71 =1135 182.25 
86 ES) 225 
96 11.5 13225 
84 =) 25 


3. In the third step, the deviation scores are squared (multiplied by themselves) to 
eliminate negative numbers. This has been carried out in the third column above. 

4. The squared deviation scores are then added together; and this sum is divided by 
the number of scores. In the example above, the sum of the squared deviation 
scores is 6.25+12.25+ 182.25+2.25+132.25+.25=335.50, and 335.50+6=55.916. 

5. The final step is to take the square root of the resulting number. The square root of 
55.916 is 7.4777—which is the standard deviation of the distribution of scores. 

6. To summarize, the standard deviation is calculated using the formula 








(score — mean)? 
N 


Original score minus the mean of 84.5 

(Note: Because this formula provides the standard deviation for a sample, the sum of 
the deviation scores is divided by the number of scores, N. However, in some cases, in 
which we might wish to generalize beyond the specific sample to a larger population, 
the standard deviation is calculated by using the number of scores minus 1, or N—1.) 


FIGURE 2 How to calculate a standard deviation. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 57 Measures of Variability A-13 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 2. By simply eyeing the following sets of numbers, predict 
which will have a higher standard deviation and why: 
ao OnlOn LO 2e IS 
1s 210), Sh, IL, IG, 1%, ils 

3. Calculate the mean and standard deviation for sets a and 
b in Question 2. 


How can we assess the variability of a set of data? 

e The range and standard deviation are two measures of 
variability, which is the spread, or dispersion, of scores 
in a distribution. The range is the distance between the 
largest score in a distribution and the smallest score. The 
standard deviation is an index of the extent to which the 
average score in a distribution deviates from the center RETHINK 


Chee ee Uo A) 1. If you were interested in understanding the number of 


people living below the poverty line in the United States, 
EVALUATE why might the range and the standard deviation provide 
you with a better understanding of the extent of poverty 
than would measures of central tendency (the mean, me- 
dian, and mode)? 


1. A measure of variability based solely on the distance be- 
tween the most extreme scores is the 


a. Spread 

b. Standard deviation Answers to Evaluate Questions 

c. Deviation score 08'S = UOYeIAap prepurjs ‘TI = eow qg OTZ = UONeIAap prepur}s 
d. Range ‘OL = Ueow "eg ‘pasiedstp ATOpIM aIOU are SIaquinu ay} asnedeq ‘q z “P'T 


KEY TERMS 


variability p. A-10 range p. A-10 standard deviation p. A-11 


How do we generalize 
from data? 


How can we determine the 
nature of a relationship, and 
the significance of differences, 
between two sets of scores? 


All the members of a 
group of interest. 


A representative subgroup 
of a population of interest. 


The branch of 
statistics that uses data from samples 
to make predictions about the larger 
population from which the sample is 
drawn. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Suppose you were a psychologist who was interested in whether there is a relation- 
ship between smoking and anxiety. Would it be reasonable to simply look at a group 
of smokers and measure their anxiety using some rating scale? Probably not. It 
clearly would be more informative if you compared their anxiety with the anxiety 
exhibited by a group of nonsmokers. 

Once you decided to observe anxiety in two groups of people, you would have 
to determine just who would be your subjects. In an ideal world with unlimited 
resources, you might contact every smoker and nonsmoker because these are the 
two populations with which you are concerned. A population consists of all the 
members of a group of interest. Obviously, however, this would be impossible 
because of the all-encompassing size of the two groups; instead, you would limit 
your subjects to a sample of smokers and nonsmokers. A sample, in formal statis- 
tical terms, is a subgroup of a population of interest that is intended to be repre- 
sentative of the larger population. Once you had identified samples representative 
of the population of interest to you, it would be possible to carry out your study 
that would yield two distributions of scores—one from the smokers and one from 
the nonsmokers. 

The obvious question is whether the two samples differ in the degree of anxiety 
their members display. The statistical procedures that we discussed earlier are help- 
ful in answering this question because each of the two samples can be examined in 
terms of central tendency and variability. The more important question, though, is 
whether the magnitude of difference between the two distributions is sufficient to 
conclude that the distributions truly differ from one another, or if, instead, the dif- 
ferences are attributable merely to chance. 

To answer the question of whether samples are truly different from one another, 
psychologists use inferential statistics. Inferential statistics is the branch of statistics 
that uses data from samples to make predictions about a larger population, permit- 
ting generalizations to be drawn. To take a simple example, suppose you had two 
coins that both were flipped 100 times. Suppose further that one coin came up heads 
41 times, and the other came up heads 65 times. Are both coins fair? We know that 
a fair coin should come up heads about 50 times in 100 flips. But a little thought 
would also suggest it is unlikely that even a fair coin would come up heads exactly 
50 times in 100 flips. The question is, then, how far a coin could deviate from 50 heads 
before that coin would be considered unfair. 

Questions such as this—as well as whether the results found are due to chance 
or represent unexpected, nonchance findings—revolve around how “probable” 
certain events are. Using coin flipping as an example, 53 heads in 100 flips would 
be a highly probable outcome because it departs only slightly from the expected 
outcome of 50 heads. In contrast, if a coin was flipped 100 times and 90 of those 
times it came up heads, that would be a highly improbable outcome. In fact, 90 heads 
out of 100 flips should occur by chance only once in 2 million trials of 100 flips 
of a fair coin. Ninety heads in 100 flips, then, is an extremely improbable outcome; 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 58 Using Statistics to Answer Questions: Inferential Statistics and Correlation A-15 


if 90 heads did appear, the odds would be that the coin or the flipping process 
was rigged. 

Inferential statistics are used to mathematically determine the probability of 
observed events. By using inferential statistics to evaluate the result of an experiment, 
psychologists are able to calculate the likelihood that the difference is a reflection of 
a true difference between populations. For example, suppose we find that the mean 
on an anxiety scale is 68 for smokers and 48 for nonsmokers. Inferential statistical 
procedures allow us to determine whether this difference is really meaningful or 
whether we might expect the same difference to occur merely because of chance fac- 
tors (Gaffney & Henry, 2007). 

The results of inferential statistical procedures are described in terms of mea- 
sures of significance. To a psychologist, a significant outcome is one in which the 
observed outcome would be expected to have occurred only by chance with a 
probability of .05 or less. Put another way, a significant difference between two 
means says that there are only 5 chances out of 100 (or less) that the difference an 
experimenter has found is due to chance rather than to an actual difference between 
the means. 

Obtaining a significant outcome in a study does not necessarily imply that the 
results of an experiment have real-world importance. An experiment may demon- 
strate that two groups differ significantly from one another, but the meaning of the 
differences in terms of what occurs outside the laboratory may be limited. Still, 
finding a significant outcome tells us something important: The differences a 
researcher has found are overwhelmingly likely to be true differences that are not 
only due to chance. 


The Correlation Coefficient: 
Measuring Relationships 


How do we know if television viewing is related to aggression, if reading romance 
novels is related to sexual behavior, or if mothers’ IQs are related to their daugh- 
ters’ IQs? 

Each of these questions revolves around the issue of the degree of relationship 
between two variables. One way of answering them is to draw a scatterplot, a means 
of graphically illustrating the relationship between two variables. We would first 
collect two sets of paired measures and assign one score to the horizontal axis (vari- 
able x) and the other score to the vertical axis (variable y). Then we would draw a 
dot at the place where the two scores meet on the graph. The first two scatterplots 
in Figure 1 present typical situations. In (a) and (b), there is a positive relationship 
in which high values of variable x are associated with high values of variable y and 
low values of x are associated with low values of y. In (c) and (d), there is a negative 
relationship: As values of variable x increase, the values of variable y decrease. In 
(e), no clear relationship exists between variable x and variable y. 

It is also possible to consider scores in terms of their mathematical relationship 
to one another rather than simply the way they appear on a scatterplot. Suppose, 
for example, a psychologist was interested in the degree to which a daughter’s IQ 
was related to her mother’s IQ—specifically, if a mother with a high IQ tended to 
have a daughter who also had a high IQ—and whether a mother with a low IQ 
tended to have a daughter with a low IQ. To examine the issue, suppose the psy- 
chologist measured the IQs of 10 mothers and daughters and arranged their IQs as 
presented in Figure 2 on page A-17. 

Looking at the data in the table, it is obvious that mothers and daughters do 
not have identical IQs. Moreover, they do not even have IQs that are rank-ordered 
the same in the two columns. For example, the mother with the highest IQ does not 
have the daughter with the highest IQ, and the mother with the lowest IQ does not 





significant outcome An outcome in 
which the observed outcome would be 
expected to have occurred by chance 
with a probability of .05 or less. 


positive relationship A relationship 
established by data that shows high 
values of one variable corresponding 
with high values of another, and low 
values of the first variable correspond- 
ing with low values of the other. 


negative relationship A relationship 
established by data that shows high 
values of one variable corresponding 
with low values of the other. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


A-16 Appendix Going by the Numbers: Statistics in Psychology 


(a) Strong positive relationship 
(correlation = +.90) 





c) Moderate negative relationshi 
8 P 
(correlation = —.50) 








correlation coefficient A numerical 
measure that indicates the extent of the 
relationship between two variables. 





(b) Moderate positive relationship 
(correlation = +.60) 





(d) Strong negative relationship (e) No relationship 
(correlation = —.90) (correlation = .00) 

















0 
0 l 2 3 4 5 6 0 l 2 3 4 5 6 


FIGURE 1 These scatterplots show relationships of different strengths. In (a) and (b), the 
relationships are positive, although in (a) the relationship is considerably stronger than in 
(b). In contrast, the relationships in (c) and (d) are negative, with (d) representing a stronger 
negative relationship. Finally, (e) illustrates a case where no systematic relationship exists 
between the variables. 


have the daughter with the lowest IQ. It is apparent, then, that there is not a perfect 
relationship between the IQ of a mother and the IQ of a daughter. However, it 
would be a mistake to conclude that there is a zero, or no, relationship between the 
IQs of the mothers and daughters because it is clear that there is a tendency for 
mothers who have high IQs to have daughters with high IQs, and that mothers with 
low IQs tend to have daughters with low IQs. 

The statistic that provides a precise mathematical index of the degree to which 
two variables are related is the correlation coefficient. A correlation coefficient is a 
numerical measure that indicates the extent of the relationship between two vari- 
ables. It ranges in value from +1.00 to —1.00. A value of +1.00 would indicate that 
two variables had a perfect positive relationship with one another, which means that 
the highest score on one variable would be associated with the highest score on the 
other variable, the second-highest score on the first variable would be associated with 
the second-highest score on the second variable, and so on. A value of —1.00 would 
indicate that there was a perfect negative relationship between the two variables; the 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Module 58 Using Statistics to Answer Questions: Inferential Statistics and Correlation A-17 





FIGURE 2 In this distribution, each mother’s IQ score is compared to her daughter's IQ score. 
Using a correlation coefficient, we can calculate the strength of the relationship between the 
set of pairs of scores. 


highest score on the first variable would be associated with the lowest score on the 
second variable, the second highest score would be associated with the second low- 
est score, and so forth. 

Correlation coefficients at or only slightly greater or slightly less than zero indi- 
cate that there is no relationship between the two variables. In such cases, there is 
no tendency for high values on one variable to be associated with either high or low 
values on the second variable. 

Correlation coefficients that range between zero and +/-—1.00 reflect varying 
degrees of relationship between the two variables. For instance, a value of +.20 or 
—.20 would indicate that there was a slight relationship between the two variables; 
a value of around +.50 or -.50 would indicate a moderate relationship; a value of 
+.80 or —.80 would indicate a relatively strong relationship. As an example, if we 
were to calculate the correlation of the two sets of variables in Figure 2 (most 
advanced calculators do the necessary calculations automatically), we would find a 
correlation that is quite strong: The coefficient is +.86. 

It is important to note that finding a strong correlation between two variables 
does not in any way indicate that changes in one variable cause changes in 
another—only that the variables are associated with one another. Although it may 
seem plausible to us that it is the mother’s intelligence that causes higher intel- 
ligence in a daughter, for example, it is just as possible that a daughter’s intelli- 
gence affects how the mother performs on an IQ test (Perhaps the daughter’s 
behavior affects the general home environment, influencing the mother’s perfor- 
mance on IQ tests.) It is even plausible that some unmeasured—and previously 
unconsidered—third variable is causing both mother’s and daughter’s IQs to 
increase or decrease simultaneously. In a clear example of this possibility, even if 
we found that ice cream sales and rates of violent crime are positively correlated 
with one another (as they happen to be), we would not presume that they are 
causally related. In this case, it is likely that both are influenced by a third factor— 
the weather. 

The crucial point is that even if we find a perfect correlation between two sets 
of variables, we will not be able to say that the two variables are linked causally— 
only that they are strongly related to one another. 


IQ Scores of Mothers and Daughters 


Mother’s IQ 
35 
28 
25 
20 
l4 
10 
02 
96 
90 
86 





Daughter’s IQ 
122 
130 
110 
132 
100 
116 
108 

89 
84 
oD) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


A-18 Appendix Going by the Numbers: Statistics in Psychology 


RECAP/EVALUATE/RETHINK 


RECAP 3: 


How do we generalize from data? 

e Inferential statistics—techniques that use data from sam- 
ples to make predictions about a larger population—are 
useful in deciding whether differences between distribu- 
tions of data are attributable to real differences or to 
chance variation. (p. A-14) 


How can we determine the nature of a relationship, and the 

significance of differences, between two sets of scores? 

e Measures of relationship provide a numerical index of 
the extent to which two variables are related. The corre- 
lation coefficient ranges in value from +1.00 to —1.00, 
with +1.00 indicating a perfect positive relationship 
and —1.00 indicating a perfect negative relationship. 
Correlations close to or at zero indicate there is little or 
no relationship between two variables. (p. A-16) 


A hypothetical set of data drawn from a sample of college 

sophomores at a university found that as the rate of caf- 

feine consumption increases, the amount of sleep de- 

creases. The scatterplot for these data is apt to show a 
relationship. 


. What would the value of the correlation coefficient be for 


the following? 

a. A perfect negative relationship 
b. A perfect positive relationship 
c. No relationship 


. If we observed a correlation coefficient of —.90 in Question 


3, we would probably be safe in saying that caffeine 
consumption causes lack of sleep in college students. True 
or false? 


. The researchers in Question 3 decide to extend the find- 


ings they obtain from their sample of college sophomores 
to all adults. Would you accept their generalization? Why 
or why not? 


EVALUATE 
RETHINK 
1. Researchers would like to estimate the level of stress for 
first-year college students for a given year at a large uni- 1. For many years, cigarette manufacturers argued that be- 


versity. A stress index is given to a randomly assigned 
group of 500 first-year students. The class size is 6,000 for 
that year. In this example the group of 500 is known as a 

, and the entire class of first-year students is 
known as the : 

. Dr. Sanders states that the results of his experiment show 
a difference between the two groups, and that there is a 
90% probability that the results are due to a true difference 
between the groups and not due to chance. Are his results 
statistically significant in the way the term significant typi- 
cally is used by psychologists? 


cause the data linking smoking and disease was correla- 
tional, one could not infer that there was a causal 
connection between them and therefore no reason not to 
smoke. Did the manufacturers have a valid argument? 
How could you refute their argument? 


Answers to Evaluate Questions 


(synpe pje) uoyefndod ay} yuasardar you səop (seroutoydos a8aTjoo 
woy UMeIp) əjdwes əy} ‘ou *g ‘UOTeTDOSse ue ATUO ‘uone esnea 
g oumnsse youd am 'ƏS[LF 'G {09 '00T+ ‘4 ‘00 L— LP aU g 
UeITFIUSIS ST }[NSAI V WIEP 0} YSIM OYM s}S13ojoy2Ásd Aq parmbar 

Aqqestd Ay [aAeq go’ ay} W yOu Jsvaq ye ‘ou z ‘uonemdod ‘otdutes ‘Tf 


KEY TERMS 


population p. A-14 
sample p. A-14 


inferential statistics p. A-14 
significant outcome p. A-15 


positive relationship p. A-15 
negative relationship p. A-15 


correlation coefficient p. A-16 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 





Psychology on the Web 


1. Search the web for the results of a recent survey, such as one conducted by the 
Gallup polling organization (www.gallup.com). Describe the results by summarizing 
the methodology and the sample size for the survey. How does the sample address 
the population to which the survey is being generalized? 


E © | | OQ U e In this group of modules, we've seen how psychologists 


use statistical methods to summarize data and support 
hypotheses. Return, for a moment, to the prologue about Selma Vorwerk, who immi- 
grated to this country in the early 1900s; use the statistical concepts that we covered to 
answer the following questions. 





1. What was the sample size anti-immigrationist Lathrop Stoddard used and to what 
population was he generalizing? 

2. What are the statistical flaws in the arguments critics of immigration made in the 
early 1900s? 

3. What statistical methods would you find useful in refuting Stoddard’s conclusions 
about immigrants? 


Join Us on Fb WhatsApp & Telegram 


Chor Only Booka Votes tu pdf 
Fo.com/groups/458 1844109658 70 


TELEGRAM.ME/CSSASPIRANTSFORUM 


WIAA SMP? NONDE 
PO2=30- HB -6—C3 n 


SEPARATE GROUP FOR FEMALES ASPIRANTS 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


This page intentionally left blank 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Glossary 


abnormal behavior Behavior that causes people 
to experience distress and prevents them from 
functioning in their daily lives (Module 46). 


absolute threshold The smallest intensity of a 
stimulus that must be present for the stimulus 
to be detected (Module 10). 


acquired immune deficiency syndrome 
(AIDS) A sexually transmitted infection 
caused by a virus that destroys the body’s 
immune system (Module 34). 


action potential An electric nerve impulse that 
travels through a neuron’s axon when it is set 
off by a “trigger,” changing the neuron’s charge 
from negative to positive (Module 7). 


activation-synthesis theory Hobson’s theory 
that the brain produces random electrical 
energy during REM sleep that stimulates mem- 
ories stored in the brain (Module 14). 


activity theory of aging A theory that sug- 
gests that the elderly who are most successful 
while aging are those who maintain the inter- 
ests and activities they had during middle age 
(Module 39). 


adaptation An adjustment in sensory capacity 
after prolonged exposure to unchanging stimuli 
(Module 10). 


addictive drugs Drugs that produce a biologi- 
cal or psychological dependence in the user so 
that withdrawal from them leads to a craving 
for the drug that, in some cases, may be nearly 
irresistible (Module 16). 


adolescence The developmental stage between 
childhood and adulthood (Module 38). 


age of viability The point at which a fetus can 
survive if born prematurely (Module 36). 


aggression The intentional injury of, or harm 
to, another person (Module 55). 


algorithm A rule that, if applied appropriately, 
guarantees a solution to a problem (Module 23). 


all-or-none law The rule that neurons are 
either on or off (Module 7). 


altruism Helping behavior that is beneficial to 
others but clearly requires self-sacrifice 
(Module 55). 


Alzheimer’s disease A progressive brain dis- 
order that leads to a gradual and irreversible 
decline in cognitive abilities (Modules 22, 39). 


amnesia Memory loss that occurs without 
other mental difficulties (Module 22). 


anal stage According to Freud, a stage from 
age 12 to 18 months to 3 years of age, in which a 
child’s pleasure is centered on the anus 
(Module 40). 


androgens Male sex hormones secreted by the 
testes (Module 33). 


anorexia nervosa A severe eating disorder in 
which people may refuse to eat while denying 
that their behavior and appearance—which can 
become skeleton-like—are unusual (Module 30). 


anorgasmia (an-or-GAZ-mee-uh) A female’s 
lack of orgasm (Module 34). 


anterograde amnesia Amnesia in which mem- 
ory is lost for events that follow an injury 
(Module 22). 


antianxiety drugs Drugs that reduce the level 
of anxiety a person experiences essentially by 
reducing excitability and increasing feelings of 
well-being (Module 51). 


antidepressant drugs Medications that 
improve a severely depressed patient’s mood 
and feeling of well-being (Module 51). 


antipsychotic drugs Drugs that temporarily 
reduce psychotic symptoms such as agitation, 
hallucinations, and delusions (Module 51). 


antisocial personality disorder A disorder in 
which individuals show no regard for the moral 
and ethical rules of society or the rights of oth- 
ers (Module 47). 


anxiety disorder The occurrence of anxiety 
without an obvious external cause that affects 
daily functioning (Module 47). 


archetypes According to Jung, universal 
symbolic representations of a particular person, 
object, or experience (such as good and evil) 
(Module 40). 


archival research Research in which existing 
data, such as census documents, college records, 
and newspaper clippings, are examined to test a 
hypothesis (Module 5). 


arousal approaches to motivation The belief 
that we try to maintain certain levels of stimula- 
tion and activity increasing or reducing them as 
necessary (Module 29). 


association areas One of the major regions of 
the cerebral cortex; the site of the higher mental 
processes, such as thought, language, memory, 
and speech (Module 9). 


assumed-similarity bias The tendency to 
think of people as being similar to oneself 
even when meeting them for the first time 
(Module 52). 


attachment The positive emotional bond that 
develops between a child and a particular indi- 
vidual (Module 37). 


attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder 
(ADHD) A disorder marked by inattention, 


impulsiveness, a low tolerance for frustration, 
and a great deal of inappropriate activity 
(Module 47). 


attitudes Evaluations of a particular person, 
behavior, belief, or concept (Module 52). 


attribution theory The theory of personality 
that seeks to explain how we decide, on the 
basis of samples of an individual’s behavior, 
what the specific causes of that person’s 
behavior are (Module 52). 


authoritarian parents Parents who are rigid 
and punitive and value unquestioning obedi- 
ence from their children (Module 37). 


authoritative parents Parents who are firm, set 
clear limits, reason with their children, and 
explain things to them (Module 37). 


autism A severe developmental disability that 
impairs children’s ability to communicate and 
relate to others (Module 47). 


autobiographical memories Our recollections 
of circumstances and episodes from our own 
lives (Module 21). 


autonomic division The part of the peripheral 
nervous system that controls involuntary move- 
ment of the heart, glands, lungs, and other 
organs (Module 8). 


autonomy-versus-shame-and-doubt stage The 
period during which, according to Erikson, tod- 
dlers (ages 1% to 3 years) develop independence 
and autonomy if exploration and freedom are 
encouraged or shame and self-doubt if they are 
restricted and overprotected (Module 37). 


aversive conditioning A form of therapy that 
reduces the frequency of undesired behavior by 
pairing an aversive, unpleasant stimulus with 
undesired behavior (Module 49). 


axon The part of the neuron that carries mes- 
sages destined for other neurons (Module 7). 


babble Meaningless speechlike sounds made 
by children from around the age of 3 months 
through 1 year (Module 25). 


background stressors (“daily hassles”) 
Everyday annoyances, such as being stuck in 
traffic, that cause minor irritations and may have 
long-term ill effects if they continue or are com- 
pounded by other stressful events (Module 43). 


basilar membrane A vibrating structure that 
runs through the center of the cochlea, dividing 
it into an upper chamber and a lower chamber 
and containing sense receptors for sound 
(Module 12). 


behavior modification A formalized technique 
for promoting the frequency of desirable 


G-1 


G-2 Glossary 


behaviors and decreasing the incidence of 
unwanted ones (Module 18). 


behavioral assessment Direct measures of an 
individual’s behavior used to describe person- 
ality characteristics (Module 42). 


behavioral genetics The study of the effects of 
heredity on behavior (Module 8). 


behavioral neuroscientists (or biopsycholo- 
gists) Psychologists who specialize in consider- 
ing the ways in which the biological structures 
and functions of the body affect behavior 
(Module 7). 


behavioral perspective The approach that sug- 
gests that observable, measurable behavior 
should be the focus of study (Modules 2, 46). 


behavioral treatment approaches Treatment 
approaches that build on the basic processes of 
learning, such as reinforcement and extinction, 
and assume that normal and abnormal behavior 
are both learned (Module 49). 


biofeedback A procedure in which a person 
learns to control through conscious thought 
internal physiological processes such as blood 
pressure, heart and respiration rate, skin tem- 
perature, sweating, and the constriction of 
particular muscles (Module 9). 


biological and evolutionary approaches to per- 
sonality Theories that suggest that important 
components of personality are inherited 
(Module 41). 


biomedical therapy Therapy that relies on 
drugs and other medical procedures to improve 
psychological functioning (Module 49). 


bipolar disorder A disorder in which a person 
alternates between periods of euphoric feelings 
of mania and periods of depression (Module 47). 


bisexuals Persons who are sexually attracted 
to people of the same sex and the other sex 
(Module 34). 


borderline personality disorder A disorder in 
which individuals have difficulty developing a 
secure sense of who they are (Module 47). 


bottom-up processing Perception that consists 
of the progression of recognizing and process- 
ing information from individual components of 
a stimuli and moving to the perception of the 
whole (Module 13). 


bulimia A disorder in which a person binges 
on large quantities of food, followed by efforts 
to purge the food through vomiting or other 
means (Module 30). 


Cannon-Bard theory of emotion The belief 
that both physiological arousal and emotional 
experience are produced simultaneously by the 
same nerve stimulus (Module 31). 


case study An in-depth, intensive investigation 
of an individual or small group of people 
(Module 5). 


cataclysmic events Strong stressors that occur 
suddenly and typically affect many people at 
once (e.g., natural disasters) (Module 43). 


catharsis The process of discharging built-up 
aggressive energy (Module 55). 


central core The “old brain,” which controls 
basic functions such as eating and sleeping and 
is common to all vertebrates (Module 9). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


central nervous system (CNS) The part of the 
nervous system that includes the brain and spi- 
nal cord (Module 8). 


central route processing Message interpreta- 
tion characterized by thoughtful consideration 
of the issues and arguments used to persuade 
(Module 52). 


central tendency An index of the central loca- 
tion within a distribution of scores; the most 
representative score in a distribution of scores 
(the mean, median, and mode are measures of 
central tendency) (Module 56). 


central traits The major traits considered in 
forming impressions of others (Module 52). 


cerebellum (ser-uh-BELL-um) The part of the 
brain that controls bodily balance (Module 9). 


cerebral cortex The “new brain,” responsible 
for the most sophisticated information pro- 
cessing in the brain; contains four lobes 
(Module 9). 


chromosomes Rod-shaped structures that 
contain all basic hereditary information 
(Module 36). 


chunk A meaningful grouping of stimuli that 
can be stored as a unit in short-term memory 
(Module 20). 


circadian rhythms Biological processes that 
occur regularly on approximately a 24-hour 
cycle (Module 14). 


classical conditioning A type of learning in 
which a neutral stimulus comes to bring about a 
response after it is paired with a stimulus that 
naturally brings about that response (Module 17). 


cochlea (KOKE-lee-uh) A coiled tube in the ear 
filled with fluid that vibrates in response to 
sound (Module 12). 


cognitive approaches to motivation Theories 
suggesting that motivation is a product of peo- 
ple’s thoughts, expectations, and goals—their 
cognitions (Module 29). 


cognitive-behavioral approach A treatment 
approach that incorporates basic principles of 
learning to change the way people think 
(Module 49). 


cognitive development The process by which 
a child’s understanding of the world changes as 
a function of age and experience (Module 37). 


cognitive dissonance The conflict that occurs 
when a person holds two contradictory atti- 
tudes or thoughts (referred to as cognitions) 
(Module 52). 


cognitive learning theory An approach to the 
study of learning that focuses on the thought 
processes that underlie learning (Module 19). 


cognitive perspective The approach that 
focuses on how people think, understand, and 
know about the world (Module 2). 


cognitive perspective on psychological dis- 
orders The perspective that suggests that 
people’s thoughts and beliefs are a central 
component of abnormal behavior (Module 46). 


cognitive psychology The branch of psychol- 
ogy that focuses on the study of higher mental 
processes, including thinking, language, mem- 
ory, problem solving, knowing, reasoning, judg- 
ing, and decision making (Module 23). 


cognitive treatment approaches Treatment 
approaches that teach people to think in more 
adaptive ways by changing their dysfunctional 
cognitions about the world and themselves 
(Module 49). 


collective unconscious According to Jung, a 
common set of ideas, feelings, images, and sym- 
bols that we inherit from our ancestors, the 
whole human race, and even animal ancestors 
from the distant past (Module 40). 


community psychology A branch of psychol- 
ogy that focuses on the prevention and mini- 
mization of psychological disorders in the 
community (Module 51). 


companionate love The strong affection we 
have for those with whom our lives are deeply 
involved (Module 55). 


compliance Behavior that occurs in response to 
direct social pressure (Module 53). 


compulsion An irresistible urge to repeatedly 
carry out some act that seems strange and 
unreasonable (Module 47). 


concepts A mental grouping of similar objects, 
events, or people (Module 23). 


concrete operational stage According to 
Piaget, the period from 7 to 12 years of age that 
is characterized by logical thought and a loss of 
egocentrism (Module 37). 


conditioned response (CR) A response that, 
after conditioning, follows a previously neutral 
stimulus (e.g., salivation at the ringing of a bell) 
(Module 17). 


conditioned stimulus (CS) A once-neutral 
stimulus that has been paired with an uncondi- 
tioned stimulus to bring about a response for- 
merly caused only by the unconditioned 
stimulus (Module 17). 


cones Cone-shaped, light-sensitive receptor 
cells in the retina that are responsible for sharp 
focus and color perception, particularly in 
bright light (Module 11). 


confirmation bias The tendency to seek out 
and weight more heavily information that 
supports one’s initial hypotheses and to ignore 
contradictory information that supports alter- 
native hypotheses or solutions (Module 24). 


conformity A change in behavior or attitudes 
brought about by a desire to follow the beliefs 
or standards of other people (Module 53). 


consciousness The awareness of the sensa- 
tions, thoughts, and feelings being experienced 
at a given moment (Module 14). 


constructive processes Processes in which 
memories are influenced by the meaning we 
give to events (Module 21). 


continuous reinforcement schedule 
Reinforcing of a behavior every time it occurs 
(Module 18). 


control group A group participating in an 
experiment that receives no treatment 
(Module 5). 


convergent thinking The ability to produce 
responses that are based primarily on knowl- 
edge and logic (Module 24). 


conversion disorder A major somatoform disor- 
der that involves an actual physical disturbance, 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


such as the inability to use a sensory organ or the 
complete or partial inability to move an arm or 
leg (Module 47). 


coping The efforts to control, reduce, or learn 
to tolerate the threats that lead to stress 
(Module 43). 


correlation coefficient A numerical measure 
that indicates the extent of the relationship 
between two variables (Module 58). 


correlational research Research in which the 
relationship between two sets of variables is 
examined to determine whether they are associ- 
ated, or “correlated” (Module 5). 


creativity The ability to generate original ideas 
or solve problems in novel ways (Module 24). 


cross-sectional research A research method 
that compares people of different ages at the 
same point in time (Module 35). 


crystallized intelligence The accumulation 
of information, skills, and strategies that are 
learned through experience and can be 
applied in problem-solving situations 
(Module 26). 


cue-dependent forgetting Forgetting that occurs 
when there are insufficient retrieval cues to rekin- 
dle information that is in memory (Module 22). 


culture-fair IQ test A test that does not dis- 
criminate against the members of any minority 
group (Module 28). 


date rape Rape in which the rapist is either a 
date or a romantic acquaintance (Module 34). 


daydreams Fantasies that people construct 
while awake (Module 14). 


decay The loss of information in memory 
through its nonuse (Module 22). 


declarative memory Memory for factual infor- 
mation: names, faces, dates, and the like 
(Module 20). 


defense mechanisms In Freudian theory, 
unconscious strategies that people use to 
reduce anxiety by concealing the source of it 
from themselves and others (Module 40). 


deinstitutionalization The transfer of former 
mental patients from institutions to the commu- 
nity (Module 51). 


dendrite A cluster of fibers at one end of a neu- 
ron that receives messages from other neurons 
(Module 7). 


dependent variable The variable that is mea- 
sured and is expected to change as a result of 
changes caused by the experimenter’s manipu- 
lation of the independent variable (Module 5). 


depressants Drugs that slow down the nervous 
system (Module 16). 


depth perception The ability to view the world 
in three dimensions and to perceive distance 
(Module 13). 


descriptive statistics The branch of statistics 
that provides a means of summarizing data 
(Module 56). 


determinism The idea that people’s behavior is 
produced primarily by factors outside of their 
willful control (Module 3). 


developmental psychology The branch of 
psychology that studies the patterns of 


growth and change that occur throughout life 
(Module 35). 


Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental 
Disorders, Fourth Edition, Text Revision 
(DSM-IV-TR) A system, devised by the American 
Psychiatric Association, used by most profession- 
als to diagnose and classify abnormal behavior 
(Module 46). 


dialectical behavior therapy A form of treat- 
ment in which the focus is on getting people to 
accept who they are regardless of whether it 
matches their ideal (Module 49). 


difference threshold (just noticeable difference) 
The smallest level of added or reduced stimula- 
tion required to sense that a change in stimula- 
tion has occurred (Module 10). 


diffusion of responsibility The tendency for 
people to feel that responsibility for acting is 
shared, or diffused, among those present 
(Module 55). 


discrimination Behavior directed toward indi- 
viduals on the basis of their membership in a 
particular group (Module 54). 


disengagement theory of aging A theory that 
suggests that aging produces a gradual 
withdrawal from the world on physical, 
psychological, and social levels (Module 39). 


dispositional causes (of behavior) Perceived 
causes of behavior that are based on internal 
traits or personality factors (Module 52). 


dissociative amnesia A disorder in whicha 
significant, selective memory loss occurs 
(Module 47). 


dissociative disorders Psychological dysfunc- 
tions characterized by the separation of differ- 
ent facets of a person’s personality that are 
normally integrated (Module 47). 


dissociative fugue A form of amnesia in which 
the individual leaves home and sometimes 
assumes a new identity (Module 47). 


dissociative identity disorder (DID) A dis- 
order in which a person displays characteris- 
tics of two or more distinct personalities 
(Module 47). 


divergent thinking The ability to generate 
unusual, yet nonetheless appropriate, responses 
to problems or questions (Module 24). 


double standard The view that premarital sex 
is permissible for males but not for females 
(Module 34). 


dreams-for-survival theory The theory sug- 
gesting that dreams permit information that is 
critical for our daily survival to be reconsidered 
and reprocessed during sleep (Module 14). 


drive Motivational tension, or arousal, 
that energizes behavior to fulfill a need 
(Module 29). 


drive-reduction approaches to motiva- 

tion Theories suggesting that a lack of a basic 
biological requirement such as water produces 
a drive to obtain that requirement (in this case, 
the thirst drive) (Module 29). 


drug therapy Control of psychological disor- 
ders through the use of drugs (Module 51). 


eardrum The part of the ear that vibrates when 
sound waves hit it (Module 12). 


Glossary G-3 


ego The part of the personality that provides a 
buffer between the id and the outside world 
(Module 40). 


egocentric thought A way of thinking in which 
a child views the world entirely from his or her 
own perspective (Module 37). 


ego-integrity-versus-despair stage According 
to Erikson, a period from late adulthood until 
death during which we review life’s accom- 
plishments and failures (Module 38). 


electroconvulsive therapy (ECT) A procedure 
used in the treatment of severe depression in 
which an electric current of 70-150 volts is briefly 
administered to a patient’s head (Module 51). 


embryo A developed zygote that has a heart, a 
brain, and other organs (Module 36). 


emerging adulthood The period beginning in 
the late teenage years and extending into the 
mid-20 (Module 39). 


emotional intelligence The set of skills that 
underlie the accurate assessment, evaluation, 
expression, and regulation of emotions 
(Module 26). 


emotions Feelings that generally have both 
physiological and cognitive elements and that 
influence behavior (Module 31). 


endocrine system A chemical communication 
network that sends messages throughout the 
body via the bloodstream (Module 8). 


episodic memory Memory for events that 
occur in a particular time, place, or context 
(Module 20). 


erectile dysfunction A male’s inability to 
achieve or maintain an erection (Module 34). 


erogenous zones Areas of the body that are 
particularly sensitive because of the presence of 
an unusually rich array of nerve receptors 
(Module 33). 


estrogens Class of female sex hormones 
(Module 33). 


evolutionary psychology The branch of psy- 
chology that seeks to identify behavior patterns 
that are a result of our genetic inheritance from 
our ancestors (Module 8). 


excitatory message A chemical message that 
makes it more likely that a receiving neuron 
will fire and an action potential will travel 
down its axon (Module 7). 


excitement phase The period in which an 
arousing stimulus begins a sequence that 
prepares the genitals for sexual intercourse 
(Module 33). 


experiment The investigation of the relationship 
between two (or more) variables by deliberately 
producing a change in one variable in a situation 
and observing the effects of that change on other 
aspects of the situation (Module 5). 


experimental bias Factors that distort how the 
independent variable affects the dependent 
variable in an experiment (Module 6). 


experimental group Any group participating 
in an experiment that receives a treatment 
(Module 5). 


experimental manipulation The change that 
an experimenter deliberately produces in a situ- 
ation (Module 5). 


G-4 Glossary 


explicit memory Intentional or conscious recol- 
lection of information (Module 21). 


exposure A behavioral treatment for anxiety 
in which people are confronted either sud- 
denly or gradually with a stimulus that they 
fear (Module 49). 


extinction A basic phenomenon of learning 
that occurs when a previously conditioned 
response decreases in frequency and eventually 
disappears (Module 17). 


extramarital sex Sexual activity between a mar- 
ried person and someone who is not his or her 
spouse (Module 34). 


facial-affect program Activation of a set of 
nerve impulses that make the face display the 
appropriate expression (Module 31). 


facial-feedback hypothesis The hypothesis 
that facial expressions not only reflect 
emotional experience but also help determine 
how people experience and label emotions 
(Module 31). 


familial retardation Mental retardation in 
which no apparent biological defect exists but 
there is a history of retardation in the family 
(Module 27). 


family therapy An approach that focuses on 
the family and its dynamics (Module 50). 


feature detection The activation of neurons in 
the cortex by visual stimuli of specific shapes or 
patterns (Module 11). 


fetal alcohol syndrome The most common 
cause of mental retardation in newborns, occur- 
ring when the mother uses alcohol during 
pregnancy (Module 27). 


fetus A developing individual from eight 
weeks after conception until birth (Module 36). 


fixations Conflicts or concerns that persist 
beyond the developmental period in which 
they first occur (Module 40). 


fixed-interval schedule A schedule that 
provides reinforcement for a response only if a 
fixed time period has elapsed, making overall 
rates of response relatively low (Module 18). 


fixed-ratio schedule A schedule by which rein- 
forcement is given only after a specific number 
of responses are made (Module 18). 


flashbulb memories Memories centered on a 
specific, important, or surprising event that are 
so vivid it is as if they represented a snapshot of 
the event (Module 21). 


fluid intelligence Intelligence that reflects 
information-processing capabilities, reasoning, 
and memory (Module 26). 


formal operational stage According to Piaget, 
the period from age 12 to adulthood that is char- 
acterized by abstract thought (Module 37). 


free will The idea that behavior is caused 
primarily by choices that are made freely by the 
individual (Module 3). 


frequency distribution An arrangement of 
scores from a sample that indicates how often a 
particular score is present (Module 56). 


frequency theory of hearing The theory that 
the entire basilar membrane acts like a micro- 
phone, vibrating as a whole in response to a 
sound (Module 12). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


functional fixedness The tendency to think 
of an object only in terms of its typical use 
(Module 24). 


functionalism An early approach to psychol- 
ogy that concentrated on what the mind does— 
the functions of mental activity—and the role of 
behavior in allowing people to adapt to their 
environments (Module 2). 


fundamental attribution error A tendency to 
overattribute others’ behavior to dispositional 
causes and minimize of the importance of situa- 
tional causes (Module 52). 


g or g-factor The single, general factor for 
mental ability assumed to underlie intelli- 

gence in some early theories of intelligence 
(Module 26). 


gate-control theory of pain The theory that 
particular nerve receptors in the spinal cord 
lead to specific areas of the brain related to pain 
(Module 12). 


gender The perception of being male or female 
(Module 32). 


gender roles The set of expectations, defined 
by a particular society, that indicate what is 
appropriate behavior for men and women 
(Module 32). 


gender schema A mental framework that orga- 
nizes and guides a child’s understanding of 
information relevant to gender (Module 32). 


general adaptation syndrome (GAS) A theory 
developed by Selye that suggests that a per- 
son’s response to a stressor consists of three 
stages: alarm and mobilization, resistance, and 
exhaustion (Module 43). 


generalized anxiety disorder The experience 
of long-term, persistent anxiety and worry 
(Module 47). 


generativity-versus-stagnation stage 
According to Erikson, a period in middle 
adulthood during which we take stock of our 
contributions to family and society (Module 38). 


genes The parts of the chromosomes through 
which genetic information is transmitted 
(Module 36). 


genetic preprogramming theories of aging 
Theories that suggest that human cells have a 
built-in time limit to their reproduction and that 
they are no longer able to divide after a certain 
time (Module 39). 


genital stage According to Freud, the period 
from puberty until death, marked by mature 
sexual behavior (that is, sexual intercourse) 
(Module 40). 


genitals The male and female sex organs 
(Module 33). 


gestalt laws of organization A series of princi- 
ples that describe how we organize bits and 
pieces of information into meaningful wholes 
(Module 13). 


gestalt psychology An approach to psychol- 
ogy that focuses on the organization of percep- 
tion and thinking in a “whole” sense rather 
than on the individual elements of perception 
(Module 2). 


grammar The system of rules that determine 
how our thoughts can be expressed (Module 25). 


group Two or more people who interact with 
one another, perceive themselves as part of a 
group, and are interdependent (Module 53). 


group therapy Therapy in which people meet 
in a group with a therapist to discuss problems 
(Module 50). 


groupthink A type of thinking in which group 
members share such a strong motivation to 
achieve consensus that they lose the ability to 
critically evaluate alternative points of view 
(Module 53). 


habituation The decrease in the response to a 
stimulus that occurs after repeated presenta- 
tions of the same stimulus (Module 37). 


hair cells Tiny cells covering the basilar 
membrane that, when bent by vibrations 
entering the cochlea, transmit neural messages 
to the brain (Module 12). 


hallucinogen A drug that is capable of 
producing hallucinations, or changes in the 
perceptual process (Module 16). 


halo effect A phenomenon in which an initial 
understanding that a person has positive traits 
is used to infer other uniformly positive charac- 
teristics (Module 52). 


hardiness A personality characteristic that 

is associated with a lower rate of stress-related 
illness and consists of three components: 
commitment, challenge, and control 

(Module 43). 


health psychology The branch of psychology 
that investigates the psychological factors 
related to wellness and illness, including the 
prevention, diagnosis, and treatment of medical 
problems (Module 43). 


hemispheres Symmetrical left and right 
halves of the brain that control the side of 
the body opposite to their location (Module 9). 


heritability A measure of the degree to which a 
characteristic is related to genetic, inherited fac- 
tors (Module 28). 


heterosexuality Sexual attraction and behavior 
directed to the other sex (Module 34). 


heuristic A thinking strategy that may lead us 
to a solution to a problem or decision, but— 
unlike algorithms—may sometimes lead to 
errors (Module 23). 


histogram Bar graph (Module 56). 


homeostasis The body’s tendency to maintain 
a steady internal state (Module 29). 


homosexuals Persons who are sexually 
attracted to members of their own sex 
(Module 34). 


hormones Chemicals that circulate through the 
blood and regulate the functioning or growth of 
the body (Module 8). 


humanistic approaches to personality 
Theories that emphasize people’s innate 
goodness and desire to achieve higher levels 
of functioning (Module 41). 


humanistic perspective The approach that 
suggests that all individuals naturally strive to 
grow, develop, and be in control of their lives 
and behavior (Module 2). 


humanistic perspective on psychological dis- 
orders The perspective that emphasizes the 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


responsibility people have for their own behav- 
ior, even when such behavior is abnormal 
(Module 46). 


humanistic therapy Therapy in which the 
underlying rationale is that people have control 
of their behavior, can make choices about their 
lives, and are essentially responsible for solving 
their own problems (Module 50). 


hypnosis A trancelike state of heightened 
susceptibility to the suggestions of others 
(Module 15). 


hypochondriasis A disorder in which people 
have a constant fear of illness and a preoccupa- 
tion with their health (Module 47). 


hypothalamus A tiny part of the brain, located 
below the thalamus, that maintains homeostasis 
and produces and regulates vital behavior, such 
as eating, drinking, and sexual behavior 
(Module 9). 


hypothesis A prediction, stemming from a 
theory, stated in a way that allows it to be 
tested (Module 4). 


id The raw, unorganized, inborn part of person- 
ality whose sole purpose is to reduce tension 
created by primitive drives related to hunger, 
sex, aggression, and irrational impulses 
(Module 40). 


identical twins Twins who are genetically 
identical (Module 35). 


identification The process of wanting to be like 
another person as much as possible, imitating 
that person’s behavior and adopting similar 
beliefs and values (Module 40). 


identity The distinguishing character of the 
individual: who each of us is, what our roles are, 
and what we are capable of (Module 38). 


identity-versus-role-confusion stage Accord- 
ing to Erikson, a time in adolescence of major 
testing to determine one’s unique qualities 
(Module 38). 


implicit memory Memories of which people 
are not consciously aware but that can affect 
subsequent performance and behavior 
(Module 21). 


incentive approaches to motivation Theories 
suggesting that motivation stems from the 
desire to obtain valued external goals, or incen- 
tives (Module 29). 


independent variable The variable that is 
manipulated by an experimenter (Module 5). 


industrial-organizational (I/O) psychology 
The branch of psychology focusing on work- 
and job-related issues, including worker moti- 
vation, satisfaction, safety, and productivity 
(Module 53). 


industry-versus-inferiority stage According to 
Erikson, the last stage of childhood, during 
which children age 6 to 12 years may develop 
positive social interactions with others or may 
feel inadequate and become less sociable 
(Module 37). 


inferential statistics The branch of statistics 
that uses data from samples to make predic- 

tions about the larger population from which 
the sample is drawn (Module 58). 


inferiority complex According to Adler, a 
problem affecting adults who have not been 


able to overcome the feelings of inferiority that 
they developed as children, when they were 
small and limited in their knowledge about the 
world (Module 40). 


information processing The way in which 
people take in, use, and store information 
(Module 37). 


informed consent A document signed by 
participants affirming that they have been told 
the basic outlines of the study and are aware 
of what their participation will involve 
(Module 6). 


inhibited ejaculation A male’s inability to 
ejaculate when he wants to, if at all (Module 34). 


inhibited sexual desire A sexual dysfunction 
in which the motivation for sexual activity is 
restrained or lacking entirely (Module 34). 


inhibitory message A chemical message that 
prevents or decreases the likelihood that a 
receiving neuron will fire (Module 7). 


initiative-versus-guilt stage According to 
Erikson, the period during which children ages 
3 to 6 years experience conflict between inde- 
pendence of action and the sometimes negative 
results of that action (Module 37). 


insight A sudden awareness of the relation- 
ships among various elements that had previ- 
ously appeared to be independent of one 
another (Module 24). 


instincts Inborn patterns of behavior that are 
biologically determined rather than learned 
(Module 29). 


intellectually gifted The 2%—4% of the popula- 
tion who have IQ scores greater than 130 
(Module 27). 


intelligence The capacity to understand the 
world, think rationally, and use resources 
effectively when faced with challenges 
(Module 26). 


intelligence quotient (IQ) A score that takes 
into account an individual’s mental and chron- 
ological ages (Module 26). 


intelligence tests Tests devised to quantify a 
person’s level of intelligence (Module 26). 


interactionist approach (to language develop- 
ment) The view that language development is 
produced through a combination of genetically 
determined predispositions and environmental 
circumstances that help teach language 
(Module 25). 


interference The phenomenon by which infor- 
mation in memory disrupts the recall of other 
information (Module 22). 


interneurons Neurons that connect sensory 
and motor neurons, carrying messages between 
the two (Module 8). 


interpersonal attraction (or close relation- 
ship) Positive feelings for others; liking and 
loving (Module 55). 


interpersonal therapy (IPT) Short-term ther- 
apy that focuses on the context of current social 
relationships (Module 50). 


intimacy-versus-isolation stage According to 
Erikson, a period during early adulthood that 
focuses on developing close relationships 
(Module 38). 


Glossary G-5 


introspection A procedure used to study the 
structure of the mind in which subjects are 
asked to describe in detail what they are experi- 
encing when they are exposed to a stimulus 
(Module 2). 


James-Lange theory of emotion The belief 
that emotional experience is a reaction to 
bodily events occurring as a result of an exter- 
nal situation (“I feel sad because I am crying”) 
(Module 31). 


Korsakoff’s syndrome A disease that afflicts 
long-term alcoholics, leaving some abilities 
intact but including hallucinations and a ten- 
dency to repeat the same story (Module 22). 


language The communication of information 
through symbols arranged according to system- 
atic rules (Module 25). 


language-acquisition device A neural system 
of the brain hypothesized by Noam Chomsky 
to permit understanding of language 

(Module 25). 


latency period According to Freud, the period 
between the phallic stage and puberty during 
which children’s sexual concerns are temporar- 
ily put aside (Module 40). 


latent content of dreams According to Freud, 
the “disguised” meanings of dreams, hidden by 
more obvious subjects (Module 14). 


latent learning Learning in which a new 
behavior is acquired but is not demonstrated 
until some incentive is provided for displaying 
it (Module 19). 


lateralization The dominance of one hemi- 
sphere of the brain in specific functions, such as 
language (Module 9). 


learned helplessness A state in which people 
conclude that unpleasant or aversive stimuli 
cannot be controlled— a view of the world that 
becomes so ingrained that they cease trying to 
remedy the aversive circumstances even if they 
actually can exert some influence on the situa- 
tion (Module 43). 


learning A relatively permanent change in behav- 
ior brought about by experience (Module 17). 


learning-theory approach (to language devel- 
opment) The theory that language acquisition 
follows the principles of reinforcement and con- 
ditioning (Module 25). 


levels-of-processing theory The theory of 
memory that emphasizes the degree to 
which new material is mentally analyzed 
(Module 21). 


life review The process by which people exam- 
ine and evaluate their lives (Module 39). 


limbic system The part of the brain that con- 
trols eating, aggression, and reproduction 
(Module 9). 


linguistic-relativity hypothesis The notion 
that language shapes and may determine the 
way people in a particular culture perceive and 
understand the world (Module 25). 


lobes The four major sections of the cerebral 
cortex: frontal, parietal, temporal, and occipital 
(Module 9). 


longitudinal research A research method that 
investigates behavior as participants age 
(Module 35). 


G-6 Glossary 


long-term memory Memory that stores infor- 
mation on a relatively permanent basis, 
although it may be difficult to retrieve 
(Module 20). 


major depression A severe form of depression 
that interferes with concentration, decision 
making, and sociability (Module 47). 


mania An extended state of intense, wild 
elation (Module 47). 


manifest content of dreams According to 
Freud, the apparent story line of dreams 
(Module 14). 


masturbation Sexual self-stimulation 
(Module 34). 


mean The average of all scores, arrived at by 
adding scores together and dividing by the 
number of scores (Module 56). 


means-ends analysis Involves repeated tests 
for differences between the desired outcome 
and what currently exists (Module 24). 


median The point in a distribution of scores 
that divides the distribution exactly in half 
when the scores are listed in numerical order 
(Module 56). 


medical perspective The perspective that sug- 
gests that when an individual displays symp- 
toms of abnormal behavior, the root cause will 
be found in a physical examination of the indi- 
vidual, which may reveal a hormonal imbal- 
ance, a chemical deficiency, or a brain injury 
(Module 46). 


meditation A learned technique for refocusing 
attention that brings about an altered state of 
consciousness (Module 15). 


memory The process by which we encode, 
store, and retrieve information (Module 20). 


menopause The period during which women 
stop menstruating and are no longer fertile 
(Module 39). 


mental age The age for which a given level of 
performance is average or typical (Module 26). 


mental images Representations in the mind of 
an object or event (Module 23). 


mental retardation (or intellectual disability) 
A condition characterized by significant limita- 
tions both in intellectual functioning and in 
conceptual, social, and practical adaptive skills 
(Module 27). 


mental set The tendency for old patterns of 
problem solving to persist (Module 24). 


metabolism The rate at which food is con- 
verted to energy and expended by the body 
(Module 30). 


metacognition An awareness and under- 
standing of one’s own cognitive processes 
(Module 37). 


Minnesota Multiphasic Personality 
Inventory-2 (MMPI-2) A widely used 
self-report test that identifies people with 
psychological difficulties and is employed to 
predict some everyday behaviors (Module 42). 


mirror neurons Specialized neurons that fire 
not only when a person enacts a particular 
behavior, but also when a person simply 
observes another individual carrying out the 
same behavior (Module 7). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


mode The most frequently occurring score ina 
set of scores (Module 56). 


mood disorder A disturbance in emotional 
experience that is strong enough to intrude on 
everyday living (Module 47). 


mood stabilizers Drugs used to treat mood 
disorders that prevent manic episodes of bipo- 
lar disorder (Module 51). 


motivation The factors that direct and energize 
the behavior of humans and other organisms 
(Module 29). 


motor area The part of the cortex that is largely 
responsible for the body’s voluntary movement 
(Module 9). 


motor (efferent) neurons Neurons that com- 
municate information from the nervous system 
to muscles and glands (Module 8). 


myelin sheath A protective coat of fat and pro- 
tein that wraps around the axon (Module 7). 


narcissistic personality disorder A personality 
disturbance characterized by an exaggerated 
sense of self-importance (Module 47). 


narcotics Drugs that increase relaxation and 
relieve pain and anxiety (Module 16). 


nativist approach (to language develop- 
ment) The theory that a genetically deter- 
mined, innate mechanism directs language 
development (Module 25). 


naturalistic observation Research in which an 
investigator simply observes some naturally 
occurring behavior and does not make a change 
in the situation (Module 5). 


nature-nurture issue The issue of the degree to 
which environment and heredity influence 
behavior (Module 35). 


need for achievement A stable, learned charac- 
teristic in which a person obtains satisfaction by 
striving for and attaining a level of excellence 
(Module 30). 


need for affiliation An interest in establishing 
and maintaining relationships with other peo- 
ple (Module 30). 


need for power A tendency to seek impact, 
control, or influence over others and to be seen 
as a powerful individual (Module 30). 


negative reinforcer An unpleasant stimulus 
whose removal leads to an increase in the prob- 
ability that a preceding response will be 
repeated in the future (Module 18). 


negative relationship A relationship estab- 
lished by data that shows high values of one 
variable corresponding with low values of the 
other (Module 58). 


neo-Freudian psychoanalysts Psychoanalysts 
who were trained in traditional Freudian theory 
but who later rejected some of its major points 
(Module 40). 


neonate A newborn child (Module 37). 


neurogenesis The creation of new neurons 
(Module 9). 


neurons Nerve cells, the basic elements of the 
nervous system (Module 7). 


neuroplasticity Changes in the brain that occur 
throughout the life span relating to the addition 
of new neurons, new interconnections between 


neurons, and the reorganization of information- 
processing areas (Module 9). 


neuroscience perspective The approach that 
views behavior from the perspective of the 
brain, the nervous system, and other biological 
functions (Module 2). 


neurotransmitters Chemicals that carry mes- 
sages across the synapse to the dendrite (and 
sometimes the cell body) of a receiver neuron 
(Module 7). 


neutral stimulus A stimulus that, before condi- 
tioning, does not naturally bring about the 
response of interest (Module 17). 


normal distribution A distribution of scores 
that produces a symmetrical, bell-shaped curve 
in which the right half mirrors the left half and 
in which the mean, median, and mode all have 
the same value (Module 56). 


norms Standards of test performance that per- 
mit the comparison of one person’s score on a 
test with the scores of other individuals who 
have taken the same test (Module 26). 


obedience A change in behavior in response to 
the commands of others (Module 53). 


obesity Body weight that is more than 20% 
above the average weight for a person of a par- 
ticular height (Module 30). 


object permanence The awareness that 
objects—and people—continue to exist even if 
they are out of sight (Module 37). 


observational learning Learning by observing 
the behavior of another person, or model 
(Module 19). 


obsession A persistent, unwanted thought or 
idea that keeps recurring (Module 47). 


obsessive-compulsive disorder A disorder 
characterized by obsessions or compulsions 
(Module 47). 


Oedipal conflict A child’s sexual interest in his 
or her opposite-sex parent, typically resolved 
through identification with the same-sex parent 
(Module 40). 


operant conditioning Learning in which a vol- 
untary response is strengthened or weakened, 
depending on its favorable or unfavorable con- 
sequences (Module 18). 


operational definition The translation of a 
hypothesis into specific, testable procedures 
that can be measured and observed (Module 4). 


opponent-process theory of color vision The 
theory that receptor cells for color are linked in 
pairs, working in opposition to each other 
(Module 11). 


optic nerve A bundle of ganglion axons that 
carry visual information to the brain (Module 11). 


oral stage According to Freud, a stage from 
birth to age 12 to 18 months, in which an 
infant’s center of pleasure is the mouth 
(Module 40). 


orgasm The peak of sexual excitement, during 
which rhythmic muscular contractions occur in 
the genitals (Module 33). 


overgeneralization The phenomenon by 
which children apply language rules even 
when the application results in an error 
(Module 25). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


ovulation The point at which an egg is released 
from the ovaries (Module 33). 


panic disorder Anxiety disorder that takes the 
form of panic attacks lasting from a few 
seconds to several hours (Module 47). 


parasympathetic division The part of the auto- 
nomic division of the nervous system that acts 
to calm the body after an emergency has ended 
(Module 8). 


partial (or intermittent) reinforcement sched- 
ule Reinforcing of a behavior some but not all 
of the time (Module 18). 


passionate (or romantic) love A state of 
intense absorption in someone that includes 
intense physiological arousal, psychological 
interest, and caring for the needs of another 
(Module 55). 


perception The sorting out, interpretation, 
analysis, and integration of stimuli by the sense 
organs and brain (Module 10). 


perceptual constancy The phenomenon in 
which physical objects are perceived as unvary- 
ing and consistent despite changes in their 
appearance or in the physical environment 
(Module 13). 


peripheral nervous system The part of the ner- 
vous system that includes the autonomic and 
somatic subdivisions; made up of neurons with 
long axons and dendrites, it branches out from 
the spinal cord and brain and reaches the 
extremities of the body (Module 8). 


peripheral route processing Message interpre- 
tation characterized by consideration of the 
source and related general information rather 
than of the message itself (Module 52). 


permissive parents Parents who give their 
children relaxed or inconsistent direction and, 
although they are warm, require little of them 
(Module 37). 


personal stressors Major life events, such as 
the death of a family member, that have imme- 
diate negative consequences that generally fade 
with time (Module 43). 


personality The pattern of enduring character- 
istics that produce consistency and individual- 
ity in a given person (Module 40). 


personality disorder A disorder characterized 
by a set of inflexible, maladaptive behavior pat- 
terns that keep a person from functioning 
appropriately in society (Module 47). 


person-centered therapy Therapy in which 
the goal is to reach one’s potential for self- 
actualization (Module 50). 


phallic stage According to Freud, a period 
beginning around age 3 during which a 
child’s pleasure focuses on the genitals 
(Module 40). 


phobias Intense, irrational fears of specific 
objects or situations (Module 47). 


phonemes The smallest units of speech 
(Module 25). 


phonology The study of the smallest units of 
speech, called phonemes (Module 25). 


pituitary gland The major component of 
the endocrine system, or “master gland,” 
which secretes hormones that control growth 


and other parts of the endocrine system 
(Module 8). 


placebo A false treatment, such as a pill, 
“drug,” or other substance, without any signifi- 
cant chemical properties or active ingredient 
(Module 6). 


place theory of hearing The theory that differ- 
ent areas of the basilar membrane respond to 
different frequencies (Module 12). 


plateau phase The period in which the maxi- 
mum level of arousal is attained, the penis and 
clitoris swell with blood, and the body prepares 
for orgasm (Module 33). 


population All the members of a group of 
interest (Module 58). 


positive reinforcer A stimulus added to the 
environment that brings about an increase in a 
preceding response (Module 18). 


positive relationship A relationship estab- 
lished by data that shows high values of one 
variable corresponding with high values of 
another, and low values of the first variable 
corresponding with low values of the other 
(Module 58). 


posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) 

A phenomenon in which victims of major 
catastrophes or strong personal stressors feel 
long-lasting effects that may include re- 
experiencing the event in vivid flashbacks or 
dreams (Module 43). 


practical intelligence According to Sternberg, 
intelligence related to overall success in living 
(Module 26). 


prejudice A negative (or positive) evaluation of 
a particular group and its members (Module 54). 


premature ejaculation A male’s inability to 
delay orgasm as long as he wishes (Module 34). 


preoperational stage According to Piaget, 
the period from 2 to 7 years of age that is 
characterized by language development 
(Module 37). 


priming A phenomenon in which exposure to 
a word or concept (called a prime) later makes 
it easier to recall related information, even 
when there is no conscious memory of the word 
or concept (Module 21). 


principle of conservation The knowledge that 
quantity is unrelated to the arrangement and 
physical appearance of objects (Module 37). 


proactive interference Interference in which 
information learned earlier disrupts the recall of 
newer material (Module 22). 


procedural memory Memory for skills and 
habits, such as riding a bike or hitting a base- 
ball; sometimes referred to as nondeclarative 
memory (Module 20). 


progesterone A female sex hormone secreted 
by the ovaries (Module 33). 


projective personality test A test in whicha 
person is shown an ambiguous stimulus and 
asked to describe it or tell a story about it 
(Module 42). 


prosocial behavior Helping behavior 
(Module 55). 


prototypes Typical, highly representative 
examples of a concept (Module 23). 


Glossary G-7 


psychoactive drugs Drugs that influence a per- 
son’s emotions, perceptions, and behavior 
(Module 16). 


psychoanalysis Freudian psychotherapy in 
which the goal is to release hidden unconscious 
thoughts and feelings in order to reduce their 
power in controlling behavior (Module 49). 


psychoanalytic perspective The perspective 
that suggests that abnormal behavior stems 
from childhood conflicts over opposing wishes 
regarding sex and aggression (Module 46). 


psychoanalytic theory Freud’s theory that 
unconscious forces act as determinants of per- 
sonality (Module 40). 


psychodynamic approaches to personality 
Approaches that assume that personality is 
motivated by inner forces and conflicts about 
which people have little awareness and over 
which they have no control (Module 40). 


psychodynamic perspective The approach 
based on the view that behavior is motivated by 
unconscious inner forces over which the indi- 
vidual has little control (Module 2). 


psychodynamic therapy Therapy that seeks to 
bring unresolved past conflicts and unaccept- 
able impulses from the unconscious into the 
conscious, where patients may deal with the 
problems more effectively (Module 49). 


psychological tests Standard measures devised 
to assess behavior objectively; used by psychol- 
ogists to help people make decisions about their 
lives and understand more about themselves 
(Module 42). 


psychology The scientific study of behavior 
and mental processes (Module 1). 


psychoneuroimmunology (PNI) The study of 
the relationship among psychological factors, 
the immune system, and the brain (Module 43). 


psychophysics The study of the relationship 
between the physical aspects of stimuli and our 
psychological experience of them (Module 10). 


psychophysiological disorders Medical prob- 
lems influenced by an interaction of psycho- 
logical, emotional, and physical difficulties 
(Module 43). 


psychosexual stages Developmental periods 
that children pass through during which 

they encounter conflicts between the demands 
of society and their own sexual urges 

(Module 40). 


psychosocial development Development of 
individuals’ interactions and understanding of 
each other and of their knowledge and under- 
standing of themselves as members of society 
(Module 37). 


psychosurgery Brain surgery once used to 
reduce the symptoms of mental disorder but 
rarely used today (Module 51). 


psychotherapy Treatment in which a trained 
professional—a therapist—uses psychological 
techniques to help a person overcome psycho- 
logical difficulties and disorders, resolve prob- 
lems in living, or bring about personal growth 
(Module 49). 


puberty The period at which maturation of the 
sexual organs occurs, beginning at about age 11 
or 12 for girls and 13 or 14 for boys (Module 38). 


G-8 Glossary 


punishment A stimulus that decreases the 
probability that a previous behavior will occur 
again (Module 18). 


random assignment to condition A procedure 
in which participants are assigned to different 
experimental groups or “conditions” on the basis 
of chance and chance alone (Module 5). 


range The difference between the highest 
score and the lowest score in a distribution 
(Module 57). 


rape The act by which one person forces 
another person to submit to sexual activity 
(Module 34). 


rapid eye movement (REM) sleep Sleep occu- 
pying 20% of an adult’s sleeping time, charac- 
terized by increased heart rate, blood pressure, 
and breathing rate; erections; eye movements; 
and the experience of dreaming (Module 14). 


rational-emotive behavior therapy A form of 
therapy that attempts to restructure a person’s 
belief system into a more realistic, rational, and 
logical set of views by challenging dysfunc- 
tional beliefs that maintain irrational behavior 
(Module 49). 


reactance A negative emotional and cognitive 
reaction that results from the restriction of one’s 
freedom (Module 45). 


recall Memory task in which specific informa- 
tion must be retrieved (Module 21). 


reciprocity-of-liking effect A tendency to like 
those who like us (Module 55). 


recognition Memory task in which individuals 
are presented with a stimulus and asked 
whether they have been exposed to it in the 
past or to identify it from a list of alternatives 
(Module 21). 


reflex An automatic, involuntary response to 
an incoming stimulus (Module 8). 


reflexes Unlearned, involuntary responses that 
occur automatically in the presence of certain 
stimuli (Module 37). 


refractory period A temporary period that fol- 
lows the resolution stage and during which 
the male cannot develop an erection again 
(Module 33). 


rehearsal The repetition of information that 
has entered short-term memory (Module 20). 


reinforcement The process by which a stimu- 
lus increases the probability that a preceding 
behavior will be repeated (Module 18). 


reinforcer Any stimulus that increases the 
probability that a preceding behavior will occur 
again (Module 18). 


reliability The property by which tests 
measure consistently what they are trying to 
measure (Module 26). 


replicated research Research that is repeated, 
sometimes using other procedures, settings, 
and groups of participants, to increase confi- 
dence in prior findings (Module 5). 


repression The primary defense mechanism in 
which unacceptable or unpleasant id impulses are 
pushed back into the unconscious (Module 40). 


resolution stage The interval after orgasm in 
which the body returns to its unaroused state, 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


reversing the changes brought about by arousal 
(Module 33). 


resting state The state in which there is a nega- 
tive electrical charge of about -70 millivolts 
within a neuron (Module 7). 


reticular formation The part of the brain 
extending from the medulla through the pons 
and made up of groups of nerve cells that can 
immediately activate other parts of the brain to 
produce general bodily arousal (Module 9). 


retina The part of the eye that converts the elec- 
tromagnetic energy of light to electrical impulses 
for transmission to the brain 

(Module 11). 


retroactive interference Interference in which 
there is difficulty in the recall of information 
learned earlier because of later exposure to dif- 
ferent material (Module 22). 


retrograde amnesia Amnesia in which mem- 
ory is lost for occurrences prior to a certain 
event (Module 22). 


reuptake The reabsorption of neurotransmit- 
ters by a terminal button (Module 7). 


rods Thin, cylindrical receptor cells in the retina 
that are highly sensitive to light (Module 11). 


Rorschach test A test that involves showing a 
series of symmetrical visual stimuli to people 
who then are asked what the figures represent 
to them (Module 42). 


sample A representative subgroup of a popula- 
tion of interest (Module 58). 


Schachter-Singer theory of emotion The belief 
that emotions are determined jointly by a non- 
specific kind of physiological arousal and its 
interpretation, based on environmental cues 
(Module 31). 


schedules of reinforcement Different patterns 
of frequency and timing of reinforcement fol- 
lowing desired behavior (Module 18). 


schemas Organized bodies of information 
stored in memory that bias the way new infor- 
mation is interpreted, stored, and recalled 
(Modules 21, 52). 


schizophrenia A class of disorders in which 
severe distortion of reality occurs (Module 47). 


scientific method The approach through which 
psychologists systematically acquire knowledge 
and understanding about behavior and other 
phenomena of interest (Module 4). 


self-actualization A state of self-fulfillment in 
which people realize their highest potential in 
their own unique way (Modules 29, 41). 


self-efficacy Belief in one’s personal capabili- 
ties. Self-efficacy underlies people’s faith in 
their ability to carry out a particular behavior 
or produce a desired outcome (Module 41). 


self-esteem The component of personality that 
encompasses our positive and negative self- 
evaluations (Module 41). 


self-report measures A method of gathering 
data about people by asking them questions 
about a sample of their behavior (Module 42). 


self-serving bias The tendency to attribute 
personal success to personal factors (skill, abil- 
ity, or effort) and to attribute failure to factors 
outside oneself (Module 52). 


semantic memory Memory for general knowl- 
edge and facts about the world, as well as 
memory for the rules of logic that are used to 
deduce other facts (Module 20). 


semantic networks Mental representations of 
clusters of interconnected information 
(Module 20). 


semantics The rules governing the meaning of 
words and sentences (Module 25). 


semicircular canals Three tubelike structures 
of the inner ear containing fluid that sloshes 
through them when the head moves, signaling 
rotational or angular movement to the brain 
(Module 12). 


sensation The activation of the sense organs by 
a source of physical energy (Module 10). 


sensorimotor stage According to Piaget, the 
stage from birth to 2 years, during which a 
child has little competence in representing the 
environment by using images, language, or 
other symbols (Module 37). 


sensory (afferent) neurons Neurons that 
transmit information from the perimeter of 
the body to the central nervous system 
(Module 8). 


sensory area The site in the brain of the tissue 
that corresponds to each of the senses, with the 
degree of sensitivity related to the amount of 
tissue (Module 9). 


sensory memory The initial, momentary stor- 
age of information, lasting only an instant 
(Module 20). 


sequential research A research method that 
combines cross-sectional and longitudinal 
research by considering a number of different 
age groups and examining them at several 
points in time (Module 35). 


sexism Negative attitudes and behavior 
toward a person based on that person’s gender 
(Module 32). 


sexually transmitted infection (STI) A disease 
acquired through sexual contact (Module 34). 


shaping The process of teaching a complex 
behavior by rewarding closer and close 
approximations of the desired behavior 
(Module 18). 


short-term memory Memory that holds infor- 
mation for 15 to 25 seconds (Module 20). 


significant outcome An outcome in which the 
observed outcome would be expected to have 
occurred by chance with a probability of .05 or 
less (Modules 5, 58). 


situational causes (of behavior) Perceived 
causes of behavior that are based on environ- 
mental factors (Module 52). 


skin senses The senses of touch, pressure, 
temperature, and pain (Module 12). 


social cognition The cognitive processes by 
which people understand and make sense of 
others and themselves (Module 52). 


social cognitive approaches to personality 
Theories that emphasize the influence of a 
person’s cognitions—thoughts, feelings, expec- 
tations, and values—as well as observation of 
others’ behavior, in determining personality 
(Module 41). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


social influence The process by which the 
actions of an individual or group affect the 
behavior of others (Module 53). 


social neuroscience The subfield of social psy- 
chology that seeks to identify the neural basis 
of social behavior (Module 54). 


social psychology The scientific study of how 
people’s thoughts, feelings, and actions are 
affected by others (Module 52). 


social support A mutual network of caring, 
interested others (Module 43). 


social supporter A group member whose dis- 
senting views make nonconformity to the 
group easier (Module 53). 


sociocultural perspective The perspective that 
assumes that people’s behavior—both normal 
and abnormal—is shaped by the kind of family 
group, society, and culture in which they live 
(Module 46). 


somatic division The part of the peripheral 
nervous system that specializes in the control of 
voluntary movements and the communication 
of information to and from the sense organs 
(Module 8). 


somatoform disorders Psychological difficul- 
ties that take on a physical (somatic) form, but 
for which there is no medical cause (Module 47). 


sound The movement of air molecules brought 
about by a source of vibration (Module 12). 


spinal cord A bundle of neurons that leaves the 
brain and runs down the length of the back and 
is the main means for transmitting messages 
between the brain and the body (Module 8). 


spontaneous recovery The reemergence of an 
extinguished conditioned response after a 
period of rest and with no further conditioning 
(Module 17). 


spontaneous remission Recovery without 
treatment (Module 50). 


stage 1 sleep The state of transition between 
wakefulness and sleep, characterized by rela- 
tively rapid, low-amplitude brain waves 
(Module 14). 


stage 2 sleep Asleep deeper than that of stage 
1, characterized by a slower, more regular wave 
pattern, along with momentary interruptions of 
“sleep spindles” (Module 14). 


stage 3 sleep Asleep characterized by slow 
brain waves, with greater peaks and valleys 
in the wave pattern than in stage 2 sleep 
(Module 14). 


stage 4 sleep The deepest stage of sleep, dur- 
ing which we are least responsive to outside 
stimulation (Module 14). 


standard deviation An index of the average 
deviation of a set of scores from the center of 
the distribution (Module 57). 


statistics The branch of mathematics concerned 
with collecting, organizing, analyzing, and 
drawing conclusions from numerical data 
(Module 56). 


status The social rank held within a group 
(Module 53). 


stereotype A set of generalized beliefs and 
expectations about a particular group and its 
members (Module 54). 


stimulants Drugs that have an arousal effect on 
the central nervous system, causing a rise in 
heart rate, blood pressure, and muscular ten- 
sion (Module 16). 


stimulus Energy that produces a response in a 
sense organ (Module 10). 


stimulus discrimination The process that 
occurs if two stimuli are sufficiently distinct 
from one another that one evokes a conditioned 
response but the other does not; the ability to 
differentiate between stimuli (Module 17). 


stimulus generalization A process in which, 
after a stimulus has been conditioned to pro- 
duce a particular response, stimuli that are sim- 
ilar to the original stimulus produce the same 
response (Module 17). 


stress A person’s response to events that are 
threatening or challenging (Module 43). 


structuralism Wundt’s approach, which 
focuses on uncovering the fundamental mental 
components of consciousness, thinking, and 
other kinds of mental states and activities 
(Module 2). 


subjective well-being People’s own evaluation 
of their lives in terms of both their thoughts and 
their emotions (Module 45). 


superego According to Freud, the final person- 
ality structure to develop; it represents the 
rights and wrongs of society as handed down 
by a person’s parents, teachers, and other 
important figures (Module 40). 


survey research Research in which people cho- 
sen to represent a larger population are asked a 
series of questions about their behavior, 
thoughts, or attitudes (Module 5). 


syllogistic reasoning Formal reasoning in 
which people draw a conclusion from a set of 
assumptions (Module 23). 


sympathetic division The part of the auto- 
nomic division of the nervous system that acts 
to prepare the body for action in stressful situa- 
tions, engaging all the organism’s resources to 
respond to a threat (Module 8). 


synapse The space between two neurons 
where the axon of a sending neuron communi- 
cates with the dendrites of a receiving neuron 
by using chemical messages (Module 7). 


syntax Ways in which words and phrases can 
be combined to form sentences (Module 25). 


systematic desensitization A behavioral 
technique in which gradual exposure to an 
anxiety-producing stimulus is paired with 
relaxation to extinguish the response of anxiety 
(Module 49). 


telegraphic speech Sentences in which words not 
critical to the message are left out (Module 25). 


temperament The innate disposition that 
emerges early in life (Modules 37, 41). 


teratogens Environmental agents such as a 
drug, chemical, virus, or other factor that 
produce a birth defect (Module 36). 


terminal buttons Small bulges at the end of 
axons that send messages to other neurons 
(Module 7). 


test standardization A technique used to vali- 
date questions in personality tests by studying 


Glossary G-9 


the responses of people with known diagnoses 
(Module 42). 


thalamus The part of the brain located in the 
middle of the central core that acts primarily to 
relay information about the senses (Module 9). 


Thematic Apperception Test (TAT) A test 
consisting of a series of pictures about which a 
person is asked to write a story (Module 42). 


theories Broad explanations and predictions 
concerning phenomena of interest (Module 4). 


theory of multiple intelligences Gardner’s 
intelligence theory that proposes that there 
are eight distinct spheres of intelligence 
(Module 26). 


thinking The manipulation of mental represen- 
tations of information (Module 23). 


tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon The inability to 
recall information that one realizes one knows— 
a result of the difficulty of retrieving informa- 
tion from long-term memory (Module 21). 


top-down processing Perception that is guided 
by higher-level knowledge, experience, expec- 
tations, and motivations (Module 13). 


traits Consistent personality characteristics and 
behaviors displayed in different situations 
(Module 41). 


trait theory A model of personality that seeks 
to identify the basic traits necessary to describe 
personality (Module 41). 


transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) A 
depression treatment in which a precise mag- 
netic pulse is directed to a specific area of the 
brain (Module 51). 


transference The transfer of feelings to a psy- 
choanalyst of love or anger that had been origi- 
nally directed to a patient’s parents or other 
authority figures (Module 49). 


transsexuals People who believe they were 
born with the body of the other gender 
(Module 34). 


treatment The manipulation implemented by 
the experimenter (Module 5). 


trichromatic theory of color vision The theory 
that there are three kinds of cones in the retina, 
each of which responds primarily to a specific 
range of wavelengths (Module 11). 


trust-versus-mistrust stage According to 
Erikson, the first stage of psychosocial develop- 
ment, occurring from birth to age 1% years, 
during which time infants develop feelings of 
trust or lack of trust (Module 37). 


Type A behavior pattern A cluster of behaviors 
involving hostility, competitiveness, time 
urgency, and feeling driven (Module 44). 


Type B behavior pattern A cluster of behaviors 
characterized by a patient, cooperative, non- 
competitive, and nonagegressive manner 
(Module 44). 


unconditional positive regard An attitude 
of acceptance and respect on the part of an 
observer, no matter what a person says or 
does (Module 41). 


unconditioned response (UCR) A response 
that is natural and needs no training (e.g., sali- 
vation at the smell of food) (Module 17). 


G-10 Glossary 


unconditioned stimulus (UCS) A stimulus that 
naturally brings about a particular response 
without having been learned (Module 17). 


unconscious A part of the personality that con- 
tains the memories, knowledge, beliefs, feel- 
ings, urges, drives, and instincts of which the 
individual is not aware (Module 40). 


unconscious wish fulfillment theory Sigmund 
Freud’s theory that dreams represent uncon- 
scious wishes that dreamers desire to see ful- 
filled (Module 14). 


uninvolved parents Parents who show little 
interest in their children and are emotionally 
detached (Module 37). 


universal grammar Noam Chomsky’s theory 
that all the world’s languages share a common 
underlying structure (Module 25). 


validity The property by which tests actually 
measure what they are supposed to measure 
(Module 26). 


Join Us on Fb WhatsApp & Telegr 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


variability The spread, or dispersion, of scores 
in a distribution (Module 57). 


variable-interval schedule A schedule by 
which the time between reinforcements varies 
around some average rather than being fixed 
(Module 18). 


variable-ratio schedule A schedule by which 
reinforcement occurs after a varying number of 
responses rather than after a fixed number 
(Module 18). 


variables Behaviors, events, or other charac- 
teristics that can change, or vary, in some way 
(Module 5). 


visual illusions Physical stimuli that consis- 


tently produce errors in perception (Module 13). 


wear-and-tear theories of aging Theories 
that suggest that the mechanical functions of 
the body simply stop working efficiently 
(Module 39). 


Weber’s law A basic law of psychophysics stat- 
ing that a just noticeable difference is a constant 
proportion to the intensity of an initial stimulus 
(rather than a constant amount) (Module 10). 


weight set point The particular level of weight 
that the body strives to maintain (Module 30). 


working memory A set of active, temporary 
memory stores that actively manipulate and 
rehearse information (Module 20). 


zone of proximal development (ZPD) Accord- 
ing to Vygotsky, the level at which a child can 
almost, but not fully, comprehend or perform a 
task on his or her own (Module 37). 


zygote The new cell formed by the union of an 
egg and sperm (Module 36). 


Chor Ouly Gooks Potes tu pdf 
Fo.com/groups/4581844109658 70 


TELEGRAM.ME/CSSASPIRANTSFORUM 


WIAA SLAP? INUMEh 
Pe 2= BI-LO -O-US 


SEPARATE GROUP FOR FEMALES ASPIRANTS 


A 


» 


VF 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


References 


AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety. (2008). Cell 
phones and driving: Research update. 
Retrieved from www.aaafoundation.org / 
pdf/CellPhonesandDrivingReport.pdf 


AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety. (2009). 
Distracted Driving the Top Reason that 35 
Percent of Drivers Feel Less Safe Than Five 
Years Ago, According to the AAA Foundation. 
Retrieved from www.aaafoundation. 
org/pdf/2009TSCIndexPR.pdf 


AAUW. See American Association of University 
Women. 


Aartsen, M. J., Martin, M., & Zimprich, D. (2002). 
Gender differences in level and change in 
cognitive functioning: Results from the longi- 
tudinal aging study Amsterdam. Gerontology, 
50, 35-38. 


Aazh, H., & Moore, B. C. J. (2007). Dead regions 
in the cochlea at 4 kHz in elderly adults: 
Relation to absolute threshold, steepness of 
audiogram, and pure-tone average. Journal of 
the American Academy of Audiology, 18, 97-106. 


Abboud, L. (2005, July 27). The next phase in 
psychiatry. The Wall Street Journal, pp. D1, D5. 


Abelman, R. (2007). Fighting the war on inde- 
cency: Mediating TV, Internet, and video-game 
usage among achieving and underachieving 
gifted children. Roeper Review, 29, 100-112. 


Ablon, J. S., & Jones, E. E. (2005). On analytic 
process. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic 
Association, 53, 541-568. 


Aboitiz, F., Garcia, R., & Brunetti, E. (2006). The 
origin of Broca’s area and its connections 
from an ancestral working memory network. 
In Y. Grodzinsky & K. Amunts, Broca’s region. 
New York: Oxford University Press. 


Abraham, P. F., & Calabrese, J. R. (2007). Review 
of: Lithium treatment of mood disorders: A 
practical guide (6th rev. ed.). Bipolar Disorders, 
9, 548. 


Abramowitz, J. S., Olatunji, B. O., & Deacon, B. J. 
(2007). Health anxiety, hypochondriasis, and 
the anxiety disorders. Behavior Therapy, 38, 
86-94. 


Abrams, R. L., Klinger, M. R., & Greenwald, A. G. 
(2002). Subliminal words activate semantic 
categories (not automated responses). 
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9, 100-106. 


Accardi, M., & Milling, L. (2009, August). The 
effectiveness of hypnosis for reducing proce- 
dure-related pain in children and adolescents: 
A comprehensive methodological review. 
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 32, 328-339. 


Ackard, D. M., & Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2002). 
Date violence and date rape among 


adolescents: Associations with disordered 
eating behaviors and psychological health. 
Child Abuse and Neglect, 26, 455-473. 


Adams, G., & Dzokoto, V. A. (2007). Genital- 
shrinking panic in Ghana: A cultural 
psychological analysis. Culture & Psychology, 
13, 83-104. 


Adams, K. B. (2004). Changing investment in 
activities and interests in elders’ lives: Theory 
and measurement. International Journal of 
Aging and Human Development, 58, 87-108. 


Adams, M., Zuniga, X., Hackman, H. W., 
Castaneda, C. R., & Blumenfeld, W. J. (2000). 
Readings for diversity and social justice: An 
anthology on racism, sexism, anti-Semitism, 
heterosexism, classism, and ableism. New York: 
Routledge. 


Adams-Byers, J., Squilkr, S., & Moon, S. M. 
(2004). Gifted students’ perceptions of the 
academic and social/emotional effects of 
homogeneous and heterogeneous grouping. 
Gifted Child Quarterly, 48, 7-20. 


Addus, A. A., Chen, D., & Khan, A. S. (2007). 
Academic performance and advisement of 
university students: A case study. College 
Student Journal, 41, 316-326. 


Adler, J. (1984, April 23). The fight to conquer 
fear. Newsweek, pp. 66-72. 


Advokat, C. (2005). Differential effects of cloza- 
pine versus other antipsychotics on clinical 
outcome and dopamine release in the brain. 
Essential Psychopharmacology, 6, 73-90. 


Aftanas, L., & Golosheykin, S. (2005). Impact of 
regular meditation practice on EEG activity at 
rest and during evoked negative emotions. 
International Journal of Neuroscience, 115, 
893-909. 


Aguilar, J., Prato, F., Bravo, C., & Rivas, F. (2009). 
A multi-agent system for the management of 
abnormal situations in an artificially gas-lifted 
well. Applied Artificial Intelligence, 23, 406—426. 

Ahiima, R. S., & Osei, S. Y. (2004). Leptin 
signaling. Physiology and Behavior, 81, 223-241. 

Aiello, R. J. (2006). Hypermasculinity and 
psychosocial development as predictors of 
potential date-rape perpetration on campus. 


Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: 
The Sciences and Engineering, 67(2-B), 1178. 


Aiken, L. (2000). Dying, death, and bereavement 
(4th ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 


Aiken, L. R. (1996). Assessment of intellectual 
functioning (2nd ed.). New York: Plenum. 


Aiken, L. R. (1997). Psychological testing and 


assessment (9th ed.). Needham Heights, MA: 
Allyn & Bacon. 


Ainsworth, M. D. S., Blehar, M. C., Waters, E., & 
Wall, S. (1978). Patterns of attachment: A psycho- 
logical study of the strange situation. Hillsdale, 
NJ: Erlbaum. 


Ajzen, I. (2002). Residual effects of past on later 
behavior: Habituation and reasoned action 
perspectives. Personality and Social Psychology 
Review, 6, 107-122. 


Akil, H., & Morano, M. I. (1996). The biology of 
stress: From periphery to brain. In S. J. 
Watson (Ed.), Biology of schizophrenia and affec- 
tive disease. Washington, DC: American 
Psychiatric Press. 


Akirav, IL., Raizel, H., & Maroun, M. (2006). 
Enhancement of conditioned fear extinction 
by infusion of the GABA-sub(A) agonist mus- 
cimol into the rat prefrontal cortex and amyg- 
dala. European Journal of Neuroscience, 23, 
758-764. 


Alberts, A., Elkind, D., & Ginsberg, S. (2007). 
The personal fable and risk-taking in early 
adolescence. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 
36, 71-76. 


Albo, K., Vorobyev, V. A., Medvedev, S. V., 
Pakhomov, S. V., Starchenko, M. G., 
Terganiemi, M., & Näätänen, R. (2006). 
Selective attention to human voice enhances 
brain activity bilaterally in the superior 
temporal sulcus. Brain Research, 1075, 142-150. 


Alloy, L. B., Jacobson, N. S., & Acocella, J. (1999). 
Abnormal psychology (8th ed.). New York: 
McGraw-Hill. 


Allport, G. W. (1961). Pattern and growth in 
personality. New York: Holt, Rinehart and 
Winston. 


Allport, G. W. (1966). Traits revisited. American 
Psychologist, 21, 1-10. 

Allport, G. W., & Postman, L. J. (1958). The basic 
psychology of rumor. In E. D. Maccoby, T. M. 
Newcomb, & E. L. Hartley (Eds.), Readings in 
social psychology (3rd ed.). New York: Holt, 
Rinehart and Winston. 


Allwood, M. A. (2007). The relations of violence 
exposure, trauma symptoms and aggressive 
cognitions to youth violent behavior. 
Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: 
The Sciences and Engineering, 67, 5387. 


Aloia, M. S., Smith, K., & Arnedt, J. T. (2007). 
Brief behavioral therapies reduce early posi- 
tive airway pressure discontinuation rates in 
sleep apnea syndrome: Preliminary findings 
Behavioral Sleep Medicine, 5, 89-104. 


Alon, I., & Brett, J. M. (2007). Perceptions of time 
and their impact on negotiations in the 
Arabic-speaking Islamic world. Negotiation 
Journal, 23, 55-73. 


R-1 


R-2 References 


Alonso, A., Alonso, S., & Piper, W. (2003). Group 
psychotherapy. In G. Stricker, T. A. Widiger, et 
al. (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Clinical 
psychology (Vol. 8). New York: Wiley. 


Altman, N. (1996). The accommodation of 
diversity in psychoanalysis. In R. P. Foster, 
M. Moskowitz, & R. A. Javier (Eds.), Reaching 
across boundaries of culture and class: Widening 
the scope of psychotherapy. Northvale, NJ: Jason 
Aronson. 


Amato, L., Davoili, M., Perucci, C. A., Ferri, M., 
Faggiano, F., & Mattick R. P. (2005). An 
overview of systematic reviews of the effec- 
tiveness of opiate maintenance therapies: 
Available evidence to inform clinical practice 
and research. Journal of Substance Abuse 
Treatment, 28, 321-329. 


American Association of Mental Retardation 
(AAMR). (2002). Mental retardation: Definition, 
classification, and systems of supports (10th ed.). 
Washington, DC: AAMR. 


American Association of University Women 
(AAUW). (1992). How schools shortchange 
women: The A.A.U.W. Report. Washington, 
DC: AAUW Educational Foundation. 
American Association of University Women. 


American Association of University Women 
(AAUW). (2001). Hostile hallways: Bullying, 
teasing, and sexual harassment in school. 
Washington, DC: American Association of 
University Women. 


American College Health Association (2006). 
Acquaintance and date rape: What everyone 
should know. Baltimore, MD: American 
College Health Association. 


American Psychological Association. (APA). 
(1993b, January/February). Subgroup norming 
and the Civil Rights Act. Psychological Science 
Agenda, 5, 6. 


American Psychological Association (APA). (2000). 
Psychology careers for the twenty-first century. 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


American Psychological Association (APA). 
(2002, August 21). APA ethics code, 2002. 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


American Psychological Association (APA). 
(2007). Where psychologists work. Washington, 
DC: American Psychological Association. 


American Psychological Association Presidential 
Task Force on Evidence-Based Practice. (2006). 
Evidence-based practice in psychology, 61, 271-285. 


American Psychological Association Task Force 
on Intelligence. (1996). Intelligence: Knowns 
and unknowns. Washington, DC: American 
Psychological Association. 


Amos, A., Wiltshire, S., & Haw, S. (2006). 
Ambivalence and uncertainty: Experiences of 
and attitudes towards addiction and smoking 
cessation in the mid-to-late teens. Health 
Education Research, 21, 181-191. 


Anastasi, A., & Urbina, S. (1997). Psychological 
testing (7th ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice 
Hall. 

Anderson, B. F. (1980). The complete thinker: A 
handbook of techniques for creative and critical 


problem solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice 
Hall. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Anderson, C., & Carnagey, N. (2009). Causal 
effects of violent sports video games on 
aggression: Is it competitiveness or violent 
content? Journal of Experimental Social 
Psychology, 45, 731-739. 


Anderson, C., Carnagey, N. L., Flanagan, M., 
Benjamin, A. J., Jr., Eubanks, J., & Valentine, 
J. C. (2004). Violent video games: Specific 
effects of violent content on aggressive 
thoughts and behavior. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), 
Advances in experimental social psychology (Vol. 
36). San Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press. 


Anderson, J. (1988). Cognitive styles and multi- 
cultural populations. Journal of Teacher 
Education, 39, 2-9. 


Anderson, C., & Home, J. A. (2006). Sleepiness 
enhances distraction during monotonous 
task. Sleep: Journal of Sleep and Sleep Disorders 
Research, 29, 573-576. 


Anderson, J. A., & Adams, M. (1992). Acknowl- 
edging the learning styles of diverse student 
populations: Implications for instructional 
design. New Directions for Teaching and 
Learning, 49, 19-33. 


Anderson, J. R. (1981). Interference: The relation- 
ship between response latency and response 
accuracy. Journal of Experimental Psychology: 
Human Learning and Memory, 7, 311-325. 


Anderson, K. B., Cooper, H., & Okamura, L. 
(1997). Individual differences and attitudes 
toward rape: A meta-analytic review. 
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 23, 
295-315. 

Andrasik, F. (2006). Psychophysiological dis- 
orders: Headache as a case in point. In 
F. Andrasik, Comprehensive handbook of person- 
ality and psychopathology, Vol. 2: Adult psycho- 
pathology. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. 


Andrasik, F. (2007). What does the evidence 
show? Efficacy of behavioural treatments for 
recurrent headaches in adults. Neurological 
Science, 28, Supplement, S70-S77. 


Andreasen, N. C. (2005). Research advances in 
genetics and genomics: Implications for psychia- 
try. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric 
Publishing. 


Andrew, M., McCanlies, E., Burchfiel, C., Charles, 
L., Hartley, T., Fekedulegn, D., et al. (2008). 
Hardiness and psychological distress in a 
cohort of police officers. International Journal 
of Emergency Mental Health, 10, 137-148. 


Andrews, F. M., & Withey, S. B. (1976). Social indi- 
cators of well-being: Americans’ perceptions of life 
quality. New York: Plenum. 


Angier, N., & Chang, K. (2005, January 24). Gray 
matter and the sexes: Still a scientific gray 
area. The New York Times, pp. A1, A15. 


Angoff, W. H. (1988). The nature-nurture debate, 
aptitudes, and group differences. American 
Psychologist, 43, 713-720. 


Ansaldo, A. I., Arguin, M., & Roch Locours, L. A. 
(2002). The contribution of the right cerebral 
hemisphere to the recovery from aphasia: A 
single longitudinal case study. Brain Languages, 
82, 206-222. 

Antonini, A., & Barone, P. (2008, December). 
Dopamine agonist-based strategies in the 


treatment of Parkinson’s disease. Neurological 
Sciences, 29, S371-SS374. 


Antony, M. M., Brown, T. A., & Barlow, D. H. 
(1992). Current perspectives on panic and 
panic disorder. Current Directions in Psy- 
chological Science, 1, 79-82. 


Apanovich, A. M., McCarthy, D., & Salovey, P. 
(2003). Using message framing to motivate 
HIV testing among low-income, ethnic 
minority women. Health Psychology, 22, 88-94. 


Aponte, J. F., & Wohl, J. (2000). Psychological 
intervention and cultural diversity. Needham 
Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. 


Arafat, I., & Cotton, W. L. (1974). Masturbation 
practices of males and females. Journal of Sex 
Research, 10, 293-307. 


Arbuthnott, A., & Sharpe, D. (2009). The effect of 
physician—patient collaboration on patient 
adherence in non-psychiatric medicine. 
Patient Education and Counseling, 77, 60-67. 


Archer, J., & Lloyd, B. B. (2002). Sex and gender 
(2nd ed.). New York: Cambridge University 
Press. 


Arena, J. M. (1984, April). A look at the opposite 
sex. Newsweek on Campus, p. 21. 


Ariely, D., & Norton, M. I. (2009). Conceptual 
consumption. Annual Review of Psychology, 60, 
475-499. 


Ariyanto, A., Hornsey, M. J., & Gallois, C. (2006). 
Group-directed criticism in Indonesia: Role of 
message source and audience. Asian Journal of 
Social Psychology, 9, 96-102. 


Arnett, J. J. (2000). Emerging adulthood. American 
Psychologist, 55, 469-480. 


Arnett, J. J. (2001). Conceptions of the transition 
to adulthood: Perspectives from adolescence 
through midlife, Journal of Adult Development, 
8(2), pp. 133-144. 


Arnett, J. J. (2006). Emerging adulthood: The wind- 
ing road from the late teens through the twenties 
New York: Oxford University Press. 


Arnett, J. J. (2007), Afterword: Aging out of 
care—Toward realizing the possibilities of 
emerging adulthood. New Directions for Youth 
Development, 113, 151-161, 


Aronson, J., & Steele, Claude M. (2005). Stereotypes 
and the fragility of academic competence, 
motivation, and self-concept. In A. J. Elliot & 
C. S. Dweck (Eds.), Handbook of competence and 
motivation. New York, NY: Guilford 
Publications. 


Asch, S. E. (1951). Effects of group pressure upon 
the modification and distortion of judgments. 
In H. Guetzkow (Ed.), Groups, leadership, and 
men. Pittsburgh: Carnegie Press. 


Aschcraft, M. H. (1994). Human memory and 
cognition (2nd ed.). New York: Harper-Collins. 


Aspinwall, L. G., & Taylor, S. E. (1997). A stitch in 
time: Self-regulation and proactive coping. 
Psychological Bulletin, 121, 417-436. 


Associated Press. (2009, May 21). AP-mtvU: 
College students and depression. Retrieved 
from http://surveys.ap.org/data/Edison/ 
APMTV%20Topline%20marginals.pdf 


Association for the Advancement of Artificial 
Intelligence. (2009). AAAI Presidential Panel 
on Long-Term AI Futures: 2008-2009 Study. 
Retrieved from http://research.microsoft 
.com/en-us/um/people/horvitz/AAAI 
_Presidential_Panel_2008-2009.htm 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Astbury-Ward, E. (2002). From Kama Sutra to 
dot.com: The history, myths and management 
of premature ejaculation. Sexual and Relationship 
Therapy, 17, 367-380. 


Astin, A. W., Korn, W. S., & Berz, E. R. (2004). The 
American freshman: National norms for fall 2004. 
Los Angeles, CA: Higher Education Research 
Institute, UCLA. 


Atkinson, H. (Ed.). (1997, January 21). Under- 
standing your diagnosis. Health-News, p. 3. 


Atkinson, H. G. (2003, August). Are you a “good” 
patient? HealthNews, p. 5. 


Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human 
memory: A proposed system and its control 
processes. In K. W. Spence & J. T. Spence 
(Eds.), The psychology of learning and motiva- 
tion: Advances in research and theory (Vol. 2). 
New York: Academic Press. 


Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1971). The con- 
trol of short-term memory. Scientific American, 
225, pp. 82-90. 


Auer, J. A., Goodship, A., Arnoczky, S., Pearce, S., 
Price, J., Claes, L., von Rechenberg, B., 
Hofmann-Amtenbrinck, M., Schneider, E., 
Muller-Terpitz, R., Thiele, F., Rippe, K. P., & 
Grainger, D. W. (2007). Refining animal 
models in fracture research: seeking consen- 
sus for changing the agenda in optimising 
both animal welfare and scientific validity for 
appropriate biomedical use. BMC 
Musculoskeletal Disorders, 8, 72. 


Aujoulat, I., Luminet, O., & Deccache, A. (2007). 
The perspective of patients on their experi- 
ence of powerlessness. Quality Health 
Research, 17, 772-785. 


Auld, F., Hyman, M., & Rudzinski, D. (2005). 
Theory and strategy of dream interpretation. 
In F. Auld & M. Hyman (Eds.), Resolution of 
inner conflict: An introduction to psychoanalytic 
therapy (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American 
Psychological Association. 


Aussilloux, C., & Bagdadli, A. (2006). Handicap 
mental et société: Soigner, éduquer, intégrer. 
Mental handicap and society. Neuropsychiatrie 
de l'Enfance et de l’ Adolescence, 54, 336-340. 


Auyeung, B., Baron-Cohen, S., Ashwin, E., 
Taylor, K., Hines, M., Hackett, G., et al. (2009). 
Fetal testosterone predicts sexually differenti- 
ated childhood behavior in girls and in boys. 
Psychological Science, 20, 144-148. 


Averill, J. R. (1975). A semantic atlas of emotional 
concepts. Catalog of Selected Documents in 
Psychology, 5, 330. 


Avery, D., McKay, P., & Wilson, D. (2008). What 
are the odds? How demographic similarity 
affects the prevalence of perceived employ- 
ment discrimination. Journal of Applied 
Psychology, 93, 235-249. 


Baars, B., & Seth, A.K. (2009). Consciousness: 
Theories and models. In W. Banks, (Ed). 
Encyclopedia of consciousness. New York: 
Elsevier. 

Babson, K., Feldner, M., Trainor, C., & Smith, R. 
(2009, September). An experimental investiga- 
tion of the effects of acute sleep deprivation 
on panic-relevant biological challenge 
responding. Behavior Therapy, 40, 239-250. 

Bacchiochi, J. R. (2006). Development and 
validation of the Malingering Discriminant 


Function Index (M-DFI) for the Minnesota 
Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 (MMPI-2). 
Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: The 
Sciences and Engineering, 66(10-B), 5673. 


Baddeley, A. (2001). Is working memory still 
working? American Psychologist, 56, 851-864. 


Baddeley, A., Chincotta, D., & Adlam, A. (2001). 
Working memory and the control of action: 
Evidence from task switching. Journal of 
Experimental Psychology: General, 130, 641-657. 


Baddeley, A., & Wilson, B. (1985). Phonological 
coding and short-term memory in patients 
without speech. Journal of Memory and 
Language, 24, 490-502. 


Bagge, C., & Sher, K. (2008). Adolescent alcohol 
involvement and suicide attempts: Toward 
the development of a conceptual framework. 
Clinical Psychology Review, 28, 1283-1296. 


Bahrick, H. P., Hall, L. K., & Berger, S. A. (1996). 
Accuracy and distortion in memory for high 
school grades. Psychological Science, 7, 265-269. 


Bai, L. (2005). Children at play: A childhood 
beyond the Confucian shadow. Childhood: A 
Global Journal of Child Research, 12, 9-32. 


Bains, O. S. (2006). Insomnia: Difficulty falling 
and staying asleep. In N. F. Watson, & B. V. 
Bradley, Clinician’s guide to sleep disorders. 
Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis. 

Bair, D. (2003). Jung: A biography. New York: 
Little, Brown, and Company. 

Baker, J., & Berenbaum, H. (2007). Emotional 
approach and problem-focused coping: A 
comparison of potentially adaptive strategies. 
Cognition and Emotion, 21, 95-118. 


Baker, S. E., Johnson, P. J., & Slater, D. (2007). 
Learned food aversion with and without an 
odour cue for protecting untreated baits from 
wild mammal foraging [Special issue: 
Conservation, enrichment, and animal 
behavior]. Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 
102, 410-428. 


Balaban, C. D. (2002). Neural substrates linking 
balance control and anxiety [Special issue: 
The Pittsburgh special issue]. Physiology and 
Behavior, 77, 469-475. 


Balaban, C. D., McBurney, D. H., & Affeltranger, 
M. A. (2005). Three distinct categories of time 
course of pain produced by oral capsaicin. 
The Journal of Pain, 6, 315-322. 


Ball, D. (2004). Genetic approaches to alcohol 
dependence. British Journal of Psychiatry, 185, 
449-451. 


Ball, H., Arseneault, L., Taylor, A., Maughan, B., 
Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. (2008, January). Genetic 
and environmental influences on victims, 
bullies and bully-victims in childhood. Journal 
of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 104-112. 


Baltes, P. B., & Kunzmann, U. (2003). Wisdom. 
Psychologist, 16, 131-133. 


Bandura, A. (1977). Social learning theory. 
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. 


Bandura, A. (1986). Social foundations of thought 
and action: A social cognitive theory. Englewood 
Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. 


Bandura, A. (1994). Social cognitive theory of 
mass communication. In J. Bryant, & D. 
Zillmann (Eds.), Media effects: Advances in 
theory and research: LEA’s communication series. 
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 


References R-3 


Bandura, A. (1999). Social cognitive theory of per- 
sonality. In D. Cervone & Y. Shod (Eds.), The 
coherence of personality. New York: Guilford. 


Bandura, A. (2000). Self-efficacy: The foundation 
of agency. In W. J. Perrig & A. Grob (Eds.), 
Control of human behavior, mental processes, and 
consciousness: Essays in honor of the 60th 
birthday of August Flammer. Mahwah, NJ: 
Erlbaum. 


Bandura, A., Grusec, J. E., & Menlove, F. L. 
(1967). Vicarious extinction of avoidance 
behavior. Journal of Personality and Social 
Psychology, 5, 16-23. 


Bandura, A., & Locke, E. A. (2003). Negative self- 
efficacy and goal effects revisited. Journal of 
Applied Psychology, 88, 87-99. 

Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. (1963a). Imitation 
of film-mediated aggressive models. Journal of 
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 66, 3-11. 


Bandura, A., Ross, D., & Ross, S. (1963b). Vicarious 
reinforcement and imitative learning. Journal of 
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 67, 601-607. 


Banich, T., & Heller, W. (1998). Evolving perspec- 
tives on lateralization of function. Current 
Directions in Psychological Science, 7, 1-2. 


Banks, J. A. (2006). Improving race relations in 
schools: From theory and research to practice. 
Journal of Social Issues, 62, 607-614. 


Bannon, L. (2000, February 14). Why boys and 
girls get different toys. The Wall Street Journal, 
pp. B1, B4. 


Baraas, R. C., Foster, D. H., & Amano, K. (2006). 
Anomalous trichromats’ judgments of surface 
color in natural scenes under different day- 
lights. Neuroscience, 23, 629-635. 


Barandiaran, A. A., Pascual, A. C., & Samaniego, 
C. M. (2006). A criticism of the Kohlberg the- 
ory: The moral development in adults and 
educative implications. Revista de Psicologia 
General y Aplicada, 59, 165-182. 


Bargh, J. A., & Chartrand, T. L. (2000). The mind 
in the middle: A practical guide to priming 
and automaticity research. In H. T. Reis & 

C. M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods 
in social and personality psychology. New York: 
Cambridge University Press. 


Barker, J., & Jones, M. (2008, June). The effects of 
hypnosis on self-efficacy, affect, and soccer 
performance: A case study. Journal of Clinical 
Sport Psychology, 2, 127-147. 


Barkley, R. (2000). Taking charge of ADHD (rev. 
ed.). New York: Guilford Press. 


Barkley, R. (2005). ADHD and the nature of self- 
control. New York: Guildford. 


Barlow, D. H. (2007). Clinical handbook of psycho- 
logical disorders: A step-by-step treatment manual 
(4th ed.). New York: Guilford Press. 


Barmeyer, C. I. (2004). Learning styles and their 
impact on cross-cultural training: An interna- 
tional comparison in France, Germany and 
Quebec. International Journal of Intercultural 
Relations, 28, 577-594. 


Barnes, T., & Eardley, I. (2007). Premature ejacula- 
tion: The scope of the problem. Journal of Sex 
& Marital Therapy, 33, 151-170. 

Barnes, V. A., Davis, H. C., Murzynowski, J., & 
Treiber, F. A. (2004). Impact of meditation on 
resting and ambulatory blood pressure and 
heart rate in youth. Medicine, 66, 909-914. 


R-4 References 


Barnett, J. E., Wise, E. H., & Johnson-Greene, D. 
(2007). Informed consent: Too much of a good 
thing or not enough? Professional Psychology: 
Research and Practice, 38, 179-186. 


Barnett, R. C. (2004). Woman and work: Where 
are we, where did we come from, and where 
are we going? [Preface]. Journal of Social Issues, 
60, 667-674. 


Baron, R. S. (2005). So right it’s wrong: Groupthink 
and the ubiquitous nature of polarized group 
decision making. In M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances 
in experimental social psychology (Vol. 37). San 
Diego, CA: Elsevier Academic Press. 


Barresi, J. (2007). Consciousness and intentional- 
ity. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 14, Special 
issue: Concepts of Consciousness: Integrating an 
Emerging Science, 77-93. 

Barrett, L. F., & Salovey, P. (Eds.). (2002). The 
wisdom in feeling: Psychological processes in 
emotional intelligence. New York: Guilford Press. 


Barrett, L. F., & Wager, T. D. (2006). The structure 
of emotion: Evidence from neuroimaging 
studies. Current Directions in Psychological 
Science, 15, 79-83. 


Barron, F. (1990). Creativity and psychological 
health: Origins of personal vitality and creative 
freedom. Buffalo, NY: Creative Education 
Foundation. 


Barron, G., & Yechiam, E. (2002). Private e-mail 
requests and the diffusion of responsibility. 
Computers in Human Behavior, 18, 507-520. 


Bartecchi, C. E., MacKenzie, T. D., & Schrier, 
R. W. (1995, May). The global tobacco epi- 
demic. Scientific American, pp. 44-51. 


Bartholow, B. D., Bushman, B. J., & Sestir, M. A. 
(2005). Chronic violent video game exposure 
and desensitization to violence: Behavioral 
and event-related brain potential data. Journal 
of Experimental Social Psychology, 42, 532-539. 


Bartlett, F. (1932). Remembering: A study in experi- 
mental and social psychology. Cambridge, 
England: Cambridge University Press. 


Bartlett, M. Y., & DeSteno, D. (2006). Gratitude 
and prosocial behavior: Helping when it costs 
you. Psychological Science, 17, 319-325. 


Bartocci, G. (2004). Transcendence techniques 
and psychobiological mechanisms underlying 
religious experience. Mental Health, Religion 
and Culture, 7, 171-181. 


Bartolomeo, J., Cruz, C., Messer, L., Dodd, J., 
Herndon, J., & Warrick, P. (2009, June 29). 
Chastity Bono’s sex change: Becoming Chaz. 
People, p. 54. 


Bartone, P., Roland, R., Picano, J., & Williams, T. 
(2008). Psychological hardiness predicts suc- 
cess in U.S. Army Special Forces candidates. 
International Journal of Selection and Assessment, 
16, 78-81. 


Bartoshuk, L. (2000, July/August). The bitter with 
the sweet. APS Observer, 11, 33. 


Bartoshuk, L., & Lucchina, L. (1997, January 13). 
Are you a supertaster? U.S. News & World 
Report, pp. 58-59. 


Bartzokis, G., Nuechterlein, K. H., Lu, P. H., Gitlin, 
M., Rogers, S., & Mintz, J. (2003). 
Dysregulated brain development in adult 
men with schizophrenia: A magnetic 
resonance imaging study. Biological Psychiatry, 
53, 412-421. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Baruss, I. (2003). Alterations of consciousness: 
An empirical analysis for social scientists. 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


Basak, C., Boot, W., Voss, M., & Kramer, A. (2008). 
Can training in a real-time strategy video 
game attenuate cognitive decline in older 
adults? Psychology and Aging, 23, 765-777. 


Bassottdi, G., Villanacci, V., Fisogni, S., Rossi, E., 
Baronio, P., Clerici, C., et al. (2007). Enteric 
glial cells and their role in gastrointestinal 
motor abnormalities: Introducing the neuro- 
gliopathies. World Journal of Gastroenterology, 
14, 4035-4041. 


Bates, E. (2005). Plasticity, localization, and lan- 
guage development. In S. T. Parker & J. Langer 
(Eds.), Biology and knowledge revisited: From 
neurogenesis to psychogenesis. Mahwah, NJ: 
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 


Bates, P. E., Cuvo, T., Miner, C. A., & Korabek, 
C. A. (2001). Simulated and community-based 
instruction involving persons with mild and 
moderate mental retardation. Research in 
Developmental Disabilities, 22, 95-115. 


Bates, R. (2002). Liking and similarity as predic- 
tors of multi-source ratings. Personnel Review, 
31, 540-552. 


Batson, C. D., & Powell, A. A. (2003). Altruism 
and prosocial behavior. In T. Millon & M. J. 
Lerner (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Per- 
sonality and social psychology (Vol. 5). New 
York: Wiley. 


Bauer, P. (2008). Toward a neuro-developmental 
account of the development of declarative 
memory. Developmental Psychobiology, 50, 19-31. 


Baum, A. (1994). Behavioral, biological, and envi- 
ronmental interactions in disease processes. 
In S. Blumenthal, K. Matthews, & S. Weiss 
(Eds.), New research frontiers in behavioral medi- 
cine: Proceedings of the National Conference. 
Washington, DC: NIH Publications. 


Bauman, S., & Kopp, T. G. (2006). Integrating a 
humanistic approach in outpatient sex 
offender groups. Journal for Specialists in Group 
Work, 31, 247-261. 


Baumeister, R. F., & Stillman, T. (2006). Erotic 
plasticity: Nature, culture, gender, and sexu- 
ality. In R. D. McAnulty, & M. M. Burnette, 
Sex and sexuality, Vol 1: Sexuality today: Trends 
and controversies. Westport, CT: Praeger 
Publishers /Greenwood Publishing. 


Baumeister, R. F., Twenge, J. M., & Nuss, C. K. 
(2002). Effects of social exclusion on cognitive 
processes: Anticipated aloneness reduces 
intelligent thought. Journal of Personality and 
Social Psychology, 83, 817-827. 


Baumgartner, F. (2002). The effect of hardiness in 
the choice of coping strategies in stressful sit- 
uations. Studia Psychologica, 44, 69-75. 


Baumrind, D. (2005). Patterns of parental author- 
ity and adolescent autonomy. New Directions 
for Child and Adolescent Development, 108, 61-69. 


Bayliss, D. M., Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., & 
Gunn, D. M. (2005a). The relationship 
between short-term memory and working 
memory: Complex span made simple? 
Memory, 13, 414-421. 


Bayliss, D. M., Jarrold, C., Baddeley, A. D., Gunn, 
D. M., & Leigh, E. (2005b). Mapping the 


developmental constraints on working 
memory span performance. Developmental 
Psychology, 41, 579-597. 


Bayne, R. (2005). Ideas and evidence: Critical reflec- 
tions on MBTI® theory and practice. Gainesville, 
FL: Center for Applications of Psychological 
Type, CAPT. 


Bazalakova, M. H., Wright, J., Schneble, E. J., 
McDonald, M. P., Heilman, C. J., Levey, A. I., 
& Blakely, R. D. (2007). Deficits in acetylcho- 
line homeostasis, receptors and behaviors in 
choline transporter heterozygous mice. Genes, 
Brain & Behavior, 6, 411—424. 


Beardslee, W., Wright, E., Gladstone, T., & 
Forbes, P. (2007). Long-term effects from a 
randomized trial of two public health 
preventive interventions for parental 
depression. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 
703-713. 


Bearman, C. R., Ball, L. J., & Ormerod, T. C. 
(2007). The structure and function of sponta- 
neous analogising in domain-based problem 
solving. Thinking & Reasoning, 13, 273-294. 


Beatty, J. (2000). The human brain: Essentials of 
behavioral neuroscience. Thousand Oaks, CA: 
Sage. 

Bechara, A., Damasio, A. R., Damasio, H., & 
Anderson, S. (1994). Insensitivity to future 
consequences following damage to human 
prefrontal cortex. Cognition, 50, 7-15. 


Beck, A. P., & Lewis, C. M. (Eds.). (2000). The 
process of group psychotherapy: Systems for 
analyzing change. Washington, DC: American 
Psychological Association. 


Beck, A. T. (1995). Cognitive therapy: Past, 
present, and future. In M. J. Mahoney (Ed.), 
Cognitive and constructive psychotherapies: 
Theory, research, and practice. New York: 
Springer. 

Beck, A. T. (2004). Cognitive therapy, behavior 
therapy, psychoanalysis, and pharmacother- 
apy: A cognitive continuum. In A. Freeman, 
M. J. Mahoney, P. Devito, & D. Martin (Eds.), 
Cognition and Psychotherapy (2nd ed.). New 
York: Springer Publishing Co. 

Beck, A. T., & Emery, G., with Greenberg, R. L. 
(1985). Anxiety disorders and phobias: A cogni- 
tive perspective. New York: Basic Books. 


Beck, A. T., Freeman, A., & Davis, D. D. (2004). 
Cognitive therapy of personality disorders (2nd 
ed.). New York: Guilford Press. 


Beck, A. T., & Rector, N. A. (2005). Cognitive 
approaches to schizophrenia: Theory and 
therapy. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 
1, 577—606. 


Beck, H. P., Levinson, S., & Irons, G. (2009). Finding 
little Albert: A journey to John B. Watson’s 
infant laboratory. American Psychologist, 64, 
605-614. 


Becker, B. E., & Luthar, S. S. (2007). Peer- 
perceived admiration and social preference: 
Contextual correlates of positive peer regard 
among suburban and urban adolescents. 
Journal of Research on Adolescence, 17, 117-144. 


Becker, T. (2003). Is emotional intelligence a viable 
concept? Academy of Management Review, 28, 
192-195. 

Bedard, W. W., & Parsinger, M. A. (1995). 
Prednisolone blocks extreme intermale social 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


aggression in seizure-induced, brain- 
damaged rats: Implications for the amygda- 
loid central nucleus, corticotrophin-releasing 
factor, and electrical seizures. Psychological 
Reports, 77, 3-9. 


Beersma, D. G. M., & Gordijn, M. C. M. (2007). 
Circadian control of the sleep-wake cycle. 
Physiology & Behavior, 90. 


Begeny, J. C., & Martens, B. K. (2007). Inclusionary 
education in Italy: A literature review and call 
for more empirical research. Remedial and 
Special Education, 28, 80-94. 


Begg, D., & Langley, J. (2001). Changes in risky 
driving behavior from age 21 to 26 years. 
Journal of Safety Research, 32, 491-499. 


Begley, S. (2002, September 13). The memory of 
September 11 is seared in your mind; but is it 
really true? The Wall Street Journal, p. B1. 


Begley, S. (2005, April 29). Evolution psychology 
may not help explain our behavior after all. 
The Wall Street Journal, p. D1. 


Begley, S. (2009, February 16). Will the BlackBerry 
sink the presidency? Newsweek, p. 37. 


Behrens, M., Lendon, C., & Roe, C. (2009). A com- 
mon biological mechanism in cancer and 
Alzheimer’s disease? Current Alzheimer 
Research, 6, 196-204. 


Beidel, D. C., & Turner, S. M. (2007). Etiology of 
social anxiety disorder. In D. C. Beidel, & 
S. M. Turner, Shy children, phobic adults: Nature 
and treatment of social anxiety disorders (2nd ed.). 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2005). When high- 
powered people fail: Working memory and 
“choking under pressure” in math. Psy- 
chological Science, 16, 101-105. 


Belar, C. (2008, April). Clinical health psychology: 
A health care specialty in professional psy- 
chology. Professional Psychology: Research and 
Practice, 39, 229-233. 


Bellezza, F. S. (2000). Mnemonic devices. In A. E. 
Kazdin (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychology (Vol. 
5). Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


Belov, D. I., & Armstrong, R. D. (2009). Direct and 
inverse problems of item pool design for com- 
puterized adaptive testing. Educational and 
Psychological Measurement, 69, 533-547. 


Belsky, J., & Pluess, M. (2009). The nature (and 
nurture?) of plasticity in early human devel- 
opment. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 4, 
345-351. 

Bem, D. J. (1996). Exotic becomes erotic: A devel- 
opmental theory of sexual orientation. Psy- 
chological Review, 103, 320-335. 

Bem, D. J., & Honorton, C. (1994). Does psi exist? 
Replicable evidence for an anomalous process 


of information transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 
115, 4-18. 

Bem, S. L. (1998). An unconventional family. New 
Haven, CT: Yale University Press. 

Benac, N., & Tompson, T. (2009, May 21). AP poll: 
Most students stressed, some depressed. 


Retrieved from http://www.denverpost 
.com/popular /ci_12418610 


Benderly, B. L. (2004). Looking beyond the SAT. 
American Psychological Society, 17, 12-18. 


Benet-Martinez, V., Lee, F., & Leu, J. (2006). 
Biculturalism and cognitive complexity: 
Expertise in cultural representations. Journal 
of Cross-Cultural Psychology, 37, 386-407. 


Benham, G., Woody, E. Z., & Wilson, K. S. (2006). 
Expect the unexpected: Ability, attitude, and 
responsiveness to hypnosis. Journal of Per- 
sonality and Social Psychology, 91, 342-350. 


Benight, C. C. (2004). Collective efficacy follow- 
ing a series of natural disasters. Stress and 
Coping: An International Journal, 17, 401-420. 


Benjamin, L. T., Jr. (1985, February). Defining 
aggression: An exercise for classroom 
discussion. Teaching of Psychology, 12(1), 40-42. 


Ben-Porath, Y., & Archer, R. (2008). The MMPI-2 
and MMPI-A. Personality assessment. New 
York: Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. 


Benson, E. (2003, April). The science of sexual 
arousal. Monitor on Psychology, pp. 50-56. 


Benson, H., Kornhaber, A., Kornhaber, C., 
LeChanu, M. N., et al. (1994). Increases in 
positive psychological characteristics with a 
new relaxation-response curriculum in high 
school students. Journal of Research and 
Development in Education, 27, 226-231. 


Benton, S. A., Robertson, J. M., Tseng, W. C., 
Newton, F. B., & Benton, S. L. (2003). Changes 
in counseling center client problems across 13 
years. Professional Psychology: Research and 
Practice, 34, 66-72. 


Beresnevaité, M., Taylor, G. J., & Bagby, R. M. (2007). 
Assessing alexithymia and type A behavior in 
coronary heart disease patients: A multimethod 
approach. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 76, 
186-192. 


Berk, L. E. (2005). Why parenting matters. In 
S. Olfman (Ed.), Childhood lost: How American 
culture is failing our kids. Westport, CT: Praeger 
Publishers/ Greenwood Publishing Group. 


Berkowitz, L. (2001). On the formation and regu- 
lation of anger and aggression: A cognitive- 
neoassociationistic analysis. In W. G. Parrott 
(Ed.), Emotions in social psychology: Essential 
readings. New York: Psychology Press. 


Berle, D. (2007). Graded exposure therapy for 
long-standing disgust-related cockroach 
avoidance in an older male. Clinical Case 
Studies, 6, 339-347. 


Berliner, L., & Elliott, D. M. (2002). Sexual abuse 
of children. In J. E. B. Myers, L. Berliner, et al. 
(Eds.), The APSAC handbook on child maltreat- 
ment (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 

Berman, A. L.. Jobes, D. A., & Silverman, M. M. 
(2006). An integrative-eclectic approach to 
treatment. In A. L. Berman, D. A. Jobes, & 

M. M. Silverman, Adolescent suicide: Assessment 
and intervention (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: 
American Psychological Association. 

Bernal, G., Trimble, J. E., Burlew, A. K., & Leong, 
F. T. (Eds.) (2002). Handbook of racial and ethnic 
minority psychology. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 

Bernard, L. L. (1924). Instinct: A study in social psy- 
chology. New York: Holt. 

Bernstein, D., & Loftus, E. (2009a). How to tell if a 
particular memory is true or false. Perspectives 
on Psychological Science, 4, 370-374. 

Bernstein, D., & Loftus, E. (2009b). The conse- 
quences of false memories for food prefer- 


References R-5 


ences and choices. Perspectives on Psychological 
Science, 4, 135-139. 


Bernstein, D. M., Loftus, G. R., & Meltzoff, A. N. 
(2005). Object identification in preschool 
children and adults. Developmental Science, 8, 
151-161. 


Berntsen, D., & Rubin, D. C. (2004). Cultural life 
scripts structure recall from autobiographical 
memory. Memory and Cognition, 32, 427-442. 


Berntsen, D., & Thomsen, D. K. (2005). Personal 
memories for remote historical events: Accu- 
racy and clarity of flashbulb memories related 
to World War II. Journal of Experimental 
Psychology: General, 134, 242-257. 


Berntson, G. G., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Tranel, 
D., & Cacioppo, J. T. (2007). Amygdala contri- 
bution to selective dimensions of emotion. 
Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 
123-129. 


Berridge, K. C. (2004). Motivation concepts in 
behavioral neuroscience. Physiology and 
Behavior, 81, 179-209. 


Berrios, G. E. (1996). The history of mental symp- 
toms: Descriptive psychopathology since the 19th 
century. Cambridge: Cambridge University 
Press. 


Bertakis, K. (2009). The influence of gender on 
the doctor-patient interaction. Patient 
Education and Counseling, 76, 356-360. 


Bertakis, K., Franks, P., & Epstein, R. (2009). 
Patient-centered communication in primary 
care: Physician and patient gender and gen- 
der concordance. Journal of Women’s Health, 
18, 539-545. 


Berthoud, H. R. (2002). Multiple neural systems 
controlling food intake and body weight. 
Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 26, 
393-428. 


Betz, N. (2007). Career self-efficacy: Exemplary 
recent research and emerging directions. 
Journal of Career Assessment, 15, 403-422. 


Beyene, Y., Gilliss, C., & Lee, K. (2007). “I take the 
good with the bad, and I moisturize”: 
Defying middle age in the new millennium. 
Menopause, 14, 734-741. 


Bhar, S., Gelfand, L., Schmid, S., Gallop, R., 
DeRubeis, R., Hollon, S., et al. (2008). 
Sequence of improvement in depressive 
symptoms across cognitive therapy and 
pharmacotherapy. Journal of Affective 
Disorders, 110, 161-166. 


Bialystok, E., & Martin, M. M. (2004). Attention 
and inhibition in bilingual children: Evidence 
from the dimensional change card sort task. 
Developmental Science, 7, 325-339. 


Bianchi, S. M., & Casper, L. M. (2000). American 
families. Population Bulletin, 55(4). 


Billiard, M. (2008). Narcolepsy: Current treatment 
options and future approaches. Neuropsychiatric 
Disease and Treatment, 4, 557-566. 


Bindemann, M., Burton, A., Leuthold, H., & 
Schweinberger, S. (2008, July). Brain potential 
correlates of face recognition: Geometric dis- 
tortions and the N250r brain response to stim- 
ulus repetitions. Psychophysiology, 45, 535-544. 


Binet, A., & Simon, T. (1916). The development of 
intelligence in children (The Binet-Simon Scale). 
Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins. 


R-6 References 


Bingenheimer, J. B., Brennan, R. T., & Earls, F. J. 
(2005, May 27). Firearm violence exposure 
and serious violent behavior. Science, 308, 
1323-1327. 


Binstock, R., & George, L. K. (Eds.). (1996). 
Handbook of aging and the social sciences (4th 
ed.). San Diego, CA: Academic Press. 


Birren, J. E. (Ed.). (1996). Encyclopedia of gerontol- 
ogy: Age, aging and the aged. San Diego, CA: 
Academic Press. 

Bishop, M. (2005). Quality of life and psychoso- 
cial adaptation to chronic illness and disabil- 
ity: Preliminary analysis of a conceptual and 
theoretical synthesis. Rehabilitation Counseling 
Bulletin, 48, 219-231. 


Bitterman, M. E. (2006). Classical conditioning 
since Pavlov. Review of General Psychology, 10, 
365-376. 


Bittles, A. H., Bower, C., & Hussain, R. (2007). 
The four ages of Down syndrome. European 
Journal of Public Health, 17, 121-225. 

Bizley, J., Walker, K., Silverman, B., King, A., & 
Schnupp, J. (2009, February). Interdependent 
encoding of pitch, timbre, and spatial location 
in auditory cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 
2064-2075. 


Bjorklund, D. F., & Ellis, B. J. (2005). Evolutionary 
psychology and child development: An emerging 
synthesis. New York: Guilford Press. 


Bjornstad, R. (2006). Learned helplessness, dis- 
couraged workers, and multiple unemploy- 


ment equilibria. The Journal of Socio-Economics, 
35, 458-475. 


Black, A. L. & McCafferty, D. (1998, July 3-5). The 
age of contentment. USA Weekend, pp. 4-6. 


Black, B. M., & Weisz, A. N. (2004). Dating vio- 
lence: A qualitative analysis of Mexican 
American youths’ views. Journal of Ethnic and 
Cultural Diversity in Social Work, 13, 69-90. 


Black, P. (2006). Thrust to wholeness: The nature 
of self-protection. Review of General Psychology, 
10, 191-209. 


Blair, C. A., Thompson, L. F., & Wuensch, K. L. 
(2005). Electronic helping behavior: The vir- 
tual presence of others makes a difference. 
Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 27, 171-178. 


Blakeslee, S. (1991, August 7). Levels of caffeine 
in various foods. The New York Times. 


Blakeslee, S. (1992, August 11). Finding a new 
messenger for the brain’s signals to the body. 
The New York Times, p. C3. 


Blass, T. (1996). Attribution of responsibility and 
trust in the Milgram obedience experiment. 
Journal of Applied Social Psychology, 26, 1529-1535. 


Blass, T. (2004). The man who shocked the world: The 
life and legacy of Stanley Milgram. New York: 
Basic Books. 


Blass, T. (2009). From New Haven to Santa Clara: 
A historical perspective on the Milgram obe- 
dience experiments. American Psychologist, 64, 
37-45. 


Blass, T. (Ed.) (2000). Obedience to authority: 
Current perspectives on the Milgram Paradigm. 
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 


Blatter, K., & Cajochen, C. (2007). Circadian 
rhythms in cognitive performance: 
Methodological constraints, protocols, theo- 
retical underpinnings. Physiology & Behavior, 
90, 196-208. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Blixen, C. E., Singh, A., & Xu, M. (2006). What 
women want: Understanding obesity and 
preferences for primary care weight reduction 
interventions among African-American and 
Caucasian women. Journal of the National 
Medical Association, 98, 1160-1170. 


Bloom, P. N., McBride, C. M., & Pollak, K. I. 
(2006). Recruiting teen smokers in shopping 
malls to a smoking-cessation program using 
the foot-in-the-door technique. Journal of 
Applied Social Psychology, 36, 1129-1144. 


Blum, D. (2002). Love at goon park: Harry Harlow 
and the science of affection. Cambridge, MA: 
Perseus. 


Boahen, K. (2005, May). Neuromorphic micro- 
chips. Scientific American, pp. 56-64. 


Boake, C. (2008, April). Clinical neuropsychology. 
Professional Psychology: Research and Practice, 
39, 234-239. 


Board of Educational Affairs. (2002). 2002 APA task 
force report on undergraduate student competen- 
cies. Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


Bode, C., de Ridder, D. T., Kuijer, R. G., & Bensing, 
J. M. (2007). Effects of an intervention promot- 
ing proactive coping competencies in middle 
and late adulthood. Gerontologist, 47, 42-51. 


Boden, J. M., Fergusson, D. M., & Horwood, L. J. 
(2007). Anxiety disorders and suicidal behav- 
iours in adolescence and young adulthood: 
Findings from a longitudinal study. Psychological 
Medicine, 37, 431—440. 


Bodin, G. (2006). Review of harvesting free 
association. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 75, 
629-632. 


Boehm, K. E., & Campbell, N. B. (1995). Suicide: 
A review of calls to an adolescent peer listen- 
ing phone service. Child Psychiatry and Human 
Development, 26, 61-66. 


Bogart, R. K., McDaniel, R. J., Dunn, W. J., 
Hunter, C., Peterson, A. L., & Write, E. E. 
(2007). Efficacy of group cognitive behavior 
therapy for the treatment of masticatory myo- 
fascial pain. Military Medicine, 172, 169-174. 


Bogenschutz, M. P., Geppert, C. M., & George, J. 
(2006). The role of twelve-step approaches in 
dual diagnosis treatment and recovery. 
American Journal of Addiction, 15, 50-60. 


Bohart, A. C. (2006). Understanding person- 
centered therapy: A review of Paul Wilkins’ 
person-centered therapy in focus. Person- 
Centered and Experiential Psychotherapies, 5, 
138-143. 


Bohn, A., & Berntsen, D. (2007). Pleasantness bias 
in flashbulb memories: Positive and negative 
flashbulb memories of the fall of the Berlin 
Wall among East and West Germans. Memory 
and Cognition, 35, 565-577. 


Boisvert, C. M., & Faust, D. (2003). Leading 
researchers’ consensus on psychotherapy 
research findings: Implications for the teach- 
ing and conduct of psychotherapy. Professional 
Psychology: Research and Practice, 34, 508-513. 

Boles, D. B. (2005). A large-sample study of sex 
differences in functional cerebral lateraliza- 
tion. Journal of Clinical and Experimental 
Neuropsychology, 27, 759-768. 

Bolger, N., & Amarel, D. (2007). Effects of social 
support visibility on adjustment to stress: 


Experimental evidence. Journal of Personality 
and Social Psychology, 92, 458-475. 


Boller, F. (2004). Rational basis of rehabilitation 
following cerebral lesions: A review of the 
concept of cerebral plasticity. Functional 
Neurology: New Trends in Adaptive and 
Behavioral Disorders, 19, 65-72. 


Bonanni, R., Pasqualetti, P., Caltagirone, C., & 
Carlesimo, G. (2007). Primacy and recency 
effects in immediate free recall of sequences 
of spatial positions. Perceptual and Motor Skills, 
105, 483-500. 


Bonanno, G.A. (2004). Loss, trauma, and human 
resilience: Have we underestimated the 
human capacity to thrive after extremely 
aversive events? American Psychologist, 59, 
20-28. 


Bond, J. T., Thompson, C., Galinsky, E., & Prottas, 
D. (2003). Highlights of the 2002 national survey 
of the changing workforce (No 3). New York: 
Families and Work Institute. 


Bond, M. (2006).Psychodynamic psychotherapy 
in the treatment of mood disorders. Current 
Opinion in Psychiatry, 19, 40-43. 


Bonham, V. L., Warshauer-Baker, E., & Collins, 
F. S. (2005). Race and ethnicity in the genome 
era: The complexity of the constructs. 
American Psychologist, 60, 9-15. 


Bonnardel, V. (2006). Color naming and categori- 
zation in inherited color vision deficiencies. 
Visual Neuroscience, 23, 637-643. 


Borbély, A. (1988). Secrets of sleep (p. 43, graph). 
New York: Basic Books. 


Bordnick, P. S., Elkins, R. L., Orr, T. E., Walters, 
P., & Thyer, B. A. (2004). Evaluating the 
relative effectiveness of three aversion thera- 
pies designed to reduce craving among 
cocaine abusers. Behavioral Interventions, 

19, 1-24. 


Borisenko, J. (2007). Fatherhood as a personality 
development factor in men. The Spanish 
Journal of Psychology, 10, 82-90. 


Bornstein, R. F. (2003). Psychodynamic models of 
personality. In T. Millon & M. J. Lerner (Eds.), 
Handbook of psychology: Personality and social 
psychology (Vol. 5). New York: Wiley. 


Bosma, H., van Boxtel, M. P. J., Ponds, R. W. H. M., 
Houx, P. J. H., & Jolles, J. (2003). Education 
and age-related cognitive decline: The contri- 
bution of mental workload. Educational 
Gerontology, 29, 165-173. 


Botvinick, M. (2004, August 6). Probing the neu- 
ral basis of body ownership. Science, 305, 
782-783. 


Bouchard, C., & Bray, G. A. (Eds.). (1996). Regu- 
lation of body weight: Biological and behavioral 
mechanisms. New York: Wiley. 


Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (2004). Genetic influence on 
human psychological traits: A survey. Current 
Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 148-151. 


Bouchard, T. J., Jr., Segal, N. L., Tellegen, A., 
McGue, M., Keyes, M. & Krueger, R. (2004). 
Genetic influence on social attitudes: 
Another challenge to psychology from 
behavior genetics. In L. F. DiLalla (Ed.), 
Behavior genetics principles: Perspectives in 
development, personality, and psychopathology. 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Bourne, L. E., & Dominowski, R. L. (1979). 
Cognitive processes. Upper Saddle River, NJ: 
Pearson Education. 


Bourne, L. E., Dominowski, R. L., Loftus, E. F., & 
Healy, A. F. (1986). Cognitive processes (2nd 
ed.). Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. 


Bower, G. H., Thompson, S. S., & Tulving, E. (1994). 
Reducing retroactive interference: An interfer- 
ence analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology 
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20, 51—66. 


Bower, J. M., & Parsons, L. M. (2003, August). 
Rethinking the “lesser brain.” Scientific 
American, pp. 51-57. 


Boxer, P., Huesmann, L., Bushman, B., O’Brien, 
M., & Moceri, D. (2009). The role of violent 
media preference in cumulative developmen- 
tal risk for violence and general aggression. 
Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38, 417—428. 


Boyce, W. T., & Ellis, B. J. (2005). Biological sensitiv- 
ity to context: An evolutionary-developmental 
theory of the origins and functions of stress 
reactivity. Development and Psychopathology, 17, 
271-301. 


Boyd-Wilson, B. M., McClure, J., & Walkey, F. H. 
(2004). Are well-being and illusory perceptions 
linked? The answer may be yes, but... . 
Australian Journal of Psychology, 56, 1-9. 


Boyle, G. J., Goldman, R., Svoboda, J. S., & 
Fernandez, E. (2002). Male circumcision: Pain, 
trauma and psychosexual sequelae. Journal of 
Health Psychology, 7, 329-343. 


Boyle, S. H., Williams, R. B., Mark, D. B., 
Brummett, B. H., Siegler, I. C., & Barefoot, 
J. C. (2005). Hostility, age, and mortality in a 
sample of cardiac patients. American Journal of 
Cardiology, 96, 64-72. 


Bozarth, J. D., Zimring, F. M., & Tausch, R. (2002). 
Client-centered therapy: The evolution of a 
revolution. In D. J. Cain (Ed.), Humanistic 
psychotherapies: Handbook of research and practice. 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


Brambilla, P., Cipriani, A., Hotopf, M., & Barbui, 
C. (2005). Side-effect profile of fluoxetine in 
comparison with other SSRIs, tricyclic and 
newer antidepressants: A meta-analysis of 
clinical trial data. Pharmacopsychiatry, 38, 69-77. 

Brasic, J. R. (2002). Conversion disorder in child- 
hood. German Journal of Psychiatry, 5, 54-61. 

Braun, A. R., Balkin, T. J., Wesensten, N. J., 
Gwadry, F., Carson, R. E., Varga, M., et al. 
(1998). Dissociated pattern of activity in 
visual cortices and their projections during 
human rapid eye movement sleep. Science, 
279, pp. 91-95. 

Brausch, A. M. & Gutierrez, P. M. (2009). 
Differences in non-suicidal self-injury and 
suicide attempts in adolescents. Journal of 
Youth and Adolescence, 21, 46-51. 

Brazelton, T. B. (1969). Infants and mothers: 
Differences in development. New York: Dell. 

Breland, K., & Breland, M. (1966). Animal behav- 
ior. New York: Macmillan. 

Brennen, T., Vikan, A., & Dybdahl, R. (2007). Are 
tip-of-the-tongue states universal? Evidence 
from the speakers of an unwritten language. 
Memory, 15, 167-176. 

Breslin, M. M. (2004, June 27). Boy, interrupted. 
Chicago Tribune, p. 12. 


Brewer, J. B., Zhao, Z., Desmond, J. E., Glover, 
G. H., & Gabrieli, J. D. E. (1998, August 21). 
Making memories: Brain activity that predicts 
how well visual experience will be remem- 
bered. Science, 281, 1185-1187. 


Brewer, M. B., & Hewstone, M. (Eds.). (2003). 
Social cognition. Malden, MA: Blackwell 
Publishers. 


Bright, P., Buckman, J., & Fradera, A. (2006). 
Retrograde amnesia in patients with hippo- 
campal, medial temporal, temporal lobe, or 
frontal pathology. Learning & Memory, 13, 
545-557. 


Brislin, R., Worthley, R., & MacNab, B. (2006). 
Cultural intelligence: Understanding behav- 
iors that serve people’s goals. Group & 
Organization Management, 31, 40-55. 


Brock, T. C., & Green, M. C. (Eds.). (2005). 
Persuasion: Psychological insights and perspec- 
tives (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage 
Publications. 


Bröder, A., & Schiffer, S. (2006). Stimulus format 
and working memory in fast and frugal strat- 
egy selection. Journal of Behavioral Decision 
Making, 19, 361-380. 


Brody, J. (2008, May 20). Trying to break nicotine’s 
grip. The New York Times, p. E9. 


Broidy, L. M., Nagin, D. S., & Tremblay, R. E. (2003). 
Developmental trajectories of childhood 
disruptive behaviors and adolescent delin- 
quency: A six-site, cross-national study. 
Developmental Psychology, 39, 222-245. 


Broman, C. L. (2005). Stress, race and substance 
use in college. College Student Journal, 39, 
340-352. 


Bronson, P., & Merryman, A. (2009). NurtureShock. 
New York: Twelve. 


Brooker, R. J., Widmaier, E. P., Graham, L., & 
Stiling, P. (2008). Biology. New York: 
McGraw-Hill. 


Brown, D. L. (2008, July 16). The impassive 
bystander. Someone is hurt, in need of com- 
passion. Is it human instinct to do nothing? 
Washington Post, C4. 


Brown, E. (2001, September 17). The World 
Health Organization takes on big tobacco (but 
don’t hold your breath): Anti-smoking 
advocates are mounting a global campaign: 
It’s going to be a long, hard fight. Forbes, 
pp. 37-41. 


Brown, J. (2006). Attribution: Theories, affect and 
evolution. Dissertation Abstracts International: 
Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 67(2-B), 
1201. 

Brown, L. S., & Pope, K. S. (1996). Recovered mem- 
ories of abuse: Assessment, therapy, forensics. 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 

Brown, P. K., & Wald, G. (1964). Visual pigments 
in single rod and cones of the human retina. 
Science, 144, 45-52. 


Brown, R. (1958). How shall a thing be called? 
Psychological Review, 65, 14-21. 

Brown, R. J. (2006). Different types of “dissocia- 
tion” have different psychological mecha- 
nisms. Journal of Trauma Dissociation, 6, 7-28. 


Brown, R., & Robertson, E. (2007). Off-line pro- 
cessing: Reciprocal interactions between 


References R-7 


declarative and procedural memories. The 
Journal of Neuroscience, 27(39), 10468-10475. 


Brown, S. I., & Walter, M. I. (Eds.). (1993). Problem 
posing: Reflections and applications. Hillsdale, 
NJ: Erlbaum. 


Brown, S., & Martinez, M. J. (2007). Activation of 
premotor vocal areas during musical discrim- 
ination. Brain and Cognition, 63, 9-69. 


Brown, S., Martinez, M. J., & Parson, L. M. (2006). 
Music and language side by side in the brain: 
A PET study of the generation of melodies 
and sentences. European Journal of Neuroscience, 
23, 2791-2803. 


Brownlee, K. (2007). What works for whom? A 
critical review of psychotherapy research. 
Psychiatric Rehabilitation Journal, 30, 239-240. 


Bruce, V., Green, P. R., & Georgeson, M. (1997). 
Visual perception: Physiology, psychology and 
ecology (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 


Bruggeman, H., Yonas, A., & Konczak, J. (2007). 
The processing of linear perspective and bin- 
ocular information for action and perception. 
Neuropsychologia, 45, 1420-1426. 


Brunet, A., Orr, S., Tremblay, J., Robertson, K., 
Nader, K., & Pitman, R. (2008). Effect of post- 
retrieval propranolol on psychophysiologic 
responding during subsequent script-driven 
traumatic imagery in post-traumatic stress. 
Journal of Psychiatric Research, 42, 503-506. 


Brunet, A., Orr, S. P., Tremblay, J., Robertson, K., 
Nader, K., & Pitman, R. K. (2007). Effect of 
post-retrieval propranolol on psychophysio- 
logic responding during subsequent 
script-driven traumatic imagery in post- 
traumatic stress disorder. Journal of Psychiatric 
Research, 22, 301-315. 


Bryant, R. M., Coker, A. D., Durodoye, B. A., 
McCollum, V. J., Pack-Brown, S. P., Constantine, 
M. G., & O'Bryant, B. J. (2005). Having our say: 
African American women, diversity, and 
counseling. Journal of Counseling and 
Development, 83, 313-319. 


Brydon, L., Edwards, S., Mohamed-Ali, V., & 
Steptoe, A. (2004). Socioeconomic status and 
stress-induced increases in interleukin-6. 
Brain, Behavior, and Immunity, 18, 281-290. 


Brzustowicz, L. M., Hodgkinson, K. A., Chow, 
E. W. C., Honer, W. G., & Bassett, A. S. (2000, 
April 28). Location of major susceptibility 
locus for familial schizophrenia on chromo- 
some 1q21-q22. Science, 288, 678—682. 


Buchanan, R. W, Javitt, D. C., Marder, S. R., 
Schooler, N. R., Gold, J. M., McMahon, R. P., 
et al. (2007). The Cognitive and Negative 
Symptoms in Schizophrenia Trial (CONSIST): 
The efficacy of glutamatergic agents for 
negative symptoms and cognitive impair- 
ments. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 
1593-1602. 


Buchanan, T., & Selmon, N. (2008). Race and gen- 
der differences in self-efficacy: Assessing the 
role of gender role attitudes and family back- 
ground. Sex Roles, 58, 822-836. 


Buchanan, T. W., & Adolphs, R. (2004). The neu- 
roanatomy of emotional memory in humans. 
In D. Reisberg & P. Hertel (Eds.), Memory and 
emotion. London: Oxford University Press. 

Buchert, R., Thomasius, R., Wilke, F., Petersen, K., 
Nebeling, B., Obrocki, J., Schulze, O., 


R-8 References 


Schmidt, U., & Clausen, M. (2004). A voxel- 
based PET investigation of the long-term 
effects of “ecstasy” consumption on brain 
serotonin transporters. American Journal of 
Psychiatry, 161, 1181-1189. 


Buehner, M., Krumm, S., & Ziegler, M. (2006). 
Cognitive abilities and their interplay: 
Reasoning, crystallized intelligence, working 
memory components, and sustained atten- 
tion. Journal of Individual Differences, 27, 57-72. 


Bukobza, G. (2009). Relations between rebellious- 
ness, risk-taking behavior, and identity status 
during emerging adulthood. Identity, 9, 
159-177. 


Bulik, C. M., Tozzi, F., Anderson, C., Mazzeo, S. E., 
Aggen, S., & Sullivan, P. F. (2003). The relation 
between eating disorders and components of 
perfectionism. American Journal of Psychiatry, 
160, 366-368. 


Bunge, S. A., & Wallis, J. D. (2008). Neuroscience 
of rule-guided behavior. New York: Oxford 
University Press. 


Bunting, M. (2006). Proactive interference and 
item similarity in working memory. Journal of 
Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and 
Cognition, 32, 183-196. 


Burbach, M. E., Matkin, G. S., & Fritz, S. M. (2004). 
Teaching critical thinking in an introductory 
leadership course utilizing active learning 
strategies: A confirmatory study. College 
Student Journal, 38, 482—493. 


Burchinal, M. R., Roberts, J. E., & Riggins, R., Jr. 
(2000). Relating quality of center-based child 
care to early cognitive and language develop- 
ment longitudinally. Child Development, 71, 
338-357. 


Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2007). American time 
use survey. Washington, DC: Bureau of Labor 
Statistics. 


Burger, J. M. (2009). Replicating Milgram: Would 
people still obey today? American Psychologist, 
64, 1-11. 

Burger, J. M., & Caldwell, D. F. (2003). The effects 
of monetary incentives and labeling on the 
foot-in-the-door effect: Evidence for a self- 


perception process. Basic and Applied Social 
Psychology, 25, 235-241. 


Burger, J. M., Reed, M., & DeCesare, K. (1999). 
The effects of initial request size on compli- 
ance: More about the that’s-not-all technique. 
Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 21, 243-249. 


Burgoon, J. K., & Bacue, A. E. (2003). Nonverbal 
communication skills. In J. O. Greene & B. R. 
Burleson (Eds.), Handbook of communication 
and social interaction skills. Mahwah, NJ: 
Lawrence Erlbaum. 


Burgoon, J. K., Bonito, J. A., Ramirez, A. J. R., 
Dunbar, N. E., Kam, K., & Fischer, J. (2002). 
Testing the interactivity principle: Effects of 
mediation, propinquity, and verbal and non- 
verbal modalities in interpersonal interaction 
[Special Issue: Research on the relationship 
between verbal and nonverbal communica- 
tion: Emerging integrations]. Journal of 
Communication, 52, 657—677. 


Burke, C., & McDaniel, J. (2001). A special kind of 
hero. Lincoln, NE: Doubleday. 


Burns, J. W., Quartana, P. J., & Bruehl, S. (2007). 
Anger management style moderates effects of 


emotion suppression during initial stress on 
pain and cardiovascular responses during 
subsequent pain-induction. Annals of Behavioral 
Medicine, 34, 154-165. 


Burns, N. R., Bryan, J., & Nettelbeck, T. (2006). 
Ginkgo biloba: No robust effect on cognitive 
abilities or mood in healthy young or older 
adults. Human Psychopharmacology: Clinical 
and Experimental, 21, 27-37. 


Busey, T. A., & Loftus, G. R. (2007). Cognitive 
science and the law. Trends in Cognitive 
Science, 11, 111-117. 


Bush, J. (2008). Viability of virtual reality exposure 
therapy as a treatment alternative. Computers 
in Human Behavior, 24, 1032-1040. 


Bushman, B. J., & Anderson, C. (2001). Media 
violence and the American public: Scientific 
facts versus media misinformation. American 
Psychologist, 56, 477-489. 


Bushman, B. J., Wang, M. C., & Anderson, C. 
(2005). Is the curve relating temperature to 
aggression linear or curvilinear? Assaults 
and temperature in Minneapolis reexamined. 
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89, 
62-66. 


Buss, D. (2003). Evolutionary psychology. Boston: 
Allyn & Bacon. 


Buss, D. (2009). How can evolutionary psychol- 
ogy successfully explain personality and 
individual differences? Perspectives on 
Psychological Science, 4, 359-366. 


Buss, D. M. (2001). Human nature and culture: 
An evolutionary psychological perspective. 
Journal of Personality, 69, 955-978. 


Buss, D. M. (2003a). The evolution of desire: Strategies 
of human mating. New York: Basic Books. 


Buss, D. M. (2004). Sex differences in human 
mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses 
tested in 37 cultures. In H. T. Reis & C. E. 
Rusbult (Eds.), Close relationships: Key readings. 
Philadelphia: Taylor & Francis. 


Buss, D. M., Abbott, M., & Angleitner, A. (1990). 
International preferences in selecting mates: A 
study of 37 cultures. Journal of Cross-Cultural 
Psychology, 21, 5-47. 


Buss, D. M., Larsen, R. J., Westen, D., & Semmelroth, 
J. (1992). Sex differences in jealousy: Evolution, 
physiology, and psychology. Psychological 
Science, 3, 251-255. 

Butcher, J. N. (2005). A beginner's guide to the 
MMPI-2 (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American 
Psychological Association. 


Butler, A. C., Chapman, J. E., Forman, E. M., & 
Beck, A. T. (2006). The empirical status of 
cognitive-behavioral therapy: A review of meta- 
analyses. Clinical Psychology Review, 26, 17-31. 


Butler, L. D., Koopman, C., Neri, E., Giese-Davis, 
J., Palesh, O., Thorne-Yocam, K. A., et al. (2009). 
Effects of supportive-expressive group ther- 
apy on pain in women with metastatic breast 
cancer. Health Psychology, 28, 579-587. 


Butler, L. T., & Berry, D. C. (2004). Understanding 
the relationship between repetition priming 
and mere exposure. British Journal of Psychology, 
95, 467-487. 

Buzi, R., Weinman, M., & Smith, P. (2007). The 
relationship between adolescent depression 


and a history of sexual abuse. Adolescence, 
42(168), 679-688. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Byne, W. (1996). Biology and homosexuality: 
Implications of neuroendocrinological and 
neuroanatomical studies. In R. P. Cabaj & T. S. 
Stein (Eds.), Textbook of homosexuality and 
mental health. Washington, DC: American 
Psychiatric Press. 


Byrne, B. M., & Watkins, D. (2003). The issue of 
measurement invariance revisited. Journal of 
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 34, 155-175. 


Byrne-Davis, L., & Vedhara, K. (2008). Psycho- 
neuroimmunology. Social and Personality 
Psychology Compass, 2, 751-764. 


Cabanac, M., & Frankham, P. (2002). Evidence 
that transient nicotine lowers the body 
weight set point. Physiology & Behavior, 76, 
539-542. 


Cabioglu, M., Ergene, N., & Tan, U. (2007, May). 
Smoking cessation after acupuncture treat- 
ment. International Journal of Neuroscience, 117, 
571-578. 


Cachelin, F. M., & Regan, P. C. (2006). Prevalence 
and correlates of chronic dieting in a multi- 
ethnic U.S. community sample. Eating and 
Weight Disorders, 11, 91-99. 


Cacioppo, J. T., Berntson, G. G., & Crites, S. L., Jr. 
(1996). Social neuroscience: Principles of psy- 
chophysiological arousal and response. In 
E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski (Eds.), Social 
psychology: Handbook of basic principles. New 
York: Guilford. 


Cacioppo, J. T., & Decety, J. (2009). What are the 
brain mechanisms on which psychological 
processes are based? Perspectives on 
Psychological Science, 4, 10-18. 


Cacioppo, J. T., Visser, P. S., & Pickett, C. L. 
(2005). Social neuroscience: People thinking about 
thinking people. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 


Cadenhead, K. & Braff, D. L. (1995). Neurophy- 
siology of schizophrenia: Attention, informa- 
tion processing, and inhibitory processes in 
schizophrenia. In J. A. Den Boer., H. G. M. 
Westenberg & H. M. van Praag (Eds.), 
Advances in the neurobiology of schizophrenia. 
Oxford, England: John Wiley & Sons. 


Caelian, C. F. (2006).The role of perfectionism 
and stress in the suicidal behaviour of 
depressed adolescents. Dissertation Abstracts 
International: Section B: The Sciences and 
Engineering, 66(12-B), 6915. 


Cahill, L. (2005, May). His brain, her brain. 
Scientific American, pp. 40—47. 


Cai, D., Shuman, T., Gorman, M., Sage, J., & 
Anagnostaras, S. (2009). Sleep selectively 
enhances hippocampus-dependent memory 
in mice. Behavioral Neuroscience, 123, 713-719. 

Cain, D. J. (Ed.). (2002). Humanistic psychotherapies: 
Handbook of research and practice. Washington, 
DC: American Psychological Association. 

Calin-Jageman, R. J., & Fischer, T. M. (2007). 
Behavioral adaptation of the aplysia siphon- 
withdrawal response is accompanied by sen- 
sory adaptation. Behavioral Neuroscience, 121, 
200-211. 


Cameron, O. G. (2002). Visceral sensory neuroscience: 
Interoception. London: Oxford University Press. 


Campbell, R. (2008). The psychological impact 
of rape victims. American Psychologist, 63, 
702-717. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Campbell, T. S., & Ditto, B. (2002). Exaggeration 
of blood pressure—related hypoalgesia and 
reduction of blood pressure with low fre- 
quency transcutaneous electrical nerve stimu- 
lation. Psychophysiology, 39, 473-481. 


Cannon, W. B. (1929). Organization for physio- 
logical homeostatics. Physiological Review, 9, 
280-289. 


Cantwell, R. H., & Andrews, B. (2002). Cognitive 
and psychological factors underlying second- 
ary school students’ feelings towards group 
work. Educational Psychology, 22, 75-91. 


Capaldi, E. D. (Ed.). (1996). Why we eat what we 
eat: The psychology of eating. Washington, DC: 
American Psychological Association. 


Caplan, D., Waters, G., & Dede, G. (2007). A 
study of syntactic processing in aphasia I: 
Behavioral (psycholinguistic) aspects. Brain 
and Language, 101, 103-150. 


Cardemil, E. V., Pinedo, T. M., & Miller, I. W. 
(2005). Developing a culturally appropriate 
depression prevention program: The family 
coping skills program. Cultural Diversity and 
Ethnic Minority Psychology, 11, 99-112. 


Carey, B. (2004, December 21). When pressure is on, 
good students suffer. The New York Times, p. D7. 


Carey, B. (2008, May 29). Monkeys think, moving 
artificial arm as own. The New York Times, 
p- Al. 


Carey, B. (2009a, April 6). So you just want to for- 
get? Science is working on an eraser. The New 
York Times, p. A1. 


Carey, B. (2009b, July 28). In battle, hunches 
prove to be valuable assets. The New York 
Times, p. Al. 


Carey, B. (2009c, November 27). Surgery for men- 
tal ills offers hope and risk. The New York 
Times, p. A1 


Carhart-Harris, R., (2007). Speed > Ecstasy > 
Ritalin: The science of amphetamines. Journal 
of Psychopharmacology, 21, 225. 


Carnagey, N., Anderson, C., & Bartholow, B. 
(2007). Media violence and social neurosci- 
ence: New questions and new opportunities. 
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 
178-182. 


Carnagey, N., Anderson, C. A., & Bushman, B. J. 
(2007). The effect of video game violence on 
physiological desensitization to real-life vio- 
lence. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 
43, 489—496. 


Carney, D., Colvin, C., & Hall, J. (2007). A thin 
slice perspective on the accuracy of first 
impressions. Journal of Research in Personality, 
41, 1054-1072. 


Carney, R. N., & Levin, J. R. (1998). Coming to 
terms with the keyword method in introduc- 
tory psychology: A “neuromnemonic” exam- 
ple. Teaching of Psychology, 25, 132-135. 

Carney, R. N., & Levin, J. R. (2003). Promoting 
higher-order learning benefits by building 
lower-order mnemonic connections. Applied 
Cognitive Psychology, 17, 563-575. 

Carpenter, S. (2002, April). What can resolve the 
paradox of mental health disparities? APA 
Monitor, 33, 18. 

Carr, A. (2002). Avoiding risky sex in adolescence. 
New York: Blackwell. 


Carr, P., & Steele, C. (2009). Stereotype threat and 
inflexible perseverance in problem solving. 
Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 45, 
853-859. 


Carrillo, M., Ricci, L., Coppersmith, G., & 
Melloni, R. (2009, August). The effect of 
increased serotonergic neurotransmission on 
aggression: A critical meta-analytical review 
of preclinical studies. Psychopharmacology, 205, 
349-368. 


Carter, R. T. (2003). Becoming racially and cultur- 
ally competent: The racial-cultural counseling 
laboratory. Journal of Multicultural Counseling 
and Development, 31, 20-30. 


Cartwright, R. (2006). A neuroscientist looks at 
how the brain makes up our minds. 
PsycCRITIQUES, 51, 35—41. 


Cartwright, R., Agargum, M. Y., & Kirkby, J. 
(2006). Relation of dreams to waking con- 
cerns. Psychiatry Research, 141, 261-270. 


Caruso, E. (2008). Use of experienced retrieval 
ease in self and social judgments. Journal of 
Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 148-155. 


Carvalho, F. M., Pereira, S. R. C., Pires, R. G. W., 
Ferraz, V. P., Romano-Silva, M. A., Oliveira- 
Silva, I. F., & Ribeiro, A. M. (2006). Thiamine 
deficiency decreases glutamate uptake in the 
prefrontal cortex and impairs spatial memory 
performance in a water maze test. Pharma- 
cology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 83, 481-489. 


Carver, C., & Scheier, M. (2002). Coping processes 
and adjustment to chronic illness. In A. 
Christensen & M. Antoni (Eds.), Chronic 
physical disorders: Behavioral medicine's perspec- 
tive. Malden: Blackwell Publishers. 


Cary, P. (2007). A brief history of the concept of 
free will: Issues that are and are not germane 
to legal reasoning. Behavioral Sciences & the 
Law, 25, Special issue: Free will, 165-181. 


Casad, B. J. (2007). Gender stereotype violation 
among job applicants: An examination of the- 
ory and measurement. Dissertation Abstracts 
International: Section B: The Sciences and 
Engineering, 67(7-B), 4154. 

Casasanto, D. (2008). Who’s afraid of the big bad 
whorf? Crosslinguistic differences in tempo- 
ral language and thought. Language Learning, 
58, 63-79. 


Case, R., & Okamoto, Y. (1996). The role of central 
conceptual structures in the development of 
children’s thought. Monographs of the Society 
for Research in Child Development, 61, v-265. 


Casey, S. D., Cooper-Brown, L. J., & Wacher, D. P. 
(2006). The use of descriptive analysis to iden- 
tify and manipulate schedules of reinforce- 
ment in the treatment of food refusal. Journal 
of Behavioral Education, 15, 41-52. 


Cashon, C. H., & Cohen, L. B. (2004). Beyond 
U-shaped development in infants’ process- 
ing of faces: An information-processing 
account. Journal of Cognition and Development, 
5, 59-80. 

Cassells, J. V. S. (2007). The virtuous roles of truth 
and justice in integral dialogue: Research, 
theory, and model practice of the evolution of 
collective consciousness. Dissertation Abstracts 
International Section A: Humanities and Social 
Sciences, 67(10-A), 4005. 


Catalano, S. M. (2006, September). Criminal 
victimization, 2005. Bureau of Justice Statistics 


References R-9 


Bulletin. Washington, DC: Bureau of Justice 
Statistics. 


Cattell, R. B., Cattell, A. K., & Catell, H. E. P. (1993). 
Sixteen personality factor questionnaire (16PF) 
(5th ed.). San Antonio, TX: Harcourt Brace. 


Cattell, R. B., Cattell, A. K., & Cattell, H. E. P. (2000). 
The sixteen personality factor™ (16PF®°) 
questionnaire. Champaign, IL: Institute for 
Personality and Ability Testing. 


Cauce, A. M. (2007). Bringing community psy- 
chology home: The leadership, community 
and values initiative. American Journal of 
Community Psychology, 39, 1-11. 


Cavallini, E., Pagnin, A., & Vecchi, T. (2003). 
Aging and everyday memory: The beneficial 
effect of memory training. Archives of Geronto- 
logy & Geriatrics, 37, 241-257. 


Cavenett, T., & Nixon, R. D. V. (2006). The effect 
of arousal on memory for emotionally-relevant 
information: A study of skydivers. Behaviour 
Research and Therapy, 44, 1461-1469. 


Centers for Disease Control and Prevention 
(CDC). (2000). Suicide prevention fact sheet. 
Atlanta: National Center for Injury Prevention 
and Control. 


Centers for Disease Control and Prevention 
(CDC). (2004a). Chlamydia—CDC fact sheet. 
Washington, DC. 


Centers for Disease Control and Prevention 
(CDC). (2004b, June 11). Suicide and 
attempted suicide. MMWR, 53, 471. 


Centers for Disease Control and Prevention 
(2008). STD surveillance tables. Atlanta: Centers 
for Disease Control and Prevention. 


Chamberlain, K., & Zika, S. (1990). The minor 
events approach to stress: Support for the use 
of daily hassles. British Journal of Psychology, 
81, 469-481. 


Chamberlain, S. R., Menzies, L., Hampshire, A., 
Suckling, J., Fineberg, N. A., del Campo, N., et 
al. (2008, July 18). Orbitofrontal dysfunction 
in patients with obsessive-compulsive disor- 
der and their unaffected relatives. Science, 321, 
421-422. 


Chambless, D. L., Crits-Christoph, P., Wampold, 
B. E., Norcross, J. C., Lambert, M. J., Bohart, 
A. C, et al. (2006). What should be validated? 
In J. C. Norcross, L. E. Beutler, & R. F. Levant, 
(Eds.). Evidence-based practices in mental health: 
Debate and dialogue on the fundamental questions. 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


Chandra, P. (2007). Review of Language, mind, 
and brain: Some psychological and neurologi- 
cal constraints on theories of grammar. 
Cognitive Systems Research, 8, 53-56. 


Chandran, S., & Menon, G. (2004). When a day 
means more than a year: Effects of temporal 
framing on judgments of health risk. Journal of 
Consumer Research, 31, 375-389. 


Chang, J., & Sue, S. (2005). Culturally sensitive 
research: Where have we gone wrong and 
what do we need to do now? In M. G. 
Constantine, Strategies for building multicul- 
tural competence in mental health and educational 
settings. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. 


Chang, L., & Geary, D. C. (2007). The future of 
psychology: Evolutionary approach to scien- 
tific psychology [Special issue: Evolutionary 


R-10 References 


psychology]. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 39, 
381-382. 


Chang, S. W., & Ansley, T. N. (2003). A compara- 
tive study of item exposure control methods 
in computerized adaptive testing. Journal of 
Educational Measurement, 40, 71-103. 

Chapkis, W., & Webb, R. (2008). Dying to get high: 
Marijuana as medicine. New York: New York 
University Press. 


Chapman, J. (2006). Anxiety and defective deci- 
sion making: An elaboration of the group-think 
model. Management Decision, 44, 1391-1404. 


Chapman, L. J., & Chapman, J. P. (1973). Disordered 
thought in schizophrenia. New York: Appleton- 
Century-Crofts. 


Charlton, S. (2009, January). Driving while con- 
versing: Cell phones that distract and passen- 
gers who react. Accident Analysis & Prevention, 
41, 160-173. 


Charman, D. P. (2004). Core processes in brief psy- 
chodynamic psychotherapy: Advancing effective 
practice. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum 
Associates. 


Cheakalos, C., & Heyn, E. (1998, November 2). 
Mercy mission. People Weekly, pp. 149-150. 


Chechile, R. A. (2003). Review of ‘Elements of 
Psychophysical Theory.’ Journal of Mathematical 
Psychology, 47, 385. 


Chen, A., Zhou, Y., & Gong, H. (2004). Firing rates 
and dynamic correlated activities of ganglion 
cells both contribute to retinal information pro- 
cessing. Brain Research, 1017, 13-20. 


Chen, C., & Stevenson, H. W. (1995). Motivation 
and mathematics achievement: A compara- 
tive study of Asian-American, Caucasian- 
American, and East Asian high school 
students. Child Development, 66, 1215-1234. 


Cheney, C. D. (1996). Medical non adherence: A 
behavior analysis. In J. R. Cautela & W. Ishaq 
(Eds.), Contemporary issues in behavior therapy: 
Improving the human condition: Applied Clinical 
Psychology. New York: Plenum Press. 


Cheng, C., & Cheung, M. L. (2005). Cognitive 
processes underlying coping flexibility: 
Differentiation and integration. Journal of 
Personality, 73, 859-886. 


Cheng, S., & Kwan, K. (2008). Attachment dimen- 
sions and contingencies of self-worth: The 
moderating role of culture. Personality and 
Individual Differences, 45, 509-514. 


Cheston, S. E. (2000). A new paradigm for teach- 
ing counseling theory and practice. Counselor 
Education & Supervision, 39, 254-269. 

Chiodo, D., Wolfe, D., Crooks, C., Hughes, R., & 
Jaffe, P. (2009). Impact of sexual harassment vic- 
timization by peers on subsequent adolescent 
victimization and adjustment: A longitudinal 
study. Journal of Adolescent Health, 45, 246-252. 

Cho, A. (2000, June 16). What’s shakin’ in the ear? 
Science, 288, 1954-1955. 

Cho, S., Holyoak, K. J., & Cannon, T. D. (2007). 
Analogical reasoning in working memory: 
Resources shared among relational integra- 
tion, interference resolution, and mainte- 
nance. Memory & Cognition, 35, 1445-1455. 

Chomsky, N. (1968). Language and mind. New 
York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. 

Chomsky, N. (1978). On the biological basis 
of language capacities. In G. A. Miller & 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


E. Lennenberg (Eds.), Psychology and biology 
of language and thought. New York: Academic 
Press. 


Chomsky, N. (1991). Linguistics and cognitive 
science: Problems and mysteries. In A. Kasher 
(Ed.), The Chomskyan turn. Cambridge, MA: 
Blackwell. 


Chou, K. (2005). Everyday competence and 
depressive symptoms: Social support and 
sense of control as mediators or moderators? 
Aging and Mental Health, 9, 177-183. 

Choy, Y., Fyer, A. J., & Lipsitz, J. D. (2007). Treat- 
ment of specific phobia in adults. Clinical 
Psychology Review, 27, 266-286. 


Christ, S. E., Steiner, R. D., & Grange, D. K. (2006). 
Inhibitory control in children with phenylke- 
tonuria. Developmental Neuropsychology, 30, 
845-864. 


Christakis, A., & Fowler, J. (2007). The spread of 
obesity in a large social network over 32 
years. The New England Journal of Medicine, 
357, 370-379. 


Christakis, N. A., & Fowler, J. H. (2008). The col- 
lective dynamics of smoking in a large social 
network. The New England Journal of Medicine, 
358, 2249-2258. 


Christensen, A. J., & Johnson, J. A. (2002). Patient 
adherence with medical treatment regimens: 
An interactive approach. Current Directions in 
Psychological Science, 11, 94-101. 


Christian, C. J., Lencz, T., Robinson, D. G., Burdick, 
K. E., Ashtari, M., Malhotra, A. K., et al. (2008). 
Gray matter structural alterations in obsessive- 
compulsive disorder: Relationship to neuropsy- 
chological functions. Neuroimaging, 164, 
123-131. 


Chronicle, E. P., MacGregor, J. N., & Ormerod, 
T. C. (2004). What makes an insight problem? 
The roles of heuristics, goal conception, and 
solution recoding in knowledge-lean prob- 
lems. Journal of Experimental Psychology: 
Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 30, 14-27. 


Chrysikou, E. G. (2006). When a shoe becomes 
a hammer: Problem solving as goal-derived, 
ad hoc categorization. Dissertation Abstracts 
International: Section B: The Sciences and Engi- 
neering, 67(1-B), 569. 


Cialdini, R. B. (2006). Influence: The psychology of 
persuasion. New York: Collins. 


Cialdini, R. B., & Sagarin, B. J. (2005). Principles 
of interpersonal influence. In T. C. Brock & 
M. C. Green (Eds.), Persuasion: Psychological 
insights and perspectives (2nd ed.). Thousand 
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. 


Cialdini, R. B., Schaller, M., Houlihan, D., Arps, 
K., Fultz, J., & Beaman, A. L. (1975). Reciprocal 
concessions procedure for inducing compli- 
ance: The door-in-the-face technique. Journal of 
Personality and Social Psychology, 31, 206-215. 


Clark, D. A. (2004). Cognitive-behavioral therapy for 
OCD. New York: Guilford. 


Clark, D. A. (2007). Obsessions and compulsions. 
In N. Kazantzis & L. L’Abate, Handbook of home- 
work assignments in psychotherapy: Research, prac- 
tice, prevention. New York: Springer Science + 
Business Media. 


Clark, L., Bechara, A., Damasio, H., Sahakian, B., 


Robbins, T., & Aitken, M. (2008). Differential 
effects of insular and ventromedial prefrontal 


cortex lesions on risky decision-making. 
Brain: A Journal of Neurology, 131, 
1311-1322. 


Clarke-Stewart, K. A., Vandell, D. L., McCartney, 
K., Owen, M. T., & Booth, C. (2000). Effects of 
parental separation and divorce on very 
young children. Journal of Family Psychology, 
14, 304-326. 


Clarkin, J. F., & Lenzenweger, M. F. (Eds.) (2004). 
Major theories of personality disorders (2nd ed.). 
New York: Guilford. 


Clay D. L. (2000). Commentary: Rethinking our 
interventions in pediatric chronic pain and 
treatment research. Journal of Pediatric 
Psychology, 25, 53-55. 


Clayton, K., & Lundberg-Love, P. (2009). Caffeine: 
Pharmacology and effects of the world’s most 
popular drug. The Praeger international collec- 
tion on addictions, Vol 2: Psychobiological profiles. 
Santa Barbara, CA: Praeger / ABC-CLIO. 


Cleary, A. M. (2006). Relating familiarity-based 
recognition and the tip-of-the-tongue phe- 
nomenon: Detecting a word’s recency in the 
absence of access to the word. Memory & 
Cognition, 34, 804-816. 


Clements, A. M., Rimrodt, S. L., & Abel, J. R. 
(2006). Sex differences in cerebral laterality of 
language and visuospatial processing. Brain 
and Language, 98, 150-158. 


Clements, M. (1994, August 7). Making love, how 
old, how often. Parade, p. 18. 


Clemons, T. L. (2006). Underachieving gifted stu- 
dents: A social cognitive model. Dissertation 
Abstracts International Section A: Humanities and 
Social Sciences, 66(9-A), 3208. 


Cloud, J. (2000, June 5). The lure of ecstasy. Time, 
pp. 60-68. 


Cloud, J. (2009, January 19). Minds on the Edge. 
Time, pp. 40—46. 


Cloud, J. (2009, June 22). Staying sane may be 
easier than you think. Time, p. 72. 


Coan, J. A., Schaefer, H. S., & Davidson, R. J. (2006). 
Lending a hand: Social regulation of the neu- 
ral response to threat. Psychological Science, 
17(12), 1032-1039. 


Coates, S. L., Butler, L. T., & Berry, D. C. (2006). 
Implicit memory and consumer choice: The 
mediating role of brand familiarity. Applied 
Cognitive Psychology, 20, 1101-1116. 


Coats, E. J., & Feldman, R. S. (1996). Gender 
differences in nonverbal correlates of social 
status. Personality and Social Psychology 
Bulletin, 22, 1014-1022. 


Cobos, P., Sanchez, M., Garcia, C., Vera, M. N., & 
Vila, J. (2002). Revisiting the James versus 
Cannon debate on emotion: Startle and auto- 
nomic modulation in patients with spinal cord 
injuries. Biological Psychology, 61, 251-269. 


Cochran, S. D. (2000). Emerging issues in 
research on lesbians’ and gay men’s mental 
health: Does sexual orientation really matter? 
American Psychologist, 56, 33-41. 


Coffman, S. J., Martell, C. R., Dimidjian, S., Gallop, 
R., & Holon, S. D. (2007). Extreme non- 
response in cognitive therapy: Can behav- 
ioral activation succeed where cognitive 
therapy fails? Journal of Consulting Clinical 
Psychology, 75, 531-545. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Cohen, B. H. (2002). Explaining psychological sta- 
tistics (2nd ed.). New York: Wiley. 


Cohen, G. L., Garcia, J., Apfel, N., & Master, A. 
(2006). Reducing the racial achievement gap: 
A social-psychological intervention. Science, 
313, 1307-1310. 


Cohen, J. (2003). Things I have learned (so far). 
In A. E. Kazdin (Ed.), Methodological issues 
and strategies in clinical research (2rd ed.). 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


Cohen, L., & Cashon, C. (2003). Infant perception 
and cognition. In R. Lerner & M. Easterbrooks 
(Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Developmental 
psychology (Vol. 6). New York: Wiley. 


Cohen, P. (2009). Medical marijuana: The conflict 
between scientific evidence and political 
ideology. Part one of two. Journal of Pain & 
Palliative Care Pharmacotherapy, 23, 4-25. 


Cohen, P., Slomkowski, C., & Robins, L. N. (Eds.). 
(1999). Historical and geographical influences on 
psychopathology. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 


Cohen, S. (2004, November). Social relationships 
and health. American Psychologist, 676-684. 


Cohen, S., Doyle, W. J., Turner, R., Alper, C. M., & 
Skoner, D. P. (2003). Sociability and suscepti- 
bility to the common cold. Psychological 
Science, 14, 389-395. 


Cohen, S., Hamrick, N., & Rodriguez, M. (2002). 
Reactivity and vulnerability to stress- 
associated risk for upper respiratory illness. 
Psychosomatic Medicine, 64, 302-310. 


Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., & Mermelstein, R. (1983). 
A global measure of perceived stress. Journal 
of Health and Social Behavior, 24, 385-396. 


Coleman, E. (2002). Masturbation as a means of 
achieving sexual health. Journal of Psychology 
and Human Sexuality, 14, 5-16. 


Coles, R. (1997). The moral intelligence of children. 
New York: Random House. 


Coles, R., & Stokes, G. (1985). Sex and the American 
teenager. New York: Harper & Row. 


Colland, V. T., Van Essen-Zandvliet, L. E. M., Lans, 
C., Denteneer, A., Westers, P., & Brackel, 
H. J. L. (2004). Poor adherence to self-medication 
instructions in children with asthma and their 
parents. Patient Education and Counseling, 55, 
416—421. 


Collins, A. M., & Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading- 
activation theory of semantic processing. 
Psychological Review, 82, 407—428. 


Collins, A. M., & Quillian, M. R. (1969). Retrieval 
times from semantic memory. Journal of Verbal 
Learning and Verbal Behavior, 8, 240-247. 

Colom, R., Haier, R. J., Head, K., Álvarez-Linera, 
J., Quiroga M. Á., Shih, P. C., et al. (2009). Gray 
matter correlates of fluid, crystallized, and 
spatial intelligence: Testing the P-FIT model. 
Intelligence, 37, 124-135. 

Colom, R., Jung, R. E., & Haier, R. J. (2006). Finding 
the g-factor in brain structure using the method 
of correlated vectors. Intelligence, 34, 561-570. 


Coltheart, M., Langdon, R., & McKay, R. (2007). 
Schizophrenia and monothematic delusions. 
Schizophrenia Bulletin, 33, 642—647. 

Coltraine, S., & Messineo, M. (2000). The perpetu- 
ation of subtle prejudice: Race and gender 


imagery in 1990s television advertising. Sex 
Roles, 42, 363-389. 


Colwell, M. J., & Lindsey, E. W. (2005). Preschool 
children’s pretend and physical play and sex 
of play partner: Connections to peer compe- 
tence. Sex Roles, 52, 497-509. 


Combrink-Graham, L., & McKenna, S. B. (2006). 
Families with children with disrupted attach- 
ments. In L. Combrink-Graham, Children in 
family contexts: Perspectives on treatment. New 
York: Guilford Press. 


Compagni, A., & Manderscheid, R. W. (2006). A 
neuroscientist-consumer alliance to transform 
mental health care, Journal of Behavioral Health 
Services & Research, 33, 265-274. 


Comuzzie, A. G., & Allison, D. B. (1998, May 29). 
The search for human obesity genes. Science, 
280, 1374-1377. 


Conduit, R., Crewther, S. G., & Coleman, G. (2004). 
Spontaneous eyelid movements (ELMS) dur- 
ing sleep are related to dream recall on awak- 
ening. Journal of Sleep Research, 13, 137-144. 


Conger, R., & Donnellan, M. (2007). An interac- 
tionist perspective on the socioeconomic con- 
text of human development. Annual Review of 
Psychology, 58, 175-199. 


Conger, R. D., Wallace, L. E., Sun, Y., Simons, R. L., 
McLoyd, V. C., & Brody, G. H. (2002). Economic 
pressure in African American families: A repli- 
cation and extension of the family stress model. 
Developmental Psychology, 38, 179-193. 


Conner, M., Povey, R., Sparks, P., James, R., & 
Shepherd, R. (2003). Moderating role of attitu- 
dinal ambivalence within the theory of 
planned behaviour. British Journal of Social 
Psychology, 42, 75-94. 


Connolly, A. C. (2007). Concepts and their fea- 
tures: Can cognitive science make good on 
the promises of concept empiricism? 
Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: 
The Sciences and Engineering, 67(7-B), 4125. 


Conte, H. R., Plutchik, R., & Draguns, J. G. (2004). 
The measurement of ego defenses in clinical 
research. In U. Hentschel, G. Smith, J. G. 
Draguns, & W. Ehlers (Eds.), Defense mecha- 
nisms: Theoretical, research and clinical perspec- 
tives. Oxford, England: Elsevier Science Ltd. 


Conway, M. A. (Ed.) (2002). Levels of processing 30 
years on special issue of memory. Hove, UK: 
Psychology Press. 


Cooke, J. R., & Ancoli-Israel, S. (2006). Sleep and 
its disorders in older adults. Psychiatric Clinics 
of North America, 29, 1077-1093. 


Cooklin, A. (2000). Therapy, the family and 
others. In H. Maxwell, Clinical psychotherapy 
for health professionals. Philadelphia: Whurr 
Publishers. 


Cooper, H., & Patall, E. (2009, June). The relative 
benefits of meta-analysis conducted with 
individual participant data versus aggregated 
data. Psychological Methods, 14, 165-176. 

Cooper, J. (2007). Cognitive dissonance: Fifty years 
of a classic theory. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage 
Publications. 

Cooper, J., Mirabile, R., & Scher, S. J. (2005). 
Actions and attitudes: The theory of cognitive 
dissonance. In T. C. Brock & M. C. Green 
(Eds.), Persuasion: Psychological insights and 


References R-11 


perspectives (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: 
Sage Publications. 


Cooper, J., & Strayer, D. (2008). Effects of simula- 
tor practice and real world experience on 
cell-phone-related driver distraction. Human 
Factors, 50, 893-902. 


Cooper, M. (2007). Humanizing psychotherapy. 
Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy, 37, 11-16. 


Cooper, Z., & Shafran, R. (2008). Cognitive 
behaviour therapy for eating disorders. 
Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 36, 
713-722. 


Cope, D. (2001). Virtual music. Cambridge, MA: 
MIT. 


Cope, D. (2003). Computer analysis of musical 
allusions. Computer Music Journal, 27, 11-28. 


Copeland, J. B., & Proudfoot, D. (2007). Artificial 
intelligence: History, foundations, and 
philosophical issues. In P. Thagard, Philosophy 
of psychology and cognitive science. Amsterdam, 
Netherlands: North Holland/Elsevier. 


Coplan, R., Reichel, M., & Rowan, K. (2009). 
Exploring the associations between maternal 
personality, child temperament, and parent- 
ing: A focus on emotions. Personality and 
Individual Differences, 46, 241-246. 


Copolov, D. L., Seal, M. L., Maruff, P., Ulusoy, R., 
Wong, M. T. H., TochonDanguy, et al. (2003). 
Cortical activation associated with the human 
experience of auditory hallucinations and 
perception of human speech in schizophrenia: 
A PET correlation study. Psychiatry Research: 
Neuroimaging, 123, 139-152. 


Cordnoldi, C., De Beni, R., & Helstrup, T. (2007). 
Memory sensitivity in autobiographical mem- 
ory. In S. Magnussen, & T. Helstrup, Everyday 
memory. New York: Psychology Press. 

Coren, S. (1992). The moon illusion: A different 
view through the legs. Perceptual and Motor 
Skills, 75, 827-831. 


Coren, S. (2004). Sensation and perception. In I. B. 
Weiner. Handbook of Psychology (Vol. 1). 
Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. 


Coren, S., & Ward, L. M. (1989). Sensation and per- 
ception (3rd ed.). San Diego, CA: Harcourt 
Brace Jovanovich. 


Cornelius, M. D., Taylor, P. M., Geva, D., & Day, 
N. L. (1995). Prenatal tobacco and marijuana 
use among adolescents: Effects on offspring 
gestational age, growth, and morphology. 
Pediatrics, 95, 57—68. 


Cornell, C. B. (2006). A graduated scale for deter- 
mining mental age. Dissertation Abstracts 
International: Section B: The Sciences and 
Engineering, 66(9-B), 5121. 

Corsello, A. (2005). The wronged man. In The best 
American magazine writing, 2005. New York: 
Columbia University Press. 

Cosmides, L., & Tooby, J. (2004). Social exchange: 
The evolutionary design of a neurocognitive 
system. In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), Cognitive 
neurosciences (3rd ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT. 

Costa, P. T., Jr., & Widiger, T. A. (Eds.). (2002). 
Personality disorders and the Five-Factor Model 
of personality (2nd ed.). Washington, DC: 
American Psychological Association. 

Cotton, P. (1993, July 7). Psychiatrists set to 
approve DSM-IV. Journal of the American 
Medical Association, 270, 13-15. 


R-12 References 


Council of National Psychological Associations 
for the Advancement of Ethnic Minority 
Interests (CNPAAEMI). (2000, January). 
Guidelines for research in ethnic minority com- 
munities. Washington, DC: American Psy- 
chological Association. 


Couturier, J., & Lock, J. (2006). Eating disorders: 
Anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa, and binge 
eating disorder. In T. G. Plante, Mental disor- 
ders of the new millennium: Biology and function 
(Vol 3.). Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers / 
Greenwood Publishing. 


Coventry, K. R., Venn, S. F., Smith, G. D., & 
Morley, A. M. (2003). Spatial problem solving 
and functional relations. European Journal of 
Cognitive Psychology, 15, 71-99. 


Cowan, N., Towse, J. N., Hamilton, Z., Saults, 

J. S., Elliott, E. M., Lacey, J. F., et al. (2003). 
Children’s working-memory processes: A 
response-timing analysis. Journal of Experimental 
Psychology: General, 132, 113-132. 

Cowley, G. (2000, January 31). Alzheimer’s: 
Unlocking the mystery. Time, pp. 46-54. 

Cowley, G. (2003, February 24). Our bodies, our 
fears. Newsweek, pp. 43—44. 

Cox, J. (2003, May 6). How far would you go to 
save your life? Denver Post, p. F1. 

Cox, R., Baker, S. E., Macdonald, D. W., & Berdoy, 
M. (2004). Protecting egg prey from carrion 
crows: The potential of aversive conditioning. 
Applied Animal Behaviour Science, 87, 325-342. 

Coyle, N. (2006). The hard work of living in the 
face of death. Journal of Pain and Symptom 
Management, 32, 266-274. 

Coyle, T. R. (2006). Test-retest changes on scholastic 
aptitude tests are not related to g. Intelligence, 
34, 15-27. 

Craik, F. I. M. (1990). Levels of processing. In 
M. E. Eysenck (Ed.), The Blackwell dictionary of 
cognitive psychology. London: Blackwell. 

Craik, F., & Lockhart, R. (2008). Levels of process- 
ing and Zinchenko’s approach to memory 
research. Journal of Russian & East European 
Psychology, 46, 52—60. 

Cramer, P. (2007). Longitudinal study of defense 
mechanisms: Late childhood to late adoles- 
cence. Journal of Personality, 75, 1-23. 

Crawford, M., & Unger, R. (2004). Women and 
gender: A feminist psychology (4th ed.). New 
York: McGraw-Hill. 

Creasey, G. L. (2005). Research methods in lifespan 
development (6th ed.). Boston: Allyn & Bacon. 

Creswell, J., & Landon, T. (2009, January 25). 
The talented Mr. Madoff. The New York Times, 
p.l. 

Crews, F. (1996). The verdict on Freud. Psychological 
Science, 7, 63-68. 

Criswell, H., Ming, Z., Kelm, M., & Breese, G. 
(2008, August). Brain regional differences in 
the effect of ethanol on GABA release from 
presynaptic terminals. Journal of Pharmacology 
and Experimental Therapeutics, 326, 596-603. 

Critelli, J., & Bivona, J. (2008). Women’s erotic 
rape fantasies: An evaluation of theory and 
research. Journal of Sex Research, 45, 57-70. 

Crits-Christoph, P. (1992). The efficacy of brief 
dynamic psychotherapy: A meta-analysis. 
American Journal of Psychiatry, 149, 151-158. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Crombag, H. S., & Robinson, R. E. (2004). Drugs, 
environment, brain, and behavior. Current 
Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 107-111. 


Cropley, A. (2006). In praise of convergent think- 
ing. Creativity Research Journal, 18, 391-404. 


Crosnoe, R., & Elder, G. H., Jr. (2002). Successful 
adaptation in the later years: A life course 
approach to aging. Social Psychology Quarterly, 
65, 309-328. 


Crum, A. J., & Langer, E. J. (2007). Mind-set 
matters: Exercise and the placebo effect. 
Psychological Science, 18, 165-171. 


Cruz, A., & Green, B. G. (2000). Thermal stimula- 
tion of taste. Nature, 403, 889-892. 


Cuddy, A. J. C., Fiske, S. T., & Glick, P. (2004). 
When professionals become mothers, warmth 
doesn’t cut the ice. Journal of Social Issues, 60, 
701-718. 


Cuijpers, P., van Straten, A., Andersson, G., & van 
Oppen, P. (2008). Psychotherapy for depres- 
sion in adults: A meta-analysis of comparative 
outcome studies. Journal of Consulting and 
Clinical Psychology, 76, 909-922. 


Culhane-Pera, K. A., Borkan, J. M., & Patten, S. 
(2007). Culture and ethnicity. In O. J. Z. Sahler 
& J. E. Carr, The behavioral sciences and health 
care (2nd rev. and updated ed.). Ashland, OH: 
Hogrefe & Huber Publishers. 


Cullinane, C. A., Chu, D. Z. J., & Mamelak, A. N. 
(2002). Current surgical options in the control 
of cancer pain. Cancer Practice, 10, s21-s26. 


Cummings, A., Ceponiene, R., & Koyama, A. 
(2006). Auditory semantic networks for words 
and natural sounds. Brain Research, 1115, 
92-107. 


Cunningham, P. (2006). Early years teachers and 
the influence of Piaget: Evidence from oral 
history. Early Years An International Journal of 
Research and Development, 26, 5-16. 


Cwikel, J., Behar, L., & Rabson-Hare, J. (2000). A 
comparison of a vote count and a meta-analysis 
review of intervention research with adult 
cancer patients. Research on Social Work Practice, 
10, 139-158. 


Cynkar, A. (2007). The changing gender composi- 
tion of psychology. Monitor on Psychology, 38, 
46-48. 


Czopp, A. M., & Monteith, M. J. (2006). Thinking 
well of African Americans: Measuring 
complimentary stereotypes and negative 
prejudice. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 
28, 233-250. 


Daftary, F., & Meri, J. W. (2002). Culture and mem- 
ory in medieval Islam. London: I. B. Tauris. 


Dai, D. Y., & Wang, X. (2007). The role of need 
for cognition and reader beliefs in text 
comprehension and interest development. 
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 32, 
332-347. 


Daines, B. (2006). Violations of agreed and 
implicit sexual and emotional boundaries in 
couple relationships—some thoughts arising 
from Levine’s ‘A clinical perspective on cou- 
ple infidelity.’ Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 
21, 45-53. 


Dalal, A. K., & Misra, G. (2006). Psychology of 
health and well-being: Some emerging per- 
spectives [Special issue: Psychology of health 


and well-being]. Psychological Studies, 51, 
91-104. 


Dale, A. (2006). Quality issues with survey 
research. International Journal of Social Research 
Methodology: Theory & Practice, 9, Special issue: 
Quality in Social Research, 143-158. 


Dale, J. W., & von Schantz, M. (2007). From genes 
to genomes: Concepts and applications of DNA 
technology. New York: John Wiley & Sons. 


Daley, E. M., McDermott, R. J., Brown, K. R. M., 
& Kittleson, M. J. (2003). Conducting Web- 
based survey research: A lesson in Internet 
designs. American Journal of Health Behavior, 
27, 116-124. 


Damasio, A. (2003, May 15). Mental self: The 
person within. Nature, 423, 227. 


Damon, W. (1999, August). The moral develop- 
ment of children. Scientific American, pp. 72-78. 


Danaei, G., Vender Hoorn, S., Lopez, A. D., 
Murray, C. J. L. & Ezzati, M. (2005). Causes 
of cancer in the world: Comparative risk 
assessment of nine behavioural and environ- 
mental risk factors. Comparative Risk 
Assessment collaborating group (Cancers). 
Lancet, 366, 1784-1793. 


Dani, J. A., & Montague, P. (2007). Disrupting 
addiction through the loss of drug-associated 
internal states. Nature Neuroscience, 10, 
403-404. 


D'Arcy, R., C. N., Bolster, R., B., & Ryner, L. (2007). 
A site directed fMRI approach for evaluating 
functional status in the anterolateral temporal 
lobes. Neuroscience Research, 57, 120-128. 


Dardenne, B., Dumont, M., & Bollier, T. (2007). 
Insidious dangers of benevolent sexism: 
Consequences for women’s performance. 
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 
764-779. 


Darley, J. M., & Latané, B. (1968). Bystanders’ 
intervention in emergencies: Diffusion of 
responsibility. Journal of Personality and Social 
Psychology, 8, 377-383. 


Darwin, C. J., Turvey, M. T., & Crowder, R. G. 
(1972). An auditory analogue of the Sperling 
partial-report procedure: Evidence for brief 
auditory storage. Cognitive Psychology, 3, 
255-267. 


Das, A. (2007). Masturbation in the United 
States. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 33, 
301-317. 


Das, A., Parish, W., & Laumann, E. (2009). 
Masturbation in urban China. Archives of 
Sexual Behavior, 38, 108-120. 


Davidson, J. E., Deuser, R., & Sternberg, R. J. 
(1994). The role of metacognition in problem 
solving. In J. Metcalfe & A. P. Shimamura 
(Eds.), Metacognition: Knowing about knowing. 
Cambridge, MA: MIT. 


Davidson, R. J., Gray, J. A., LeDoux, J. E., Levenson, 
R. W., Pankseep, J., & Ekman, P. (1994). Is there 
emotion-specific physiology? In P. Ekman & 

R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of emotion. 
New York: Oxford University Press. 


Davies, S., Jackson, P., Lewis, G., Hood, S., Nutt, 
D., & Potokar, J. (2008). Is the association of 
hypertension and panic disorder explained by 
clustering of autonomic panic symptoms in 
hypertensive patients? Journal of Affective 
Disorders, 111, 344-350. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Davis, J., Senghas, A., & Ochsner, K. (2009). How 
does facial feedback modulate emotional 
experience? Journal of Research in Personality, 
43, 822-829. 


Davis, L. J. (2009, June 15). Sotomayor and the 
New Haven firefighters case: More myths 
than facts. The Washington Times, p. A04. 


Davis, O., Haworth, C., & Plomin, R. (2009, 
January). Learning abilities and disabilities: 
Generalist genes in early adolescence. 
Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 14, 312-331. 


Davis, R. D., & Millon, T. (1999). Models of per- 
sonality and its disorders. In T. Millon, P. H. 
Blaney, & R. D. Davis, (Eds), Oxford textbook of 
psychopathology. New York: Oxford University 
Press. 


Davis, S. F. (2007). The nose knows best. 
PsycCRITIQUES, 52, 22-31. 


Day, A. L., & Livingstone, H. A. (2003). Gender 
differences in perceptions of stressors and 
utilization of social support among university 
students. Canadian Journal of Behavioural Science, 
35, 73-83. 


Day, R. D., & Lamb, M. E. (2004). Conceptualizing 
and measuring father involvement. Mahwah, NJ: 
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 


DeAngelis, D., & Monahan, J. (2008). Professional 
credentials and professional regulations: 
Social work professional development. In 
B. W. White, K. M. Sowers, & C. N. Dulmus, 
(Eds.), Comprehensive handbook of social work 
and social welfare, Vol. 1: The profession of social 
work. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. 

De Bini, R., Pazzaglia, F., & Gardini, S. (2007). 
The generation and maintenance of visual 
mental images: Evidence from image type 
and aging. Brain and Cognition, 63, 271-278. 


Dean, C., & Dresbach, T. (2006). Neuroligins and 
neurexins: Linking cell adhesion, synapse 
formation and cognitive function. International 
Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical Practice, 10 
(Suppl.), 5-11. 


Dean-Borenstein, M. T. (2007). The long-term 
psychosocial effects of trauma on survivors of 
human-caused extreme stress situations. 
Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: 
The Sciences and Engineering, 67(11-B), 6733. 


Dearing, E., McCartney, K., & Taylor, B. (2009). 
Does higher quality early child care promote 
low-income children’s math and reading 
achievement in middle childhood? Child 
Development, 80, 1329-1349. 


Deary, I. J., & Der, G. (2005). Reaction time, age, 
and cognitive ability: Longitudinal findings 
from age 16 to 63 years in representative pop- 
ulation samples. Aging, Neuropsychology, & 
Cognition, 12, 187-215. 


Deater-Deckard, K., Ivy, L., & Smith, J. (2005). 
Resilience in gene-environment transactions. 
In S. Goldstein & R. B. Brooks (Eds.), 
Handbook of resilience in children. New York: 
Kluwer Academic/Plenum Publishers. 

Deci, E. L., & Ryan, R. M. (2008). Facilitating opti- 
mal motivation and psychological well-being 
across life’s domains. Canadian Psychology/ 
Psychologie Canadienne, 49, 14-23. 

Dediu, D., & Ladd, D. R. (2007). From the Cover: 
Linguistic tone is related to the population 
frequency of the adaptive haplogroups of two 
brain size genes, ASPM and microcephalin. 


Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 
104, 10944-10949. 


DeGaton, J. F., Weed, S., & Jensen, L. (1996). 
Understanding gender differences in 
adolescent sexuality. Adolescence, 31, 217-231. 


de Gelder, B. (2000). More to seeing than meets 
the eye. Science, 289, 1148-1149. 


deGroot, A. (1978). Thought and choice in chess. 
Paris: Mouton de Gruyter. 


deGroot, A. D. (1966). Perception and memory 
versus thought: Some old ideas and recent 
findings. In B. Kleinmuntz (Ed.), Problem 
solving: Research, method, and theory. New York: 
Wiley. 

DeLamater, J. D., & Sill, M. (2005). Sexual desire 
in later life. Journal of Sex Research, 42, 138-149. 


Delcourt, M. A. B., Cornell, D. G., & Goldberg, 
M. D. (2007). Cognitive and affective learning 
outcomes of gifted elementary school stu- 
dents. The Gifted Child Quarterly, 51, 359-382. 


Delgado, M. R., Labouliere, C. D., & Phelps, E. A. 
(2006). Fear of losing money? Aversive condi- 
tioning with secondary reinforcers [Special 
issue: Genetic, comparative and cognitive 
studies of social behavior]. Social Cognitive and 
Affective Neuroscience, 1, 250-259. 


Delinsky, S. S., Latner, J. D., & Wilson, G. T. (2006). 
Binge eating and weight loss in a self-help 
behavior modification program. Obesity, 14, 
1244-1249, 


DeLoache, J., & LoBue, V. (2009). The narrow 
fellow in the grass: Human infants associate 
snakes and fear. Developmental Science, 12, 
201-207. 


del Rosal, E., Alonso, L., & Moreno, R. (2006). 
Simulation of habituation to simple and 
multiple stimuli. Behavioural Processes, 73, 
272-277. 


Demaree, H. A., & Everhart, D. E. (2004). Healthy 
high-hostiles: Reduced para-sympathetic 
activity and decreased sympathovagal 
flexibility during negative emotional 
processing. Personality and Individual 
Differences, 36, 457-469. 


Dement, W. C., & Wolpert, E. A. (1958). The 
relation of eye movements, body mobility, 
and external stimuli to dream content. Journal 
of Experimental Psychology, 55, 543-553. 


Dempster, F. N. (1981). Memory span: Sources for 
individual and developmental differences. 
Psychological Bulletin, 89, 63-100. 


Denmark, G. L., & Fernandez, L. C. (1993). 
Historical development of the psychology of 
women. In F. L. Denmark & M. A. Paludi 
(Eds.), A handbook of issues and theories. 
Westport, CT: Greenwood Press. 

Dennett, D. C. (2003). Freedom evolves. New York: 
Viking. 

Dennis, I. (2007). Halo effects in grading student 
projects. Journal of Applied Psychology, 92, 
1169-1176. 

Dennis, T. A., Cole, P. M., Zahn-Waxler, C., & 
Mizuta, I. (2002). Self in context: Autonomy 
and relatedness in Japanese and U.S. mother- 
preschooler dyads. Child Development, 73, 
1803-1817. 

Denollet, J. (2005). DS14: Standard assessment of 
negative affectivity, social inhibition, and 


References R-13 


Type D personality. Psychosomatic Medicine, 
67, 89-97. 


Deouell, L. Y., Parnes, A., & Pickard, N. (2006). 
Spatial location is accurately tracked by 
human auditory sensory memory: Evidence 
from the mismatch negativity. European 
Journal of Neuroscience, 24, 1488-1494. 


Deregowski, J. B. (1973). Illusion and culture. 
In R. L. Gregory & G. H. Combrich (Eds.), 
Illusion in nature and art. New York: 
Scribner. 


Derryberry, W. P. (2006). Review of social motiva- 
tion: Conscious and unconscious processes. 
Journal of Moral Education, 35, 276-278. 


DeRubeis, R., Hollon, S., & Shelton, R. (2003, May 
23). Presentation, American Psychiatric 
Association meeting, Philadelphia. 


Deshields, T., Tibbs, T., Fan, M. Y., & Taylor, M. 
(2006). Differences in patterns of depression 
after treatment for breast cancer [Electronic 
article published August 12, 2005]. Psycho- 
Oncology, 15(5), 398-406. 


Des Jarlais, D. C., Sloboda, A., Friedman, S. R., 
Tempakski, B., McKnight, C., & Braine, N. 
(2006). Diffusion of the D.A.R.E and Syringe 
Exchange Programs. American Journal of Public 
Health, 96, 1354-1357. 


Desimone, R. (1992, October 9). The physiology 
of memory: Recordings of things past. Science, 
258, 245-255. 


Dessing, J. C., Peper, C. E., Bullock, D., & Beek, 
P. J. (2005). How position, velocity, and tem- 
poral information combine in the prospective 
control of catching: Data and model. Journal of 
Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 668-686. 


DeSteno, D., Bartlett, M. Y., Braverman, J., & 
Salovey, P. (2002). Sex differences in jealousy: 
Evolutionary mechanism or artifact of mea- 
surement? Journal of Personality and Social 
Psychology, 83, 1103-1116. 


Detoledo-Morrell, L., Stoub, T. R., & Wang, C. 
(2007). Hippocampal atrophy and disconnec- 
tion in incipient and mild Alzheimer’s 
disease. Progressive Brain Research, 163C, 
741-823. 


Detterman, D. K., Gabriel, L. T., & Ruthsatz, J. M. 
(2000). Intelligence and mental retardation. In 
R. J. Sternberg et al. (Eds.), Handbook of intelli- 
gence. New York: Cambridge University Press. 


Devonport, J. J., & Lane, A. M. (2006). 
Relationships between self-efficacy, coping 
and student retention. Social Behavior and 
Personality, 34, 127-138. 


Dey, E. L., Astin, A. W., Korn, W. S., & Berz, 
E. R. (2004). The American freshman: National 
norms for fall, 1990. Los Angeles: Higher 
Education Research Institute, Graduate 
School of Education, UCLA. 


Dhillon, S., Yang, L., & Curran, M. (2008). Spotlight 
on bupropion in major depressive disorder. 
CNS Drugs, 22, 613-617. 


Diamond, M. (2009). Human intersexuality: 
Difference or disorder? Archives of Sexual 
Behavior, 38, 172. 


DiCano, P., & Everitt, B. J. (2002). Reinstatement 
and spontaneous recovery of cocaine-seeking 
following extinction and different durations 
of withdrawal. Behavioural Pharmacology, 13, 
397-406. 


R-14 References 


Dickens, W. T., & Flynn, J. R. (2001). Heritability 
estimates versus large environmental effects: 
The IQ paradox resolved. Psychological Review, 
108, 291-310. 


Dickerson, S. S., Kemeny, M. E., Aziz, N., Kim, 
K. H., & Fahey, J. L. (2004). Immunological 
effects of induced shame and guilt. 
Psychosomatic Medicine, 66, 124-131. 


Diener, E., & Biswas-Diener, R. (2002). Will 
money increase subjective well-being? Social 
Indicators Research, 57, 119-169. 


Diener, E., & Clifton, D. (2002). Life satisfaction 
and religiosity in broad probability samples. 
Psychological Inquiry, 13, 206-209. 


Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., & Oishi, S. (2002). Sub- 
jective well-being: The science of happiness 
and life satisfaction. In C. R. Snyder & S. J. 
Lopez (Eds.), Handbook of positive psychology. 
London: Oxford University Press. 


Diener, E., Lucas, R. E., & Scollon, C. N. (2006). 
Beyond the hedonic treadmill: Revising the 
adaptation theory of well-being. American 
Psychologist, 61, 305-314. 


Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2002). Very 
happy people. Psychological Science, 18, 81-84. 


Diener, E., & Seligman, M. E. P. (2004). Beyond 
money: Toward an economy of well-being. 
Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 5, 
1-31. 


Diener, M, Isabella, R., Behunin, M., & Wong, 
M. (2008). Attachment to mothers and fathers 
during middle childhood: Associations with 
child gender, grade, and competence. Social 
Development, 17, 84-101. 


Di Fabio, A., & Palazzeschi, L. (2009). An 
in-depth look at scholastic success: Fluid 
intelligence, personality traits or emotional 
intelligence? Personality and Individual 
Differences, 46, 581-585. 


Di Forti, M., Lappin, J., & Murray, R. (2007, 
March). Risk factors for schizophrenia—All 
roads lead to dopamine. European Neuro- 
psychopharmacology, 17, S101-SS107. 


DiGiovanna, A. G. (1994). Human aging: Biological 
perspectives. New York: McGraw-Hill. 


Dijksterhuis, A., Chartrand, T. L., & Aarts, H. (2007). 
Effects of Priming and Perception on Social 
Behavior and Goal Pursuit. Frontiers of Social 
Psychology, 17, 33-40. 


Dill, K., & Thill, K. (2007). Video game characters 
and the socialization of gender roles: Young 
people’s perceptions mirror sexist media 
depictions. Sex Roles, 57, 851-864. 


Dillard, J. P., & Shen, L. (2004). On the nature of 
reactance and its role in persuasive health 
communication. Communication Monographs, 
72, 144-168. 


Dillard, J. P., & Shen, L. (2007). Self-report mea- 
sures of discrete emotions. In R. A. Reynolds, 
R. Woods, & J. D. Baker, Handbook of research 
on electronic surveys and measurements. 
Hershey, PA: Idea Group Reference /IGI 
Global, 2007. 


Dillon, J. (2008, April). Reclaiming humanistic 
psychology from modernity: Problems and 
solutions. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 48, 
221-242. 


DiLorenzo, P. M., & Yougentob, S. L. (2003). 
Olfaction and taste. In M. Gallagher & R. J. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Nelson, Handbook of psychology: Biological psy- 
chology (Vol 3). New York: Wiley. 


Dixon, R. A., & Cohen, A. L. (2003). Cognitive 
development in adulthood. In R. M. Lerner, 
M. A. Easterbrooks, et al. (Eds.), Handbook of 
psychology: Developmental psychology (Vol. 6). 
New York: Wiley. 


Dixon, R. A., Rust, T. B., & Feltmate, S. E. (2007). 
Memory and aging: Selected research direc- 
tions and application issues. Canadian Psy- 
chology Psychologie Canadienne, 48, 67-76. 


Do, V. T. (2006). Asian American men and the 
media: The relationship between ethnic 
identity, self-esteem, and the endorsement of 
stereotypes. Dissertation Abstracts International: 
Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 67(6-B), 
3446. 


Dobbins, A. C., Jeo, R. M., Fiser, J., & Allman, 
J. M. (1998, July 24). Distance modulation of 
neural activity in the visual cortex. Science, 
281, 552-555. 


Dodge, K. A. (2004). The nature-nurture debate 
and public policy [Special issue: 50th anniver- 
sary issue, part 2: The maturing of the human 
development sciences—Appraising past, 
present, and prospective agendas]. Merrill- 
Palmer Quarterly: Journal of Developmental 
Psychology, 50, 418-427. 


Doi, T. (1990). The cultural assumptions of psy- 
choanalysis. In J. W. Stigler, R. A. Shweder, & 
G. Herdt (Eds.), Cultural psychology: Essays on 
comparative human development. New York: 
Cambridge University Press. 


Dolan, P., & White, M. P. (2007). How can 
measures of subjective well-being be used 
to inform public policy? Perspectives on Psy- 
chological Science, 2, 71-85. 


Dolan, R. J. (2002, November 8). Emotion, 
cognition, and behavior. Science, 298, 
1191-1194. 


Dolbier, C. L., Smith, S. E., & Steinhardt, M. A. 
(2007). Relationships of protective factors to 
stress and symptoms of illness. American 
Journal of Health Behavior, 31, 423-433. 


Dollinger, S. J. (2003). Need for uniqueness, need 
for cognition and creativity. Journal of Creative 
Behavior, 37, 99-116. 


Domhoff, G. W. (1996). Finding meaning in dreams: 
A quantitative approach. New York: Plenum 
Press. 


Donahoe, J. W. (2003). Selectionism. In K. A. Lattal, 
& P.N. Chase (Eds.), Behavior theory and 
philosophy. New York: Kluwer Academic/ 
Plenum Publishers. 


Donahoe, J. W., & Vegas, R. (2004). Pavlovian 
Conditioning: The CSUR Relation. Journal of 
Experimental Psychology: Animal Behavior 
Processes, 30, 17-33. 


Doty, R. L., Green, P. A., Ram, C., & Yankell, S. L. 
(1982). Communication of gender from 
human breath odors: Relationship to per- 
ceived intensity and pleasantness. Hormones 
and Behavior, 16, 13-22. 


Dougall, A. L., & Baum, A. (2004). Psychoneuro- 
immunology and trauma. In P. P. Schnurr & 
B. L. Green (Eds.), Trauma and health: Physical 
health consequences of exposure to extreme stress. 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


Douglas Brown, R., Goldstein, E., & Bjorklund, 
D. F. (2000). The history and zeitgeist of the 
repressed-false-memory debate: Scientific and 
sociological perspectives on suggestibility 
and childhood memory. In D. F. Bjorklund 
(Ed.), False-memory creation in children and 
adults: Theory, research, and implications. 
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. 


Douglas, O., Burton, K. S., & Reese-Durham, N. 
(2008). The effects of the multiple intelligence 
teaching strategy on the academic achieve- 
ment of eighth grade math students. Journal of 
Instructional Psychology, 35, 182-187. 


Dovidio, J. F., & Gaertner, S. L. (2006). A multi- 
level perspective on prejudice: Crossing disci- 
plinary boundaries. In P. A. M. Van Lange, 
Bridging social psychology: Benefits of transdisci- 
plinary approaches. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence 
Erlbaum Associates. 


Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., & Kawakami, 
K. (2003). Intergroup contact: The past, 
present, and the future. Group Processes and 
Intergroup Relations, 6, 5-20. 


Dovidio, J. F., Gaertner, S. L., & Pearson, A. R. 
(2005). On the nature of prejudice: The 
psychological foundations of hate. In R. J. 
Sternberg (Ed.), Psychology of hate. 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


Dovidio, J. F. Piliavin, J. A., Schroeder, D. A., & 
Penner, L. A. (2006). The social psychology of 
prosocial behavior. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence 
Erlbaum Associates. 


Dowling, N., Jackson, A., & Thomas, S. (2008). 
Behavioral interventions in the treatment of 
pathological gambling: A review of activity 
scheduling and desensitization. International 
Journal of Behavioral Consultation and Therapy, 
4, 172-187. 


Doyle, R. (2002, December). Deinstitutionalization. 
Scientific American, 38. 


Drewes, A. A. (2005). Play in selected cultures: 
Diversity and universality. In E. Gil & A. A. 
Drewes, Cultural issues in play therapy. New 
York: Guilford Press. 


Drews, F., Pasupathi, M., & Strayer, D. (2008, 
December). Passenger and cell phone conver- 
sations in simulated driving. Journal of 
Experimental Psychology: Applied, 14, 392-400. 


Drob, S. (2005). The mystical symbol: Some com- 
ments on Ankor, Giegerich, Scholem, and 
Jung. Journal of Jungian Theory & Practice, 7, 
25-29. 


Dryden, W., & David, D. (2008). Rational emotive 
behavior therapy: Current status. Journal of 
Cognitive Psychotherapy, 22, 195-209. 


Ducharme, J. M., Sanjuan, E., & Drain, T. (2007). 
Errorless compliance training: Success- 
focused behavioral treatment of children with 
Asperger syndrome. Behavior Modification, 31, 
329-344. 

Duffy, M., Gillig, S. E., Tureen, R. M., & Ybarra, 
M. A. (2002). A critical look at the DSM-IV. 
Journal of Individual Psychology, 58, 363-373. 

Dugger, C. W. (1996, December 28). Tug of 
taboos: African genital rite vs. U.S. law. The 
New York Times, pp. 1, 9. 

Duke, M., & Nowicki, S., Jr. (1979). Abnormal 
psychology: Perspectives on being different. 
Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Dumont, M., & Dumont, D. (2008). Deinstitu- 
tionalization in the United States and Italy: A 
historical survey. International Journal of 
Mental Health, 37, 61-70. 


Duncker, K. (1945). On problem solving. Psy- 
chological Monographs, 58 (5, whole no. 270). 


Durik, A. M., Hyde, J. S., & Marks, A. C. (2006). 
Ethnicity and gender stereotypes of emotion. 
Sex Roles, 54, 429-445. 


Eagly, A., & Chaiken, S. (1998). Attitude structure 
and function. In D. T. Gilbert & S. T. Fiske 
(Eds.), Handbook of social psychology (Vol. 1, 4th 
ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. 


Eagly, A. H., Beall, A., & Sternberg, R. S. (Eds.). 
(2004). The psychology of gender (2nd ed.). New 
York: Guilford Press. 


Eaker, E. D., Sullivan, L. M., Kelly-Hayes, M., 
D'Agostino, R. B., Sr., & Benjamin, E. J. (2004). 
Anger and hostility predict the development 
of atrial fibrillation in men in the Framingham 
Offspring Study. Circulation, 109, 1267-1271. 


Ebbinghaus, H. (1885/1913). Memory: A contribu- 
tion to experimental psychology (H. A. Roger & 
C. E. Bussenius, Trans.). New York: Columbia 
University Press. 


Eberhard, K. M., Cutting, J. C., & Bock, K. (2005). 
Making syntax of sense: Number agreement 
in sentence production. Psychological Review, 
112, 531-559. 


Eberhardt, J. L., Goff, P. A., Purdie, V. J., & 
Davies, P. G. (2004). Seeming black: Race, 
crime, and visual processing. Journal of 
Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 876-893. 


Eberling, J., Jagust, W., Christine, C., Starr, P., 
Larson, P., Bankiewicz, K., et al. (2008, May). 
Results from a phase I safety trial of hAADC 
gene therapy for Parkinson disease. Neurology, 
70(21), 1980-1983. 


Ebstein, R. P., Benjamin, J., & Belmaker, R. H. (2003). 
Behavioral genetics, genomics, and personality. 
In R. Plomin & J. C. DeFries (Eds.), Behavioral 
genetics in the postgenomic era. Washington, DC: 
American Psychological Association. 

Ebster, C., & Neumayr, B. (2008). Applying the 
door-in-the-face compliance technique to 
retailing. The International Review of Retail, 
Distribution and Consumer Research, 18, 
121-128. 


Ecenbarger, W. (1993, April 1). America’s new 
merchants of death. The Reader’s Digest, p. 50. 


Eckardt, M. H. (2005). Karen Horney: A portrait: 
The 120th anniversary, Karen Horney, 
September 16, 1885. American Journal of 
Psychoanalysis, 65, 95-101. 


Edwards, R. R., & Fillingim, R. B. (2007). Self- 
reported pain sensitivity: Lack of correlation 
with pain threshold and tolerance. European 
Journal of Pain, 11, 594-598. 


Egan, K. (2005). Students’ development in theory 
and practice: The doubtful role of research. 
Harvard Educational Review, 75, 25—41. 

Egliston, K., & Rapee, R. (2007). Inhibition of fear 
acquisition in toddlers following positive 
modelling by their mothers. Behaviour 
Research and Therapy, 45, 1871-1882. 

Einarsson, C., & Granstroem, K. (2002). Gender- 
biased interaction in the classroom: The 
influence of gender and age in the relation- 


ship between teacher and pupil. Scandinavian 
Journal of Educational Research, 46, 117-127. 


Eisch, A., Cameron, H., Encinas, J., Meltzer, L., 
Ming, G., & Overstreet-Wadiche, L. (2008, 
November). Adult neurogenesis, mental 
health, and mental illness: Hope or hype? 
Journal of Neuroscience, 28(46), 1785-1791. 


Eisenberg, N., Guthrie, I. K., & Cumberland, A. 
(2002). Prosocial development in early adult- 
hood: A longitudinal study. Journal of Person- 
ality and Social Psychology, 82, 993-1006. 


Eizenberg, M. M., & Zaslavsky, O. (2004). 
Students’ verification strategies for combina- 
torial problems. Mathematical Thinking and 
Learning, 6, 15-36. 


Ekman, P. (1972). Universals and cultural differ- 
ences in facial expressions of emotion. In 
J. Cole (Ed.), Darwin and facial expression: A 
century of research in review. New York: 
Academic Press. 


Ekman, P. (1994a). All emotions are basic. In 
P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of 
emotion: Fundamental questions. New York: 
Oxford University Press. 


Ekman, P. (1994b). Strong evidence for universals 
in facial expressions: A reply to Russell’s 
mistaken critique. Psychological Bulletin, 115, 
268-287. 


Ekman, P. (2003). Emotions revealed: Recognizing 
faces and feelings to improve communication and 
emotional life. New York: Times Books. 


Ekman, P., Levenson, R. W., & Friesen, W. V. (1983, 
September 16). Autonomic nervous system 
activity distinguishes among emotions. 
Science, 223, 1208-1210. 


Ekman, P., & O'Sullivan, M. (1991). Facial expres- 
sion: Methods, means, and moues. In R. S. 
Feldman & B. Rimé (Eds.), Fundamentals of 
nonverbal behavior. Cambridge, England: 
Cambridge University Press. 


Ekroll, V., & Scherzer, T. R. (2009). Apparent 
visual motion of the observer’s own limbs. 
Perception, 38, 778-780. 


Elfhag, K., Tynelius, P., & Rasmussen, F. (2007). 
Sugar-sweetened and artificially sweetened 
soft drinks in association to restrained, exter- 
nal and emotional eating. Physiology & 
Behavior, 91, 191-195. 


El-Hai, J. (2005). The lobotomist: A maverick medical 
genius and his tragic quest to rid the world of 
mental illness. New York: Wiley. 


Elkins, D. (2009). Why humanistic psychology 
lost its power and influence in American 
psychology: Implications for advancing 
humanistic psychology. Journal of Humanistic 
Psychology, 49, 267-291. 


Elkins, G., Marcus, J., Bates, J., Hasan, R. M., & 
Cook, T. (2006). Intensive hypnotherapy for 
smoking cessation: a prospective study. 
International Journal of Clinical Experimental 
Hypnosis, 54, 303-315. 


Ellason, J. W., & Ross, C. A. (2004). SCL-90-R 
norms for dissociative identity disorder. 
Journal of Trauma and Dissociation, 5, 85-91. 


Ellins, E., Halcox, J., Donald, A., Field, B., 
Brydon, L., Deanfield, J., et al. (2008). Arterial 
stiffness and inflammatory response to 
psychophysiological stress. Brain, Behavior, 
and Immunity, 22, 941-948. 


References R-15 


Elliott, A. (2002). Psychoanalytic theory: An introduc- 
tion (2nd ed.). Durham, NC: Duke University 
Press. 


Elliott, J., Berman, H., & Kim, S. (2002). Critical 
ethnography of Korean Canadian women’s 
menopause experience. Health Care for Women 
International, 23, 377-388. 


Ellis, A. (1974). Growth through reason. Hollywood, 
CA: Wilshire Books. 


Ellis, A. (2000). How to control your anger before it 
controls you. New York: Citadel. 


Ellis, A. (2002). Overcoming resistance: A rational 
emotive behavior therapy integrated approach 
(2nd ed.). New York: Springer. 


Ellis, A. (2004). Expanding the ABCs of rational 
emotive behavior therapy. In A. Freeman, 
M. J. Mahoney, P. Devito, & D. Martin (Eds.), 
Cognition and psychotherapy (2nd ed.). New 
York: Springer Publishing Co. 


El-Mallakh, R. S., & Abraham, H. D. (2007). 
MDMA (Ecstasy). Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 
19, 45-52. 


Emick, J., & Welsh, M. (2005). Association 
between formal operational thought and 
executive function as measured by the Tower 
of Hanoi-Revised. Learning and Individual 
Differences, 15, 177-188. 


Endres, T., & Fendt, M. (2007). Conditioned behav- 
ioral responses to a context paired with the 
predator odor trimethylthiazoline. Behavioral 
Neuroscience, 121, 594-601. 


Engen, T. (1987). Remembering odors and their 
names. American Scientist, 75, 497-503. 


Engler, J., & Goleman, D. (1992). The consumer's 
guide to psychotherapy. New York: Simon & 
Schuster. 


Epstein, R. (1996). Cognition, creativity, and behav- 
ior: Selected essays. Westport, CT: Praeger / 
Greenwood. 


Eranti, S. V., & McLoughlin, D. M. (2003). 
Electroconvulsive therapy: State of the art. 
British Journal of Psychiatry, 182, 8-9. 


Erickson, R. (2008, February). A study of the sci- 
ence of taste: On the origins and influence of 
the core ideas. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 
31, 59-75. 


Erikson, E. H. (1963). Childhood and society. New 
York: Norton. 


Ervik, S., Abdelnoor, M., & Heier, M. S. (2006). 
Health-related quality of life in narcolepsy. 
Acta Neurologica Scandinavica, 114, 198-204. 


Etchegary, H. (2004). Psychological aspects of 
predictive genetic-test decision: What do we 
know so far? Analyses of Social Issues and 
Public Policy, 4, 13-31. 


Evans, A. M. (2007). Transference in the nurse- 
patient relationship. Journal of Psychiatric and 
Mental Health Nursing, 14, 189-195. 


Evans, D. E., & Rothbart, M. K. (2007). 
Developing a model for adult temperament. 
Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 868-888. 


Evans, D., & Rothbart, M. (2009). A two-factor 
model of temperament. Personality and 
Individual Differences, 47, 565-570. 


Evans, J. B. T. (2004). Informal reasoning: Theory 
and method. Canadian Journal of Experimental 
Psychology, 58, 69-74. 


R-16 References 


Evans, J. B. T., & Feeney, A. (2004). The role of 
prior belief in reasoning. In J. P. Leighton 
(Ed.), Nature of reasoning. New York: 
Cambridge University Press. 


Evcik, D., Kavuncu, V., Cakir, T., Subasi, V., & 
Yaman, M. (2007). Laser therapy in the 
treatment of carpal tunnel syndrome: A 
randomized controlled trial. Photomedical 
Laser Surgery, 25, 34-39. 


Everette, M. (2008). Gestational weight and 
dietary intake during pregnancy: Perspectives 
of African American women. Maternal & Child 
Health Journal, 12, 718-724. 


Everly, G. S., Jr., & Lating, J. M. (2007). Psy- 
chotherapy: A cognitive perspective. In 
A. Monat, R. S. Lazarus, & G. Reevy, The 
Praeger handbook on stress and coping (Vol. 2). 
Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers /Greenwood 
Publishing. 

Eysenck, H. J. (1990). Biological dimensions of 
personality. In L. A. Pervin (Ed.), Handbook of 


personality: Theory and research. New York: 
Guilford Press. 


Eysenck, H. J. (1995). Eysenck on extraversion. New 
York: Wiley. 


Fagan, J. F., & Holland, C. R. (2002). Equal 
opportunity and racial differences in IQ. 
Intelligence, 30, 361-387. 


Fagan, J. F., & Holland, C. R. (2007). Racial equality 
in intelligence: Predictions from a theory of 
intelligence as processing. Intelligence, 35, 
319-334. 


Fagan, J. F., & Holland, C. R. (2009). Culture-fair 
prediction of academic achievement. Intel- 
ligence, 37, 62-67. 


Fagan, J., & Press, J. (2008). Father influences on 
employed mothers’ work-family balance. 
Journal of Family Issues, 29, 1136-1160. 


Falck-Ytter, T., & Gredeback, G. (2006). Infants 
predict other people’s action goals. Nature 
Neuroscience, 9, 878-879. 


Fallon, A. (2006). Informed consent in the practice 
of group psychotherapy. International Journal 
of Group Psychotherapy, 56, 431-453. 


Fallon, B. A., & Feinstein, S. (2001). Hypochon- 
driasis. In K. A. Phillips (Ed.), Somatoform and 
factitious disorders. Washington, DC: American 
Psychiatric Association. 


Fanselow, M. S., & Poulos, A. M. (2005). The neu- 
roscience of mammalian associative learning. 
Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 207-234. 


Farrell, E. F. (2005, July 15). To test or not to test? 
The Chronicle of Higher Education, pp. A39-A40. 


Fearing, V. G., & Clark, J. (Eds.). (2000). Individuals 
in context: A practical guide to client-centered 
practice. Chicago: Slack Publishing. 


Fedeli, A., Braconi, S., Economidou, D., Cannella, 
N., Kallupi, M., Guerrini, R., et al. (2009). The 
paraventricular nucleus of the hypothalamus 
is a neuroanatomical substrate for the inhibi- 
tion of palatable food intake by neuropeptide 
S. European Journal of Neuroscience, 30, 
1594-1602. 


Fee, E., Brown, T. M., Lazarus, J., & Theerman, P. 
(2002). Exploring acupuncture: Ancient ideas, 
modern techniques. American Journal of Public 
Health, 92, 1592. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Feinberg, A. W. (2002, April). Homo-cysteine may 
raise Alzheimer’s risk: A physician's perspec- 
tive. HealthNews, p. 4. 

Feingold, A. (1994). Gender differences in person- 
ality: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 
116, 429-456. 


Feldhusen, J. F., (2006). The role of the knowledge 
base in creative thinking. In J. C. Kaufman, & 
J. Baer, Creativity and reason in cognitive develop- 
ment. New York: Cambridge University Press. 


Feldman, D. H. (2003). Cognitive development in 
childhood. In R. M. Lerner, M. A. Easterbrooks, 
et al. (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Develop- 
mental psychology (Vol. 6.). New York: Wiley. 


Feldman, D. H. (2004). Piaget’s stages: The unfin- 
ished symphony of cognitive development. 
New Ideas in Psychology, 22, 175-231. 


Feldman, R. S. (2010). P.O.W.E.R. Learning: 
Strategies for Success in College and Life (5th ed). 
New York: McGraw-Hill. 


Feldman, R. S. (Ed.). (1993). Applications of non- 
verbal behavioral theories and research. Hillsdale, 
NJ: Erlbaum. 


Feldt, L. S. (2005). Estimating the reliability of 
cichotomous or trichotomous scores. Edu- 
cational and Psychological Measurement, 65, 
28-41. 


Fenter, V. L. (2006). Concerns about Prozac and 
direct-to-consumer advertising of prescrip- 
tion drugs. International Journal of Risk & 
Safety in Medicine, 18, 1-7. 

Feret, A., Steinweg, S., Griffin, H. C., & Glover, 
S. (2007). Macular degeneration: types, 
causes, and possible interventions. Geriatric 
Nursing, 28, 387-392. 


Fernandez, A. M., Vera-Villarroel, P., & Sierra, 
J. C. (2007). Distress in response to emotional 
and sexual infidelity: Evidence of evolved 
gender differences in Spanish students. 
Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and 
Applied, 141, 17-24. 


Feshbach, S., & Tangney, J. (2008, September). 
Television viewing and aggression: Some 
alternative perspectives. Perspectives on 
Psychological Science, 3, 387-389. 


Festinger, D., Marlowe, D., Croft, J., Dugosh, K., 
Arabia, P., & Benasutti, K. (2009). Monetary 
incentives improve recall of research con- 
sent information: It pays to remember. 
Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 
17, 99-104. 

Festinger, L. (1957). A theory of cognitive dissonance. 
Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. 

Festinger, L., & Carlsmith, J. M. (1959). Cognitive 
consequences of forced compliance. Journal of 
Abnormal and Social Psychology, 58, 203-210. 

Fichtenberg, C. M., & Glantz, S. A. (2006). 
Association of the California tobacco control 
program with declines in cigarette con- 
sumption and mortality from heart disease. 
In K. E. Warner, Tobacco control policy. San 
Francisco, CA: Jossey-Bass. 

Fields, R. D. (2004, April). The other half of the 
brain. Scientific American, pp. 55-61. 

Fields-Meyer, T. (1995, September 25). Having 
their say. People, pp. 50-60. 

Fine, R., & Fine, L. (2003). Basic chess endings. 
New York: Random House. 


Fingelkurts, A., Fingelkurts, A. A., & Kallio, S. 
(2007). Hypnosis induces a changed composi- 
tion of brain oscillations in EEG: A case study. 
Contemporary Hypnosis, 24, 3-18. 


Fink, B., Manning, J. T., & Williams J. H. G. (2007). 
The 2nd to 4th digit ratio and developmental 
psychopathology in school-aged children. Per- 
sonality and Individual Differences, 42, 369-379. 


Fink, G. (Ed.). (2000). Encyclopedia of stress. New 
York: Academic Press. 


Finkelhor, D. (2002). Sexual abuse in the United 
States. Durham, NH: University of New 
Hampshire. 


Finkelhor, D., & Jones, L. M. (2004, January). 
Explanations for the decline in child sexual 
abuse cases. Juvenile Justice Bulletin. 
Washington, DC: Office of Juvenile Justice 
and Delinquency Prevention. 


Finkelhor, D., Ormrod, R., & Turner, H. (2005). The 
victimization of children and youth: A compre- 
hensive, national survey. Child Maltreatment: 
Journal of the American Professional Society on the 
Abuse of Children, 10, 5-25. 


Finkelstein, M. (2009). Intrinsic vs. extrinsic 
motivational orientations and the volunteer 
process. Personality and Individual Differences, 
46, 653-658. 


Finkler, K. (2004). Traditional healers in Mexico: 
The effectiveness of spiritual practices. In 
U. P. Gielen, J. M. Fish, & J. G. Draguns (Eds.), 
Handbook of culture, therapy, and healing. 
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 


Finlay, F. O., Jones, R., & Coleman, J. (2002). Is 
puberty getting earlier? The views of doctors 
and teachers. Child: Care, Health and Develop- 
ment, 28, 205-209. 


Finley, C. L., & Cowley, B. J. (2005). The effects of a 
consistent sleep schedule on time taken to 
achieve sleep. Clinical Case Studies, 4, 304-311. 


Firestein, B. A. (Ed.). (1996). Bisexuality: The 
psychology and politics of an invisible minority. 
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 


First, M. B., Frances, A., & Pincus, H. A. (2002). 
DSM-IV-TR handbook of differential diagnosis. 
Arlington, VA: American Psychiatric 
Publishing. 


Fischer, K. W., Shaver, P. R., & Carnochan, P. (1990). 
How emotions develop and how they organize 
development. Cognition and Emotion, 4, 81-127. 


Fishbach, A., Dhar, R., & Zhang, Y. (2006). 
Subgoals as substitutes or complements: The 
role of goal accessibility. Journal of Personality 
and Social Psychology, 91, 232-242. 


Fisher, C. B. (2003). Decoding the ethics code: A 
practical guide for psychologists. Thousand 
Oaks, CA: Sage. 


Fisher, C. B., Hoagwood, K., Boyce, C., Duster, T., 
Frank, D. A., Grisso, T., et al. (2002). Research 
ethics for mental health science involving 
ethnic minority children and youths. American 
Psychologist, 57, 1024—1040. 


Fisk, J. E., Bury, A. S., & Holden, R. (2006). 
Reasoning about complex probabilistic con- 
cepts in childhood. Scandinavian Journal of 
Psychology, 47, 497-504. 

Fisk, J. E., & Sharp, C. (2002). Syllogistic reason- 
ing and cognitive aging. Quarterly Journal of 
Experimental Psychology: Human Experimental 
Psychology, 55A, 1273-1293. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Fitzgerald, H., Mann, T., Cabrera, N., & Wong, 
M. M. (2003). Diversity in caregiving contexts. 
In R. M. Lerner, M. A. Easterbrooks, et al. 
(Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Developmental 
psychology (Vol. 6). New York: Wiley. 


Fitzgerald, P., & Daskalakis, Z. (2008, January). 
The use of repetitive transcranial magnetic 
stimulation and vagal nerve stimulation in 
the treatment of depression. Current Opinion 
in Psychiatry, 21, 25-29. 


Flam, F. (1991, June 14). Queasy riders. Science, 
252, 1488. 


Flavell, S. W., Cowan, C. W., Kim, T., Greer, P. L., 
Lin, Y., Paradis, S., et al. (2006, February 17). 
Activity-dependent regulation of MEF2 tran- 
scription factors suppresses excitatory syn- 
apse number. Science, 311, 1008-1010. 


Fleck, J. I., Green, D. L., Payne, L., Stevenson, 
J. L., Bowden, E. M., Jung-Beeman, M., et al. 
(2008). The transliminal brain at rest: Baseline 
EEG, unusual experiences, and access to 
unconscious mental activity. Cortex, 44, 
1353-1363. 


Fleischman, D. A., Wilson, R. S., Gabrieli, J. D. E., 
Bienias, J. L., & Bennett, D. A. (2004). A longi- 
tudinal study of implicit and explicit memory 
in old persons. Psychology and Aging, 19, 
617-625. 


Fleming, J. (2000). Affirmative action and 
standardized test scores. Journal of Negro 
Education, 69, 27-37. 


Flynn, J. R. (2000). IQ gains and fluid g. American 
Psychologist, 55, 543. 


Flynn, J. R. (2007). What is intelligence? Beyond the 
Flynn effect. New York: Cambridge University 
Press. 


Foderaro, L. W. (2006, February 16). Westchester 
lawyer, his memory lost, is found in Chicago 
shelter after 6 months. The New York Times, 

p. B3. 


Fogarty, J. S., & Young, G. A., Jr. (2000). Patient- 
physician communication. Journal of the 
American Medical Association, 289, 92. 


Folk, C., & Remington, R. (2008, January). 
Bottom-up priming of top-down attentional 
control settings. Visual Cognition, 16, 215-231. 


Folkman, S., & Moskowitz, J. T. (2000). Stress, 
positive emotion, and coping. Current 
Directions in Psychological Science, 9, 115-118. 


Folkman, S., & Moskowitz, J. T. (2004). Coping: 
Pitfalls and promise. Annual Review of 
Psychology, 55, 745-774. 


Follett, K., & Hess, T. M. (2002). Aging, cognitive 
complexity, and the fundamental attribution 
error. Journal of Gerontology: Series B: Psy- 
chological Sciences and Social Sciences, 57B, 
P312-P323. 


Forbes, G. B., Jung, J., & Haas, K. B. (2006). 
Benevolent sexism and cosmetic use: A repli- 
cation with three college samples and one 
adult sample. Journal of Social Psychology, 146, 
635-640. 

Forbey, J., & Ben-Porath, Y. (2007). Computerized 
adaptive personality testing: A review and 
illustration with the MMPI-2 computerized 
adaptive version. Psychological Assessment, 19, 
14-24. 

Ford, C. S., & Beach, F. A. (1951). Patterns of sexual 
behavior. New York: Harper. 


Forer, B. (1949). The fallacy of personal valida- 
tion: A classroom demonstration of gullibility. 
Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 44, 
118-123. 


Forgas, J. P., & Laham, S. M. (2005). The interac- 
tion between affect and motivation in social 
judgments and behavior. In J. P. Forgas, K. P. 
Williams, & S. M. Laham (Eds.), Social motiva- 
tion: Conscious and unconscious processes. New 
York: Cambridge University Press. 


Forlenza, M. J., & Baum, M. J. (2004). Psycho- 
neuroimmunology. In T. J. Boll, R. G. Frank, 
A. Baum, & J. L. Wallander (Eds). Handbook of 
clinical health psychology, Vol. 3: Models and per- 
spectives in health psychology. Washington, DC: 
American Psychological Association 81-114. 


Foster, K. M. (2005). Introduction: John Uzo Ogbu 
(1939-2003): How do you ensure the fair con- 
sideration of a complex ancestor? Multiple 
approaches to assessing the work and legacy 
of John Uzo Ogbu. International Journal of 
Qualitative Studies in Education, 18, 559-564. 


Foster, K., & Stiffman, A. (2009). Child welfare 
workers’ adoption of decision support tech- 
nology. Journal of Technology in Human 
Services, 27, 106-126. 


Foster, P., Drago, V., FitzGerald, D., Skoblar, B., 
Crucian, G., & Heilman, K. (2008). Spreading 
activation of lexical-semantic networks in 
Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychologia, 46, 
1908-1914. 


Foulds, J., Gandhi, K. K., Steinberg, M. B., 
Richardson, D. L., Williams, J. M., Burke, 
M. V., et al. (2006). Factors associated with 
quitting smoking at a tobacco dependence 
treatment clinic. American Journal of Health 
Behavior, 30, 400-412. 


Fountas, K. N., & Smith, J. R. (2007). Historical 
evolution of stereotactic amygdalotomy for 
the management of severe aggression. Journal 
of Neurosurgery, 106, 716-713. 


Fournier, J., Deremaux, S., & Bernier, M. (2008). 
Content, characteristics and function of 


mental images. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 
9, 734-748. 


Fowler, C. A., & Galantucci, B. (2008). The 
relation of speech perception and speech 
production. In Pisoni, D. B. & Remez, R. E. 
(Eds)., The handbook of speech perception. 
Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishing. 


Fowler, J., & Christakis, N. (2009). Dynamic 
spread of happiness in a large social network: 
Longitudinal analysis over 20 years in the 
Framingham Heart Study. British Medical 
Journal, 338, 1-13. 


Fox, C. R. (2006). The availability heuristic in the 
classroom: How soliciting more criticism can 
boost your course ratings. Judgment and 
Decision Making, 1, 86-90. 


Fox, S., & Spector, P. E. (2000). Relations of emo- 
tional intelligence, practical intelligence, gen- 
eral intelligence, and trait affectivity with 
interview outcomes: It’s not all just “G.” 
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 21, 203-220. 


Frankenberg, W. K., et al. (1992). Denver II training 
manual. Denver, CO: Denver Developmental 
Materials. 

Franklin, A., Pilling, M., & Davies, I. (2005). The 
nature of infant color categorization: Evidence 
from eye movements on a target decision 


References R-17 


task. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 
91, 227-248. 


Franklin, M. E., March, J. S., & Garcia, A. (2007). 
Treating obsessive-compulsive disorder in 
children and adolescents. In C. Purdon, 

M. M. Antony, & L. J. Summerfeldt, (Eds.), 
Psychological treatment of obsessive-compulsive 
disorder: Fundamentals and beyond. Washington, 
DC: American Psychological Association. 


Franko, D., & Striegel-Moore, R. (2002). The role 
of body dissatisfaction as a risk factor for 
depression in adolescent girls: Are the 
differences black and white? Journal of 
Psychosomatic Research, 53, 975-983. 


Franks, D. D., & Smith, T. S. (1999) (Eds.). Mind, 
brain, and society: Toward a neuro-sociology of 
emotion. Stamford, CT: JAI Press. 


Franzek, E., & Beckmann, H. (1996). Gene- 
environment interaction in schizophrenia: 
Season-of-birth effect reveals etiologically dif- 
ferent subgroups. Psychopathology, 29, 14-26. 


Frasure-Smith, N., Lesperance, F., & Talajic, M. 
(2000). The prognostic importance of depres- 
sion, anxiety, anger, and social support fol- 
lowing myocardial infarction: Opportunities 
for improving survival. In P. M. McCabe, 

N. Schneiderman, T. M. Field, & A. R. Wellens 
(Eds.), Stress, coping, and cardiovascular disease. 
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 


Frawley, T. (2008). Gender schema and prejudi- 
cial recall: How children misremember, 
fabricate, and distort gendered picture book 
information. Journal of Research in Childhood 
Education, 22, 291-303. 


Fredrickson, B. L., & Branigan, C. (2005). Positive 
emotions broaden the scope of attention and 
thought-action repertoires. Cognition and 
Emotion, 19, 313-332. 


Freedman, D. S. (1995). The importance of body 
fat distribution in early life. American Journal 
of the Medical Sciences, 310, S72-S76. 


Freedman, J. L., & Fraser, S. C. (1966). Comp- 
liance without pressure: The foot-in-the-door 
technique. Journal of Personality and Social 
Psychology, 4, 195-202. 


Freeman, J. E. (2006). Psychology of race and 
ethnicity. In W. Buskit, & S. F. Davis. Handbook 
of the teaching of psychology. Malden, MA: 
Blackwell Publishing. 


Frensch, P. A., & Riinger, D. (2003). Implicit learn- 
ing. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 
12, 13-18. 

Freud, S. (1922/1959). Group psychology and the 
analysis of the ego. London: Hogarth. 


Friborg, O., Barlaug, D., Martinussen, M., 
Rosenvinge, J. H., & Hjemdal, O. (2005). 
Resilience in relation to personality and 
intelligence. International Journal of Methods 
in Psychiatric Research, 14, 29—42. 


Friborg, O., Hjemdal, O., & Rosenvinge, J. H. (2006). 
Resilience as a moderator of pain and stress. 
Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 61, 213-219. 


Friedberg, R. D. (2006). A cognitive-behavioral 
approach to family therapy. Journal of Contem- 
porary Psychotherapy, 36, 159-165. 


Friedman, J. N. W., Oltmanns, T. F., & Turkheimer, 
E. (2007). Interpersonal perception and per- 
sonality disorders: Utilization of a thin slice 
approach. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 
667—688. 


R-18 References 


Friedman, M. J. (2006). Posttraumatic stress 
disorder among military returnees from 
Afghanistan and Iraq. American Journal of 
Psychiatry, 163, 586-593. 


Frijda, N. H. (2005). Emotion experience. 
Cognition and Emotion, 19, 473-497. 


Frincke, J. L., & Pate, W. E., II. (2004, March). 
Yesterday, today, and tomorrow. Careers in 
Psychology 2004, what students need to know. 
Paper presented at the Annual Convention of 
the Southeastern Psychological Association, 
Atlanta, GA. 


Frings, L., Wagner, K., Unterrainer, J., Spreer, J., 
Halsband, U., & Schulze-Bonhage, A. (2006). 
Gender-related differences in lateralization of 
hippocampal activation and cognitive strat- 
egy. Neuroreport, 17, 417-421. 


Fritsch, T., McClendon, M. J., Smyth, K. A., 
Lerner, A. J., Friedland, R. P., & Larsen, J. D. 
(2007). Cognitive functioning in healthy 
aging: The role of reserve and lifestyle factors 
early in life. Gerontologist, 47, 307-322. 


Frost, L. E., & Bonnie, R. J. (Eds.). (2001). The 
evolution of mental health law. Washington, DC: 
American Psychological Association. 


Frost, R. O., & Steketee, G. (Eds.). (2002). Cognitive 
approaches to obsessions and compulsions: Theory, 
assessment, and treatment. New York: Pergamon 
Press. 


Fuller, A. (2006). Hypnosis and ideomotor com- 
pliance in the treatment of smoking tobacco 
and cannabis. Australian Journal of Clinical 
Hypnotherapy and Hypnosis, 27, 14-18. 


Funder, D. C. (1991). Global traits: A neo-Allportian 
approach to personality. Psychological Science, 2, 
31-39. 


Furnham, A., & Crump, J. (2005). Personality 
traits, types, and disorders: An examination 
of the relationship between three self-report 
measures. European Journal of Personality, 19, 
167-184. 


Furnham, A., & Swami, V. (2007). Perception of 
female buttocks and breast size in profile. 
Social Behavior and Personality, 35, 1-8. 


Furumoto, L., & Scarborough, E. (2002). Placing 
women in the history of psychology: The first 
American women psychologists. In W. E. 
Pickren (Ed.), Evolving perspectives on the his- 
tory of psychology. Washington, DC: American 
Psychological Association. 


Fusari, A., & Ballesteros, S. (2008, August). 
Identification of odors of edible and nonedi- 
ble stimuli as affected by age and gender. 
Behavior Research Methods, 40, 752-759. 


Gaab, J., Rohleder, N., Nater, U. M., & Ehlert, U. 
(2005). Psychological determinants of the 
cortisol stress response: The role of anticipatory 
cognitive appraisal. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 
30, 599-610. 


Gaffney, K. F., & Henry, L. L. (2007). Identifying 
risk factors for postpartum tobacco use. 
Journal of Nursing Scholarship, 39, 126-132. 


Gagnon, G. H. (1977). Human sexualities. Glenview, 
IL: Scott, Foresman. 


Galanter, E. (1962). Contemporary psychophysics. 
In R. Brown, E. Galanter, E. Hess, & G. Maroler 
(Eds.), New directions in psychology. New York: 
Holt. 


Galanter, M. (2007). Spirituality and recovery in 
12-step programs: An empirical model. Journal 
of Substance Abuse Treatment, 33, 265-272. 


Galatzer-Levy, R. M., & Cohler, B. J. (1997). 
Essential psychoanalysis: A contemporary intro- 
duction. New York: Basic Books. 


Galef, D. (2001, April 27). The information you 
provide is anonymous, but what was your 
name again? The Chronicle of Higher Education, 
47, p. B5. 


Gallup News Service (1998). Adults who find that 
premarital sex is not wrong. Washington, DC: 
Gallup News Service. 


Gallup Poll. (2001, June 8). Americans’ belief in 
psychic and paranormal phenomena is up over last 
decade. Washington, DC: The Gallup 
Organization. 


Gami, A. S., Howard, D. E., Olson, E. J., & 
Somers, V. K. (2005). Day-night pattern of 
sudden death in obstructive sleep apnea. New 
England Journal of Medicine, 353, 1206-1214. 


Gangestad, S. W., Simpson, J. A., Cousins, A. J., 
Garver-Apgar, C. E., & Christensen, P. N. 
(2004). Women’s preferences for male behav- 
ioral displays change across the menstrual 
cycle. Psychological Science, 15, 203-207. 


Ganong, L. H., & Coleman, M. (1999). Changing 
families, changing responsibilities: Family 
obligations following divorce and remarriage. 
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 


Garb, H. N., Wood, J. M., Lilenfeld, S. O., & 
Nezworski, M. T. (2005). Roots of the Ror- 
schach controversy. Clinical Psychology 
Review, 25, 97-118. 


Garber, J., Clarke, G., Weersing, V., Beardslee, W., 
Brent, D., Gladstone, T., et al. (2009). Preven- 
tion of depression in at-risk adolescents: A 
randomized controlled trial. Journal of the 
American Medical Association, 301, 2215-2224. 


Garber, J., & Horowitz, J. L. (2002). Depression in 
children. In I. H. Gotlib & C. L. Hammen 
(Eds.), Handbook of depression. New York: 
Guilford Press. 


Garcia, J. (1990). Learning without memory. 
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2, 287-305. 


Garcia, J. (2003). Psychology is not an enclave. In 
R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Psychologists defying the 
crowd: Stories of those who battled the establish- 
ment and won. Washington, DC: American 
Psychological Association. 


Garcia, S. M., Weaver, K., Moskowitz, G. B., & 
Darley, J. M. (2002). Crowded minds: The 
implicit bystander effect. Journal of Personality 
and Social Psychology, 83, 843-853. 


Garcia-Andrade, C., Wall, T. L. & Ehlers, C. L. 
(1997). The firewater myth and response to 
alcohol in Mission Indians. Journal of 
Psychiatry, 154, 983-988. 


Garcia-Palacios, A., Hoffman, H., & Carlin, 
A. (2002). Virtual reality in the treatment of 
spider phobia: A controlled study. Behavior 
Research & Therapy, 40, 983-993. 


Gardini, S., Cornoldi, C., De Beni, R., & Venneri, 
A. (2009). Cognitive and neuronal processes 
involved in sequential generation of general 
and specific mental images. Psychological 
Research/Psychologische Forschung, 73, 
633-643. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Gardner, B., & O'Connor, D. (2008). A review of 
the cognitive effects of electroconvulsive 
therapy in older adults. The Journal of ECT, 24, 
68-80. 


Gardner, E. P., & Kandel, E. R. (2000). Touch. In 
E. R. Kandel, J. H. Schwartz, & T. M. Jessell 
(Eds.), Principles of neural science (4th ed.). New 
York: McGraw-Hill. 


Gardner, F., & Moore, Z. (2008). Understanding 
clinical anger and violence: The anger avoid- 
ance model. Behavior Modification, 32, 897-912. 


Gardner, H. (1975). The shattered mind: The person 
after brain damage. New York: Knopf. 


Gardner, H. (1999). Intelligence reframed: Multiple 
intelligences for the 21st century. New York: 
Basic Books. 


Gardner, H. (2000). The giftedness matrix: A 
developmental perspective. In R. C. Friedman 
& B. M. Shore (Eds.). Talents unfolding: Cogni- 
tion and development. Washington, DC: 
American Psychological Association. 


Gardner, H. (2005). Scientific psychology: Should 
we bury it or praise it? In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), 
Unity in psychology: Possibility or pipe dream? 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


Gardner, H. (2006). Multiple Intelligences: New 
Horizons in theory and practice by New York: 
Basic Books. 


Garlow, S. J., Purselle, D. C., & Heninger, M. 
(2007). Cocaine and alcohol use preceding 
suicide in African American and White 
adolescents. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 41, 
530-536. 


Garn, C. L., Allen, M. D., Larsen, J. D., & Cortex, 
A. (2009). An fMRI study of sex differences in 
brain activation during object naming. Journal 
Devoted to the Study of the Nervous System and 
Behavior, 45, 610—618. 


Garrigan, P., & Kellman, P. (2008, February). 
Perceptual learning depends on perceptual 
constancy. PNAS Proceedings of the National 
Academy of Sciences of the United States of 
America, 105, 2248-2253. 


Garwick, G. B. (2007). Intelligence-related terms 
in mental retardation, learning disability, and 
gifted/talented professional usage, 1983- 
2001: The 1992 mental retardation redefinition 
as natural experiment. Dissertation Abstracts 
International Section A: Humanities and Social 
Sciences, 67(9-A), 3296. 


Garwood, C. L., & Potts, L. A. (2007). Emerging 
pharmacotherapies for smoking cessation. 
American Journal of Health Systems Pharma- 
cology, 64, 1693-1698. 


Garza-Guerrero, C. (2000). Idealization and 
mourning in love relationships: Normal and 
pathological spectra. Psychoanalytic Quarterly, 
69, 121-150. 


Gass, C. S., Luis, C. A., Meyers, T. L., & Kuljis, 
R. O. (2000). Familial Creutzfeldt-Jakob disease: 
A neuro-psychological case study. Archives of 
Clinical Neuropsychology, 15, 165-175. 


Gatchel, R. J., & Oordt. M. S. (2003). Obesity. In 
R. J. Gatchel & M. S. Oordt, Clinical health psy- 
chology and primary care: Practical advice and 
clinical guidance for successful collaboration. 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Gatchel, R. J. & Weisberg, J. N. (2000). Personality 
characteristics of patients with pain. Washington, 
DC: APA Books. 


Gathercole, S. E., & Baddeley, A.D. (1993). 
Working memory and language processing. 
Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. 


Gazzaniga, M. S. (1998, July). The split brain 
revisited. Scientific American, pp. 50-55. 


Gazzaniga, M. S., Ivry, R. B., & Mangun, G. R. 
(2002). Cognitive neuroscience: The biology of the 
mind (2nd ed.). New York: W. W. Norton. 

Ge, X., Kim, I. J., Brody, G. H., Conger, R. D., 
Simons, R. L., Gibbons, F. X., et al. (2003). It’s 
about timing and change: Pubertal transition 
effects on symptoms of major depression 
among African American youths. 
Developmental Psychology, 39, 430-439. 


Gegenfurtner, K. R. (2003). Color vision. Annual 
Review of Neuroscience, 26, 181-206. 


Gelbard-Sagiv, H., Mukamel, R., Harel, M., 
Malach, R., & Fried, I. (March 2008). Internally 
generated reactivation of single neurons in 
human hippocampus during free recall. 
Science, 322, 96-101. 


Gelfand, M. M. (2000). Sexuality among older 
women. Journal of Women’s Health and Gender 
Based Medicine, 9(Suppl. 1), S15-S20. 


Gelman, R., & Baillargeon, R. (1983). A review of 
some Piagetian concepts. In J. H. Flavell & 
E. M. Markman (Eds.), Handbook of child psy- 
chology, Vol. 3: Cognitive development (4th ed.). 
New York: Wiley. 


Gelman, R., & Kit-Fong Au, T. (Eds.). (1996). 
Perceptual and cognitive development. New 
York: Academic Press. 


Gennaro, R. J. (2004). Higher-order theories of 
consciousness: An anthology. Amsterdam, 
Netherlands: John Benjamins. 


Genovese, J. E. C. (2006). Piaget, pedagogy, and 
evolutionary psychology. Evolutionary 
Psychology, 4, 2127-2137. 


Gentile, B., Grabe, S., Dolan-Pascoe, B., Twenge, 
J., Wells, B., & Maitino, A. (2009). Gender 
differences in domain-specific self-esteem: A 
meta-analysis. Review of General Psychology, 
13, 34-45. 


Gentner, D., Goldin, S., & Goldin-Meadow, S. 
(Eds.). (2003). Language in mind: Advances in 
the study of language and cognition. Cambridge, 
MA: MIT. 


George, M. S., Wassermann, E. M., Williams, 
W. A., Callahan, A., Ketter, T. A., Baser, P., et al. 
(1995). Daily repetitive transcranial magnetic 
stimulations (rTMS) improves mood in 
depression. Neuroreport: An International 
Journal for the Rapid Communication of Research 
in Neuroscience, 6, 1853-1856. 


George, S., & Moselhy, H. (2005). Cocaine-induced 
trichotillomania. Addiction, 100, 255-256. 


George, T. P. (1999). Design, measurement, and 
analysis in developmental research. In 
M. Bornstein & M. Lamb, Developmental psy- 
chology. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 

Georgiou, G. A., Bleakley, C., Hayward, J., Russo, 
R., Dutton, K., Eltiti, S., et al. (2005). Focusing 
on fear: Attentional disengagement from 
emotional faces. Visual Cognition, 12, 145-158. 

Gerdes, A., Uhl, G., & Alpers, G. (2009). Spiders 
are special: Fear and disgust evoked by pic- 


tures of arthropods. Evolution and Human 
Behavior, 30, 66-73. 


Gershkoff-Stowe, L., Connell, B., & Smith, L. (2006). 
Priming overgeneralizations in two- and four- 
year-old children. Journal of Child Language, 
33, 461-486. 


Gerstel, N. (2005, April 8). In search of time. 
Science, 308, 204-205. 


Getner, D., & Holyoak, K. J. (1997, January). 
Reasoning and learning by analogy. American 
Psychologist, 52, 32-34. 


Giacobbi, P. R., Jr., Lynn, T. K., Wetherington, 
J. M., Jenkins, J., Bodendorf, M., & Langley, 
B. (2004). Stress and coping during the transi- 
tion to university for first-year female athletes. 
Sports Psychologist, 18, 1-20. 


Giacomini, M., Baylis, F., & Robert, J. (2007). 
Banking on it: Public policy and the ethics of 
stem cell research and development. Social 
Sciences Medicine, 22, 88-84. 


Gibb, K., Tunbridge, D., Chua, A., & Frederickson, 
N. (2007). Pathways to inclusion: Moving from 
special school to mainstream. Educational 
Psychology in Practice, 23, 109-127. 


Gibbons, R. D., Brown, C. H., Hur, K., Marcus, 
S. M., Bhamik, D. K., Erkens, J. A., et al. (2007). 
Early evidence on the effects of regulators’ 
suicidal warnings on SSRI prescriptions and 
suicide in children and adolescents. American 
Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 1356-1363. 


Gibbs, N. (2005, August 8). Being 13. Time, 
pp. 41-55. 


Gibbs, W. W. (2002, August.) From mouth to 
mind. Scientific American, p. 26. 


Gigerenzer, G. (2007). Gut feelings: The intelligence 
of the unconscious. Penguin Group: New York. 


Gilbert, D. T., McNulty, S. E., Guiliano, T. A., & 
Benson, J. E. (1992). Blurry words and fuzzy 
deeds: The attribution of obscure behavior. 
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 62, 
18-25. 


Gilbert, D. T., Miller, A. G., & Ross, L. (1998). 
Speeding with Ned: A personal view of the 
correspondence bias. In J. M. Darley & 

J. Cooper (Eds.), Attribution and social interac- 
tion: The legacy of Edward E. Jones. Washington, 
DC: American Psychological Association. 


Gilbert, P. D. (2007). Spirituality and mental 
health: A very preliminary overview. Current 
Opinions in Psychiatry, 20, 594-598. 


Gilboa, A., Winocur, G., & Rosenbaum, R. S. (2006). 
Hippocampal contributions to recollection in 
retrograde and anterograde amnesia. Hippo- 
campus, 16, 966-980. 


Gilchrist, A., Cowan, N., & Naveh-Benjamin, 
M. (2009). Investigating the childhood devel- 
opment of working memory using sentences: 
New evidence for the growth of chunk capac- 
ity. Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 
104, 252-265. 


Gilestro, G. F., Tononi, G., & Cirelli, C. (2009, 
April 3). Widespread changes in synaptic 
markers as a function of sleep and wakeful- 
ness in Drosophila. Science, 324, 109-112. 


Gilligan, C. (1996). The centrality of relationships 
in psychological development: A puzzle, 
some evidence, and a theory. In G. G. Noam 
& K. W. Fischer (Eds.), Development and vul- 


References R-19 


nerability in close relationships. Hillsdale, NJ: 
Erlbaum. 


Gilligan, C. (2004). Recovering psyche: Reflec- 
tions on life-history and history. Annual of 
Psychoanalysis, 32, 131-147. 


Gladwell, M. (2004, September 20). Annals of 
psychology: Personality, plus how corpora- 
tions figure out who you are. The New Yorker, 
42-45. 


Gladwin, T. (1964). Culture and logical process. 
In N. Goodenough (Ed.), Explorations in 
cultural anthropology: Essays in honor of George 
Peter Murdoch. New York: McGraw-Hill. 


Glass, K., Flory, K., Hankin, B., Kloos, B., & 
Turecki, G. (2009). Are coping strategies, 
social support, and hope associated with 
psychological distress among Hurricane 
Katrina survivors? Journal of Social and 
Clinical Psychology, 28, 779-795. 


Glick, P., Lameiras, M., Fiske, S. T., Eckes, T., 
Masser, B., Volpato, C., et al. (2004). Bad but 
bold: Ambivalent attitudes toward men 
predict gender inequality in 16 nations. 
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 
713-728. 


Glickler, J. (2006). Advancing in advancement: A 
self-efficacy study of development practitio- 
ners in higher education. Dissertation Abstracts 
International: Section B: The Sciences and 
Engineering, 67(2-B), 1190. 


Glicksohn, J., & Nahari, G. (2007). Interacting 
personality traits? Smoking as a test case. 
European Journal of Personality, 21, 225-234. 


Glisky, E. L. (2007). Changes in cognitive 
function in human aging. In D. R. Riddle, 
Brain aging: Models, methods, and mechanisms. 
Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. 


Goffin, R. D., Jelley, R. B., & Wagner, S. H. (2003). 
Is halo helpful? Effects of inducing halo on 
performance rating accuracy. Social Behavior 
and Personality, 31, 625-636. 


Goin, M. K. (2005). A current perspective on the 
psychotherapies. Psychiatric Services, 56, 
255-257. 


Gold, P. E., Cahill, L., & Wenk, G. L. (2002). 
Ginkgo biloba: A cognitive enhancer? 
Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 3, 
2-7. 

Golden, R. N., Gaynes, B. N., Ekstsrom, R. D., 
Hamer, R. M., Jacobsen, F. M., Suppes, T., et 
al. (2005). The efficacy of light therapy in the 
treatment of mood disorders: A review and 
meta-analysis of the evidence. The American 
Journal of Psychiatry, 162, 656—662. 


Golden, W. L. (2006). Hypnotherapy for anxiety, 
phobias and psychophysiological disorders. 
In R. A. Chapman, The clinical use of hypnosis 
in cognitive behavior therapy: A practitioner’s 
casebook. New York: Springer Publishing. 


Goldfried, M. R., & Pachankis, J. E. (2007). On the 
next generation of process research. Clinical 
Psychology Review, 27, 760-768. 


Goldstein, I. (2000). Female sexual arousal 
disorder: New insights. International Journal of 
Impotence Research, 12(Suppl. 4), 5152-8157. 


Goldstein, S. N. (2006). The exploration of spiri- 
tuality and identity status in adolescence. 
Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: 
The Sciences and Engineering, 67(6-B), 3481. 


R-20 References 


Goldstone, R. L., & Kersten, A. (2003). Concepts 
and categorization. In A. F. Healy & R. W. 
Proctor (Eds.), Handbook of psychology: 
Experimental psychology (Vol. 4). New York: 
Wiley. 

Goleman, D. (1988, January 21). Doctor and 
patient; physicians may bungle key part of 
treatment: The medical interview. The New 
York Times, p. B16. 


Goleman, D. (1993, July 21). “Expert” babies 
found to teach others. The New York Times, 
p- C10. 


Golimbet, V. E., Alfimova, M. V., Gritsenko, I. K., 
& Ebstein, R. P. (2007). Relationship between 
dopamine system genes and extraversion and 
novelty seeking. Neuroscience Behavior and 
Physiology, 37, 601—606. 


Golombok, S., Cook, R., Bish, A., & Murray, C. 
(1995). Families created by the new reproduc- 
tive technologies: Quality of parenting and 
social and emotional development of the chil- 
dren. Child Development, 66, 285-298. 


Gontier, N. (2008). Genes, brains, and language: 
An epistemological examination of how 
genes can underlie human cognitive behavior. 
Review of General Psychology, 12, 170-180. 


Gontkovsky, S. T. (2005). Neurobiological bases 
and neuropsychological correlates of aggres- 
sion and violence. In J. P. Morgan (Ed.), Psy- 
chology of aggression. Hauppauge, NY: Nova 
Science Publishers. 


Gontkovsky, S. T., & Beatty, W. W. (2006). 
Practical methods for the clinical assessment 
of information processing speed. Inter- 
national Journal of Neuroscience, 116, 
1317-1325. 


Goode, E. (1999, April 13). If things taste bad, 
“phantoms” may be at work. The New York 
Times, pp. D1-D2. 


Goode, E. (2003, January 28). Even in the age of 
Prozac, some still prefer the couch. The New 
York Times, Section F, p. 1. 


Goodman, G., & Quas, J. (2008). Repeated inter- 
views and children’s memory: It’s more than 
just how many. Current Directions in Psycho- 
logical Science, 17, 386-390. 


Goodman, W., K., Rudorfer, M. V., & Maser, J. D. 
(2000). Obsessive-compulsive disorder: Contem- 
porary issues in treatment. Mahwah, NJ: 
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 


Goodwin, R., Costa, P., & Adonu, J. (2004). Social 
support and its consequences: ‘Positive’ and 
‘deficiency’ values and their implications for 
support and self-esteem. British Journal of 
Social Psychology, 43, 465-474. 


Goodwin, R. D., & Hamilton, S. P. (2003). Lifetime 
comorbidity of antisocial personality disorder 
and anxiety disorders among adults in the 
community. Psychiatry Research, 117, 159-166. 


Gooren, L. (2006). The biology of human psycho- 
sexual differentiation. Hormones and Behavior, 
50, 589-601. 

Gordon, E. F. (2001). Mockingbird years: A life in 
and out of therapy. New York: Basic Books. 

Gossop, M., Stewart, D., & Marsden, J. (2008). 
Attendance at Narcotics Anonymous and 
Alcoholics Anonymous meetings, frequency 
of attendance and substance use outcomes 
after residential treatment for drug depen- 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


dence: A 5-year follow-up study. Addiction, 
103, 119-125. 


Gotlib, I. H., Krasnoperova, E., Yue, D. N., & 
Joorman, J. (2004). Attentional biases for nega- 
tive interpersonal stimuli in clinical depression. 
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 113, 127-135. 


Gottesman, I. I. (1991). Schizophrenia genesis: The 
origins of madness. New York: Freeman. 


Gottesman, I. I., & Hanson, D. R. (2005). Human 
development: Biological and genetic processes. 
Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 263-286. 


Gottlieb, D. A. (2004). Acquisition with partial and 
continuous reinforcement in pigeon autoshap- 
ing. Learning and Behavior, 32, 321-334. 


Gottlieb, D. A. (2006). Effects of partial reinforce- 
ment and time between reinforced trials on 
terminal response rate in pigeon autoshaping. 
Behavioural Processes, 72, 6-13. 


Gould, E., Reeves, A. J., Graziano, M.S. A., & 
Gross, C. G. (1999, October 15). Neurogenesis 
in the neocortex of adult primates. Science, 
548-552. 


Gould, R. L. (1978). Transformations. New York: 
Simon & Schuster. 


Govindarajan, A., Kelleher, R. J., & Tonegawa, S. 
(2006). A clustered plasticity model of long- 
term memory engrams. Nature Reviews 
Neuroscience, 7, 575-583. 


Gowaty, P. A. (2003). Power asymmetries between 
the sexes, mate preferences, and components 
of fitness. In C. B. Travis (Ed.), Evolution, 
gender, and rape. Cambridge, MA: MIT. 


Gradinaru, V., Mogri, M., Thompson, K. R., 
Henderson, J. M., & Deisseroth, K. (2009). 
Optical deconstruction of Parkinsonian neu- 
ral circuitry. Science, 324, 354-359. 


Grady, D., & Altman, L. K. (2008, December 29). 
Lessons for other smokers in Obama’s efforts 
to quit. The New York Times, p. A12. 


Graham, C. A., Bancroft, J., & Doll, H. A. (2007). 
Does oral contraceptive-induced reduction in 
free testosterone adversely affect the sexuality 


or mood of women? Psychoneuroendocrinology, 
32, 246-255. 


Graham, S. (1992). Most of the subjects were 
white and middle class. American Psychologist 
47(5), 629-639. 


Grahek, N. (2007). Feeling pain and being in pain 
(2nd ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. 


Granic, I., Hollenstein, T., & Dishion, T. (2003). 
Longitudinal analysis of flexibility and 
reorganization in early adolescence: A 
dynamic systems study of family interactions. 
Developmental Psychology, 39, 606-617. 


Grann, J. D. (2007). Confidence in knowledge past: 
An empirical basis for a differential decay the- 
ory of very long-term memory monitoring. 
Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: 
Humanities and Social Sciences, 67, 2462. 


Grant, A. (2008). Does intrinsic motivation fuel 
the prosocial fire? Motivational synergy in 
predicting persistence, performance, and 
productivity. Journal of Applied Psychology, 93, 
48-58. 


Gray, G. C. (2006). The regulation of corporate 
violations: Punishment, compliance, and the 
blurring of responsibility. British Journal of 
Criminology, 46, 875-892. 


Gray, J. R., Chabris, C. F., & Braver, T. S. (2003, 
February 18). Neural mechanisms of general 
fluid intelligence. Nature: Neurosciences 
Retrieved from http://www.nature.com/ 
cgitaf / DynaPage.taf?file=/neuro/journal/ 
vaop/ncurrent/abs/nn1014.html 


Graziano, M. S., Taylor, C. S., & Moore, T. (2002). 
Complex movements evolved by microstimu- 
lation of precentral cortex. Neuron, 34, 841-851. 


Graziano, W. G., Habashi, M. M., Sheese, B. E., & 
Tobin, R. M. (2007). Agreeableness, empathy, 
and helping: A person situation perspective. 
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 
583-599. 


Green, B. G., & George, P. (2004). Thermal taste 
predicts higher responsiveness to chemical 
taste and flavor. Chemical Senses, 29, 617-628. 


Green, J., Lynn, S., & Montgomery, G. (2008, 
January). Gender-related differences in 
hypnosis-based treatments for smoking: A 
follow-up meta-analysis. American Journal of 
Clinical Hypnosis, 50, 259-271. 


Green, J. S., Henderson, F. R., & Collinge, M. D. 
(2003). Prevention and control of wildlife damage: 
Coyotes. Lincoln: University of Nebraska, In- 
stitute of Agriculture and Natural Resources. 
Retrieved from http://wildlifedamage.unl. 
edu/handbook/handbook/carnivor/ 
ca_c51.pdf 


Greenberg, G., & Rosenheck, R. (2008). Jail 
incarceration, homelessness, and mental 
health: A national study. Psychiatric Services, 
59, 170-177. 


Greenberg, R., & Goldman, E. (2009). Antide- 
pressants, psychotherapy or their combina- 
tion: Weighing options for depression 
treatments. Journal of Contemporary 
Psychotherapy, 39, 83-91. 


Greenberg, R. M., & Kellner, C. H. (2005). 
Electroconvulsive therapy: A selected review. 
The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 13, 
268-281. 


Greene, J. D., & Paxton, J. M. (2009). Patterns of 
neural activity associated with honest and 
dishonest moral decisions. PNAS Proceedings 
of the National Academy of Sciences of the United 
States of America, 106(30), 12506-12511. 


Greene, J. D., Sommerville, R. B., Nystrom, L. E., 
Darley, J. M., & Cohen, J. D. (2001, September 
14). An fMRI investigation of emotional 
engagement in moral judgment. Science, 293, 
2105-2108. 


Greenfield, P. M. (1997). You can’t take it with 
you: Why ability assessments don’t cross 
cultures. American Psychologist, 52, 1115-1124. 


Greenfield, S. (2002). Mind, brain and con- 
sciousness. British Journal of Psychiatry, 181, 
91-93. 


Greenspan, S. (2006). Functional concepts in 
mental retardation: Finding the natural 
essence of an artificial category. Exceptionality, 
14, 205-224. 

Greenwald, A. G., Draine S. C., & Abrams, R. L. 
(1996, September 20). Three cognitive markers 


of unconscious semantic activation. Science, 
272, 1699-1702. 


Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Banaji, M. R. 
(2003). Understanding and using the Implicit 
Association Test: 1. An improved scoring 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


algorithm. Journal of Personality and Social 
Psychology 85, 197-216. 


Greenwald, A. G., Nosek, B. A., & Sririam, N. 
(2006). Consequential validity of the implicit 
association test: Comment on Blanton and 
Jaccard. American Psychologist, 61, 56—61. 


Greenwald, A., Poehlman, T., Uhlmann, E., & 
Banaji, M. (2009). Understanding and using 
the Implicit Association Test: III. Meta- 
analysis of predictive validity. Journal of 
Personality and Social Psychology, 97, 17-41. 


Greer, R. D., Dudek-Singer, J., & Gautreaux, G. 
(2006). Observational learning. International 
Journal of Psychology, 41, 486-499. 


Gregg, J. P., Lit, L., Baron, C. A., Hertz-Picciotto, 
I., Walker, W., Davis, R. A., et al. (2007). Gene 
expression changes in children with autism. 
Genomics, 12, 88-97. 


Gregory, R. L. (1978). The psychology of seeing (3rd 
ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. 


Gregory, R. L. (2008). Emmert’s Law and the 
moon illusion. Spatial Vision, 21, 407-720. 


Gregory, S. (1856). Facts for young women. Boston. 


Gregory, S. S. (1994, March 21). At risk of mutila- 
tion. Time, pp. 45-46. 


Grigorenko, E. (2009). Speaking genes or genes for 
speaking? Deciphering the genetics of speech 
and language. Journal of Child Psychology and 
Psychiatry, 50, 116-125. 


Grigoriadis, S., & Ravitz, P. (2007). An approach 
to interpersonal psychotherapy for postpartum 
depression: Focusing on interpersonal 
changes. Canadian Family Physician, 53, 
1469-1475. 


Grimes, T., & Bergen, L. (2008, April). The 
epistemological argument against a causal 
relationship between media violence and 
sociopathic behavior among psychologically 
well viewers. American Behavioral Scientist, 51, 
1137-1154. 


Grindstaff, J. S., & Fisher, L. A. (2006). Sport 
psychology consultants’ experience of using 
hypnosis in their practice: An exploratory 
investigation. The Sport Psychologist, 20, 
368-386. 


Gronholm, P., Rinne, J. O., Vorobyev, V., & Laine, 
M. (2005). Naming of newly learned objects: 
A PET activation study. Brain Research and 
Cognitive Brain Research, 14, 22-28. 


Gross, D. M. (2006). The secret history of emotion: 
From Aristotle's rhetoric to modern brain science. 
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 


Grossmann, T., Striano, T., & Friederici, A. D. 
(2007). Developmental changes in infants’ 
processing of happy and angry facial expres- 
sions: A neurobehavioral study. Brain and 
Cognition, 64, 30-41. 


Groves, R. M., Singer, E., Lepkowski, J. M., 
Heeringa, S. G., & Alwin, D. F. In S. J. House, 
F. T. Juster, R. L. Kahn, H. Schuman, & E. 
Singer. (2004). A telescope on society: Survey 
research and social science at the University of 
Michigan and beyond. Ann Arbor, MI: University 
of Michgan Press. 


Grucza, R., Norberg, K., & Bierut, L. (2009). Binge 
drinking among youths and young adults in 
the United States: 1979-2006. Journal of the 
American Academy of Child & Adolescent 
Psychiatry, 48, 692-702. 


Grunwald, T., Boutros, N. N., Pezer, N., von 
Oertzen, J., Fernandez, G., Schaller, C., & 
Elger, C. E. (2003). Neuronal substrates of sen- 
sory gating within the human brain. Biological 
Psychiatry, 15, 511-519. 


Guadagno, R. E., & Cialdini, R. B. (2002). Online 
persuasion: An examination of gender differ- 
ences in computer-mediated interpersonal 
influence [Special issue: Groups and Internet]. 
Group Dynamics, 6, 38-51. 


Guastella, A., Mitchell, P., & Dadds, M. (2008, 
January). Oxytocin increases gaze to the eye 
region of human faces. Biological Psychiatry, 
63, 3-5. 


Guéguen, N., Marchand, M., Pascual, A., & 
Lourel, M. (2008). Foot-in-the-door technique 
using a courtship request: A field experiment. 
Psychological Reports, 103, 529-534. 


Guerrero, L., La Valley, A., & Farinelli, L. (2008, 
October). The experience and expression of 
anger, guilt, and sadness in marriage: An 
equity theory explanation. Journal of Social and 
Personal Relationships, 25, 699-724. 


Guilleminault, C., Kirisoglu, C., Bao, G., Arias, V., 
Chan, A., & Li, K. K. (2005). Adult chronic 
sleepwalking and its treatment based on poly- 
somnography. Brain, 128 (Pt. 5), 1062-1069. 


Guldemond, H., Bosker, R., Kuyper, H., & van der 
Werf, G. (2007). Do highly gifted students really 
have problems? [Special issue: Current research 
on giftedness: International perspectives]. 
Educational Research and Evaluation, 13, 555-568. 


Gurin, P. (2006). Informing theory from practice 
and applied research. Journal of Social Issues, 
62, 621-628. 


Guthrie, R. V. (1998). Even the rat was white: A his- 
torical view of psychology (2nd ed.). Needham 
Heights, MA: Allyn & Bacon. 


Gutierrez, P. M., Muehlenkamp, J. L., Konick, 
L. C., & Osman, A. (2005). What role does 
race play in adolescent suicidal ideation? 
Archives of Suicide Research, 9, 177-192. 


Gwynn, M. I., & Spanos, N. P. (1996). Hypnotic 
responsiveness, nonhypnotic suggestibility, 
and responsiveness to social influence. In 
R. G. Kunzendorf, N. P. Spahos, & B. Wallace 
(Eds.), Hypnosis and imagination. Amityville, 
NY: Baywood. 


Haberstick, B. C., Schmitz, S., Young, S. E., & 
Hewitt, J. K. (2005).Contributions of genes 
and environments to stability and change in 
externalizing and internalizing problems 
during elementary and middle school. 
Behavior Genetics, 35, 381-396. 


Hackam, D. G. (2007). Translating animal 
research into clinical benefit. British Medical 
Journal, 334, 163-1644. 


Hadjistavropoulos, T., Craig, K. D., & Fuchs- 
Lacelle, S. (2004). Social influences and the 
communication of pain. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence 
Erlbaum Associates. 

Haier, R. J. (2003). Brain imaging studies of 
intelligence: Individual differences and 
neurobiology. In R. J. Sternberg & J. Lautrey 
(Eds.), Models of intelligence: International 
perspectives. Washington, DC: American 
Psychological Association. 


Haier, R. J., Colom, R., Schroeder, D. H., Condon, 
C. A., Tang, C., Eaves, E., et al. (2009). Gray 


References R-21 


matter and intelligence factors: Is there a 
neuro-g? Intelligence, 37, 136-144. 


Haley, W. E., Clair, J. M., & Saulsberry, K. (1992). 
Family caregiver satisfaction with medical 
care of their demented relatives. Gerontologist, 
32, 219-226. 


Halford, S. (2006). Collapsing the boundaries? 
Fatherhood, organization and home-working. 
Gender, Work & Organization, 13, 383-402. 


Halkitis, P. (2009). Methamphetamine addiction: 
Biological foundations, psychological factors, and 
social consequences. Washington, DC: American 
Psychological Association. 


Hall, G. C. N. (1996). Theory-based assessment, 
treatment, and prevention of sexual aggression. 
New York: Oxford University Press. 


Hall, R. E. (2002). The Bell Curve: Implications for the 
performance of black/white athletes. Social 
Science Journal, 39, 113-118. 


Halle, M, (2009, August 4). Can drilling electrodes 
into your brain help you lose weight? Daily 
Mail (London). 


Hallschmid, M., Benedict, C., Born, J., Fehm, H., 
& Kern, W. (2004). Manipulating central 
nervous mechanisms of food intake and body 
weight regulation by intranasal administra- 
tion of neuropeptides in man. Physiology and 
Behavior, 83, 55-64. 


Halpern, D. F. (2000). Sex differences in cognitive 
abilities (3rd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 


Halpern, D. F. (2005). Psychology at the inter- 
section of work and family: Recommen- 
dations for employers, working families, and 
policy-makers. American Psychologist, 60, 
397-409. 


Halpern, D., & Riggio, H. (2002). Thinking 
critically about critical thinking. Mahwah, NJ: 
Erlbaum. 


Halpert, J. (2003, April 28). What do patients 
want? Newsweek, pp. 63-64. 


Hamani, Y., Sciaki-Tamir, Y., Deri-Hasid, R., Miller- 
Pogrund, T., Milwidsky, A., & Haimov- 
Kochman, R. (2007). Misconceptions about 
oral contraception pills among adolescents 
and physicians. Human Reproduction, 22, 
3078-3083. 


Hamann, S. (2001). Cognitive and neural mecha- 
nisms of emotional memory. Trends in 
Cognitive Sciences, 5, 394—400. 


Hamann, S. B., Ely, T. D., Hoffman, J. M., & Kilts, 
C. D. (2002). Ecstasy and agony: Activation of 
human amygdala in positive and negative 
emotion. Psychological Science, 13, 135-141. 


Hambleton, R. K. (2006). Psychometric models, 
test designs and item types for the next gener- 
ation of educational and psychological tests. 
In D. Bartram, & R. K. Hambleton, Computer- 
based testing and the Internet: Issues and 
advances. New York: John Wiley & Sons. 


Hamer, D. H., Hu, S., Magnuson, V. L., Hu, N., & 
Pattatucci, A. M. L. (1993, July 16). A linkage 
between DNA markers on the X chromo- 
some and male sexual orientation. Science, 
261, 321-327. 


Hamer, M., Taylor, A., & Steptoe, A. (2006). The 
effect of acute aerobic exercise on stress 
related blood pressure responses: A system- 
atic review and meta-analysis. Biological 
Psychology, 71, 183-190. 


R-22 References 


Hamilton, A. (2009, July 11). Can gaming slow 
mental decline in the elderly? Time. Retrieved 
from http://www.time.com/time/health/ 
article /0,8599,1909852,00.html 


Hamilton, A. C., & Martin, R. C. (2007). Semantic 
short-term memory deficits and resolution of 
interference: A case for inhibition? In D. S. 
Gorfein & C. M. Macleod, Inhibition in cognition. 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


Hamilton, W. L., Biener, L., & Brennan, R. T. (2008). 
Do local tobacco regulations influence 
perceived smoking norms? Evidence from 
adult and youth surveys in Massachusetts. 
Health Education Research, 23, 709-722. 


Hammond, C., & Gold, M. (2008). Caffeine 
dependence, withdrawal, overdose and 
treatment: A review. Directions in Psychiatry, 
28, 177-190. 


Hammond, D. C. (2007, April). Review of the 
efficacy of clinical hypnosis with headaches 
and migraines [Special issue: Evidence-based 
practice clinical hypnosis—part 1]. Inter- 
national Journal of Clinical and Experimental 
Hypnosis 55, 207-219. 


Haney, C., & Zimbardo, P. (2009). Persistent 
dispositionalism in interactionist clothing: 
Fundamental attribution error in explaining 
prison abuse. Personality and Social Psychology 
Bulletin, 35, 807-814. 


Hanley, S. J., & Abell, S. C. (2002). Maslow and 
relatedness: Creating an interpersonal model 
of self-actualization. Journal of Humanistic 
Psychology, 42, 37-56. 


Hannon, E. E., & Johnson, S. P. (2005). Infants use 
meter to categorize rhythms and melodies: 
Implications for musical structure learning. 
Cognitive Psychology, 50, 354-377. 


Harding, D.J., & Jencks, C. (2003). Changing 
Attitudes toward premarital sex: Cohort, 
period, and aging effects. The Public Opinion 
Quarterly, 67, 211-226 


Hardison, D. M. (2006). Review of phonetics and 
phonology in language comprehension and 
production: Differences and similarities. 
Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 28, 
138-140. 

Hardt, J., Sidor, A., Nickel, R., Kappis, B., Petrak, 
P., & Egle, U. (2008). Childhood adversities 
and suicide attempts: A retrospective study. 
Journal of Family Violence, 23, 713-718. 


Hardy, L. T., (2007). Attachment theory and 
reactive attachment disorder: theoretical 
perspectives and treatment implications. 
Journal of Child and Adolescent Psychiatric 
Nursing, 20, 27-39. 


Harlow, H. F., & Zimmerman, R. R. (1959). 
Affectional responses in the infant monkey. 
Science, 130, 421-432. 


Harlow, J. M. (1869). Recovery from the passage 
of an iron bar through the head. Massachusetts 
Medical Society Publication, 2, 329-347. 

Harmon-Jones, E., & Winkielman, P. (2007). Social 
neuroscience: Integrating biological and psycho- 
logical explanations of social behavior. New York, 
NY: Guilford Press. 

Harold, G. T., Fincham, F. D., Osborne, L. N., & 
Conger, R. D. (1997). Mom and dad are at it 
again: Adolescent perceptions of marital 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


conflict and adolescent psychological distress. 
Developmental Psychology, 33, 333-350. 


Harper, T. (1978, November 15). It’s not true 
about people 65 or over. Green Bay Press- 
Gazette (Wisconsin), p. D-1. 


Hart, B., & Risley, T. R. (1997). Use of language by 
three-year-old children. Courtesy of Drs. Betty 
Hart and Todd Risley, University of Kansas. 


Hartmann, E. (1967). The biology of dreaming. 
Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas Publisher. 


Hartung, C. M., & Widiger, T. A. (1998). Gender 
differences in the diagnosis of mental disor- 
ders: Conclusions and controversies of the 
DSM-IV. Psychological Bulletin, 123, 260-278. 


Harvard Mental Health Letter (HMHL). (1994, 
March). Brief psychodynamic therapy—Part 
I. Harvard Mental Health Letter, p. 10. 


Harvey, J. H., Wenzel, A., & Sprecher, S. (Eds.). 
(2004). The handbook of sexuality in close rela- 
tionships. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum 
Associates. 

Haslam, A., O’Brien, A., Jetten, J., Penna, S., & 
Vormedal, K. (2005). Taking the strain: Social 
identity, social support, and the experience of 
stress. British Journal of Social Psychology, 44, 
355-370. 


Haslam, C., & Lawrence, W. (2004). Health- 
related behavior and beliefs of pregnant 
smokers. Health Psychology, 23, 486-491. 


Hassold, T. J., & Patterson, D. (1999). Down syn- 
drome: A promising future, together. New York, 
NY: Wiley-Liss. 

Hastings, R. P., & Oakford, S. (2003). Student 
teachers’ attitudes towards the inclusion of 
children with special needs. Educational 
Psychology, 23, 87-94. 


Hatsopoulos, N. G., & Donoghue, J. P. (2009). The 
Science of Neural Interface Systems. Annual 
Review of Neuroscience, 19, 245-251. 


Hauke, C. (2006). The unconscious: Personal and 
collective. In R. K. Papadopoulos, The hand- 
book of Jungian psychology: Theory, practice and 
applications. New York: Routledge. 


Hauser, M. D. (2000). The sound and the fury: 
Primate vocalizations as reflections of 
emotion and thought. In N. L. Wallin & 

B. Merker (Eds.), The origins of music. 
Cambridge, MA: MIT. 


Hauser, M. D., Chomsky, N., & Fitch, W. T. (2002, 
November, 22). The faculty for language: 
What is it, who has it, and how did it evolve? 
Science, 298, 1569-1579. 


Hausmann, R., Tyson, L. D., & Zahidi, S. (2009). 
The Global Gender Gap Report, 2009. Geneva, 
Switzerland: World Economic Forum. 


Havermans, R. C., Mulkens, S., Nederkoorn, C., 
& Jansen, A. (2007). The efficacy of cue expo- 
sure with response prevention in extinguish- 
ing drug and alcohol cue reactivity. Behavioral 
Interventions, 22, 121-135. 


Haviland-Jones, J., & Chen, D. (1999, April 17). 
Human olfactory perception. Paper presented at 
the Association for Chemoreception Sciences, 
Sarasota, Florida. 

Haviland-Jones, J. M., & Wilson, P. J. (2008). A 
‘nose’ for emotion: Emotional information 
and challenges in odors and semiochemicals. 
In M. Lewis, J. M. Haviland-Jones, & L. G. 


Barrett (Eds.), Handbook of emotions (3rd ed.). 
New York: Guilford Press. 


Hawkes, Christopher H., & Doty, R. L. (2009). 
The neurology of olfaction. Cambridge, UK: 
Cambridge University Press. 

Hayflick, L. (2007). Biological aging is no longer 
an unsolved problem. Annals of the New York 
Academy of Sciences, 1100, 1-13. 


Haynes, P., Nixon, J. C., & West, J. F. (1990). Time 
perception and consumer behaviour: Some 
cross-cultural implications. International 
Journal of Consumer Studies, 14, 14-27. 


Hays, P. A. (2008). Addressing cultural complexities 
in practice: Assessment, diagnosis, and therapy 
(2nd ed.). Washington, DC: American 
Psychological Association. 


Health Pages. (2003, March 13). Just what the 
doctor ordered. Retrieved from http://www 
.thehealthpages.com/ articles /ar-drord.html 


Heath, R. A. (2006). The Praeger handbook of 
transsexuality: Changing gender to match 
mindset. Westport, CT: Praeger Publishers / 
Greenwood Publishing. 


Heatherton, T., & Sargent, J. (2009). Does 
watching smoking in movies promote teenage 
smoking? Current Directions in Psychological 
Science, 18, 63-67. 


Hecht, J. M. (2007). The happiness myth: Why 
what we think is right is wrong. A history of 
what really makes us happy. New York: 
HarperSanFrancisco/ HarperCollins. 


Hedgepeth, E. (2005). Different lenses, different 
vision. School Administrator, 62, 36-39. 


Hedges, D. W., Brown, B. L., Shwalk, D. A., 
Godfrey, K., & Larcher, A. M. (2007). The 
efficacy of selective serotonin reuptake 
inhibitors in adult social anxiety disorder: A 
meta-analysis of double-blind, placebo- 
controlled trials. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 
21, 102-111. 


Hegarty, P. (2007). From genius inverts to gen- 
dered intelligence: Lewis Terman and the 
power of the norm [Special issue: Power 
matters: Knowledge politics in the history of 
psychology]. History of Psychology, 10, 132-155. 


Hegarty, P., & Massey, S. (2007). Anti-homosexual 
prejudice . . . as opposed to what? Queer 
theory and the social psychology of anti- 
homosexual attitudes. Journal of Homosexuality, 
52, 47-71. 

Heilman, K. M. (2005). Creativity and the brain. 
New York: Psychology Press. 

Heinrichs, R. W. (2005). The primacy of cognition 
in schizophrenia. American Psychologist, 60, 
229-242. 


Heller, S. (2005). Freud A to Z. New York: Wiley. 


Helmbold, N., Troche, S. & Rammsayer, T. (2007). 
Processing of temporal and nontemporal 
information as predictors of psychometric 
intelligence: A structural-equation-modeling 
approach. Journal of Personality, 75, 985-1006. 

Helms, J. E. (1992). Why is there no study of cul- 
tural equivalence in standardized cognitive 
ability testing? American Psychologist, 47, 
1083-1101. 


Helmuth, L. (2000, August 25). Synapses shout to 
overcome distance. Science, 289, 1273. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Henderlong, J., & Lepper, M. R. (2002). The 
effects of praise on children’s intrinsic moti- 
vation: A review and synthesis. Psychological 
Bulletin, 128, 774-795. 


Henderson, J., Kesmodel, U., & Gray, R. (2007). 
Systematic review of the fetal effects of 
prenatal binge-drinking. Journal of Epidemio- 
logy and Community Health, 61, 1069-1073. 

Henderson, N. D. (1982). Correlations in IQ for 
pairs of people with varying degrees of 
genetic relatedness and shared environment. 
Annual Review of Psychology, 33, 219-243. 


Hennig-Fast, K., Meister, F., Frodl, T., Beraldi, A., 
Padberg, F., Engel, R., et al. (2008). The case of 
persistent retrograde amnesia following a 
dissociative fugue: Neuropsychological and 
neurofunctional underpinnings of loss of 


autobiographical memory and self-awareness. 


Neuropsychologia, 46(12), 2993-3005. 


Henningsen, D. D., Henningsen, M. L., & Eden, 
J. (2006). Examining the symptoms of group- 
think and retrospective sensemaking. Small 
Group Research, 37, 36-64. 


Henry, D., McClellen, D., Rosenthal, L., Dedrick, 
D., & Gosdin, M. (2008, February). Is sleep 
really for sissies? Understanding the role of 
work in insomnia in the US. Social Science & 
Medicine, 66, 715-726. 


Hentschel, U., Smith, G., Draguns, J. G., & 
Elhers, W. (2004). Defense mechanisms: 
Theoretical, research and clinical perspectives. 
Oxford, England: Elsevier Science. 


Herbenick, D., Reece, M., Sanders, S., Dodge, B., 
Ghassemi, A., & Fortenberry, J. (2009). 
Prevalence and characteristics of vibrator use 
by women in the United States: Results from 
a nationally representative study. Journal of 
Sexual Medicine, 6, 1857-1866. 


Herran, A., Carrera, M., & Sierra-Biddle, D. 
(2006). Panic disorder and the onset of agora- 
phobia. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 
60, 395-396. 


Herrington, D. M., & Howard, T. D. (2003). From 
presumed benefit to potential harm— 
Hormone therapy and heart disease. New 
England Journal of Medicine, 349, 519-521. 


Herrnstein, R. J., & Murray, D. (1994). The bell 
curve. New York: Free Press. 


Hertlein, K. M., Weeks, G. R., & Gambescia, N. 
(2007). The treatment of hypoactive sexual 
desire disorder: An intersystem approach. In 
L. VandeCreek, F. L. Peterson, Jr., & J. W. Bley, 
Innovations in clinical practice: Focus on sexual 
health. Sarasota, FL: Professional Resource 
Press /Professional Resource Exchange. 


Hertzog, C., Kramer, A., Wilson, R., & Linden- 
berger, U. (2008). Enrichment effects on adult 
cognitive development: Can the functional 
capacity of older adults be preserved and 
enhanced? Psychological Science in the Public 
Interest, 9, 1-65. 


Herzberg, L. (2009). Direction, causation, and 
appraisal theories of emotion. Philosophical 
Psychology, 22, 167-186. 


Heshka, S., Anderson, J. W., Atkinson, R. L., 
Greenway, F. L., Hill, J. O., Phinney, S. D., et 
al. (2003). Weight loss with self-help com- 
pared with a structured commercial program: 
A randomized trial. Journal of the American 
Medical Association, 289, 1792-1798. 


Hess, M. J., Houg, S., & Tammaro, E. (2007). The 
experience of four individuals with paraple- 
gia enrolled in an outpatient interdisciplinary 
sexuality program. Sexuality and Disability, 25, 
189-195. 


Hess, T. M., Hinson, J. T., & Statham, J. A. (2004). 
Explicit and implicit stereotype activation 
effects on memory: Do age and awareness 
moderate the impact of priming? Psychology 
and Aging, 19, 495-505. 


Heward, W. L., & Orlansky, M. D. (1988). Excep- 
tional children (3rd ed.). Columbus, OH: Merrill. 


Hewitt, B., & Egan, H. W. (2009, February 23). 
Flight 1549: The right stuff. People, 71, pp. 
10-14. 


Heyman, G. D., & Diesendruck, G. (2002). The 
Spanish ser/estar distinction in bilingual chil- 
dren’s reasoning about human psychological 
characteristics. Developmental Psychology, 38, 
407-417. 


Hibbard, P. (2007, February). A statistical model 
of binocular disparity. Visual Cognition, 15, 
149-165. 


Hiby, E. F., Rooney, N. J., & Bradshaw, J. W. S. 
(2004). Dog training methods: Their use, 
effectiveness and interaction with behaviour 
and welfare. Animal Welfare, 13, 63-69. 


Hicks, T. V., & Leitenberg, H. (2001). Sexual 
fantasies about one’s partner versus someone 
else: Gender differences in incidence and 
frequency. Journal of Sex Research, 38, 43-50. 


Hilarski, C. (2007). Antisocial personality disor- 
der. In B. A. Thyer & J. S. Wodarski, Social 
work in mental health: An evidence-based 
approach. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. 


Hilgard, E. (1992). Disassociation and theories of 
hypnosis. In E. Fromm & M. E. Nash (Eds.), 
Contemporary hypnosis research. New York: 
Guilford. 


Hill, J. O., Catenacci, V., & Wyatt, H. R. (2005). 
Obesity: Overview of an epidemic. Psychiatric 
Clinics of North America, 28, 1-23. 


Hillix, W. A. (2007). The past, present, and possi- 
ble futures of animal language research. In 
D. A. Washburn, Primate perspectives on behav- 
ior and cognition. Washington, DC: American 
Psychological Association. 


Hines, M. (2004) Brain gender. New York: Oxford 
University Press. 


Hines, M., & Alexander, G. (2008). Monkeys, 
girls, boys and toys: A confirmation letter 
regarding sex differences in toy preferences: 
Striking parallels between monkeys and 
humans. Hormones and Behavior, 54, 478-479. 


Hines, M., Golombok, S., Rust, J., Johnston, K. J., 
Golding, J., & Avon Longitudinal Study of 
Parents and Children Study Team. (2002). 
Testosterone during pregnancy and gender 
role behavior of preschool children: A longitu- 
dinal, population study. Child Development, 73, 
1678-1687. 

Hirschler, B. (2007, May 1). Doctors test gene 
therapy to treat blindness. Reuters, p. 9. 

Hirsh, I. J., & Watson, C. S. (1996). Auditory psy- 
chophysics and perception. Annual Review of 
Psychology, 47, 461-484. 

Ho, S. M. Y., Saltel, P., Machavoine, J., Rapoport- 


Hubschman, N., & Spiegel, D. (2004). Cross- 
cultural aspects of cancer care. In National 


References R-23 


Institutes of Health and Stanford University 
School of Medicine, Cancer, culture, and com- 
munication. New York: Kluwer Academic/ 
Plenum Publishers. 


Hobfoll, S. E., Freedy, J. R., Green B. L., & 
Solomon, S. D. (1996). Coping in reaction 
to extreme stress: The roles of resource loss 
and resource availability. In M. Zeidner & 
N. S. Endler (Eds.), Handbook of coping: Theory, 
research, applications. New York: Wiley. 


Hobfoll, S. E., Hall, B. J., & Canetti-Nisim, D. 
(2007). Refining our understanding of trau- 
matic growth in the face of terrorism: Moving 
from meaning cognitions to doing what is 
meaningful. Applied Psychology: An International 
Review, 56, 345-366. 


Hobson, J. A. Sleep. (1989) New York: W. H. 
Freeman. 


Hobson, J. A. (2005). In bed with Mark Solms? 
What a nightmare! A reply to Domhoff (2005). 
Dreaming, 15, 21-29. 


Hochschild, A. (2001, February). A generation 
without public passion. Atlantic Monthly, 
pp. 33-42. 


Hock, H. S., & Ploeger, A. (2006). Linking 
dynamical perceptual decisions at different 
levels of description in motion pattern 
formation: Psychophysics. Perception & 
Psychophysics, 68, 505-514. 


Hoff, E. (2003). Language development in 
childhood. In R. M. Lerner et al. (Eds.), 
Handbook of psychology: Developmental 
psychology (Vol. 6). New York: Wiley. 


Hoff, E. (2008). Language development. New York: 
Wadsworth. 


Hoffer, T. B., Selfa, L., Welch, V., Jr., Williams, K., 
Hess, M., Friedman, J., et al. (2005, March 8). 
Doctorate recipients from United States universi- 
ties: Summary report 2003. Chicago: NORC at 
the University of Chicago. 


Hofmann, S. G. (2007). Enhancing exposure- 
based therapy from a translational research 
perspective. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 
45, 1987-2001. 


Hofmann, W., Gschwendner, T., Castelli, L., & 
Schmitt, M. (2008). Implicit and explicit atti- 
tudes and interracial interaction: The moder- 
ating role of situationally available control 
resources. Group Processes & Intergroup 
Relations, 11, 69-87. 


Hogan, J., Davies, S., & Hogan, R. (2007). 
Generalizing personality-based validity 
evidence. In S. M. McPhail, Alternative 
validation strategies: Developing new and 
leveraging existing validity evidence. Hoboken, 
NJ: John Wiley & Sons. 


Hoge, C. W., & Castro, C. A. (2006, September 2). 
Post-traumatic stress disorder in UK and US 
forces deployed to Iraq. Lancet, 368, 837. 


Hogg, M. A. (2006). Social identity theory. In P. J. 
Burke, Contemporary social psychological theo- 
ries. Palo Alto, CA: Stanford University 
Press. 


Hogg, M. A., & Hains, S. C. (2001). Intergroup 
relations and group solidarity: Effects of group 
identification and social beliefs on depersonal- 
ized attraction. In M. A. Hogg & D. Abrams 
(Eds.), Intergroup relations: Essential readings. 
New York: Psychology Press. 


R-24 References 


Holden, C. (2003, January 17). Deconstructing 
schizophrenia. Science, 299, 333-335. 


Holden, C. (2007, June 29). Embryonic stem cells. 
Stem cell science advances as politics stall. 
Science, 316, 1825. 


Holden, L. M. (2005). Complex adaptive systems: 
Concept analysis. Journal of Advanced Nursing, 
52, 651-657. 


Holland, J. C., & Lewis, S. (2001). The human side 
of cancer: Living with hope, coping with uncer- 
tainty. New York: Quill. 


Holler, G. D. (2006). Relations of hypnotic suscep- 
tibility, absorption, imagery, sexual fantasy, 
sexual daydreaming, and social desirability 
to sexual satisfaction. Dissertation Abstracts 
International: Section B: The Sciences and 
Engineering, 67, 3453. 


Holleran, S., Mehl, M., & Levitt, S. (2009). Eaves- 
dropping on social life: The accuracy of 
stranger ratings of daily behavior from thin 
slices of natural conversations. Journal of 
Research in Personality, 43, 660-672. 


Hollingworth, H. L. (1943/1990). Leta Stetter 
Hollingworth: A biography. Boston: Anker. 


Hollins, K. (2007). Consequences of antenatal 
mental health problems for child health and 
development. Current Opinions on Obstetric 
Gynecology, 19, 568-573. 


Hollis, K. L. (1997, September). Contemporary 
research on Pavlovian conditioning: A “new” 
functional analysis. American Psychologist, 52, 
956-965. 


Hollon, S. D., Thase, M. E., & Markowitz, J. C. 
(2002). Treatment and prevention of depres- 
sion. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 
3, 39-77. 


Holloway, L. (2000, December 16). Chief of New 
York City schools plans to revamp bilingual 
study. The New York Times, p. A1. 


Holmes, A., Yang, R. J., Lesch, K. P., Crawley, 
J. N., & Murphy, D. L. (2003). Mice lacking the 
Serotonin Transporter Exhibit 5-HT-sub(1A) 
receptor-mediated abnormalities in tests for 
anxiety-like behavior. Neuropsychopharma- 
cology, 28, 2077-2088. 


Holowka, S., & Pettito, L. A. (2002, August 30). 
Left hemisphere cerebral specialization for 
babies while babbling. Science, 297, 1515. 

Holt, M., & Jahn, R. (2004, March, 26). Synaptic 
vesicles in the fast lane. Science, 303, 
1986-1987. 


Holy, T. E., Dulac, C., & Meister, M. (2000, 
September 1). Responses of vomeronasal 
neurons to natural stimuli. Science, 289, 
1569-1572. 


Hongchun, W., & Ming, L. (2006). About the 
research on suggestibility and false memory. 
Psychological Science (China), 29, 905-908. 

Hopkins, W., & Cantalupo, C. (2008, June). 
Theoretical speculations on the evolutionary 
origins of hemispheric specialization. Current 
Directions in Psychological Science, 17, 233-237. 


Hopwood, C., Newman, D., Donnellan, M., 
Markowitz, J., Grilo, C., Sanislow, C., et al. 
(2009). The stability of personality traits in indi- 
viduals with borderline personality disorder. 
Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 118, 806-815. 

Horinek, D., Varjassyová, A., & Hort, J. (2007). 
Magnetic resonance analysis of amygdalar 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


volume in Alzheimer’s disease. Current 
Opinion in Psychiatry, 20, 273-277. 

Horn, J. L. (2002). Selections of evidence, 
misleading assumptions, and over- 
simplifications: The political message of 
The Bell Curve. In J. M. Fish (Ed.), Race and 
intelligence: Separating science from myth. 
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 


Horney, K. (1937). Neurotic personality of our times. 
New York: Norton. 


Horton, K. D., Wilson, D. E., Vonk, J., Kirby, S. L., 
& Nielsen, T. (2005). Measuring automatic 
retrieval: A comparison of implicit memory, 
process dissociation, and speeded response 
procedures. Acta Psychologica, 119, 235-263. 


Houghtalen, R. P., & Talbot, N. (2007). 
Dissociative disorders and cognitive disor- 
ders. In O. J. Z. Sahler & J. E. Carr, The behav- 
ioral sciences and health care (2nd rev. and 
updated ed.). Ashland, OH: Hogrefe & Huber 
Publishers, 2007. 


Howe, C. J. (2002). The countering of overgeneral- 
ization. Journal of Child Language, 29, 875-895. 


Howes, O., & Kapur, S. (2009). The dopamine 
hypothesis of schizophrenia: Version IHI—The 
final common pathway. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 
35, 549-562. 

Howitt, D., & Cramer, D. (2000). First steps in 
research and statistics: A practical workbook for 
psychology students. Philadelphia: Psychology 
Press. 


Hsu, B., Kling, A., Kessler, C., Knapke, K., 
Diefenbach, P; & Elias, J. (1994). Gender dif- 
ferences in sexual fantasy and behavior ina 
college population: A ten-year replication. 
Journal of Sex and Marital Therapy, 20, 103-118. 


Hu, F. B., Li, T. Y., Colditz, G. A., Willett, W. C., & 
Manson, J. E. (2003). Television watching and 
other sedentary behaviors in relation to risk 
of obesity and type 2 diabetes mellitus in 
women. Journal of the American Medical 
Association, 289, 1785-1791. 


Hubbard, K., O’Neill, A., & Cheakalos, C. (1999, 
April 12). Out of control. People, pp. 52-72. 


Hubel, D. H., & Wiesel, T. N. (2004). Brain and 
visual perception: The story of a 25-year collabora- 
tion. New York: Oxford University Press. 


Huber, F., Beckmann, S. C., & Herrmann, A. 
(2004). Means-end analysis: Does the affective 
state influence information processing style? 
Psychology and Marketing, 21, 715-737. 


Hudson, W. (1960). Pictorial depth perception in 
subcultural groups in Africa. Journal of Social 
Psychology, 52, 183-208. 


Hudspeth, A. J. (2000). Hearing. In E. R. Kandel, 
J. H. Schwartz, & T. M. Jessell (Eds.), 
Principles of neural science (4th ed.). New York: 
McGraw-Hill. 


Huerta, M., Cortina, L. M., & Pang, J. S. (2006). 
Sex and power in the academy: Modeling sex- 
ual harassment in the lives of college women. 
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 
616-628. 

Hugdahl, K., Thomsen, T., & Ersland, L. (2006). 
Sex differences in visuo-spatial processing: An 
fMRI study of mental rotation. Neuropsy- 
chologia, 44, 1575-1583. 

Hugenberg, K., & Sacco, D. (2008). Social catego- 
rization and stereotyping: How social cate- 


gorization biases person perception and face 
memory. Social and Personality Psychology 
Compass, 2, 1052-1072. 


Huijie, T. (2006). The measurement and assess- 
ment of mental health: A longitudinal and 
cross-sectional research on undergraduates, 
adults and patients. Psychological Science 
(China), 29, 419-422. 


Hull, C. L. (1943). Principles of behavior. New York: 
Appleton-Century-Crofts. 


Hummer, T. A., & McClintock, M. K. (2009). 
Putative human pheromone and rostadienone 
attunes the mind specifically to emotional 
information. Hormones and Behavior, 55, 
548-559. 


Humphrey, N., Curran, A., & Morris, E. (2007). 
Emotional intelligence and education: A 
critical review. Educational Psychology, 27, 
235-254. 


Humphreys, G. W., & Miiller, H. (2000). A search 
asymmetry reversed by figure-ground 
assignment. Psychological Science, 11, 196-200. 


Hunt, E. (1994). Problem solving. In R. J. Sternberg 
(Ed.), Thinking and problem solving: Handbook of 
perception and cognition (2nd ed.). San Diego, 
CA: Academic Press. 


Hunt, E. (2005). Information processing and 
intelligence: Where we are and where we are 
going. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Pretz, Cognition 
and intelligence: Identifying the mechanisms of the 
mind. New York: Cambridge University Press. 


Hunt, J. S., Seifert, A. L., & Armenta, B. E. (2006). 
Stereotypes and prejudice as dynamic con- 
structs: Reminders about the nature of inter- 
group bias from the hurricane Katrina relief 
efforts. Analyses of Social Issues and Public 
Policy (ASAP), 6, 237-253. 


Hunt, M. (1974). Sexual behaviors in the 1970s. 
New York: Dell. 


Hurt, C. S., Ganerjee, S., Tunnard, C., Whitehead, 
D. L., Tsolaki, M., Mecocci, P., et al. (2005). 
Insight, cognition and quality of life in 
Alzheimer’s disease. NeuroMed Consortium, 
Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & 
Psychiatry, 81, 331-336. 


Huston, A. C., Donnerstein, E., Fairchild, H. H., 
Feshback, N. D., Katz, P., Murray, J. P., et al. 
(1992). Big world, small screen: The role of 
television in American society. Omaha, NE: 
University of Nebraska Press. 


Hutchinson, S. L., Baldwin, C. K., & Oh, S-S. 
(2006). Adolescent coping: Exploring adoles- 
cents’ leisure-based responses to stress. Leisure 
Sciences, 28, 115-131. 


Hyde, J., Mezulis, A. H., & Abramson, L. Y. (2008). 
The ABCs of depression: Integrating affective, 
biological, and cognitive models to explain 
the emergence of the gender difference in 
depression. Psychological Review, 115, 291-313. 


Hyde, J. S. (2005). The gender similarities hypoth- 
esis. American Psychologist, 50, 581-592. 


Hyde, J. S., Lindberg, S. M., Linn, M. C., Ellis, 
A. B., & Williams, C. C. (2008). Gender simi- 
larities characterize math performance. 
Science, 321, 494—495 

Hyde, J. S., & Mertz, J. E. (2009). Gender, culture, 
and mathematics performance. Proceedings of 
the National Academy of Sciences of the United 
States of America, 106, 8801-8807. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Hyde, K., Peretz, I., & Zatorre, R. (2008, February). 
Evidence for the role of the right auditory cor- 
tex in fine pitch resolution. Neuropsychologia, 
46, 632-639. 


Hyman, R. (1994). Anomaly or artifact? Comments 
on Bem and Honorton. Psychological Bulletin, 
115, 19-24. 


Hyman, S. E. (2003, September). Diagnosing 
disorders. Scientific American, pp. 96-103. 


Iachini, T., & Giusberti, F. (2004). Metric properties 
of spatial images generated from locomotion: 
The effect of absolute size on mental scanning. 
European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 16, 
573-596. 


Iacoboni, M. (2009, January). Imitation, empathy, 
and mirror neurons. Annual Review of Psycho- 
logy, 60, 653-670. 


Iaria, G., Palermo, L., Committeri, G., & Barton, J. 
(2009). Age differences in the formation and 
use of cognitive maps. Behavioural Brain 
Research, 196, 187-191. 


Tevers-Landis, C. E., Hoff, A. L., Brez, C., 
Cancilliere, M. K., McConnell, J., & Kerr, 
D. (2005). Situational analysis of dietary 
challenges of the treatment regimen for chil- 
dren and adolescents with phenylketonuria 
and their primary caregivers. Journal of 
Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 26, 
186-193. 


Iglesias, A. (2005). Awake-alert hypnosis in the 
treatment of panic disorder: A case report. 
American Journal of Clinical Hypnosis, 47, 
249-257. 


Igo, S. E. (2006). Review of A telescope on society: 
Survey research and social science at the 
University of Michigan and beyond. Journal 
of the History of the Behavioral Sciences, 42, 
95-96. 


Ihler, E. (2003). Patient-physician communication. 
Journal of the American Medical Association, 
289, 92. 


Ikonomidou, C., Bittigau, P., Ishimaru, M. J., 
Wozniak, D. F., Koch, C., Genz, K., et al. (2000, 
February 11). Ethanol-induced apoptotic neu- 
rodegeneration and fetal alcohol syndrome. 
Science, 287, 1056-1060. 


llies, R., Arvey, R. D., & Bouchard, T. J., Jr. (2006). 
Darwinism, behavioral genetics, and organi- 
zational behavior: A review and agenda for 
future research [Special issue: Darwinian 
perspectives on behavior in organizations]. 
Journal of Organizational Behavior, 27, 96-141. 


Imamura, M., & Nakamizo, S. (2006). An empiri- 
cal test of formal equivalence between 
Emmert’s Law and the size-distance invari- 
ance hypothesis. The Spanish Journal of 
Psychology, 9(2), 295-299. 


Innocenti, G. M. (2007). Subcortical regulation of 
cortical development: Some effects of early, 
selective deprivations. Progressive Brain 
Research, 164, 23-37. 


Interlandi, J. (2008, March 3). What addicts need. 
Newsweek, p. 31-16. 


International Human Genome Sequencing 
Consortium (2003). International Consortium 
completes Human Genome Project. Bethesda, 
MD: National Human Genome Research 
Institute. 


Irwin, M. (2008). Human psychoneuroimmunol- 
ogy: 20 years of discovery. Brain, Behavior, and 
Immunity, 22, 129-139. 


Irwin, R. R. (2006). Spiritual development in 
adulthood: Key concepts and models. In 
C. Hoare, Handbook of adult development and 
learning. New York: Oxford University Press. 


Isacson, O., & Kordower, J. (2008). Future of cell 
and gene therapies for Parkinson’s disease. 
Annals of Neurology, 64, $122-SS138. 


Isay, R. A. (1994). Being homosexual: Gay men and 
their development. Lanham, MD: Jason 
Aronson. 


Isbell, L. M., & Tyler, J. M. (2003). Teaching stu- 
dents about in-group favoritism and the mini- 
mal groups paradigm. Teaching of Psychology, 
30, 127-130. 


Ishikawa, S., Okajima, I., Matsuoka, H., & 
Sakano, Y. (2007). Cognitive behavioural 
therapy for anxiety disorders in children 
and adolescents: A meta-analysis. Child and 
Adolescent Mental Health, 12, 164-172. 


Iversen, S., & Iversen, L. (2007). Dopamine: 50 
years in perspective. Trends in Neurosciences, 
30, 188-193. 


Iverson, L. (2000). The science of marijuana. 
Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. 


Iverson, P., Kuhl, P. K., Reiko, A. Y., Diesch, E., 
Tohkura, Y., Ketterman, A., et al. (2003). A per- 
ceptual interference account of acquisition dif- 
ficulties for non-native phonemes. Cognition, 
87, B47-B57. 


Izard, C. E. (1990). Facial expressions and the reg- 
ulation of emotions. Journal of Personality and 
Social Psychology, 58, 487-498. 


Izard, C. E. (1994). Innate and universal facial 
expressions: Evidence from developmental 
and cross-cultural research. Psychological 
Bulletin, 115, 288-299. 


Izard, C. E., & Abe, J. A. (2004). Developmental 
changes in facial expressions of emotions in 
the strange situation during the second year 
of life. Emotion, 4, 251-265. 


Jackson, J. D. (2006). Trauma, attachment, and 
coping: Pathways to resilience. Dissertation 
Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences 
and Engineering, 67(1-B), 547. 


Jacob, K. S., Kumar, P. S., Gayathri, K., Abraham, 
S., & Prince, M. J. (2007). The diagnosis of demen- 
tia in the community [Special issue: Focus on 
psychogeriatrics in the developing world]. 
International Psychogeriatrics, 19, 669-678. 


Jacobs, J. A., & Gerson, K. (2004). The time divide: 
Work, family, and gender inequality. Cambridge, 
MA: Harvard University Press. 


Jacobs, M., Roesch, S., Wonderlich, S., Crosby, R., 
Thornton, L., Wilfley, D., et al. (2009). Anorexia 
nervosa trios: Behavioral profiles of individu- 
als with anorexia nervosa and their parents. 
Psychological Medicine, 39, 451-461. 


Jacoby, L. L., Bishara, A. J., Hessels, S., & Hughes, 
A. (2007). Probabilistic retroactive interfer- 
ence: The role of accessibility bias in interfer- 
ence effects. Journal of Experimental Psychology: 
General, 136, 200-216. 

Jaffé, A., Prasad, S. A., & Larcher, V. (2006). Gene 
therapy for children with cystic fibrosis— 
Who has the right to choose? Journal of 
Medical Ethics, 32, 361-364. 


References R-25 


Jain, S., Mills, P. J., & Von Känel, R. (2007). Effects 
of perceived stress and uplifts on inflamma- 
tion and coagulability. Psychophysiology, 44, 
154-160. 


James, H. S., Jr. (2005). Why did you do that? An 
economic examination of the effect of extrin- 
sic compensation on intrinsic motivation and 
performance. Journal of Economic Psychology, 
26, 549-566. 


James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New 
York: Holt. 


Jamieson, G. A. (2007). Hypnosis and conscious 
states: The cognitive neuroscience perspective. 
New York: Oxford University Press. 


Jamison, K. R. (1995a). An unquiet mind: A memoir 
of moods and madness. New York: Knopf. 


Janes, A. C., Frederick, B. deB., Merlo-Pich, E., 
Renshaw, P. F., Evins, A. E., Fava, M., et al. 
(2009). Brain fMRI reactivity to smoking- 
related images before and during extended 
smoking abstinence. Experimental and Clinical 
Psychopharmacology, 17(6), 365-373. 


Jang, H., Reeve, J., Ryan, R. M., & Kim, A. (2009, 
August). Can self-determination theory 
explain what underlies the productive, 
satisfying learning experiences of collectivisti- 
cally oriented Korean students? Journal of 
Educational Psychology, 101, pp. 644-661. 


Jang, S. J., You, S. H., & Ahn, S. H. (2007). 
Neurorehabilitation-induced cortical 
reorganization in brain injury: A 14-month 
longitudinal follow-up study. NeuroRehabili- 
tation, 22, 117-122. 


Janis, I. L. (1997). Groupthink. In R. P. Vecchio, 
Leadership: Understanding the dynamics of power 
and influence in organizations. Notre Dame, IN: 
University of Notre Dame Press. 


Jarlais, D. C. D., Arasteh, K., & Perlis, T. (2007). 
The transition from injection to non-injection 
drug use: Long-term outcomes among 
heroin and cocaine users in New York City. 
Addiction, 102, 778-785. 


Jayson, S. (2008, August 18). More than 50% of 
college students felt suicidal. USA Today, p. 10B. 


Jenkins, A. M., Albee, G. W., Paster, V. S., Sue, S., 
Baker, D. B., Comas-Diaz, L., et al. (2003). 
Ethnic minorities. In D. K. Freedheim, 
Handbook of psychology: History of psychology 
(Vol. 1). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. 


Jenkins, S. R. (1994). Need for power and wom- 
en’s careers over 14 years: Structural power, 
job satisfaction, and motive change. Journal of 
Personality and Social Psychology, 66, 155-165. 


Jensen, A. R. (2002). Galton’s legacy to research 
on intelligence. Journal of Biosocial Science, 34, 
145-172. 


Jensen, A. R. (2005). Psychometric g and mental 
chronometry. Cortex, 41, 230-231. 


Jequier, E. (2002). Pathways to obesity. Interna- 
tional Journal of Obesity and Related Metabolic 
Disorders, 26, S12-S17. 


Jetten, J., Hornsey, M. J., & Adarves-Yorno, I. 
(2006). When group members admit to being 
conformist: The role of relative intragroup 
status in conformity self-reports. Personality 
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 32, 162-173. 


Jha, A. (2006, February 3). Pill could make painful 
memories a thing of past: Common drug may 


R-26 References 


help to fight post traumatic stress. The Guardian 
(London), p. 15. 


Joe, G. W., Flynn, P. M., & Broome, K. M. (2007). 
Patterns of drug use and expectations in 
methadone patients. Addictive Behaviors, 32, 
1640-1656. 


Johnsdotter, S., & Essén, B. (2005). It is only a 
tradition: Making sense of Swedish Somalis’ 
narratives of female circumcision and 
avoiding submission to hegemonic political 
discourse. Critical Social Policy, 25, 577-589. 


Johnson, G. (1997, July 29). To test a powerful 
computer, play an ancient game. The New York 
Times, p. C3. 


Johnson, G. B. (2000). The Living World (p. 600), 
Boston: McGraw-Hill. 


Johnson, H. D. (2004). Gender, grade and rela- 
tionship differences in emotional closeness 
within adolescent friendships. Adolescence, 39, 
243-255. 


Johnson, J. G., Cohen, P., Smailes, E. M., Kasen, S., 
& Brook, J. S. (2002, March 29). Television 
viewing and aggressive behavior during 
adolescence and adulthood. Science, 295, 
2468-2471. 


Johnson, S. P. (2004). Development of perceptual 
completion in infancy. Psychological Science, 
15, 769-775. 


Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2006). How we reason. 
Oxford, England: Oxford University Press. 


John-Steiner, V., & Mahn, H. (2003). Sociocultural 
contexts for teaching and learning. In W. M. 
Reynolds & G. E. Miller (Eds.), Handbook of 
psychology: Educational psychology (Vol. 7). 
New York: Wiley. 


Johnston, L. D., O'Malley, P. M., & Bachman, J. G. 
(2003). Monitoring the future: National results on 
adolescent drug use: Overview of key findings, 
2002. Bethesda MD: National Institute on 
Drug Abuse. 


Johnston, L. D., O'Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., 
& Schulenberg, J. E. (2007). Monitoring the 
future national results on adolescent drug use: 
Overview of key findings, 2006 (NIH Publication 
No. 07-6202). Bethesda, MD: National Institute 
on Drug Abuse. 


Johnston, L. D., O'Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., 
& Schulenberg, J. E. (2009). Monitoring the 
future national results on adolescent drug use; 
overview of key findings, 2008 (NIH Publication 
No. 09-7401). Bethesda, MD: National 
Institute on Drug Abuse. 


Johnston, L. D., O'Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., 
& Schulenberg, J. E. (2009). Monitoring the 
future national survey results on drug use: 
1975-2008. Volume I: Secondary school students 
(NIH Publication No. 09-7402). Bethesda, MD: 
National Institute on Drug Abuse. 


Johnston, M. V. (2004). Clinical disorders of brain 
plasticity. Brain and Development, 26, 73-80. 


Jones, A. L. (2006). The contemporary psychoana- 
lyst: Karen Horney’s theory applied in 
today’s culture. PsycCRITIQUES, 51, 
127-134. 


Jones, J. E., & Corp, E. S. (2003). Effect of naltrex- 
one on food intake and body weight in Syrian 
hamsters depends on metabolic status. 
Physiology and Behavior, 78, 67-72. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Jones, J. M. (2007). Exposure to chronic community 
violence: Resilience in African American chil- 
dren. Journal of Black Psychology, 33, 125-149. 


Jones, K., Callen, F., Blagrove, M., & Parrott, A. 
(2008). Sleep, energy and self rated cognition 
across 7 nights following recreational ecstasy / 
MDMA use. Sleep and Hypnosis, 10, 2-38. 


Jones, R. K., Darroch, J. E., & Singh, S. (2005). 
Religious differentials in the sexual and 
reproductive behaviors of young women in 
the United States. Journal of Adolescent Health, 
36, 279-288. 


Jorgensen, G. (2006). Kohlberg and Gilligan: Duet 
or duel? Journal of Moral Education, 35, 179-196. 


Joyce, J. (1934). Ulysses. New York: Random 
House. 

Julien, R. M (2001). A primer of drug action (9th 
ed.). New York: Freeman. 


Jung, C. G. (1961). Freud and psychoanalysis. New 
York: Pantheon. 


Jung, J. (2002). Psychology of alcohol and other 
drugs: A research perspective. Thousand Oaks, 
CA: Sage. 

Juster, F. T., Ono, H., & Stafford, F. (2002). Report 
on housework and division of labor. Ann Arbor, 
MI: Institute for Social Research. 


Jylha, M. (2004). Old age and loneliness: Cross- 
sectional and longitudinal analyses in the 
Tampere longitudinal study on aging. Canadian 
Journal on Aging/La Revue Canadienne du 
Vieillissement, 23, 157-168. 


Kadosh, R., Henik, A., & Walsh, V. (2009, May). 
Synaesthesia: Learned or lost? Developmental 
Science, 12, 484-491. 


Kagan, J., Snidman, N., Kahn, V., & Towsley, S. 
(2007). The preservation of two infant temper- 
aments into adolescence. Monographs of the 
Society for Research in Child Development, 72, 
1-75. 


Kahneman, D., Diener, E., & Schwarz, N. (1998). 
Well-being: The foundations of hedonic psychol- 
ogy. New York: Russell Sage Foundation. 


Kalb, C. (2001, February 26). DARE checks into 
rehab. Newsweek, pp. 56. 


Kalb, C. (2003, May 19). Taking a new look at 
pain. Newsweek, pp. 51-52. 


Kaller, C. P., Unterrainer, J. M., Rahm, B., & 
Halsband, U. (2004). The impact of problem 
structure on planning: Insights from the 
Tower of London task. Cognitive Brain 
Research, 20, 462-472. 


Kallio, S., & Revonsuo, A. (2003). Hypnotic phe- 
nomena and altered states of consciousness: 

A multilevel framework of description and 
explanation. Contemporary Hypnosis, 20, 
111-164. 

Kaminski, P., Chapman, B. P., Haynes, S. D., & 
Own, L. (2005). Body image, eating behaviors, 
and attitudes toward exercise among gay and 
straight men. Eating Behaviors, 6, 179-187. 


Kandel, E. R., Schwartz, J. H., & Jessell, T. M. (Eds.) 
(2000). Principles of neural science (4th ed.). 
New York: McGraw-Hill. 

Kane, M. J., & Engle, R. W. (2002). The role of 
prefrontal cortex in working-memory capac- 
ity, executive attention, and general fluid 
intelligence: An individual-differences 


perspective. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 
9, 637-671. 


Kanner, A. D., Coyne, J. C., Schaefer, C., & 
Lazarus, R. S. (1981). Comparison of two 
modes of stress measurement: Daily hassles 
and uplifts versus major life events. Journal of 
Behavioral Medicine, 4, 14. 


Kantrowitz, B., & Underswood, A. (2007, June 
25). The teen drinking dilemma. Newsweek, 
pp. 36-37. 


Kaplan, H. S. (1974). The new sex therapy. New 
York: Brunner-Mazel. 


Kaplan, J. R., & Manuck, S. B. (1989). The effect of 
propranolol on behavioral interactions among 
adult male cynomolgus monkeys (Macaca fas- 
cicularis) housed in disrupted social group- 
ings. Psychosomatic Medicine, 51, 449-462. 


Kaplan, M. S., Huguer, N., McFarland, B. H., & 
Newsom, J. T. (2007). Suicide among male 
veterans: A prospective population-based 
study. Journal of Epidemiological Community 
Health, 61, 619—624. 


Kaplan, R. M., & Saccuzzo, D. P. (2001). Psycholo- 
gical testing: Principles, applications, and issues 
(5th ed.). Belmont, CA: Wadsworth / Thomson 
Learning. 


Kara, P., & Boyd, J. (2009, April). A micro- 
architecture for binocular disparity and 
ocular dominance in visual cortex. Nature, 
458(7238), 627—631. 


Karama, S., Ad-Dab’bagh, Y., Haier, R. J., Deary, 
I. J., Lyttelton, O. C., Lepage, C., et al. (2009). 
Positive association between cognitive ability 
and cortical thickness in a representative US 
sample of healthy 6 to 18 year-olds. Brain 
Development Cooperative Group; Intelligence, 37, 
145-155. 


Karaszewski, B. (2008). Sub-neocortical brain: 
A mechanical tool for creative generation? 
Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12, 171-172. 


Karni, A., Tanne, D., Rubenstein, B. S., Askenasy, 
J. J. M., & Sagi, D. (1994, July 29). Dependence 
on REM sleep of overnight improvement of a 
perceptual skill. Science, 265, 679-682. 


Kasof, J. (2009, May). Cultural variation in sea- 
sonal depression: Cross-national differences 
in winter versus summer patterns of seasonal 
affective disorder. Journal of Affective Disorders, 
115, 79-86. 


Kassam, K. S., Gilbert, D. T., Swencionis, J. K., & 
Wilson, T. D. (2009). Misconceptions of mem- 
ory: The Scooter Libby effect. Psychological 
Science, 20, 551-552. 


Kassel, J. D., Evatt, D. P., Greenstein, J. E., Wardle, 
M. C., Yates, M. C., & Veilleux, J. C. (2007). 
The acute effects of nicotine on positive and 
negative affect in adolescent smokers. Journal 
of Abnormal Psychology, 116, 543-553. 


Kassin, S. M. (2005). On the psychology of con- 
fessions: Does innocence put innocents at 
risk? American Psychologist, 60, 215-228. 


Kaštelan, A., Franciškovic, A., Tanja, M., & 
Moro, L. (2007). Psychotic symptoms in 
combat-related post-traumatic stress dis- 
order. Military Medicine, 172, 273-277. 

Kato, K., & Pedersen, N. L. (2005). Personality 
and coping: A study of twins reared apart and 


twins reared together. Behavior Genetics, 35, 
147-158. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Kato, N. (2009). Neurophysiological mechanisms 
of electroconvulsive therapy for depression. 
Neuroscience Research, 64, 3-11. 


Kato, R., Yokoi, H., Arieta, A. H., Yu, W., & Arai, T. 
(2009). Mutual adaptation among man and 
machine by using f-MRI analysis. Robotics and 
Autonomous Systems, 161-166. 


Kato, T. (2007). Molecular genetics of bipolar dis- 
order and depression. Psychiatry and Clinical 
Neurosciences, 61, 3-19. 


Katsiyannis, A., Zhang, D., & Woodruff, N. (2005). 
Transition supports to students with mental 
retardation: An examination of data from the 
national longitudinal transition study 2. 
Education and Training in Developmental 
Disabilities, 40, 109-116. 

Katz, L., Fotti, S., & Postl, L. (2009). Cognitive- 
behavioral therapy and dialectical behavior 
therapy: Adaptations required to treat 
adolescents. Psychiatric Clinics of North 
America, 32, 95-109. 


Katz, M. (2001). The implications of revising 
Freud’s empiricism for drive theory. 
Psychoanalysis and Contemporary Thought, 24, 
253-272. 


Kaufman, A., Johnson, C., & Liu, X. (2008). A 
CHC theory-based analysis of age differences 
on cognitive abilities and academic skills at 
ages 22 to 90 years. Journal of Psychoeducational 
Assessment, 26, 350-381. 


Kaufman, J. C., & Baer, J. (2006). Creativity and 
reason in cognitive development. New York: 
Cambridge University Press. 


Kawasaki, C., Nugent, J. K., Miyashita, H., 
Miyahara, H., & Brazelton, T. B. (1994). The 
cultural organization of infants’ sleep [Special 


issue: Environments of birth and infancy]. 
Children’s Environment, 11, 135-141. 


Kawashima, H., Izaki, Y., & Grace, A. A. (2006). 
Cooperativity between hippocampal- 
pre-frontal short-term plasticity through asso- 
ciative long-term potentiation. Brain Research, 
1109, 37-44. 


Kazar, D. B. (2006). Forensic psychology: Did we 
leave anything out? PsycCRITIQUES, 51, 88-97. 


Kazdin, A. (2008). Evidence-based treatment and 
practice: New opportunities to bridge clinical 
research and practice, enhance the knowledge 
base, and improve patient care. American 
Psychologist, 63, 146-159. 


Kearns, K. P. (2005). Broca’s aphasia. In L. L. 
LaPointe (Ed.), Aphasia and related neurogenic 
language disorders (3rd ed.). New York: Thieme 
New York. 


Keating, D. P., & Clark, L. V. (1980). Development 
of physical and social reasoning in adoles- 
cence. Developmental Psychology, 16, 23-30. 


Keehn, J. D. (1996). Master builders of modern 
psychology: From Freud to Skinner. New York: 
New York University Press. 


Keillor, J. M., Barrett, A. M., Crucian, G. P., 
Kortenkamp, S., & Heilman, K. M. (2002). 
Emotional experience and perception in the 
absence of facial feedback. Journal of the 
International Neuropsychological Society, 8, 
130-135. 


Keller, J., (2007). Stereotype threat in classroom 
settings: The interactive effect of domain 
identification, task difficulty and stereotype 


threat on female students’ math performance. 
British Journal of Educational Psychology, 77, 
323-338. 


Kelley, H. (1950). The warm-cold variable in first 
impressions of persons. Journal of Personality 
and Social Psychology, 18, 431-439. 

Kelly, D., & Tangney, B. (2006). Adapting to intelli- 
gence profile in an adaptive educational sys- 
tem. Interacting with Computers, 18, pp. 385-409. 


Kelly, J. B. (2000). Children’s adjustment in 
conflicted marriage and divorce: A decade 
review of research. Journal of the American 
Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 
963-973. 

Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L., Räikkönen, K., 
Ekelund, J., & Peltonen, L. (2004). Nature 
and nurture in novelty seeking. Molecular 
Psychiatry, 9, 308-311. 

Kemeny, M. E. (2007). Psychoneuroimmunology. 
In H. S. Friedman & R. C. Silver, Foundations 
of health psychology. New York: Oxford 
University Press. 


Kempermann, G., & Gage, F. H. (1999, May). 
New nerve cells for the adult brain. Scientific 
American, pp. 48-53. 


Kemps, E., & Tiggemann, M. (2007). Reducing 
the vividness and emotional impact of dis- 
tressing autobiographical memories: The 
importance of modality-specific interference. 
Memory, 15, 412—422. 


Kendler, K., Halberstadt, L., Butera, F., Myers, J., 
Bouchard, T., & Ekman, P. (2008). The similar- 
ity of facial expressions in response to 
emotion-inducing films in reared-apart twins. 
Psychological Medicine, 38(10), 1475-1483. 


Kendler, K. S., Gatz, M., & Gardner, C. O. (2006). 
Personality and major depression. Archives of 
General Psychiatry, 63, 1113-1120. 


Kendler, K. S., Gatz, M., & Gardner, C. O. (2006a). 
Personality and major depression. Archives of 
General Psychiatry, 63, 1113-1120. 


Kennedy, J. E. (2004). A proposal and challenge 
for proponents and skeptics of psi. Journal of 
Parapsychology, 68, 157-167. 


Kensinger, E. (2007). Negative emotion enhances 
memory accuracy: Behavioral and neuro- 
imaging evidence. Current Directions in 
Psychological Science, 16, 213-218. 


Kensinger, E. A., & Schacter, D. L. (2006). Neural 
processes underlying memory attribution on 
a reality-monitoring task. Cerebral Cortex, 16, 
1126-1133. 


Kenway, L., & Wilson, M. A. (2001). Temporally 
structured replay of awake hippocampal 
ensemble activity during rapid eye move- 
ment sleep. Neuron, 29, 145-156. 


Kesebir, P., & Diener, E. (2008). In pursuit of hap- 
piness: Empirical answers to philosophical 
questions. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 
3, 117-125. 


Kess, J. F., & Miyamoto, T. (1994). Japanese psycho- 
linguistics. Amsterdam, Netherlands: John 
Benjamins. 

Kessler, R. C., & Wang, P. S. (2008). The descrip- 
tive epidemiology of commonly occurring 
mental disorders in the United States. Annual 
Review of Public Health, 29, 115-129. 


Kettenmann, H., & Ransom, B. R. (2005). Neuroglia 
(2nd ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. 


References R-27 


Key, W. B. (2003). Subliminal sexuality: The 
fountainhead for America’s obsession. In 
T. Reichert & J. Lambaiase (Eds.), Sex in adver- 
tising: Perspectives on the erotic appeal. LEA’s 
communication series. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence 
Erlbaum. 


Khazaal, Y., Chatton, A., Claeys, F., Ribordy, F., 
Zullino, D., & Cabanac, M. (2008). 
Antipsychotic drug and body weight set- 
point. Physiology & Behavior, 95, 157-160. 


Kidd, E., & Lum, J. (2008). Sex differences in past 
tense overregularization. Developmental 
Science, 11, 882-889. 


Kiecolt, J. K. (2003). Satisfaction with work and 
family life: No evidence of a cultural reversal. 
Journal of Marriage and Family, 65, 23-35. 


Kiefer, A. K. (2006). The effects of stereotypes on 
performance attributions: How gender-math 
stereotypes lead women to internalize failure. 
Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: 
The Sciences and Engineering, 66(9-B), 5140. 


Kihlstrom, J. F. (2005a). Dissociative disorders. 
Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 1, 227-253. 


Kihlstrom, J. F. (2005b). Is hypnosis an altered 
state of consciousness or what? Comment. 
Contemporary Hypnosis, 22, 34-38. 


Kihlstrom, J. F., Schacter, D. L., Cork, R. C., Hurt, 
C. A., & Behr, S. E. (1990). Implicit and explicit 
memory following surgical anesthesia. 
Psychological Science, 1, 303-306. 


Kim, D. R., Pesiridou, A., & O’Reardon, J. P. 
(2009). Transcranial magnetic stimulation in 
the treatment of psychiatric disorders. Current 
Psychiatry Reports, 11, 447-52. 


Kim, H., Clark, D., & Dionne, R. (2009, July). 
Genetic contributions to clinical pain and 
analgesia: Avoiding pitfalls in genetic 
research. The Journal of Pain, 10, 663-693. 


Kim, K. H., Relkin, N. R., Lee, K. M., & Hirsch, 
J. (1997, July 10). Distinct cortical areas associ- 
ated with native and second languages. 
Nature, 388, 171-174. 


Kim, N. (2008). The moon illusion and the size- 
distance paradox. In Cummins-Sebree, S., 
Riley, M. A., & Shockley, K. (Eds). Studies in 
perception and action IX: Fourteenth Interna- 
tional Conference on Perception and Action. 
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 

Kim, S-E., Kim, J-W, & Kim, J-J. (2007). The neu- 
ral mechanism of imagining facial affective 
expression. Brain Research, 1145, 128-137. 


Kimbrel, N. A. (2007). A model of the develop- 
ment and maintenance of generalized social 
phobia. Clinical Psychological Review, 8, 69-75. 


Kim-Cohen, J., Caspi, A., & Moffitt, T. E. (2003). 
Prior juvenile diagnoses in adults with mental 
disorder: Developmental follow-back of a 
prospective-longitudinal cohort. Archives 
of General Psychiatry, 60, 709-717. 


Kim-Cohen, J., Moffitt, T. E., Taylor, A., Pawlby, 
S. J., & Caspi, A. (2005). Maternal depression 
and children’s antisocial behavior: Nature 
and nurture effects. Archives of General 
Psychiatry, 62, 173-181. 


Kimura, D. (1999). Sex and cognition. Cambridge, 
MA: The MIT Press. 

King-Casas, B., Sharp, C., Lomax-Bream, L., 
Lohrenz, T., Fonagy, P., & Montague, P. R. 
(2008, August, 8). The rupture and repair of 


R-28 References 


cooperation in borderline personality disor- 
der. Science, 321, 806-810. 


Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., & Martin, C. E. 
(1948). Sexual behavior in the human male. 
Philadelphia: Saunders. 


Kinsey, A. C., Pomeroy, W. B., Martin, C. E., & 
Gebhard, P. H. (1953). Sexual behavior in the 
human female. Philadelphia: Saunders. 


Kirby, D. (1977). The methods and methodologi- 
cal problems of sex research. In J. S. DeLora & 
C. A. B. Warren (Eds.), Understanding sexual 
interaction. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. 


Kirk, E. P., Jacobsen, D. J., Gibson, C., Hill, J. O., 
& Donnelly, J. E. (2003). Time course for 
changes in aerobic capacity and body 
composition in overweight men and women 
in response to long-term exercise: The 
Midwest exercise trial (MET). International 
Journal of Obesity, 27, 912-919. 


Kirk, K. M., Bailey, J. M., & Martin, N. G. (2000). 
Etiology of male sexual orientation in an 
Australian twin sample. Psychology, Evolution 
& Gender, 2, 301-311. 


Kirsch, I., & Braffman, W. (2001). Imaginative 
suggestibility and hypnotizability. Current 
Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 57-61. 


Kirsch, I., Lynn, S. J., Vigorito, M., & Miller, R. R. 
(2004). The role of cognition in classical and 
operant conditioning. Journal of Clinical 
Psychology, 60, 369-392. 


Kirschenbaum, H. (2004). Carl Rogers’s life and 
work: An assessment on the 100th anniversary 
of his birth. Journal of Counseling and 
Development, 82, 116-124. 


Kirschenbaum, H., & Jourdan, A. (2005). The 
current status of Carl Rogers and the 
person-centered approach. Psychotherapy: 
Theory, Research, Practice, Training, 42, 37-51. 


Kish, S., Fitzmaurice, P., Boileau, I., Schmunk, G., 
Ang, L., Furukawa, Y., et al. (2009). Brain 
serotonin transporter in human methamphet- 
amine users. Psychopharmacology, 202, 
649-661. 


Kiss, A. (2004). Does gender have an influence on 
the patient-physician communication? Journal 
of Men’s Health and Gender, 1, 77-82. 


Kleinman, A. (1996). How is culture important 
for DSM-IV? In J. E Mezzich, A. Kleinman, 
H. Fabrega, Jr., & D. L. Parron (Eds.), Culture 
and psychiatric diagnosis: A DSM-IV perspective. 
Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press. 

Kling, K. C., Ryff, C. D., & Love, G. (2003). 
Exploring the influence of personality on 
depressive symptoms and self-esteem across 
a significant life transition. Journal of Per- 
sonality and Social Psychology, 85, 922-932. 


Klotz, F., Garle, M., & Granath, F. (2006). 
Criminality among individuals testing posi- 
tive for the presence of anabolic androgenic 
steroids. Archives of General Psychiatry, 63, 
1274-1279. 


Kluck, A. (2008). Family factors in the develop- 
ment of disordered eating: Integrating 
dynamic and behavioral explanations. Eating 
Behaviors, 9, 471—483. 

Kluger, J. (2001, April 2). Fear not! Time, pp. 51-62. 


Kluger, J. (2006, December 4). Why we worry 
about the things we shouldn’t and ignore the 
things we should. Time, pp. 64-71. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Klump, K., & Culbert, K. (2007). Molecular 
genetic studies of eating disorders: Current 
status and future directions. Current Directions 
in Psychological Science, 16, 37-41. 


Knight, S. C., & Meyer, R. G. (2007). Forensic 
hypnosis. In A. M. Goldstein, Forensic psychol- 
ogy: Emerging topics and expanding roles. 
Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. 


Knoblich, G., & Sebanz, N. (2006). The social 
nature of perception and action. Current 
Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 
99-111. 


Kobayashi, F, Schallert, D. L., & Ogren, H. A. (2003). 
Japanese and American folk vocabularies for 
emotions. Journal of Social Psychology, 143, 
451-478. 


Koch, C., & Greenfield, S. (2007, October.) How 
does consciousness happen? Scientific American, 
pp. 76-83. 


Koch, J. (2003). Gender issues in the classroom. 
In W. M. Reynolds & G. E. Miller (Eds.), 
Handbook of psychology: Educational psychol- 
ogy (Vol. 7). New York: Wiley. 


Kohlberg, L. (1984). The psychology of moral devel- 
opment: Essays on moral development (Vol. 2). 
San Francisco: Harper & Row. 


Kohlberg, L., & Ryncarz, R. A. (1990). Beyond 
justice reasoning: Moral development and 
consideration of a seventh stage. In C. N. 
Alexander & E. J. Langer (Eds.), Higher stages 
of human development: Perspectives on adult 
growth. New York: Oxford University Press. 


Kohler, C. G., Turner, T., Stolar, N. M., Bilker, 
W. B., Brensinger, C. M., Gur, R. E., et al. (2004). 
Differences in facial expressions of four 
universal emotions. Psychiatry Research, 128, 
235-244. 


Köhler, W. (1927). The mentality of apes. London: 
Routledge & Kegan Paul. 


Kojima, M., & Kangawa, K. (2008). Structure and 
function of ghrelin. Results & Problems in Cell 
Differentiation, 46, 89-115. 


Kolata, G. (2002, December 2). With no answers 
on risks, steroid users still say “yes.” The New 
York Times, p. 1A. 


Kolb, B., Gibb, R., & Robinson, T. E. (2003). Brain 
plasticity and behavior. Current Directions in 
Psychological Science, 12, 1-5. 


Konig, R. (2005). Introduction: Plasticity, learning, 
and cognition. In R. Konig., P. Heil., E. 
Budinger & H. Scheich (Eds.), The auditory 
cortex: A synthesis of human and animal research. 
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 

Koocher, G. P., Norcross, J. C., & Hill, S. S. (2005). 
Psychologists’ desk reference (2nd ed.). New 
York: Oxford University Press. 

Kopelman, M. D., & Fleminger, S. (2002). 
Experience and perspectives on the classifica- 
tion of organic mental disorders. Psychopa- 
thology, 35, 76-81. 

Koper, R. J., & Jaasma, M. A. (2001). Interpersonal 
style: Are human social orientations guided 
by generalized interpersonal needs? Com- 
munications Reports, 14, 117-129. 


Koplewicz, H. (2002). More than moody: Rec- 
ognizing and treating adolescent depression. New 
York: Putnam. 


Korecka, J. A., Verhaagen, J., & Hol, E. M. (2007). 
Cell-replacement and gene-therapy strategies 


for Parkinson’s and Alzheimer’s disease. 
Regenerative Medicine, 2, 425-426. 


Kosambi, D. D. (1967). The Vedic “Five Tribes.” 
American Oriental Society, 14, 5-12. 


Kosfeld, M., Heinrich, M., Zak, P. J., Fischbacher, 
U., & Fehr, E. (2005, June 2). Oxytocin increases 
trust in humans. Nature, 435, 673-676. 

Koss, M. P. (1993). Rape: Scope, impact, interven- 
tions, and public policy responses. American 
Psychologist, 48, 1062-1069. 


Kosslyn, S. M. (2005). Mental images and the 
brain. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 22, 333-347. 


Kosslyn, S. M., Cacioppo, J. T., Davidson, R. J., 
Hugdahl, K., Lovallo, W. R., Spiegel, D., et al. 
(2002). Bridging psychology and biology. 
American Psychologist, 57, 341-351. 


Kotre, J., & Hall, E. (1990). Seasons of life. Boston: 
Little, Brown. 


Kounios, J., Fleck, J. I., Green, D. L., Payne, L., 
Stevenson, J. L., Bowden, E. M., et al. (2008). 
The origins of insight in resting-state brain 
activity. Neuropsychologia, 46, 281-291. 

Kovacs, A. M., & Mehler, J. (2009, July 31). 
Flexible learning of multiple speech structures 
in bilingual infants. Science, 325, 611-612. 


Kovelman, I., Baker, S .A., & Petitto, L. A. (2008). 
Bilingual and monolingual brains compared: 
A functional magnetic resonance imaging 
investigation of synatictic processing and a 
possible “neural signature” of bilingualism. 
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20(1), 153-169. 


Kovelman, I., Shalinsky, M.H., Berens, M.S., &, 
Petitto, L.A. (2008). Shining new light on the 
brain’s “bilingual signature”: A functional Near 
Infrared Spectroscopy investigation of semantic 
processing. Neuroimage, 39, 1457-1471. 


Kowert, P. A. (2002). Groupthink or deadlock: When 
do leaders learn from their advisors? SUNY Series 
on the presidency. Albany: State University of 
New York Press. 


Kozaric-Kovacic, D., & Borovecki, A. (2005). 
Prevalence of psychotic comorbidity in 
combat-related post- traumatic stress 
disorder. Military Medicine, 170, 223-226. 

Kozulin, A., Gindis, B., Ageyev, V. S., & Miller, 
S. M. (2003). Vygotsky's educational theory in 
cultural context. New York: Cambridge 
University Press. 


Kramer, P. (1993). Listening to Prozac. New York: 
Viking. 

Kreher, D., Holcomb, P., Goff, D., & Kuper- 
berg, G. (2008). Neural evidence for faster 
and further automatic spreading activation in 
schizophrenic thought disorder. Schizophrenia 
Bulletin, 34, 473—482. 


Kreuger, A. (2007). Are we having fun yet? 
Categorizing and evaluating changes in time 
allocation. Brookings Papers on Economic 
Activity (Vol. 2), 38, 193-218. 


Krijn, M., Emmelkamp, P. M. G., Olafsson, R. P., 
& Biemond, R. (2004). Virtual reality exposure 
therapy of anxiety disorders: A review. 
Clinical Psychology Review, 24, 259-281. 

Krueger, K., & Dayan, P. (2009). Flexible shaping: 
How learning in small steps helps. Cognition, 
110, 380-394. 

Krueger, R. G., Hicks, B. M., & McGue, M. (2001). 
Altruism and antisocial behavior: Independent 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


tendencies, unique personality correlates, 
distinct etiologies. Psychological Science, 12, 
397-402. 


Krull, D. S., & Anderson, C. A. (1997). The 
process of explanation. Current Directions in 
Psychological Science, 6, 1-5. 


Krusemark, E., Campbell, W., & Clementz, B. (2008). 
Attributions, deception, and event related 
potentials: An investigation of the self-serving 
bias. Psychophysiology, 45, 511-515. 


Ku, L., St. Louis, M., Farshy, C., Aral, S., Turner, 
C. F, Lindberg, L. D., et al. (2002). Risk behav- 
iors, medical care, and chlamydial infection 
among young men in the United States. 
American Journal of Public Health, 92, 1140-1143. 


Kiibler-Ross, E. (1969). On death and dying. New 
York: Macmillan. 


Kubovy, M., Epstein, W., & Gepshtein, S. (2003). 
Foundations of visual perception. In A. F. 
Healy, & R. W. Proctor (Eds.). Handbook of 
psychology: Experimental psychology (Vol. 4). 
New York: Wiley. 


Kuo, L. J. (2007). Effects of bilingualism on devel- 
opment of facets of phonological competence 
(China). Dissertation Abstracts International 
Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences, 
67(11-A), 4095. 


Kuppens, P., Ceulemans, E., Timmerman, M. E., 
Diener, E., & Kim-Prieto, C. (2006). Universal 
intracultural and intercultural dimensions of 
the recalled frequency of emotional experi- 
ence. Journal of Cross Cultural Psychology, 37, 
491-515. 


Kuriyama, K., Stickgold, R., & Walker, M. P. 
(2004). Sleep-dependent learning and 
motor-skill complexity. Learning and Memory, 
11, 705-713. 


Kuther, T. L. (2003). Your career in psychology: 
Psychology and the law. New York: Wadsworth. 


Kvavilashvili, L., & Fisher, L. (2007). Is time- 
based prospective remembering mediated by 
self-initiated rehearsals? Role of incidental 
cues, ongoing activity, age, and motivation. 
Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 136, 
112-132. 


Kwate, N. O. A. (2001). Intelligence or misorien- 
tation? Eurocentrism in the WISC-III. Journal 
of Black Psychology, 27, 221-239. 


Kwon, P., & Laurenceau, J. P. (2002). A longitudinal 
study of the hopelessness theory of depression: 
Testing the diathesis-stress model within a 
differential reactivity and exposure framework 
[Special issue: Reprioritizing the role of science 
in a realistic version of the scientist-practitioner 
model]. Journal of Clinical Psychology, 50, 
1305-1321. 


Laas, I. (2006). Self-actualization and society: A 
new application for an old theory. Journal of 
Humanistic Psychology, 46, 77-91. 


LaBar, K. (2007). Beyond fear: Emotional memory 
mechanisms in the human brain. Current 
Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 173-177. 


Lacerda, F., von Hofsten, C., & Heimann, M. 
(2001). Emerging cognitive abilities in early 
infancy. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum 
Associates. 


Lacey, M. (2002, January 6). In Kenyan family, 


ritual for girls still divides. The New York 
Times, p. 6. 


Laederach-Hofmann, K., & Messerli-Buergy, N. 
(2007). Chest pain, angina pectoris, panic 
disorder, and Syndrome X. In J. Jordan, 

B. Barde & A. M. Zeiher, Contributions toward 
evidence-based psychocardiology: A systematic 
review of the literature. Washington, DC: 
American Psychological Association. 


LaFrance, M., & Harris, J. L. (2004). Gender and 
verbal and nonverbal communication. In 
M. A. Paludi (Ed.), Praeger guide to the psychol- 
ogy of gender. Westport, CT: Praeger 
Publishers/ Greenwood Publishing Group. 


Lagacé-Séguin, D. G., & d'Entremont, M. L. 
(2006). The role of child negative affect in the 
relations between parenting styles and play. 
Early Child Development and Care, 176, 461—477. 


Lahti, J., Räikkönen, K., Ekelund, J., Peltonen, L., 
Raitakari, O. T., & Keltikangas-Järvinen, L. 
(2005). Novelty seeking: Interaction between 
parental alcohol use and dopamine D4 recep- 
tor gene exon III polymorphism over 17 
years. Psychiatric Genetics, 15, 133-139. 


Laing, R. D., & Szasz, T. (2004). “Knowing what 
ain't so.” Psychoanalytic Review, 91, 331-346. 


Laird, J. D., & Bresler, C. (1990). William James 
and the mechanisms of emotional experience. 
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 16, 
636-651. 


Lakhan, S., & Vieira, K. (2009, May 15). Schizo- 
phrenia pathophysiology: Are we any closer 
to a complete model? Annals of General 
Psychiatry, 8. 


Lal, S. (2002). Giving children security: Mamie 
Phipps Clark and the racialization of child 
psychology. American Psychologist, 57, 20-28. 


Lamal, P. A. (1979). College students’ common 
beliefs about psychology. Teaching of Psy- 
chology, 6, 155-158. 


Lamb, M. E., & Garretson, M. E. (2003). The 
effects of interviewer gender and child gender 
on the informativeness of alleged child sexual 
abuse victims in forensic interviews. Law and 
Human Behavior, 27, 157-171. 


Lamborn, S. D., & Groh, K. (2009). A four-part 
model of autonomy during emerging adult- 
hood: Associations with adjustment. Inter- 
national Journal of Behavioral Development, 33, 
393-401. 


Lane, K. A., Banaji, M. R., Nosek, B. A., & 
Greenwald, A. G. (Eds.). (2007). Understanding 
and using the implicit association test: IV: 
What we know (so far) about the method. In 
B. Wittenbrink & N. Schwarz, Implicit measures 
of attitudes. New York: Guilford Press. 


Lane, S. D., Cherek, D. R., & Tcheremissine, O. V. 
(2007). Response perseveration and adapta- 
tion in heavy marijuana-smoking adolescents. 
Addictive Behaviors, 32, 977-990. 


Lang, A. J., Sorrell, J. T., & Rodgers, C. S. (2006). 
Anxiety sensitivity as a predictor of labor 
pain. European Journal of Pain, 10, 263-270. 


Langan-Fox, J., & Grant, S. (2006). The Thematic 
Apperception Test: Toward a standard 
measure of the big three motives. Journal of 
Personality Assessment, 87, 277-291. 

Langdridge, D., & Butt, T. (2004). The fundamen- 
tal attribution error: A phenomenological 
critique. British Journal of Social Psychology, 43, 
357-369. 


References R-29 


Langreth, R. (2000, May 1). Every little bit helps: 
How even moderate exercise can have a big 
impact on your health. The Wall Street Journal, 
p. R5. 


Lankov, A. (2004). The dawn of modern Korea: 
Changes for better or worse. The Korea Times, 
p. A1. 


Lanza, S. T., & Collins, L. M. (2002). Pubertal 
timing and the onset of substance use in 
females during early adolescence. Prevention 
Science, 3, 69-82. 


Laqueur, T. W. (2003). Solitary sex: A cultural 
history of masturbation. New York: Zone. 


Larsen, R. J., & Buss, D. M. (2006). Personality 
psychology: Domains of knowledge about human 
nature with PowerWeb (2nd ed.). New York: 
McGraw-Hill. 


Lascaratos, G., Ji, D., & Wood, J. P. (2007). Visible 
light affects mitochondrial function and 
induces neuronal death in retinal cell cultures. 
Vision Research, 47, 1191-1201. 


Latané, B., & Darley, J. M. (1970). The unresponsive 
bystander: Why doesn’t he help? New York: 
Appleton-Century-Crofts. 


Laugharne, J., Janca, A., & Widiger, T. (2007). 
Posttraumatic stress disorder and terrorism: 
5 years after 9/11. Current Opinion in 
Psychiatry, 20, 36-41. 


Laumann, E. O., Paik, A., & Rosen, R. C. (1999, 
February 10). Sexual dysfunction in the 
United States: Prevalence and predictors. 
Journal of the American Medical Association, 281, 
537-544. 


Lavelli, M., & Fogel, A. (2005). Developmental 
changes in the relationship between the 
infant’s attention and emotion during early 
face-to-face communication. Developmental 
Psychology, 41, 265-280. 


Laven, A. (2008). Freshmen college student 
mental health and their resource usage. 
Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: 
Humanities and Social Sciences, 69, 136. 


Lavenex, P., & Lavenex, P. (2009). Spatial memory 
and the monkey hippocampus: Not all space 
is created equal. Hippocampus, 19, 8-19. 


Lawrence, J., Ashford, K., & Dent, Paul. (2006). 
Gender differences in coping strategies of 
undergraduate students and their impact on 
self-esteem and attainment. Active Learning in 
Higher Education, 7, 273-281. 


Lazarus, A. A. (1997). Brief but comprehensive psy- 
chotherapy: The multimodal way. New York: 
Springer. 

Lazarus, R. S. (1995). Emotions express a social 
relationship, but it is an individual mind that 
creates them. Psychological Inquiry, 6, 253-265. 


Leahy, R. L. (2003). Roadblocks in cognitive- 
behavioral therapy: Transforming challenges into 
opportunities for change. New York: Guilford 
Press. 


Leaper, C., & Ayres, M. (2007). A meta-analytic 
review of gender variations in adults’ lan- 
guage use: Talkativeness, affiliative speech, 
and assertive speech. Personality and Social 
Psychology Review, 11, 328-363. 


Leaper, C., & Friedman, C. K. (2007). The socializa- 
tion of gender. In J. E. Grusec & P. D. Hastings, 
Handbook of socialization: Theory and research. 
New York: Guilford Press. 


R-30 References 


Leary, C., Kelley, M., Morrow, J., & Mikulka, P. 
(2008). Parental use of physical punishment 
as related to family environment, psychologi- 
cal well-being, and personality in undergrad- 
uates. Journal of Family Violence, 23, 1-7. 


Leckman, J. F., & King, R. A. (2007). A develop- 
mental perspective on the controversy sur- 
rounding the use of SSRIs to treat pediatric 
depression. American Journal of Psychiatry, 164, 
1304-1306. 


Lee, A., Isaac, M. & Janca, A. (2007). Posttraumatic 
stress disorder and terrorism. In A. Monat, 
R. S. Lazarus & G. Reevy (Eds.), The Praeger 
handbook on stress and coping (Vol. 1). Westport, 
CT: Praeger Publishers/Greenwood 
Publishing Group. 


Lee, A. Y., & Aaker, J. L. (2004). Bringing the 
frame into focus: The influence of regulatory 
fit on processing fluency and persuasion. 
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 86, 
205-218. 


Lee, D., Kleinman, J., & Kleinman, A. (2007). 
Rethinking depression: An ethnographic 
study of the experiences of depression among 
Chinese. Harvard Review of Psychiatry, 15, 1-8. 


Lee, H. J., & Kwon, S. M. (2003). Two different 
types of obsession: Autogenous obsessions 
and reactive obsessions. Behaviour Research & 
Therapy, 41, 11-29. 


Lee, H. J., Kwon, S. M., Kwon, J. S., & Telch, M. J. 
(2005). Testing the autogenous reactive model 
of obsessions. Depress Anxiety, 21, 118-129. 


Lee-Chai, A. Y., & Bargh, J. A. (Eds.). (2001). The 
use and abuse of power: Multiple perspectives on 
the causes of corruption. Philadelphia, PA: 
Psychology Press. 


Lee-Chiong, T. L. (2006). Sleep: A comprehensive 
handbook. New York: Wiley-Liss. 


Lefaucheur, J. P., Brugieres, P., Menard- 
Lefaucheur, I., Wendling, S., Pommier, M., & 
Bellivier, F. (2007). The value of navigation- 
guided rTMS for the treatment of depression: 
An illustrative case. Neurophysiologic Clinics, 
37, 265-271. 


Lehar, S. (2003). The world in your head: A gestalt 
view of the mechanism of conscious experience. 
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 


Lehman, D. R., & Taylor, S. E. (1988). Date with 
an earthquake: Coping with a probable, 
unpredictable disaster. Personality and Social 
Psychology Bulletin, 13, 546-555. 


Lehrman, S. (2007). Going beyond X and Y. 
Scientific American, pp. 40-41. 


Leib, J. R., Gollust, S. E., Hull, S. C., & Wilfond, 
B. S. (2005). Carrier screening panels for 
Ashkenazi Jews: Is more better? Genetic 
Medicine, 7, 185-190. 


Leibel, R. L., Rosenbaum, M., & Hirsch, J. (1995, 
March 9). Changes in energy expenditure 
resulting from altered body. New England 
Journal of Medicine, 332, 621-628. 


Leiblum, S. R., & Chivers, M. L. (2007). Normal 
and persistent genital arousal in women: New 
perspectives. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 
33, 357-373. 


Leigh, J. H., Zinkhan, G. M. & Swaminathan, V. 
(2006). Dimensional relationships of recall 
and recognition measures with selected 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


cognitive and affective aspects of print ads. 
Journal of Advertising, 35, 105-122. 


Leiter, S., & Leiter, W. M. (2003). Affirmative action 
in antidiscrimination law and policy: An overview 
and synthesis. SUNY series in American consti- 
tutionalism. Albany: State University of New 
York Press. 


Leitner, L. M. (2007). Diversity issues, post- 
modernism, and psychodynamic therapy. 
PsycCRITIQUES, 52, no pagination specified. 


Le Maner-Idrissi, G., & Renault, L. (2006). 
Développement du ‘schéma de genre’: Une 
asymétrie entre filles et garcons? Are there sex 
differences in the acquisition of a gender 
schema? Enfance, 58, 251-265. 


Lemay, E., & Clark, M. (2008). How the head lib- 
erates the heart: Projection of communal 
responsiveness guides relationship promotion. 
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 94, 
647-671. 


Lemay, E. P., Jr., Clark, M. S., & Feeney, B. C. (2007). 
Projection of responsiveness to needs and the 
construction of satisfying communal relation- 
ships. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 
92, 834-853. 


Lemonick, M. D. (2000, December 11). Downey’s 
downfall. Time, p. 97. 


Lenhart, A. (2009, January 14). Adults and social 
network websites. Washington, DC: Pew 
Internet & American Life Project. 


Lenzenweger, M. F., & Dworkin, R. H. (Eds.). 
(1998). The origins and development of schizo- 
phrenia: Advances in experimental psychopathol- 
ogy. Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


Leo, R. J., & Latif, T. (2007). Repetitive transcra- 
nial magnetic stimulation (rTMS) in experi- 
mentally induced and chronic neuropathic 
pain: A review. The Journal of Pain, 8, 453-459. 


Leonard, E. (2009, May 18). Kirstie Alley: I’ve let 
myself go. People, p. 50. 


Lepage, J. F., & Theoret, H. (2007). The mirror 
neuron system: grasping others’ actions from 
birth? Developmental Science, 10, 513-523. 


Leppänen, J. M., Moulson, M. C., Vogel-Farley, 
V. K. & Nelson, C.A. (2007). An ERP study of 
emotional face processing in the adult and 
infant brain. Child Development, 78, 232-245. 


Lepper, M. R., Corpus, J. H., & Iyengar, S. S. (2005). 
Intrinsic and extrinsic motivational orienta- 
tions in the classroom: Age differences and 
academic correlates. Journal of Educational 
Psychology, 97, 184-196. 


Leung, F. K. S. (2002). Behind the high achieve- 
ment of East Asian students [Special issue: 
Achievements in mathematics and science in 
an international context]. Education Research 
and Evaluation, 8, 87-108. 


LeVay, S. (1993). The sexual brain. Cambridge, 
MA: MIT. 


Levi, A., Chan, K. K., & Pence, D. (2006). Real 
men do not read labels: The effects of mascu- 
linity and involvement on college students’ 
food decisions. Journal of American College 
Health, 55, 91-98. 


Levin, B. E., (2006). Metabolic sensing neurons 
and the control of energy homeostasis. 
Physiology & Behavior, 89, 486—489. 


Levin, R. J. (2007). Sexual activity, health and 
well-being—the beneficial roles of coitus and 
masturbation. Sexual and Relationship Therapy, 
22, 135-148. 


Levin, R., & Nielsen, T. (2009, April). Nightmares, 
bad dreams, and emotion dysregulation: A 
review and new neurocognitive model of 
dreaming. Current Directions in Psychological 
Science, 18, 84-88. 


Levine, J. M., & Moreland, R. L. (2006). Small 
groups: An overview. In J. M. Levine & 
R. L. Moreland (Eds.), Small groups. New 
York: Psychology Press. 


Levine, S. Z., & Rabinowitz, J. (2007). Revisiting 
the 5 dimensions of the Positive and Negative 
Syndrome Scale. Journal of Clinical Psycho- 
pharmacology, 27, 431-436. 


Levinson, D. (1992). The seasons of a woman's life. 
New York: Knopf. 


Levinson, D. J. (1990). A theory of life structure 
development in adulthood. In C. N. Alexander 
& E. J. Langer (Eds.), Higher stages of human 
development: Perspectives on adult growth. New 
York: Oxford University Press. 


Levy, B. (1996). Improving memory in old age 
through implicit self-stereotyping. Journal 
of Personality and Social Psychology, 71, 
1092-1107. 


Levy, B. R., & Myers, L. M. (2004). Preventive 
health behaviors influenced by self-perceptions 
of aging. Preventive Medicine: An International 
Journal Devoted to Practice and Theory, 39, 
625-629. 


Levy, B. R., Slade, M. D., Kunkel, S. R., & Kasl, 
S. V. (2002). Longevity increased by positive 
self-perceptions of aging. Journal of Personality 
& Social Psychology, 83, 261-270. 


Lewin, T. (2003, December 22). For more people 
in their 20s and 30s, going home is easier 
because they never left. The New York Times, 
p. A27. 


Lewinsohn, P. M., & Essau, C. A. (2002). 
Depression in adolescents. In I. H. Gotlib & 
C. L. Hammen (Eds.), Handbook of depression. 
New York: Guilford Press. 


Lewinsohn, P. M., Petit, J. W., Joiner, T. E., Jr., & 
Seeley, J. R. (2003). The symptomatic expres- 
sion of major depressive disorder in adoles- 
cents and young adults. Journal of Abnormal 
Psychology, 112, 244-252. 


Li, J. (2005). Mind or virtue: Western and Chinese 
beliefs about learning. Current Directions in 
Psychological Science, 14, 190-194. 


Li, J., Wang, L., & Fischer, K. W. (2004). The 
organization of Chinese shame concepts. 
Cognition and Emotion, 18, 767-797. 


Li, M. D., Cheng, R., Ma, J. Z., & Swan, G. E. (2003). 
A meta-analysis of estimated genetic and 
environmental effects on smoking behavior in 
male and female adult twins. Addiction, 98, 
23-31. 

Li, M. D., Lou, X., Chen, G., Ma, J. Z., & Elston, 
R. C. (2008). Gene-gene interactions among 
CHRNA4, CHRNB2, BDNF, and NTRK2 in 
nicotine dependence. Biological Psychiatry, 64, 
951-957. 

Li, T-K., Volkow, N. D., & Baler, R. D. (2007). The 
biological bases of nicotine and alcohol co- 
addiction. Biological Psychiatry, 61, 1-3. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Li, X., Wang, M., Poliakoff, E., & Luo, Y. (2007). 
Attention to threat in high and low trait- 
anxious individuals: A study using extremely 
threatening pictorial cues. Perceptual and 
Motor Skills, 104, 1097-1106. 


Liang, K. A. (2007). Acculturation, ambivalent 
sexism, and attitudes toward women who 
engage in premarital sex among Chinese 
American young adults. Dissertation Abstracts 
International: Section B: The Sciences and 
Engineering, 67(10-B), 6065. 


Liben, L. S., & Bigler, R. S. (2002). The develop- 
ment course of gender differentiations: 
Conceptualizing, measuring, and evaluation 
constructs and pathways. Monographs of the 
Society for Research in Child Development, 67, 
148-167. 


Lidz, J., & Gleitman, L. R. (2004). Argument 
structure and the child’s contribution to lan- 
guage learning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8, 
157-161. 


Lieberman, M. D. (2007). Social cognitive neuro- 
science: A review of core processes. Annual 
Review of Psychology, 58, 259-289. 


Lieberman, M. D., Hariri, A., Jarcho, J. M., 
Eisenberger, N. I., & Bookheimer, S. Y. (2005). 
An fMRI investigation of race-related amyg- 
dala activity in African-American and 
Caucasian-American individuals. Nature 
Neuroscience, 8, 720-722. 


Lien, Y-W., Chu, R-L., Jen, C-H., & Wu, C-H. 
(2006). Do Chinese commit neither funda- 
mental attribution error nor ultimate attribu- 
tion error? Chinese Journal of Psychology, 48, 
163-181. 


Lilienfeld, S. O. (2007). Psychological treatments 
that cause harm. Perspectives on Psychological 
Science, 2, 53-58. 


Lilienfeld, S. O., & Lynn, S. J., Lohr, J. M. (2003). 
Dissociative identity disorder: Multiple per- 
sonalities, multiple controversies. In S. O. 
Lilienfeld & S. J. Lynn (Eds.), Science and pseu- 
doscience in clinical psychology. New York: 
Guilford Press. 


Lin, C-H., & Lin, H-M. (2007). What price do you 
ask for the ‘extra one’? A social value orienta- 
tion perspective. Social Behavior and Personality, 
35, 9-18. 

Lin, Y. Y., Chen, W. T., Liao, K. K., Yeh, T. C., Wu, 

Z. Z., & Ho, L. T. (2005). Hemispheric balance 
in coding speech and non-speech sounds in 
Chinese participants. Neuroreport, 16, 469—473. 


Lindblad, F., Lindahl, M., & Theorell, T. (2006). 
Physiological stress reactions in 6th and 9th 
graders during test performance. Stress and 
Health: Journal of the International Society for the 
Investigation of Stress, 22, 189-195. 


Lindemann, O., & Bekkering, H. (2009). Object 
manipulation and motion perception: Evidence 
of an influence of action planning on visual 
processing. Journal of Experimental Psychology: 
Human Perception and Performance, 35, 
1062-1071. 

Lindh-Astrand, L., Brynhildsen, J., & Hoffmann, 
M. (2007). Attitudes towards the menopause 
and hormone therapy over the turn of the 
century. Maturitas, 56, 12-20. 

Lindley, L. D. (2006). The paradox of self-efficacy: 
Research with diverse populations. Journal of 
Career Assessment, 14, 143-160. 


Lindorff, M. (2005). Determinants of received 
social support: Who gives what to managers? 
Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 22, 
323-337. 


Lindsay, P. H., & Norman, D. A. (1977). Human 
information processing (2nd ed.). New York: 
Academic Press. 


Lindsay, P., Maynard, I., & Thomas, O. (2005). 
Effects of hypnosis on flow states and cycling 
performance. Sport Psychologist, 19, 164-177. 


Lindsey, E., & Colwell, M. (2003). Preschoolers’ 
emotional competence: Links to pretend and 
physical play. Child Study Journal, 33, 39-52. 

Links, P. S., Eynan, R., & Heisel, M. J. (2007). 
Affective instability and suicidal ideation and 
behavior in patients with borderline personal- 


ity disorder. Journal of Personality Disorders, 21, 
72-86. 


Lippa, R. A. (2005). Gender, nature, and nurture 
(2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 


Lips, H. M. (2003). A new psychology of women: 
Gender, culture, and ethnicity. New York: 
McGraw-Hill. 


Liszkowski, U., Schafer, M., Carpenter, M., & 
Tomasello, M. (2009). Prelinguistic infants, 
but not chimpanzees, communicate about 
absent entities. Psychological Science, 20, 654— 
660. 


Litowitz, B. E. (2007). Unconscious fantasy: A 
once and future concept. Journal of the 
American Psychoanalytic Association, 55, 
199-228. 


Little, A., Burt, D. M., & Perrett, D. I. (2006). What 
is good is beautiful: Face preference reflects 
desired personality. Personality and Individual 
Differences, 41, 1107-1118. 


Little, K., Ramssen, E., Welchko, R., Volberg, V., 
Roland, C., & Cassin, B. (2009). Decreased 
brain dopamine cell numbers in human 
cocaine users. Psychiatry Research, 168, 173-180. 


Liu, J. H., & Mills, D. (2006). Modern racism and 
neo-liberal globalization: The discourses of 
plausible deniability and their multiple 
functions. Journal of Community & Applied 
Social Psychology, 16, 83-99. 


Liu, L., He, S-Z., & Wu, Y. (2007). An analysis of 
the characteristics of single parent families 
with different structures and their children. 
Chinese Journal of Clinical Psychology, 15, 68-70. 


Livesley, W., & Jang, K. (2008). The behavioral 
genetics of personality disorder. Annual 
Review of Clinical Psychology, 4, 247-274. 


Lobato, M. I., Koff, W. J., & Manenti, C. (2006). 
Follow-up of sex reassignment surgery in 
transsexuals: A Brazilian cohort. Archives of 
Sexual Behavior, 35, 711-715. 


Lobban, F., Barrowclough, C., & Jones, S. (2006). 
Does expressed emotion need to be under- 
stood within a more systemic framework? An 
examination of discrepancies in appraisals 
between patients diagnosed with schizophre- 
nia and their relatives. Social Psychiatry and 
Psychiatric Epidemiology, 41, 50-55. 


Lobo, I., & Harris, R. (2008, July). GABA, recep- 
tors and alcohol. Pharmacology, Biochemistry 
and Behavior, 90, 90-94. 


Locicero, A., & Sinclair, S. (2008, March). Terrorism 
and terrorist leaders: Insights from develop- 


References R-31 


mental and ecological psychology. Studies in 
Conflict & Terrorism, 31, 227-250. 


Locke, J. L. (2006). Parental selection of vocal 
behavior: Crying, cooking, babbling, and the 
evolution of language. Human Nature, 17, 
155-168. 


Lockl, K., & Schneider, W. (2007). Knowledge 
about the mind: Links between theory of mind 
and later metamemory. Child Development, 78, 
148-167. 


Loehlin, J. C. (2002). The IQ paradox: Resolved? 
Still an open question. Psychological Review, 
109, 754-758. 


Lofholm, N. (2003, May 6). Climber’s kin share 
relief: Ralston saw 4 options, they say; death 
wasn’t one of them. Denver Post, p. A1. 


Loftus, E. F. (1993). Psychologists in the eyewit- 
ness world. American Psychologist, 48, 550-552. 


Loftus, E. F. (2004). Memories of things unseen. 
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13, 
145-147. 


Loftus, E. F., & Bernstein, D. M. (2005). Rich false 
memories: The royal road to success. In A. F. 
Healy, Experimental cognitive psychology and its 
applications. Washington, DC: American 
Psychological Association. 


Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C. (1974). Reconstruc- 
tion of automobile destruction: An example of 
the interface between language and memory. 
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 
13, 585-589. 


Long, G. M., & Beaton, R. J. (1982). The case for 
peripheral persistence: Effects of target and 
background luminance on a partial-report 
task. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human 
Perception and Performance, 8, 383-391. 


Lopes, A. C., Greenberg, B. D., Noren, G., Canteras, 
M. M., Busatto, G. F. de Mathis, et al. (2009). 
Treatment of resistant obsessive-compulsive 
disorder with ventral capsular /ventral striatal 
gamma capsulotomy: A pilot prospective study. 
The Journal of Neuropsychiatry and Clinical 
Neurosciences, 21, 381-392. 


Lopez, S. R., & Guarnaccia, P. J. (2005). Cultural 
dimensions of psychopathology: The social 
world’s impact on mental illness. In J. E. 
Maddux & B. A. Winstead (Eds.), Foundations 
for a contemporary understanding. Mahwah, NJ: 
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 


Lopez, S. R., & Guarnaccia, P. J. J. (2000). Cultural 
psychopathology: Uncovering the social 
world of mental illness. Annual Review of 
Psychology, 51, 571-598. 


Lorenz, K. (1966). On aggression. New York: 
Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. 


Lorenz, K. (1974). Civilized man’s eight deadly sins. 
New York: Harcourt Brace Jovanovich. 


Lothane, Z. (2005). Jung, A biography. Journal of 
the American Psychoanalytic Association, 53, 
317-324. 


Loving, T., Crockett, E., & Paxson, A. (2009). 
Passionate love and relationship thinkers: 
Experimental evidence for acute cortisol ele- 


vations in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 
34, 939-946. 

Lowe, P., Humphreys, C., & Williams, S. J. (2007). 
Night terrors: Women’s experiences of (not) 


sleeping where there is domestic violence. 
Violence against Women, 13, 549-561. 


R-32 References 


Lowery, D., Fillingim, R. B., & Wright, R. A. (2003). 
Sex differences and incentive effects on per- 
ceptual and cardiovascular responses to cold 
pressor pain. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 
284-291. 


Lu, J., Sherman, D., Devor, M., & Saper, C. B. (2006). 
A putative flip-flop switch for control of REM 
sleep. Nature, 441, 589-594. 


Lubell, K. M., Swahn, M. H., Crosby, A. E., & 
Kegler, S. R. (2004). Methods of suicide 
among persons aged 10-19 years—United 
States, 1992-2001. MMWR, 53, 471-473. 
Retrieved from http://www.cdc.gov/ 
mmwr/PDF/wk/mm5322.pdf. 


Lubinski, D., & Benbow, C. P. (2006). Study of 
mathematically precocious youth after 35 
years: Uncovering antecedents for the devel- 
opment of math-science expertise. Perspectives 
on Psychological Science, 1, 316-345. 


Lublin, H., Eberhard, J., & Levander, S. (2005). 
Current therapy issues and unmet clinical 
needs in the treatment of schizophrenia: A 
review of the new generation antipsychotics. 
International Clinical Psychopharmacology, 20, 
183-198. 


Lucas, W. (2008). Parents’ perceptions of the 
Drug Abuse Resistance Education program 
(DARE). Journal of Child & Adolescent Substance 
Abuse, 17, 99-114. 


Luchins, A. S. (1946). Classroom experiments on 
mental set. American Journal of Psychology, 59, 
295-298. 


Lucki, I., & O'Leary, O. F. (2004). Distinguishing 
roles for norepinephrine and serotonin in the 
behavioral effects of antidepressant drugs. 
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 65, 11-24. 


Luders, E., Narr, K. L., Thompson, P. M., & Toga, 
A. W. (2009). Neuroanatomical correlates of 
intelligence. Intelligence, 37, 156-163. 


Luders, E., Narr, K. L., Zaidel, E., Thompson, 
P. M., & Toga, A. W. (2006). Gender effects on 
callosal thickness in scaled and unscaled 
space. Neuroreport, 17, 1103-1106. 


Ludwig, A. M. (1996, March). Mental disturbances 
and creative achievement. The Harvard Mental 
Health Letter, pp. 4-6. 


Lun, V. M., & Bond, M. H. (2006). Achieving rela- 
tionship harmony in groups and its conse- 
quence for group performance. Asian Journal 
of Social Psychology, 9, 195-202. 

Luo, S., & Zhang, G. (2009). What leads to roman- 
tic attraction: Similarity, reciprocity, security, 
or beauty? Evidence from a speed-dating 
study. Journal of Personality, 77, 933-964. 

Luria, A. R. (1968). The mind of a mnemonist. 
Cambridge, MA: Basic Books. 

Lurie, K., Robinson, A., & Pecsenye, M. (2005). 
Cracking the GRE 2006. New York: Random 
House. 

Luthar, S. S., Cicchetti, D., & Becker, B. (2000). 
The construct of resilience: A critical evalua- 


tion and guidelines for future work. Child 
Development, 71, 543-562. 


Lutz, C. K. & Novak, M. A. (2005) Environmental 
enrichment for nonhuman primates: theory 
and application. ILAR Journal, 46, 178-191. 

Lutz, W., Lambert, M. J., Harmon, S. C., Tschitsaz, 
A., Schurch, E., & Stulz, N (2006). The proba- 
bility of treatment success, failure and 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


duration—What can be learned from empirical 
data to support decision making in clinical 
practice? Clinical Psychology & Psychotherapy, 
13, 223-232. 


Ly, D. H., Lockhart, D. J., Lerner, R. A., & Schultz, 
P. G. (2000, March 31). Mitotic misregulation 
and human aging. Science, 287, 2486-2492. 


Lykken, D. T. (1995). The antisocial personalities. 
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 


Lymberis, S. C., Parhar, P. K., Katsoulakis, E., & 
Formenti, S. C. (2004). Pharmacogenomics 
and breast cancer. Pharmacogenomics, 5, 31-55. 


Lynch, T. R., Trost, W. T, Salsman, N., & Linehan, 
M. M. (2007). Dialectical behavior therapy for 
borderline personality disorder. Annual 
Review of Clinical Psychology, 3, 181-205. 


Lyness, K. S., & Heilman, M. E. (2006). When fit is 
fundamental: performance evaluations and 
promotions of upper-level female and male 
managers. Journal of Applied Psychology, 91, 
777-785. 


Lynn, R. (2009). What has caused the Flynn 
effect? Secular increases in the Development 
Quotients of infants. Intelligence, 37, 16-24. 


Lynn, S. J., Fassler, O., & Knox, J. (2005). Hypnosis 
and the altered state debate: Something more 
or nothing more? Comment. Contemporary 
Hypnosis, 22, 39-45. 


Lynn, S. J., Kirsch, I., Barabasz, A., Cardena, E., & 
Patterson, D. (2000). Hypnosis as an empiri- 
cally supported clinical intervention: The 
state of the evidence and a look to the future. 
International Journal of Clinical and Experimental 
Hypnosis, 48, 239-259. 


Lynn, S. J., Lock, T., Loftus, E. F., Krackow, E., & 
Lilienfeld, S. O. (2003). The remembrance of 
things past: Problematic memory recovery 
techniques in psychotherapy. In S. O. Lilienfeld, 
S. J. Lynn, & J. M. Lohr (Eds.), Science and pseu- 
doscience in clinical psychology. New York: 
Guilford Press. 


Lynn, S. J., Neufeld, V., Green, J. P., Sandberg, D., et 
al. (1996). Daydreaming, fantasy, and psycho- 
pathology. In R. G. Kunzendorf, N. P. Spanos, 
& B. Wallace (Eds.), Hypnosis and imagination. 
Imagery and human development series. 
Amityville, NY: Baywood. 


Macaluso, E., & Driver, J. (2005). Multisensory 
spatial interactions: a window onto functional 
integration in the human brain. Trends in 
Neurosciences, 28, Issue 5, 264-271. 


Macaluso, E., Frith, C. D., & Driver, J. (2000, 
August 18). Modulation of human visual 
cortex by crossmodal spatial attention. 
Science, 289, 1206-1208. 


Macduff, I. (2006). Your pace or mine? Culture, time 
and negotiation. Negotiation Journal, 22, 31—45. 
Mackay, J., & Eriksen, M. (2002). The tobacco 
atlas. Geneva, Switzerland: World Health 
Organization. 

Mack, J. (2003). The museum of the mind. London: 
British Museum Publications. 

MacLean, L., Edwards, N., Garrard, M., 
Sims-Jones, N., Clinton, K., & Ashley, L. 
(2009, March). Obesity, stigma and public 
health planning. Health Promotion International, 
24, 88-93. 





MacLennan, A. (2009). Evidence-based review of 
therapies at the menopause. International 
Journal of Evidence-Based Healthcare, 7, 112-123. 


Macmillan, M. (1996). Freud evaluated: The com- 
pleted arc. Cambridge, MA: MIT. 


MacNeilage, P. F., Rogers, L. J., & Vallortigara, 
G. (2009, July). Origins of the left & right 
brain. Scientific American, pp. 60-67. 


MacPherson, K. (2002, March 17). Unfilled pink- 
collar jobs threaten service cuts. Pittsburgh 
Post-Gazette, p. 7. 


Madden, D. J. (2007). Aging and visual attention. 
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 
70-74. 


Maddi, S. R. (2007). The story of hardiness: 
Twenty years of theorizing, research, and 
practice. In A. Monat, R. S. Lazarus, & 

G. Reevy, The Praeger handbook on stress and 
coping (Vol. 2). Westport, CT: Praeger 
Publishers/ Greenwood Publishing. 


Mader, S. S. (2000). Biology (6th ed.). Boston: 
McGraw-Hill. 


Madon, S., Willard, J., & Guyll, M. (2006). Self- 
fulfilling prophecy effects of mothers’ beliefs 
on children’s alcohol use: Accumulation, dis- 
sipation, and stability over time. Journal of 
Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 911-926. 


Magida, A. J. (2006). Opening the doors of wonder: 
Reflections on religious rites of passage. Berkeley, 
CA: University of California Press. 


Magley, V. J., (2002). Coping with sexual harass- 
ment: Reconceptualizing women’s resistance. 
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83, 
930-946. 


Magoni, M., Bassani, L., Okong, P., Kituuka, P., 
Germinario, E. P., Giuliano, M., et al. (2005). 
Mode of infant feeding and HIV infection in 
children in a program for prevention of 
mother-to-child transmission in Uganda. 
AIDS, 19, 433-437. 


Magoon, M., & Critchfield, T. (2008). Concurrent 
schedules of positive and negative reinforce- 
ment: Differential-impact and differential- 
outcomes hypotheses. Journal of the 
Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 90, 1-22. 


Magruder, K., & Yeager, D. (2009). The prevalence 
of PTSD across war eras and the effect of 
deployment on PTSD: A systematic review and 
meta-analysis. Psychiatric Annals, 39, 778-788. 


Maguire, E. A., Kumaran, D., Hassabis, D., & 
Kopelman, M.D. (2010). Autobiographical 
memory in semantic dementia: A longitudinal 
fMRI study. Neuropsychologia, 48, 123-136. 

Maguire, E. A., Woollett, K., & Spiers, H. J. (2006). 
London taxi drivers and bus drivers: A 
structural MRI and neuropsychological 
analysis. Hippocampus, 16, 1091-1101. 


Mahmood, M., & Black, J. (2005). Narcolepsyca- 
taplexy: How does recent understanding help 
in evaluation and treatment. Current Treatment 
Options in Neurology, 7, 363-371. 


Maier, S. F., & Watkins, L. R. (2000). Learned 
helplessness. In A. E. Kazdin, Encyclopedia of 
psychology (Vol. 4). Washington, DC: American 
Psychological Association. 

Majdandzic, M., & van den Boom, D. C. (2007). 
Multimethod longitudinal assessment of 


temperament in early childhood. Journal of 
Personality, 75, 121-167. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Majeres, R. L. (2007). Sex differences in phono- 
logical coding: Alphabet transformation 
speed. Intelligence, 35, 335-346. 


Malamuth, N. M., Linz, D., Heavey, C. L., & 
Barnes, G. (1995). Using the confluence model 
of sexual aggression to predict men’s conflict 
with women: A 10-year follow-up study. Journal 
of Personality and Social Psychology, 69, 353-369. 


Maldonado, J. R., & Spiegel, D. (2003). Dissociative 
disorders. In R. E. Hales & S. C. Yudofsky, The 
American Psychiatric Publishing textbook of 
clinical psychiatry (4th ed.). Washington, DC: 
American Psychiatric Publishing. 


Malle, B. F. (2004). How the mind explains behavior: 
Folk explanations, meaning, and social interac- 
tion. Cambridge, MA: MIT. 


Malouff, J. M., Thorsteinsson, E. B., & Schutte, 
N. S. (2007). The efficacy of problem solving 
therapy in reducing mental and physical 
health problems: A meta-analysis. Clinical 
Psychology Review, 27, 46-57. 


Malpas, P. (2008, April). Predictive genetic 
testing of children for adult-onset diseases 
and psychological harm. Journal of Medical 
Ethics, 34, 275-278. 


Mamassis, G., & Doganis, G. (2004). The effects 
of a mental training program on juniors 
pre-competitive anxiety, self-confidence, and 
tennis performance. Journal of Applied Sport 
Psychology, 16, 118-137. 


Mancinelli, R., Binetti, R., & Ceccanti, M. (2007). 
Woman, alcohol and environment: Emerging 
risks for health. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral 
Reviews, 31, 246-253. 


Manly, J., & Echemendia, R. (2007). Race-specific 
norms: Using the model of hypertension to 
understand issues of race, culture, and 
education in neuropsychology. Archives of 
Clinical Neuropsychology, 22, 319-325. 


Manly, J. J. (2005). Advantages and disadvantages 
of separate norms for African Americans. 
Clinical Neuropsychologist, 19, 270-275. 


Manly, J. J. (2006). Deconstructing race and 
ethnicity: Implications for measurement of 
health outcomes [Special issue: Measurement 
in a multi-ethnic society]. Medical Care, 44, 
S10-S16. 


Mann, K., Ackermann, K., Croissant, B., Mundle, 
G., Nakovics, H., & Diehl, A. (2005). Neuro- 
imaging of gender differences in alcohol 
dependence: are women more vulnerable? 
Alcoholism: Clinical & Experimental Research, 29, 
896-901. 


Mann, R. E., & Hollin, C. R. (2007). Sexual 
offenders’ explanations for their offending. 
Journal of Sexual Aggression, 13, 3-9. 


Manning, M. A., & Hoyme, E. H. (2007). Fetal 
alcohol spectrum disorders: A practical 
clinical approach to diagnosis. Neuroscience & 
Biobehavioral Reviews, 31, 230-238. 


Manning, S. Y. (2005). Dialectical behavior 
therapy of severe and chronic problems. In 
L. VandeCreek (Ed.), Innovations in clinical 
practice: Focus on adults. Sarasota, FL: Pro- 
fessional Resource Press /Professional 
Resource Exchange. 


Manor, J. K., & Gailliot, M. T. (2007). Altruism 
and egoism: Prosocial motivations for helping 


depend on relationship context. European 
Journal of Social Psychology, 37, 347-358. 


Manstead, A. S. R., Frijda, N., & Fischer, A. H. 
(Eds.). (2003). Feelings and emotions: The 
Amsterdam Symposium. Cambridge, England: 
Cambridge University Press. 


Manstead, A. S. R., & Wagner, H. L. (2004). 
Experience emotion. Cambridge, England: 
Cambridge University Press. 


Marcaurelle, R., Bélanger, C., & Marchand, A. 
(2005). Marital predictors of symptom 
severity in panic disorder with agoraphobia. 
Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 19, 211-232. 


Marcus-Newhall, A., Pedersen, W. C., & Carlson, 
M. (2000). Displaced aggression is alive and 
well: A meta-analytic review. Journal of 
Personality and Social Psychology, 78, 670-689. 


Markoff, J. (2009, July 25). Scientists worry 
machines may outsmart man. The New York 
Times, p. A1. 


Marks, I. M. (2004). The Nobel prize award in 
physiology to Ivan Petrovich Pavlov-1904. 
Australian and New Zealand Journal of 
Psychiatry, 38, 674-677. 


Markus, H. R. (2007). Sociocultural psychology: 
The dynamic interdependence among self 
systems and social systems. In S. Kitayama, & 
D. Cohen, (Eds.), Handbook of cultural psychol- 
ogy. New York: Guilford Press. 


Markus, H. R., & Kitayama, S. (2003). Models 
of agency: Sociocultural diversity in the 
construction of action. In V. Murphy-Berman 
& J. J. Berman (Eds.), Cross-cultural differences 
in perspectives on the self. Lincoln, NE: 
University of Nebraska Press. 


Marmar, C. (2009). Mental health impact of 
Afghanistan and Iraq deployment: Meeting 
the challenge of a new generation of veterans. 
Depression and Anxiety, 26, 493-497. 


Maroda, K. J. (2004). A relational perspective on 
women and power. Psychoanalytic Psychology, 
21, 428-435. 


Marrero, H., & Gamez, E. (2004). Content and 
strategy in syllogistic reasoning. Canadian 
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 58, 168-180. 


Marsh, B. (2008, February 24). A growing cloud 
over the planet. The New York Times, p. WK4. 


Marsh, H. W., Hau, K. T., & Sung, R. Y. T. (2007). 
Childhood obesity, gender, actual-ideal body 
image discrepancies, and physical self- 
concept in Hong Kong children: Cultural 
differences in the value of moderation. 
Developmental Psychology, 43, 647-662. 


Marshall, K., Laing, D. G., & Jinks, A. L. (2006). 
The capacity of humans to identify compo- 
nents in complex odor-taste mixtures. 
Chemical Senses, 31, 539-545. 


Marshall, L., & Born, J. (2007, October). The 
contribution of sleep to hippocampus- 
dependent memory consolidation. Trends in 
Cognitive Sciences, 11(10), 442-450. 


Marshall, M. K. (2007). The critical factors of 
coaching practice leading to successful 
coaching outcomes. Dissertation Abstracts 
International: Section B: The Sciences and 
Engineering, 67(7-B), 4092. 


Marshall, R. D., Bryant, R. A, & Amsel, L. (2007). 
The psychology of ongoing threat: Relative 
risk appraisal, the September 11 attacks, and 


References R-33 


terrorism-related fears. American Psychologist, 
62, 304-316. 


Marshall, R. D., Bryant, R. A., Amsel, L., Suh, 
E. J., Cook, J. M., & Neria, Y. (2007). The 
psychology of ongoing threat: Relative risk 
appraisal, the September 11 attacks and ter- 
rorism-related fears. American Psychologist, 62, 
304-316. 


Marszalek, J. (2007). Computerized adaptive test- 
ing and the experience of flow in examinees. 
Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: 
Humanities and Social Sciences, 67(7-A), 2465. 


Martelle, S., Hanley, C., & Yoshino K. (2003, 
January 28). “Sopranos” scenario in slaying? 
Los Angeles Times, p. B1. 


Martin, A. J., & Marsh, H. W. (2002). Fear of 
failure: Friend or foe? Australian Psychologist, 
38, 31-38. 


Martin, L., & Pullum, G. K. (1991). The great 
Eskimo vocabulary hoax. Chicago: University of 
Chicago Press. 


Martin, P. D., & Brantley, P. J. (2004). Stress, 
coping, and social support in health and 
behavior. In J. M. Raczynski & L. C. Leviton 
(Eds.), Handbook of clinical health psychology, 
Vol. 2: Disorders of behavior and health. 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 





Martindale, C. (1981). Cognition and consciousness. 
Homewood, IL: Dorsey. 


Mascia, K., & Servis, R. (2009, August 24). Mail 
carriers to the rescue. People, p. 108-110. 


Mashour, G. A., Walker, E. E., & Martuza, R. L. 
(2005). Psychosurgery: Past, present, and 
future. Brain Research Reviews, 48, 409-419. 


Maslow, A. H. (1970). Motivation and personality. 
New York: Harper & Row. 


Maslow, A. H. (1987). Motivation and personality 
(3rd ed.). New York: Harper & Row. 


Mason, M. (1995). The making of Victorian 
sexuality. New York: Oxford University Press. 


Massaro, D. W., & Chen, T. H. (2008). The motor 
theory of speech perception revisited. 
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 15, 453-457. 
Mast, F. W., & Kosslyn, S. M. (2002). Visual 
mental images can be ambiguous: Insights 
from individual differences in spatial 
transformation abilities. Cognition, 86, 57-70. 
Masters, W. H., & Johnson, V. E. (1966). Human 
sexual response. Boston: Little, Brown. 
Masters, W. H., & Johnson, V. E. (1979). Homo- 
sexuality in perspective. Boston: Little, Brown. 


Masters, W., & Johnson, V. (1994). Hetero- 
sexuality. New York: Harper Collins. 





Masuda, M. (2003). Meta-analyses of love scales: 
Do various love scales measure the same 
psychological constructs? Japanese Psycholo- 
gical Research, 45, 25-37. 


Mataix-Cols, D., & Bartres-Faz, D. (2002). Is the 
use of the wooden and computerized ver- 
sions of the Tower of Hanoi Puzzle equiva- 
lent? Applied Neuropsychology, 9, 117-120. 

Matlin, M. W. (1996). The psychology of women (3rd. 
ed.). Orlando, FL: Harcourt Brace College 
Publishers. 

Maton, K. I., Kohout, J. L., Wicherski, M., Leary, 
G. E., & Vinokurov, A. (2006). Minority 


R-34 References 


students of color and the psychology graduate 
pipeline. American Psychologist, 61, 117-131. 


Matson, J., & LoVullo, S. (2008). A review of 
behavioral treatments for self-injurious 
behaviors of persons with autism spectrum 
disorders. Behavior Modification, 32, 61-76. 


Matsumoto, D. (2002). Methodological require- 
ments to test a possible in-group advantage in 
judging emotions across cultures: Comment 
on Elfenbein and Ambady (2002) and evi- 
dence. Psychological Bulletin, 128, 236-242. 


Matsumoto, D., & Willingham, B. (2009). 
Spontaneous facial expressions of emotion of 
congenitally and noncongenitally blind indi- 
viduals. Journal of Personality and Social 
Psychology, 96, 1-10. 


Matthews, G., & Funke, G. J. (2006). Worry and 
information-processing. In G. C. L. Davey & 
A. Wells, Worry and its psychological disorders: 
Theory, assessment and treatment. Hoboken, NJ: 
Wiley Publishing. 

Maurer, D., Lewis, T. L., Brent, H. P., & Levin, 
A. V. (1999, October 1). Rapid improvement in 
the acuity of infants after visual input. Science, 
286, 108-110. 


Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2004). 
Emotional intelligence: Theory, findings, and 
implications. Psychological Inquiry, 15, 197-215. 


Mayer, J. D., Salovey, P., & Caruso, D. R. (2008). 
Emotional intelligence: New ability or eclectic 
traits? American Psychologist, 63, 503-517. 


Maynard, A. E., & Martini, M. I. (2005). Learning 
in cultural context: Family, peers, and school. 
New York: Kluwer Academic/Plenum 
Publishers. 


Mazard, A., Laou, L., Joliot, M., & Mellet, E. 
(2005). Neural impact of the semantic content 
of visual mental images and visual percepts. 
Brain Research and Cognitive Brain Research, 24, 
423-435. 


McAdams, D. P., Diamond, A., de St. Aubin, E., & 
Mansfield, E. (1997). Stories of commitment: 
The psychosocial construction of generative 
lives. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 
72, 678-694. 


McCabe, C., & Rolls, E. T. (2007). Umami: A deli- 
cious flavor formed by convergence of taste 
and olfactory pathways in the human brain. 
European Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 
1855-1864. 


McCabe, M. (2009). Anorgasmia in women. 
Journal of Family Psychotherapy, 20, 177-197. 


McCarthy, J. (2005). Individualism and collectiv- 
ism: What do they have to do with counsel- 
ing? Journal of Multicultural Counseling and 
Development, 33, 108-117. 


McCaul, K. D., Johnson, R. J., & Rothman, A. J. 
(2002). The effects of framing and action 
instructions on whether older adults obtain 
flu shots. Health Psychology, 21, 624-628. 


McCauley, R. N., & Henrich, J. (2006), Suscepti- 
bility to the Miiller-Lyer illusion, theory- 
neutral observation, and the diachronic 
penetrability of the visual input Philosophical 
Psychology, 19, 79-101. 


McClelland, D. C. (1985). How motives, skills, 
and values determine what people do. 
American Psychologist, 40, 812-825. 





McClelland, D. C. (1993). Intelligence is not the 
best predictor of job performance. Current 
Directions in Psychological Research, 2, 5-8. 


McClelland, D. C., Atkinson, J. W., Clark, R. A., & 
Lowell, E. L. (1953). The achievement motive. 
New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. 


McClure, J., Sutton, R. M., & Sibley, C. G. (2007). 
Listening to reporters or engineers? How 
instance-based messages about building 
design affect earthquake fatalism. Journal of 
Applied Social Sciences, 37, 1956-1973. 


McCrae, R. R., & Costa, P. T., Jr. (1986). A five- 
factor theory of personality. In L. A. Pervin & 
O. P. John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: 
Theory and research (2nd ed.). New York: 
Guilford. 


McCrae, R. R., Terracciano A., & 78 Members of 
the Personality Profiles of Cultures Project. 
(2005). Universal features of personality traits 
from the observer’s perspective: Data from 50 
cultures. Journal of Personality and Social Psy- 
chology, 88, 547-561. 


McCrink, K., & Wynn, K. (2007). Ratio abstraction 
by 6-month-old infants. Psychological Science, 
18, 740-745. 


McDaniel, M. A., Maier, S. F., & Einstein, G. O. 
(2002). “Brain specific” nutrients: A memory 
cure? Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 
3, 12-18. 


McDonald, C., & Murray, R. M. (2004). Can 
structural magnetic resonance imaging 
provide an alternative phenotype for genetic 
studies of schizophrenia? In M. S. Keshavan, 
J. L. Kennedy, & R. M. Murray (Eds.), Neuro- 
development and schizophrenia. New York: 
Cambridge University Press. 


McDonald, H. E., & Hirt, E. R. (1997). When 
expectancy meets desire: Motivational effects 
in reconstructive memory. Journal of 
Personality and Social Psychology, 72, 5-23. 


McDougall, W. (1908). Introduction to social psy- 
chology. London: Methuen. 


McDowell, D. M., & Spitz, H. I. (1999). Substance 
abuse. New York: Brunner/ Mazel. 


McEwen, B. S. (1998, January 15). Protective and 
damaging effects of stress mediators [Review 
article]. New England Journal of Medicine, 338, 
171-179. 


McGaugh, J. L. (2003). Memory and emotion: The 
making of lasting memories. New York: Columbia 
University Press. 


McGilvray, J. (Ed.). (2004). The Cambridge compan- 
ion to Chomsky. Oxford, England: Cambridge 
University Press. 


McGinn, D. (2003, June 9). Testing, testing: The 
new job search. Time, pp. 36-38. 


McGregor, K. K., & Capone, N. C. (2004). Genetic 
and environmental interactions in determin- 
ing the early lexicon: Evidence from a set of 
tri-zygotic quadruplets. Journal of Child 
Language, 31, 311-337. 


McHale, S. M., Crouter, A. C., & Tucker, C. J. 
(1999). Family context and gender role 
socialization in middle childhood: 
Comparing girls to boys and sisters to 
brothers. Child Development, 70, 990-1004. 


McIntyre, K., Korn, J., & Matsuo, H. (2008). 
Sweating the small stuff: How different types 
of hassles result in the experience of stress. 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Stress and Health: Journal of the International 
Society for the Investigation of Stress, 24, 383-392. 


McKinley, M. J., Cairns, M. J., Denton, D. A., Egan, 
G., Mathai, M. L., Uschakov, A., et al. (2004). 
Physiological and pathophysiological 
influences on thirst. Physiology and Behavior, 
81, 795-803. 


McMurtray, A. M., Licht, E., Yeo, T., Krisztal, E., 
Saul, R. E., & Mendez, M. F. (2007). Positron 
emission tomography facilitates diagnosis of 
early-onset Alzheimer’s disease. European 
Neurology, 59, 31-37. 


McNamara, P. (2004). An evolutionary psychology 
of sleep and dreams. Westport, CT: Praeger 
Publishers/Greenwood Publishing Group. 


Mead, M. (1949). Male and female. New York: 
Morrow. 


Mealey, L. (2000). Sex differences: Developmental 
and evolutionary strategies. San Diego, CA: 
Academic Press. 


Means, M. K., & Edinger, J. D. (2007). Graded 
exposure therapy for addressing claustropho- 
bic reactions to continuous positive airway 
pressure: A case series report. Behavioral Sleep 
Medicine, 5, 105-116. 


Medeiros, R., Prediger, R. D. S., Passos, G. F., ` 
Pandolfo, P., et al. (2007). Connecting TNF-I+ 
signaling pathways to iNOS expression in a 
mouse model of Alzheimer’s disease: 
Relevance for the behavioral and synaptic 
deficits induced by amyloid ?? protein. Journal 
of Neuroscience, 27, 5394-5404. 


Meeter, M., & Murre, J. M. J. (2004). Consolidation 
of long-term memory: Evidence and alterna- 
tives. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 843-857. 


Mehl, M. R., Vazire, S., Ramirez-Esparza, N., 
Slatcher, R. B., & Pennebaker, J. W. (2007, 
July 6). Are women really more talkative than 
men? Science, 317, 82. 


Mehl-Madrona, L. E. (2004). Hypnosis to facili- 
tate uncomplicated birth. American Journal of 
Clinical Hypnosis, 46, 299-312. 


Meinlschmidt, G., & Heim, C. (2007). Sensitivity 
to intranasal oxytocin in adult men with 
early parental separation. Biological Psychiatry, 
61, 1109-1111. 


Mel, B. W. (2002, March 8). What the synapse tells 
the neuron. Science, 295, 1845-1846. 


Melnikov, K. S. (1993, October-December). On 
some aspects of the mechanistic approach to 
the study of processes of forgetting. Vestnik 
Moskovskogo Universiteta Seriya 14 Psikhologiya, 
pp. 64-67. 


Meltzer, H. Y. (2000). Genetics and etiology of 
schizophrenia and bipolar disorder. Biological 
Psychiatry, 47, 171-173. 


Meltzoff, A. N. (1996). The human infant as imita- 
tive generalist: A 20-year progress report on 
infant imitation with implications for compar- 
ative psychology. In C. M. Heyes & B. G. Galef, 
Jr. (Eds.), Social learning in animals: The roots of 
culture. San Diego, CA: Academic Press. 

Melzack, R., & Katz, J. (2001). The McGill Pain 
Questionnaire: Appraisal and current status. In 
D. Turk & R. Melzack, (Eds.). Handbook of pain 
assessment (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford Press. 


Melzack, R., & Katz, J. (2004). The gate control 
theory: Reaching for the brain. Mahwah, NJ: 
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Mendelsohn, J. (2003, November 7-9). What we 
know about sex. USA Weekend, pp. 6-9. 


Mental Health Information Center, U.S. 
Department of Health and Human Services. 
(2002, December). Deinstitutionalization. 
Scientific American, p. 38. 


Merikangas, K. R., Ames, M., Cui, L., Stang, P. E., 
Ustun, T. B., VonKorff, M., et al. (2007). The 
impact of comorbidity of mental and physical 
conditions on role disability in the US adult 
household population. Archives of General 
Psychiatry, 64, 1180-1188. 


Messer, S. B., & McWilliams, N. (2003). The 
impact of Sigmund Freud and The Inter- 
pretation of Dreams. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The 
anatomy of impact: What makes the great works of 
psychology great. Washington, DC: American 
Psychological Association. 


Messner, M., Reinhard, M., & Sporer, S. (2008). 
Compliance through direct persuasive 
appeals: The moderating role of communica- 
tor’s attractiveness in interpersonal persua- 
sion. Social Influence, 3, 67-83. 


Meston, C. M (2003). Validation of the female sex- 
ual function index (FSFI) in women with 
female orgasmic disorder and in women with 
hypoactive sexual desire disorder. Journal of 
Sex and Marital Therapy, 29, 39-46. 


Meyer-Bahlburg, H. (1997). The role of prenatal 
estrogens in sexual orientation. In L. Ellis & 
L. Ebertz (Eds.), Sexual orientation: Toward 
biological understanding. Westport, CT: 
Praeger. 


Meyer, I., & Ladewig, J. (2008). The relationship 
between number of training sessions per 
week and learning in dogs. Applied Animal 
Behaviour Science, 111, 311-320. 


Meyerowitz, J. (2004). How sex changed: A history 
of transsexuality in the United States. Cambridge, 
MA: Harvard University Press. 


Michael, R. T., Gagnon, J. H., Laumann, E. O., & 
Kolata, G. (1994). Sex in America: A definitive 
survey. Boston: Little, Brown. 


Midanik, L. T., Tam, T. W., & Weisner, C. (2007). 
Concurrent and simultaneous drug and 
alcohol use: Results of the 2000 national 
alcohol survey. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 
90, 72-80. 


Middlebrooks, J. C., Furukawa, S., Stecker, G. C., 
& Mickey, B. J. (2005). Distributed representa- 
tion of sound-source location in the auditory 
cortex. In R. König, P. Heil, E. Budinger, & 

H. Scheich (Eds.), Auditory cortex: A synthesis of 
human and animal research. Mahwah, NJ: 
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 


Miesenbock, G. (2008, October). Lighting up the 
brain. Scientific American, pp. 52-59. 


Mifflin, L. (1998, January 14). Study finds a 
decline in TV network violence. The New 
York Times, p. A14. 

Mignon, A., & Mollaret, P. (2002). Applying the 
affordance conception of traits: A person 


perception study. Personality and Social 
Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1327-1334. 


Mika, V. S., Wood, P. R., Weiss, B. D., & Trevino, 
L. (2007). Ask Me 3: Improving communica- 
tion in a Hispanic pediatric outpatient prac- 
tice. American Journal of Behavioral Health, 31, 
$115-S121. 


Miklowitz, D. J., & Thompson, M. C. (2003). 
Family variables and interventions in schizo- 
phrenia. In G. Sholevar & G. Pirooz (Eds.), 
Textbook of family and couples therapy: Clinical 
applications. Washington, DC: American 
Psychiatric Publishing. 


Mikulincer, M., & Shaver, P. R. (2005). Attachment 
security, compassion, and altruism. Current 
Directions in Psychological Science, 14, 

34-38. 


Miletic, M. P. (2002). The introduction of a 
feminine psychology to psychoanalysis: 
Karen Horney’s legacy [Special issue: 
Interpersonal psychoanalysis and feminism]. 
Contemporary Psychoanalysis, 38, 287-299. 


Milgram, S. (1965). Obedience [film]. New York 
University Film Library and Pennsylvania 
State University, PCR. 


Milgram, S. (2005). Obedience to authority. Pinter 
& Martin: New York. 


Miller, G. (2006). A spoonful of medicine—and a 
steady diet of normalcy. Science, 311, 464-465. 


Miller, G. (2008, May 9). The roots of morality. 
Science, 320, 734-737. 


Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, 
plus or minus two: Some limits on our capac- 
ity for processing information. Psychology 
Review, 63, 81-97. 


Miller, G. F., & Penke, L. (2007). The evolution of 
human intelligence and the coefficient of 
additive genetic variance in human brain size. 
Intelligence, 35, 97-114. 


Miller, J. A., & Leffard, S. A. (2007). Behavioral 
assessment. In S. R. Smith & L. Handler, The 
clinical assessment of children and adolescents: A 
practitioner's handbook. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence 
Erlbaum Associates. 


Miller, J. G. (1984). Culture and the development 
of everyday social explanation. Journal of 
Personality and Social Psychology, 46, 961-978. 


Miller, L. A., Taber, K. H., Gabbard, G. O., & 
Hurley, R. A. (2005). Neural underpinnings 
of fear and its modulation: Implications for 
anxiety disorders. The Journal of Neuropsychiatry 
and Clinical Neurosciences, 17, 1-6. 


Miller, L., Gur, M., Shanok, A., & Weissman, M. 
(2008). Interpersonal psychotherapy with 
pregnant adolescents: Two pilot studies. 
Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49, 
733-742. 


Miller, M. N., & Pumariega, A. J. (2001). Culture 
and eating disorders: A historical and cross- 
cultural review. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and 
Biological Processes, 64, 93-110. 


Miller, N. E., & Magruder, K. M. (Eds.). (1999). 
Cost-effectiveness of psychotherapy: A guide for 
practitioners, researchers, and policymakers. New 
York: Oxford University Press. 


Miller, W. R., & Thoresen, C. E. (2003). Spirituality, 
religion, and health: An emerging research 
field. American Psychologist, 58, 24-35. 


Miller-Jones, D. (1991). Informal reasoning in 
inner-city children. In J. F. Voss & D. N. 
Perkins (Eds.), Informal reasoning and educa- 
tion. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. 

Miller-Perrin, C., Perrin, R., & Kocur, J. (2009). 
Parental physical and psychological aggres- 
sion: Psychological symptoms in young 
adults. Child Abuse & Neglect, 33, 1-11. 





References R-35 


Millon, T., Davis, R., & Millon, C. (2000). 
Personality disorders in modern life. New York: 
Wiley. 

Milner, B. (1966). Amnesia following operation 
on temporal lobes. In C. W. M. Whitty & 

P. Zangwill (Eds.), Amnesia. London: 
Butterworth. 


Milner, B. (2005). The medial temporal-lobe 
amnesic syndrome. Psychiatric Clinics of North 
America, 28, 599-611. 


Milton, J., & Wiseman, R. (1999). Does psi exist? 
Lack of replication of an anomalous process 
of information transfer. Psychological Bulletin, 
125, 387-391. 


Miner, J., & Clarke-Stewart, K. (2008). Trajectories 
of externalizing behavior from age 2 to age 9: 
Relations with gender, temperament, ethnic- 
ity, parenting, and rater. Developmental 
Psychology, 44, 771-786. 


Miner-Rubino, K., & Cortina, L. M. (2007). 
Beyond targets: Consequences of vicarious 
exposure to misogyny at work. Journal of 
Applied Psychology, 92, 1254-1269. 


Miner-Rubino, K., Winter, D. G., & Stewart, A. J. 
(2004). Gender, social class, and the subjective 
experience of aging: Self-perceived personal- 
ity change from early adulthood to late 
midlife. Personality and Social Psychology 
Bulletin, 30, 1599-1610. 


Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory-2 
(1989). Regents of the University of 
Minnesota. 


Mintz, A., & Brule, D. (2009). Methodological 
issues in studying suicide terrorism. Political 
Psychology, 30, 361-367. 


Minuchin, S. (1999). Retelling, reimagining, and 
re-searching: A continuing conversation. 
Journal of Marital and Family Therapy, 25, 9-14. 


Miquel, J., (2006). Integración de teorías del 
envejecimiento (parte I). Integration of theo- 
ries of ageing. Revista Espanola de Geriatria y 
Gerontologia, 41, 55-63. 


Mischel, W. (2004). Toward an integrative science 
of the person. Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 
1-22. 


Mischel, W. (2009). From Personality and Assess- 
ment (1968) to Personality Science, 2009. 
Journal of Research in Personality, 43, 282-290. 


Mischel, W., & Shoda, Y. (2008). Toward a unified 
theory of personality: Integrating dispositions 
and processing dynamics within the cognitive- 
affective processing system. In O. P. Oliver, 

R. W. Robins, & L. A. Pervin (Eds.) Handbook 
of personality psychology: Theory and research (3rd 
ed). New York: Guilford Press. 


Mischoulon, D. (2000, June). Anti-depressants: 
Choices and controversy. HealthNews, p. 4. 


Miserando, M. (1991). Memory and the seven 
dwarfs. Teaching of Psychology, 18, 169-171. 


Mitchell, D. B., & Schmitt, F. A. (2006). Short- and 
long-term implicit memory in aging and 
Alzheimer’s disease. Neuropsychological 
Development and Cognition, B, Aging and 
Neuropsychological Cognition, 13, 611-635. 

Mitte, K. (2005). Meta-analysis of cognitive- 
behavioral treatments for generalized anxiety 


disorder: A comparison with pharmacotherapy. 
Psychological Bulletin, 131, 785-795. 


R-36 References 


MLA (2005). MLA Language Map; all languages 
other than English combined. Retrieved from 
http: / /www.mla.org/census_mapé&source= 
county (based on 2000 U.S. Census Bureau 
figures). 

Mobasher, B., & Tuzhilin, A. (2009). Preface to the 
special issue on data mining for personaliza- 
tion. User Modeling and User-Adapted Interaction, 
19, 1-3. 


Moffitt, T. E., & Caspi, A. (2007). Evidence from 
behavioral genetics for environmental 
contributions to antisocial conduct. In J. E. 
Grusec & P. D. Hastings, Handbook of socializa- 
tion: Theory and research. New York: Guilford 
Press. 


Moffitt, T. E., Caspi, A., & Rutter, M. (2006). 
Measured gene-environment interactions in 
psychopathology: Concepts, research strate- 
gies, and implications for research, interven- 
tion, and public understanding of genetics. 
Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 5-27. 


Mograss, M., Guillem, F., Brazzini-Poisson, V., & 
Godbout, R. (2009, May). The effects of total 
sleep deprivation on recognition memory 
processes: A study of event-related potential. 
Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 91, 
343-352. 


Mohapel, P., Leanza, G., Kokaia, M., & Lindvall, 
O. (2005). Forebrain acetylcholine regulates 
adult hippo-campal neurogenesis and 
learning. Neurobiology of Aging, 26, 939-946. 


Moher, C., Gould, D., Hegg, E., & Mahoney, A. 
(2008). Non-generalized and generalized 
conditioned reinforcers: Establishment and 
validation. Behavioral Interventions, 23, 13-38. 


Mokdad, A. H., Brewer, R. D., & Naimi, T. (2007). 
Binge drinking is a problem that cannot be 
ignored. Preventive Medicine: An International 
Journal Devoted to Practice and Theory, 44, 
303-304. 

Monnier, J., Resnick, H. S., Kilpatrick, D. G., & 
Seals, B. (2002). The relationship between 
distress and resource loss following rape. 
Violence and Victims, 17, 85-92. 


Montgomery, K. J., Isenberg, N., & Haxby, J.V. 
(2007). Communicative hand gestures and 
object-directed hand movements activated 
the mirror neuron system. Social, Cognitive and 
Affective Neuroscience, 2, 114-122. 


Montgomery, S. (2006). Serotonin noradrenaline 
reuptake inhibitors: Logical evolution of 
antidepressant development. International 
Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical Practice, 10, 5-11. 

Montgomery, S. A., Nil, R., Diirr-Pal, N., Loft, H., 
& Boulenger, J. P. (2005). A 24-week random- 
ized, double-blind, placebo-controlled study 
of escitalopram for the prevention of general- 
ized social anxiety disorder. Journal of Clinical 
Psychiatry, 66, 1270-1278. 

Montoya, R., & Insko, C. (2008). Toward a more 
complete understanding of the reciprocity 
of liking effect. European Journal of Social 
Psychology, 38, 477—498. 


Moody, H. R. (2000). Aging: Concepts and contro- 
versies. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 

Moore, D. G., Goodwin, J. E., & George, R. (2007). 
Infants perceive human point-light displays 
as solid forms. Cognition, 104, 377-396. 


Moore, M. M. (2002). Behavioral observation. In 
M. W. Wiederman & B. E. Whitley (Eds.), 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Handbook for conducting research on human sex- 
uality. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. 


Moorey, S. (2007). Cognitive therapy. In W. Dryden, 
Dryden's handbook of individual therapy (5th ed.). 
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. 


Morad, Y., Barkana, Y., Zadok, D., Hartstein, M., 
Pras, E., & Bar-Dayan, Y. (2009, July). Ocular 
parameters as an objective tool for the assess- 
ment of truck drivers fatigue. Accident Analysis 
and Prevention, 41, 856-860. 


Mora-Giral, M., Raich-Escursell, R. M., Segues, 
C.V., Torras-Claras, A. J., & Huon, G. (2004). 
Bulimia symptoms and risk factors in 
university students. Eating and Weight 
Disorders, 9, 163-169. 


Moran, A. (2009). Cognitive psychology in sport: 
Progress and prospects. Psychology of Sport 
and Exercise, 10, 420-426. 


Moretz, M., & McKay, D. (2009). The role of 
perfectionism in obsessive-compulsive 
symptoms: ‘Not just right’ experiences and 
checking compulsions. Journal of Anxiety 
Disorders, 23, 640-644. 


Morgan, A. A., Marsiske, M., & Whitfield, K. E. 
(2008). Characterizing and explaining 
differences in cognitive test performance 
between African American and European 
American older adults. Experimental Aging 
Research, 34, 80-100. 


Morone, N. E., & Greco, C. M. (2007). Mind-body 
interventions for chronic pain in older adults: 
A structured review. Pain Medicine, 8, 359-375. 


Morris, J. F, Waldo, C. R., & Rothblum, E. D. (2001). 
A model of predictors and outcomes of 
outness among lesbian and bisexual women. 
American Journal of Orthopsychiatry, 71, 61-71. 

Morrone, A. S., & Pintrich, P. R. (2006). Achieve- 
ment motivation. In G. G. Bear & K. M. 
Minke, Children’s needs III: Development, pre- 
vention, and intervention. Washington, DC: 
National Association of School Psychologists. 

Morrow, J., & Wolff, R. (1991, May). Wired for a 

miracle. Health, pp. 64-84. 

Mosher, C. J., & Akins, S. (2007). Drugs and drug 

policy: The control of consciousness alteration. 

Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. 

Moskowitz, G. B. (2004). Social cognition: Under- 

standing self and others. New York: Guilford 

Press. 

Motley, M. T. (1987, February). What I meant to 

say. Psychology Today, pp. 25-28. 





Muammar, O. M. (2007). An integration of two 
competing models to explain practical intelli- 
gence. Dissertation Abstracts International: 
Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 67(7-B), 
4128. 


Mueller, C. E. (2009). Protective factors as barriers 
to depression in gifted and nongifted adoles- 
cents. Gifted Child Quarterly, 53, 3-14. 


Mullen, B., & Rice, D. R. (2003). Ethnophaulisms 
and exclusion: The behavioral consequences 
of cognitive representation of ethnic immi- 
grant groups. Personality and Social Psychology 
Bulletin, 29, 1056-1067. 


Munakata, Y. (2006). Information processing 
approaches to development. In D. Kuhn, R. S. 
Siegler, W. Damon, & R. M. Lerner, Handbook 
of child psychology: Vol 2, Cognition, perception, 


and language (6th ed.) Hoboken, NJ: John 
Wiley & Sons. 


Munger, D. (2009, April 20). Super-recognizers: 
people with an amazing ability to recognize 
faces. Retrieved from http://scienceblogs 
.com/cognitivedaily /2009/04/super- 
recognizers_people_with.php 


Munroe, R L., Hulefeld, R., Rodgers, J. M., 
Tomeo, D. L., & Yamazaki, S. K. (2000). 
Aggression among children in four cultures. 
Cross-Cultural Research: The Journal of Compara- 
tive Social Science, 34, 3-25. 


Murphy, D., et al. (2008, November). How the 
serotonin story is being rewritten by new 
gene-based discoveries principally related to 
SLC6A4, the serotonin transporter gene, which 
functions to influence all cellular serotonin 
systems. Neuropharmacology, 55, 932-960. 


Murphy, G. J., Glickfield, L. L., Balsen, Z., & 
Isaacson, J. S. (2004). Sensory neuron signal- 
ing to the brain: Properties of transmitter 
release from olfactory nerve terminals. Journal 
of Neuroscience, 24, 3023-3030. 


Murphy, G. L. (2005). The study of concepts 
inside and outside the laboratory: Medin 
versus Medin. In W. Ahn, R. L. Goldstone, 

B. C. Love, A. B. Markman, & P. Wolff (Eds.), 
Categorization inside and outside the laboratory: 
Essays in honor of Douglas L. Medin. Washington, 
DC: American Psychological Association. 


Murphy, R. T., Wismar, K., & Freeman, K. (2003). 
Stress symptoms among African-American 
college students after the September 11, 2001 
terrorist attacks. Journal of Nervous and Mental 
Disease, 191, 108-114. 


Murphy, S., et al. (1998). Interference under the 
influence. Personality and Social Psychology 
Bulletin, 24, 517-528. 


Murphy, S. T., & Zajonc, R. B. (1993). Affect, cog- 
nition, and awareness: Affective priming with 
optimal and suboptimal stimulus exposures. 
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 64, 
723-739. 


Murray, B. (June 2002). Good news for bachelor’s 
grads. Monitor on Psychology, pp. 30-32. 

Murray, R., Lappin, J., & Di Forti, M. (2008, 
August). Schizophrenia: From developmental 
deviance to dopamine dysregulation. European 
Neuropsychopharmacology, 18, 5129-85134. 


Murray, S. L., Holmes, J. G., & Griffin, D. W. (2004). 
The benefits of positive illusions: Idealization 
and the construction of satisfaction in close 
relationships. In H. T. Reis & C. E. Rusbult 
(Eds.), Close relationships: Key readings. 
Philadelphia, PA: Taylor & Francis. 


Murthy, P., Kudlur, S., George, S., & Mathew, G. 
(2009). A clinical overview of fetal alcohol 
syndrome. Addictive Disorders & Their 
Treatment, 8, 1-12. 


Myers, D. G. (2000). The funds, friends, and faith 
of happy people. American Psychologist, 55, 
56-67. 


Myers, L. L. (2007). Anorexia nervosa, bulimia 
nervosa, and binge eating disorder. In B. A. 
Thyer, & J. S. Wodarski. Social work in mental 
health: An evidence-based approach. Hoboken, 
NJ: John Wiley & Sons. 


Myerson, J., Adams, D. R., Hale, S., & Jenkins, L. 
(2003). Analysis of group differences in pro- 
cessing speed: Brinley plots, Q-Q plots, and 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


other conspiracies. Psychonomic Bulletin and 
Review, 10, 224-237. 


Myrtek, M. (2007). Type a behavior and hostility 
as independent risk factors for coronary heart 
disease. In J. Jordan, B. Barde & A. M. Zeiher, 
Contributions toward evidence-based psychocardi- 
ology: A systematic review of the literature. 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


Mytinger, C. (2001). Headhunting in the Solomon 
Islands: Around the Coral Sea. Santa Barbara, 
CA: Narrative Press. 


Nadeem, E., & Graham, S. (2005). Early puberty, 
peer victimization, and internalizing symp- 
toms in ethnic minority adolescents. Journal of 
Early Adolescence, 25, 197-222. 


Nagai, Y., Goldstein, L. H., Fenwick, P. B. C., & 
Trimble, M. R. (2004). Clinical efficacy of gal- 
vanic skin response biofeedback training in 
reducing seizures in adult epilepsy: A prelim- 
inary randomized controlled study. Epilepsy 
and Behavior, 5, 216-223. 


Nagda, B. A., Tropp, L. R., & Paluck, E. L. (2006). 
Looking back as we look ahead: Integrating 
research, theory, and practice on intergroup 
relations. Journal of Social Research, 62, 439-451. 


Naik, G. (2004, December 29). New obesity boom 
in Arab countries has old ancestry. The Wall 
Street Journal, p. A1. 


Naik, G. (2009, February 3). Parents agonize over 
treatment in the womb. Wall Street Journal, 
p. D1. 


Najman, J. M., Aird, R., Bor, W., O’Callaghan, 
M., Williams, G. M., & Shuttlewood, G. J. 
(2004). The generational transmission of 
socioeconomic inequalities in child cognitive 
development and emotional health. Social 
Science and Medicine, 58, 1147-1158. 


Nakamura, Y. (2004). Isolation of p53-target genes 
and their functional analysis. Cancer Science, 
95, 7-11. 


Naldini, L. (2009, November 6). A comeback for 
gene therapy. Science, 326, 805-806. 


Narrow, W. E., Rae, D. S., Robins, L. N., & Regier, 
D. A. (2002). Revised prevalence estimates of 
mental disorders in the United States: Using a 
clinical significance criterion to reconcile 2 
surveys’ estimates. Archives of General 
Psychiatry, 59, 115-123. 


Nasir, N. S., & Hand, V. (2006). From the court to 
the classroom: Opportunities for engagement, 
learning, and identity in basketball and class- 
room mathematics. Journal of the Learning 
Sciences, 17, 143-179. 


Nasrallah, H., Black, D., Goldberg, J., Muzina, D., 
& Pariser, S. (2008). Issues associated with the 
use of atypical antipsychotic medications. 
Annals of Clinical Psychiatry, 20, 824-829. 


Nathan, P. E., Stuart, S. P., & Dolan, S. L. (2000). 
Research on psychotherapy efficacy and 
effectiveness: Between Scylla and Charybdis? 
Psychological Bulletin, 126, 964-981. 


National Adolescent Health Information Center. 
(2003). Fact Sheet on Demographics: Adolescents. 
San Francisco: University of California, San 
Francisco. 


National Association for the Education of Young 
Children. (2005). Position statements of the 


NAEYC. Retrieved from http://www.naeyc 
.org/about/ positions.asp#where. 


National Center for Health Statistics. (2000). 
Health United States, 2000 with adolescent health 
chartbook. National Center for Health Statistics, 
Hyattsville, MD. 


National Depression Screening Day. (2003, March 
26). Questionnaire on website. Retrieved from 
http://www. mentalhealthscreening.org/ 
dep/ depsample.htm#sampletest 


National Institute of Child Health and Human 
Development (NICHD) Early Child Care 
Research Network. (1999). Child care and 
mother-child interaction in the first 3 years of 
life. Psychology, 35, 1399-1413. 


National Institute of Child Health and Human 
Development (NICHD) Early Child Care 
Research Network. (2000). The relation of 
child care to cognitive and language develop- 
ment. Child Development, 71, 960-980. 


National Institute of Child Health and Human 
Development (NICHD) Early Child Care 
Research Network. (2001). Child-care and 
family predictors of preschool attachment 
and stability from infancy. Development 
Psychology, 37, 847-862. 


National Institute of Child Health and Human 
Development (NICHD) Early Child Care 
Research Network. (2002). Child-care 
structure—process— outcome: Direct and 
indirect effects of child-care quality on young 
children’s development. Psychological Science, 
13, 199-206. 


National Institute of Child Health and Human 
Development (NICHD) Early Child Care 
Research Network. (2006). Child-care effect 
sizes for the NICHD study of early child care 
and youth development. American Psychologist, 
61, 99-116. 


National Research Council (2001). Eager to learn: 
Educating our preschoolers. Washington, DC: 
National Academy Press. 


National Research Council and Institute of 
Medicine of the National Academies. (2009). 
Preventing mental, emotional, and behavioral dis- 
orders among young people: Progress and possibil- 
ities. Washington, DC: National Academies 
Press. 


Natvig, G. K., Albrektsen, G., & Ovarnstrom, U. 
(2003). Methods of teaching and class partici- 
pation in relation to perceived social support 
and stress: Modifiable factors for improving 
health and well-being among students. 
Educational Psychology, 23, 261-274. 


Naveh-Benjamin, M., Craik, F. I. M., Gavrilescu, D., 
& Anderson, N. D. (2000). Asymmetry between 
encoding and retrieval processes: Evidence 
from divided attention and a calibration 
analysis. Memory & Cognition, 28, 965-967. 


Naveh-Benjamin, M., Guez, J., & Sorek, S. (2007). 
The effects of divided attention on encoding 
processes in memory: Mapping the locus of 
interference. Canadian Journal of Experimental 
Psychology, 61, 1-12. 


Neher, A. (2006). Evolutionary psychology: Its 
programs, prospects, and pitfalls. American 
Journal of Psychology, 119, 517-566. 


Neitz, J., Neitz, M., & Kainz, P. M. (1996, 
November 1). Visual pigment gene structure 


References R-37 


and the severity of color vision defects. 
Science, 274, 801-804. 


Nelson, P. D. (2007). The globalization of psychol- 
ogy: What does it mean? The Educator, 5, 1-4. 


Nelson, W. M., II, & Finch, A. J., Jr. (2000). 
Managing anger in youth: A cognitive- 
behavioral intervention approach. In P. C. 
Kendall, Child & adolescent therapy: Cognitive- 
behavioral procedures (2nd ed.). New York: 
Guilford Press. 


Neron, S., & Stephenson, R. (2007). Effectiveness 
of hypnotherapy with cancer patients’ trajec- 
tory: Emesis, acute pain, and analgesia and 
anxiolysis in procedures. International Journal 
of Clinical Experimental Hypnosis, 55, 336-354. 


Nesdale, D., Maass, A., & Durkin, K. (2005). 
Group norms, threat, and children’s racial 
prejudice. Child Development, 76, 652-663. 


Nesheim, S., Henderson, S., Lindsay, M., Zuberi, 
J., Grimes, V., Buehler, J., et al. (2004). Prenatal 
HIV testing and antiretroviral prophylaxis at an 
urban hospital—Atlanta, Georgia, 1997-2000. 
Atlanta, GA: Centers for Disease Control. 


Nesse, R. M. (2000). Is depression an adaptation? 
Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 14-20. 


Nestler, E. J., & Malenka, R. C. (2004, March). 
The addicted brain. Scientific American, 
pp. 78-83. 


Nestoriuc, Y., & Martin, A. (2007, March). Efficacy 
of biofeedback for migraine: A meta-analysis. 
Pain, 128, 111-127. 


Nestoriuc, Y., Martin, A., Rief, W., & Andrasik, F. 
(2008, September). Biofeedback treatment for 
headache disorders: A comprehensive efficacy 
review. Applied Psychophysiology and Biofeedback, 
33, 125-140. 


Neubauer, A. C., & Fink, A. (2005). Basic informa- 
tion processing and the psychophysiology of 
intelligence. In R. J. Sternberg & J. E. Pretz, 
Cognition and intelligence: Identifying the mecha- 
nisms of the mind. New York: Cambridge 
University Press, 2005. 


Neumann, N., & Birbaumer, N. (2004, December). 
Thinking out loud. Scientific American: Mind, 
pp. 37—45. 


Neumark-Sztainer, D. (2009, March). Preventing 
obesity and eating disorders in adolescents: 
What can health care providers do? Journal of 
Adolescent Health, 44, 206-213. 


Newby-Clark, I. R., & Ross, M. (2003). 
Conceiving the past and future. Personality 
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 29, 807-818. 


Newell, A., & Simon, H. (1972). Human problem 
solving. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. 


Newman, C. F., Leahy, R. L., Beck, A. T., Reilly- 
Harrington, N. A., & Gyulai, L. (2002). 
Bipolar disorder: A cognitive therapy approach. 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


Newman, M., & Bakay, R. (2008, April). Thera- 
peutic potentials of human embryonic stem 
cells in Parkinson’s disease. Neurothera- 
peutics, 5, 237-251. 


Newport, F., & Carroll, J. (2002, November 27). 
Battle of the bulge: Majority of Americans 
want to lose weight. Gallup News Service, 


pp. 1-9. 











R-38 References 


The New York Times. (1993, November 15). No. 2 
pencil fades as graduate exam moves to 
computer. The New York Times, p. B9. 


Niccols, A. (2007). Fetal alcohol syndrome and 
the developing socio-emotional brain. Brain 
Cognition, 65, 135-142. 


Nicholson, I. A. M. (2003). Inventing personality: 
Gordon Allport and the science of selfhood. 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


Nickerson, R. S., & Adams, M. J. (1979). Cognitive 
Psychology, 11, 297. 


Niedenthal, P. M. (2007, May 18). Embodying 
emotion. Science, 316, 1002-1005. 


Nielsen, C., Staud, R., & Price, D. (2009, March). 
Individual differences in pain sensitivity: 
Measurement, causation, and consequences. 
The Journal of Pain, 10, 231-237. 


Nielsen, C., Stubhaug, A., Price, D., Vassend, O., 
Czajkowski, N., & Harris, J. (2008, May). 
Individual differences in pain sensitivity: 
Genetic and environmental contributions. 
Pain, 136, 21-29. 


Nielson, T. A., Stenstrom, P., & Levin, R. (2006). 
Nightmare frequency as a function of age, 
gender, and September 11, 2001: Findings 
from an internet questionnaire. Dreaming, 16, 
145-158. 


Nigg, J. T., & Goldsmith, H. H. (1994). Genetics of 
personality disorders: Perspectives from 
personality and psychopathology research. 
Psychological Bulletin, 115, 346-380. 


Nilsson, H., Juslin, P., & Olsson, H. (2008). 
Exemplars in the mist: The cognitive sub- 
strate of the representativeness heuristic. 
Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 49, 201-212. 


Nimrod, G., & Kleiber, D. A. (2007). Reconsi- 
dering change and continuity in later life: 
Toward an innovation theory of successful 
aging. International Journal of Human 
Development, 65, 1-22. 


Nisbett, R. (2003). The geography of thought. New 
York: Free Press. 


Nisbett, R. E. (2007, December 9). All brains are 
the same color. The New York Times, p. E11. 


Nisbett, R.E. (2009, February). All brains are the 
same color. Association for Psychological Science 
Observer, 22(3), 20-21. 


Nishida, M., Pearsall, J., Buckner, R., & Walker, 
M. (2009, May). REM sleep, prefrontal theta, 
and the consolidation of human emotional 
memory. Cerebral Cortex, 19, 1158-1166. 


Nishino, S. (2007, June). Clinical and neurobio- 
logical aspects of narcolepsy. Sleep Medicine, 8, 
373-399. 


Nissle, S., & Bschor, T. (2002). Winning the jackpot 
and depression: Money cannot buy happiness. 
International Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical 
Practice, 6, 183-186. 


Nittrouer, S., & Lowenstein, J. H. (2007). Children’s 
weighting strategies for word-final stop voic- 
ing are not explained by auditory sensitivi- 
ties. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing 
Research, 50, 58-73. 


Noble, H. B. (1999, March 12). New from the 
smoking wars: Success. The New York Times, 
pp. D1-D2. 








www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (2007). Abnormal psychology 
(4th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. 


Norcia, A. M., Pei, F., Bonneh, Y., Hou, C., 
Sampath, V., & Petter, M. W. (2005). 
Development of sensitivity to texture and 
contour information in the human infant. 
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17, 569-579. 


Norcross, J. C. (2002). Empirically supported 
therapy relationships. In J. C. Norcross, 
Psychotherapy relationships that work: Therapist 
contributions and responsiveness to patients. 
New York: Oxford University Press. 


Norcross, J. C., Beutler, L. E., & Levant, R. F. 
(2006). Evidence-based practices in mental health: 
Debate and dialogue on the fundamental ques- 
tions. Washington, DC: American Psycholog- 
ical Association. 


Norlander, T., Von Schedvin, H., & Archer, T. (2005). 
Thriving as a function of affective personality: 
Relation to personality factors, coping 
strategies and stress. Anxiety, Stress & Coping: 
An International Journal, 18, 105-116. 


Norton, P. J., & Price, E. C. (2007). A meta-analytic 
review of adult cognitive-behavioral treat- 
ment outcome across the anxiety disorders. 
Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 195, 
521-531. 


Noy, V. M. (2006). A psychoneuroimmunology 
program for Hispanic women with stage IHI 
breast cancer. Dissertation Abstracts International: 
Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 66(11-B), 
6287. 


Noyes, R., Jr., Stuart, S. P., Langbehn, D. R., Happel, 
R. L., Longley, S. L., Muller, B. A., et al. (2003). 
Test of an interpersonal model of hypochon- 
driasis. Psychosomatic Medicine, 65, 292-300. 


Ntinas, K. M. (2007). Behavior modification and 
the principle of normalization: Clash or syn- 
thesis? Behavioral Interventions, 22, 165-177. 


Nucci, L. P. (2002). The development of moral 
reasoning. In U. Goswami (Ed.), Blackwell 
handbook of childhood cognitive development. 
Blackwell Handbooks of developmental 
psychology. Malden, MA: Blackwell. 


Nunes, A., & Kramer, A. F. (2009). Experience- 
based mitigation of age-related performance 
declines: Evidence from air traffic control. 
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15, 
12-24. 


Nurnberger, J. I., Jr., & Bierut, L. J. (2007, April). 
Seeking the connections: Alcoholism and our 
genes. Scientific American, pp. 46-53. 


Nussbaum, A.D. & Steele, C. M. (2007). Situational 
disengagement and persistence in the face 
of adversity. Journal of Experimental Social 
Psychology, 43, 127-134. 


Nyberg, L., & Tulving, E. (1996). Classifying 
human long-term memory: Evidence from 
converging dissociations. European Journal of 
Cognitive Psychology, 8, 163-183. 

Oatley, K., Keltner, D., & Jenkins, J. M. (2006). 
Understanding emotions. Oxford, England: 
Blackwell. 

Oberauer, K. (2007). In search of the magic num- 
ber. Experimental Psychology, 54, 245-246. 


O’Brien, K. M., & LeBow, M. D. (2007). Reducing 
maladaptive weight management practices: 


Developing a psychoeducational intervention 
program. Eating Behaviors, 8, 195-210. 


Occhionero, M. (2004). Mental processes and the 
brain during dreams. Dreaming, 14, 54-64. 


O’Connor, D. B., & O’Connor, R. C. (2004). 
Perceived changes in food intake in response 
to stress: The role of conscientiousness. Stress 
and Health: Journal of the International Society 
for the Investigation of Stress, 20, 279-291. 


Odeku, K., Rembe, S., & Anwo, J. (2009). Female 
genital mutilation: A human rights perspec- 
tive. Journal of Psychology in Africa, 19, 55-62. 


O'Donohue, W. (Ed.). (1997). Sexual harassment: 
Theory, research, and treatment. Boston: Allyn & 
Bacon. 


Oehman, A., & Mineka, S. (2003). The malicious 
serpent: Snakes as a prototypical stimulus for 
an evolved module of fear. Current Directions 
in Psychological Science, 12, 5-9. 


Offer, D., Kaiz, M., Howard, K. I., & Bennett, E. S. 
(2000). The altering of reported experiences. 
Journal of the American Academy of Child & 
Adolescent Psychiatry, 39, 735-742. 


Ogbu, J. (1992). Understanding cultural diver- 
sity and learning. Educational Researcher, 21, 
5-14. 


Ogletree, S. M., Martinez, C. N., & Turner, T. R. 
(2004). Pokémon: Exploring the role of 
gender. Sex Roles, 50, 851-859. 


O'Grady, W. D., & Dobrovolsky, M. (Eds.). (1996). 
Contemporary linguistic analysis: An introduc- 
tion (3rd ed.). Toronto: Copp Clark Pitman. 


Ogren, K., & Sandlund, M. (2007). Lobotomy at a 
state mental hospital in Sweden. A survey of 
patients operated on during the period 1947— 
1958. Nordic Journal of Psychiatry, 61, 355-362. 


Ohara, K. (2007). The n-3 polyunsaturated fatty 
acid / dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia. 
Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & 
Biological Psychiatry, 31, 469-474. 


Ohira, T., Hozawa, A., Iribarren, C., Daviglus, 
M. L., Matthews, K. A., Gross, M. D., et al. 
(2007). Longitudinal association of serum 
carotenoids and tocopherols with hostility: 
The CARDIA study. American Journal of Epide- 
miology, 18, 235-241. 


Ojha, H., & Pramanick, M. (2009). Effects of age 
on intensity and priority of life needs. Journal 
of the Indian Academy of Applied Psychology, 35, 
131-136. 


O'Keefe, T., & Fox, K. (Eds.). (2003). Finding the real 
me: True tales of sex and gender diversity. San 
Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 


Olatunji, B. (2008). New directions in research on 
health anxiety and hypochondriasis: Com- 
mentary on a timely special series. Journal of 
Cognitive Psychotherapy, 22, 183-190. 


Olds, M. E., & Fobes, J. L. (1981). The central 
basis of motivation: Intracranial self- 
stimulation studies. Annual Review of 
Psychology, 32, 123-129. 

O’Leary-Kelly, A., Bowes-Sperry, L., Bates, C., & 
Lean, E. (2009). Sexual harassment at work: A 
decade (plus) of progress. Journal of Manage- 
ment, 35, 503-536. 


Olfson, M., & Marcus, S. (2008). A case-control 
study of antidepressants and attempted sui- 
cide during early phase treatment of major 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


depressive episodes. Journal of Clinical 
Psychiatry, 69, 425-432. 

Olivardia, R., & Pope, H. (2002). Body image dis- 
turbance in childhood and adolescence. In 
D. Castle & K. Phillips (Eds.), Disorders of body 
image. Petersfield, England: Wrightson 
Biomedical Publishing. 


Oliver, M. B., & Hyde, J. S. (1993). Gender 
differences in sexuality: A meta-analysis. 
Psychological Bulletin, 114, 29-51. 


Olson, D. H., & DeFrain, J. (2005). Marriages and 
families: Intimacy, diversity, and strengths with 
PowerWeb. New York: McGraw-Hill. 


Oosterhof, N. N., & Todorov, A. (2008). The 
functional basis of face evaluation. Proceedings 
of the National Academy of Sciences of the United 
States of America, 105, 11087-11092. 

Ophir, E., Nass, C., & Wagner, A. D. (2009). 
Cognitive control in media multitaskers. 
PNAS Proceedings of the National Academy of 
Sciences of the United States of America, 106, 
15583-15587. 

Opler, M., Perrin, M., Kleinhaus, K., & Malaspina, 
D. (2008). Factors in the etiology of schizo- 
phrenia: Genes, parental age, and environ- 
ment. Primary Psychiatry, 15, 37-45. 


Oppenheimer, D. M. (2004). Spontaneous 
discounting of availability in frequency 
judgment tasks. Psychological Science, 15, 
100-105. 


OpportunityNYC.org. (2009). About Opportunity 
NYC family rewards. Retrieved from http:// 
opportunitynyc.org/about 


Ornat, S. L., & Gallo, P. (2004). Acquisition, 
learning, or development of language? 
Skinner’s “Verbal behavior” revisited. Spanish 
Journal of Psychology, 7, 161-170. 


Orwin, R. G., & Condray, D. S. (1984). Smith and 
Glass’ psychotherapy conclusions need 
further probing: On Landman and Dawes’ 
re-analysis. American Psychologist, 39, 71-72. 


Oskamp, S. (Ed.). (2000). Reducing prejudice and 
discrimination. Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 


Otake, K., Shimai, S., & Tanaka-Matsumi, J. (2006). 
Happy people become happier through kind- 
ness: A counting kindnesses intervention. 
Journal of Happiness Studies, 7, 361-375. 


Ouimet, A., Gawronski, B., & Dozois, D. (2009). 
Cognitive vulnerability to anxiety: A review 
and an integrative model. Clinical Psychology 
Review, 29, 459-470. 


Oveis, C., Gruber, J., Keltner, D., Stamper, J., & 
Boyce, W. (2009). Smile intensity and warm 
touch as thin slices of child and family affec- 
tive style. Emotion, 9, 544-548. 

Oviedo-Joekes, E., et al. (2009). Diacetylmorphine 
versus methadone for the treatment of opioid 
addiction. The New England Journal of 
Medicine, 361, 777-786. 

Owens, D. (2009, May 12). Park Street hit-and- 
run victim Angel Arce Torres dies. The 
Hartford Courant. 


Paabo, S. (2001, February 16). The human 
genome and our view of ourselves. Science, 
291, 1219-1220. 


Packer, D. (2009). Avoiding groupthink: Whereas 
weakly identified members remain silent, 
strongly identified members dissent about 


collective problems. Psychological Science, 20, 
546-548. 


Pager, D., & Shepherd, H. (2008). The sociology 
of discrimination: Racial discrimination in 
employment, housing, credit, and consumer 
markets. Annual Review of Sociology, 34, 
181-209. 


Pagonis, T. A., Angelopoulos, N., & Koukoulis, 
G. N. (2006). Psychiatric side effects induced 
by supraphysiological doses of combinations 
of anabolic steroids correlate to the severity 
of abuse. European Psychiatry, 21, 551-562. 


Pagoto, S. L., Kozak, A. T., & Spates, C. R. (2006). 
Systematic desensitization for an older 
woman with a severe specific phobia: An 
application of evidenced-based practice. 
Clinical Gerontologist, 30, 89-98. 


Pallanti, S., & Bernardi, S. (2009, July). Neuro- 
biology of repeated transcranial magnetic 
stimulation in the treatment of anxiety: A 
critical review. International Clinical Psycho- 
pharmacology, 24, 163-173. 


Pandya, M., Pozuelo, L., & Malone, D. (2007). 
Electroconvulsive therapy: What the internist 
needs to know. Cleveland Clinic Journal of 
Medicine, 74, 679-685. 


Paniagua, F. A. (2000). Diagnosis in a multicultural 
context: A casebook for mental health profession- 
als. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 


Paquier, P. F., & Mariën, P. (2005). A synthesis of 
the role of the cerebellum in cognition. 
Aphasiology, 19, 3-19. 


Parish, C. L., & Arenas, E. (2007). Stem-cell-based 
strategies for the treatment of Parkinson’s 
disease. Neurodegenerative Disease, 4, 

339-347. 


Park, A. (2008, December 22). The happiness 
effect: How emotions and even behaviors can 
spread like an epidemic. Time, p. 40. 


Park, D. C. (2007). Eating disorders: A call to 
arms. American Psychologist, 62, 158. 


Park, H., & Antonioni, D. (2007). Personality, 
reciprocity, and strength of conflict resolution 
strategy. Journal of Research in Personality, 41, 
110-125. 


Parke, B. N. (2003). Discovering programs for talent 
development. Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin 
Press. 


Parke, R. D. (2004). Development in the family. 
Annual Review of Psychology, 55, 365-399. 


Parker, E. S., Cahill, L., & McGaugh, J. L. (2006). 
A case of unusual autobiographical remem- 
bering. Neurocase, 12, 35-49. 


Parker-Pope, T. (2003, April 22). The diet that 
works. The Wall Street Journal, pp. R1, R5. 


Parmley, M. C. (2007). The effects of the 
confirmation bias on diagnostic decision 
making. Dissertation Abstracts International: 
Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 67(8-B), 
4719. 


Parra, A., & Argibay, J. C. (2007). Comparing psy- 
chics and non-psychics through a ‘token- 
object’ forced-choice ESP test. Journal of the 
Society for Psychical Research, 71, 80-90. 

Pascual, A., & Guéguen, N. (2005). Foot-in-the-door 
and door-in-the-face: A comparative meta- 


analytic study. Psychological Reports, 96, 
122-128. 


References R-39 


Pascual, A., & Guéguen, N. (2006). Door-in-the-face 
technique and monetary solicitation: An 
evaluation in a field setting. Perceptual and 
Motor Skills, 103, 974-978. 


Pascual-Leone, A., Nguyet, D., Coehn, L. G., 
Brasil-Neto, J. P., Cammarota, A., & Hallett, 
M. (1995). Modulation of muscle responses 
evoked by transcranial magnetic stimulation 
during the acquisition of new fine motor 
skills. Journal of Neurophysiology, 74, 1037-1045. 


Pascual, M. A., & Rodriguez, M. A. (2006). 
Learning by operant conditioning as a 
nonlinear self-organized process. Nonlinear 
Dynamics, Psychology, and Life Sciences, 10, 
341-364. 


Passos, V. L., Berger, M. P. F., & Tan, F. E. (2007). 
Test design optimization in CAT early stage 
with the nominal response model. Applied 
Psychological Measurement, 31, 213-232. 


Pastalkova, E., Serrano, P., Pinkhasova, D., 
Wallace, E., Fenton, A., & Sacktor, T. (2006). 
Storage of Spatial Information by the 
Maintenance Mechanism of LTP. Science, 313, 
1141-1144. 


Pasterski, V., Geffner, M., Brain, C., Hindmarsh, 
P., Brook, C., & Hines, M. (2005). Prenatal hor- 
mones and postnatal socialization by parents 
as determinants of male-typical toy play in 
girls with congenital adrenal hyperplasia. 
Child Development, 76, 264-278. 


Pauk, W., & Owens, R. (2007). How to study in col- 
lege. Florence, KY: Cengage Press. 


Paukert, A., Stagner, B., & Hope, K. (2004). The 
assessment of active listening skills in 
helpline volunteers. Stress, Trauma, and Crisis: 
An International Journal, 7, 61-76. 


Paul, A. M. (2004). Cult of personality: How 
personality tests are leading us to miseducate our 
children, mismanage our companies and misun- 
derstand ourselves. New York: Free Press. 


Paulmann, S., Jessen, S., & Kotz, S. A. (2009). 
Investigating the multimodal nature of 
human communication: Insights from ERPs. 
Journal of Psychophysiology, 23, 63-76. 


Paulozzi, L. J. (2006). Opioid analgesic involve- 
ment in drug abuse deaths in American 
metropolitan areas. American Journal of Public 
Health, 96, 1755-1757. 


Pavitt, C. (2007). Impression formation. In B. B. 
Whaley & W. Samter, Explaining communica- 
tion: Contemporary theories and exemplars. 
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 


Payne, D. G. (1986). Hyperamnesia for pictures 
and words: Testing the recall level hypothesis. 
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, 
Memory, and Cognition, 12, 16-29. 


Payne, K., & Marcus, D. (2008). The efficacy of 
group psychotherapy for older adult clients: 
A meta-analysis. Group Dynamics: Theory, 
Research, and Practice, 12, 268-278. 


Pearce, J. M. S. (2007). Synaesthesia. European 
Neurology, 57, 120-124. 


Pearce, R. R., & Lin, Z. (2007). Chinese American 
post-secondary achievement and attainment: 
A cultural and structural analysis. Educational 
Review, 59, 19-36. 

Pearlstein, T., & Steiner, M. (2008). Premenstrual 
dysphoric disorder: Burden of illness and 


R-40 References 


treatment update. Journal of Psychiatry & 
Neuroscience, 33, 291-301. 


Pearson, A. R., Dovidio, J. F., & Pratto, F. (2007). 
Racial prejudice, intergroup hate, and blatant 
and subtle bias of whites toward blacks in 
legal decision making in the United States. 
International Journal of Psychology & Psychologi- 
cal Therapy, 7, 125-134. 


Pearson, J., & Clifford, C. W. G. (2005). When 
your brain decides what you see: Grouping 
across monocular, binocular, and stimulus 
rivalry. Psychological Science, 16, 516-519. 


Pedersen, P. B., Draguns, J. G., Lonner, W. J., & 
Trimble, J. E. (Eds.). (2002). Counseling across 
cultures (5th ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 


Pedraza, O., & Mungas, D. (2008). Measurement 
in cross-cultural neuropsychology. 
Neuropsychology Review, 18, 184-193. 


Peiro, J. M., & Lunt, I. (2002). The context for a 
European framework for psychologists’ 
training. European Psychologist, 7, 169-179. 


Pell, M. D., Monetta, L., Paulmann, S., & Kotz, 
S. A. (2009). Recognizing emotions in a for- 
eign language. Journal of Nonverbal Behavior, 
33, 107-120. 


Pellegrini, S., Muzio, R. N., Mustaca, A. E., & 
Papini, M. R. (2004).Successive negative 
contrast after partial reinforcement in the 
consummatory behavior of rats. Learning and 
Motivation, 35, 303-321. 


Pelli, D. G., Burns, C. W., & Farell, B. (2006). 
Feature detection and letter identification. 
Vision Research, 46, 4646-4674. 


Pellis, S. M., & Pellis, V. C. (2007). Rough-and- 
tumble play and the development of the 
social brain. Current Directions in Psychological 
Science, 16, 95-97. 


Pempek, T., Yermolayeva, Y., & Calvert, S. (2009). 
College students’ social networking experi- 
ences on Facebook. Journal of Applied 
Developmental Psychology, 30, 227-238. 


Penley, J. A., Tomaka, J., & Wiebe, J. S. (2002). The 
association of coping to physical and psycho- 
logical health outcomes: A meta-analytic 
review. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 25, 
551-603. 


Penn, D. L., Corrigan, P. W., Bentall, R. P., 
Racenstein, J. M., & Newman, L. (1997). Social 
cognition in schizophrenia. Psychological Bulle- 
tin, 121, 114-132. 


Penney, J. B., Jr. (2000). Neurochemistry. In B. S. 
Fogel, et al. (Eds.), Synopsis of neuropsychiatry. 
New York: Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. 


Penza, K. M., Heim, C., & Nemeroff, C. B (2006). 
Trauma and depression. In C. L. Keyes & S. H. 
Goodman, Women and depression: A handbook 
for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. 
New York: Cambridge University Press. 


Penzel, F. (2000). Obsessive-compulsive disorders: A 
complete guide to getting well and staying well. 
New York: Oxford University Press. 


People Weekly. (2003, June 23). Kid Doc: He’s no 
brain surgeon—yet. But Sho Yano, 12, is about 
to start med school. People Weekly, 125. 


Perez, R. M., DeBord, K. A., & Bieschke, K. J. (Eds). 
(2000). Handbook of counseling and psychother- 
apy with lesbian, gay, and bisexual clients. 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Perloff, R. M. (2003). The dynamics of persuasion: 
Communication and attitudes in the 21st century 
(2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 


Perry, B. (2008, May 19) ‘I don’t know how to 
forget.’ People, p. 143. 


Pert, C. B. (2002). The wisdom of the receptors: 
Neuropeptides, the emotions, and body- 
mind. Advances in Mind-Body Medicine, 18, 
30-35. 


Pervin, L. A. (1990). Handbook of personality: Theory 
and research. New York: Guilford Press. 


Pervin, L. A. (2003). The science of personality (2nd 
ed.). London: Oxford University Press. 


Pesmen, C. (2006). Health and wealth techniques 
to help keep chronic pain from taking over. 
Money Builder, 35, 48. 


Peterfi, Z., McGinty, D., Sarai, E., & Szymusiak, 
R. (2010). Growth hormone-releasing hor- 
mone activates sleep regulatory neurons of 
the rat preoptic hypothalamus. American 
Journal of Physiology: Regulatory, Integrative and 
Comparative Physiology, 298, R147-R156. 


Peters, E., Hess, T. M., Vastfjall, D., & Auman, C. 
(2007). Adult age differences in dual informa- 
tion processes. Perspectives on Psychological 
Science, 2, 1-23. 


Peters, J., Suchan, B., Koster, O., & Daum, I. (2007). 
Domain-specific retrieval of source informa- 
tion in the medial temporal lobe. European 
Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 1333-1343. 


Peterson, C. (2000). The future of optimism. 
American Psychologist, 55, 44-55. 


Peterson, F. L., Jr., & Fuerst, D. E. (2007). Assess- 
ment and treatment of erection dysfunction. 
In L. VandeCreek, F. L. Peterson, Jr., & J. W. 
Bley, Innovations in clinical practice: Focus on 
sexual health. Sarasota, FL: Professional 
Resource Press /Professional Resource 
Exchange. 


Peterson, L. R., & Peterson, M. J. (1959). Short- 
term retention of individual items. Journal of 
Experimental Psychology, 58, 193-198. 


Petersson, K. M., Silva, C., Castro-Caldas, A., 
Ingvar, M., & Reis, A. (2007). Literacy: A 
cultural influence on functional left-right 
differences in the inferior parietal cortex. 
European Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 791-799. 


Petrill, S. A. (2005). Introduction to this special 
issue: Genes, environment, and the develop- 
ment of reading skills. Scientific Studies of 
Reading, 9, 189-196. 


Petrill, S. A., & Deater-Deckard, K. (2004). The 
heritability of general cognitive ability: A 
within-family adoption design. Intelligence, 
32, 403-409. 


Pettigrew, T. F. (2004). Justice deferred: A half 
century after Brown v. Board of Education. 
American Psychologist, 59, 521-529. 


Pettigrew, T. F., & Tropp, L. R. (2006). A meta- 
analytic test of intergroup contact theory. 
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 90, 
751-783. 


Pettito, L. A. (1993). On the ontogenetic require- 
ments for early language acquisition. In 
B. de Boysson-Bardies, S. de Schonen, P. W. 
Jusczyk, P. McNeilage, & J. Morton (Eds.), 
Developmental neurocognition: Speech and face 
processing in the first year of life. NATO ASI 


series D: Behavioural and social sciences (Vol. 69). 
Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic. 


Petty, R. E., Cacioppo, J. T., Strathman, A. J., & 
Priester, J. R. (2005). To think or not to think: 
Exploring two routes to persuasion. In T. C. 
Brock & M. C. Green (Eds.), Persuasion: 
Psychological insights and perspectives (2nd ed.). 
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. 


Pfeffer, C. R. (2006). An evolutionary perspective 
on childhood depression. In P. S. Jensen, 
P. Knapp, & D. A. Mrazek, Toward a new diag- 
nostic system for child psychopathology: Moving 
beyond the DSM. New York: Guilford Press. 


Phelps, R. P. (2005). Defending standardized testing. 
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 


Philip, P., Sagaspe, P., Moore, N., Taillard, J., 
Charles, A., Guilleminault, C., et al. (2005). 
Fatigue, sleep restriction and driving 
performance. Accident Analysis and Prevention, 
37, 473—478. 


Phinney, J. S. (2003). Ethnic identity and accultur- 
ation. In K. M. Chun, O. Balls, & P. Organista 
(Eds.), Acculturation: Advances in theory, mea- 
surement, and applied research. Washington, 
DC: American Psychological Association. 


Piaget, J. (1970). Piaget’s theory. In P. H. Mussen 
(Ed.), Carmichael's manual of child psychology 
(3rd ed., Vol. I). New York: Wiley. 


Piaget, J., & Inhelder, B. (1958). The growth of logical 
thinking from childhood to adolescence (A. Parsons 
& S. Seagrin, Trans.). New York: Basic Books. 


Picchioni, D., Goeltzenleucher, B., Green, D. N., 
Convento, M. J., Crittenden, R., Hallgren, M., 
et al. (2002). Nightmares as a coping mecha- 
nism for stress. Dreaming: Journal of the 
Association for the Study of Dreams, 12, 155-169. 


Pickel, K. (2009). The weapon focus effect on 
memory for female versus male perpetrators. 
Memory, 17, 664-678. 


Pickering, G. J., & Gordon, R. (2006). Perception 
of mouthfeel sensations elicited by red wine 
are associated with sensitivity to 6-N- 
propylthiouracil. Journal of Sensory Studies, 21, 
249-265. 


Piechowski, M. M. (2003). From William James to 
Maslow and Dabrowski: Excitability of char- 
acter and self-actualization. In D. Ambrose, 
L. M. Cohen, et al. (Eds.), Creative intelligence: 
Toward theoretic integration: Perspectives on 
creativity. Cresskill, NJ: Hampton Press. 


Pietarinen, A-V. (2006). The evolution of seman- 
tics and language-games for meaning. 
Interaction Studies: Social Behaviour and 
Communication in Biological and Artificial 
Systems, 7, 79-104. 


Pillay, S. S., Gruber, S. A., Rogowska, J., Simpson, 
N., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2006). fMRI of 
fearful facial affect recognition in panic 
disorder: the cingulate gyrus-amygdala 
connection. Journal of Affective Disorders, 94, 
173-181. 


Pillay, S. S., Rogowska, J., Gruber, S. A., Simpson, 
N., & Yurgelun-Todd, D. A. (2007). Recognition 
of happy facial affect in panic disorder: an 
fMRI study. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 21, 
381-393. 


Pilotti, M., Chodorow, M., & Shono, Y. (2009). The 
benefits and costs of prior exposure: A large- 
scale study of interference effects in stimulus 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


identification. American Journal of Psychology, 
122, 191-208. 


Pina, A., Gannon, T., & Saunders, B. (2009). An 
overview of the literature on sexual harass- 
ment: Perpetrator, theory, and treatment 
issues. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 14, 
126-138. 


Pincus, T., & Morley, S. (2001). Cognitive- 
processing bias in chronic pain: A review and 
integration. Psychological Bulletin, 127, 
599-617. 


Pine, D. S., Klein, R. G., Coplan, J. D., Papp, L. A., 
Hoven, C. W., Martinez, J., et al. Differential 
carbon dioxide sensitivity in childhood 
anxiety disorders and nonill comparison 
group. Archives of General Psychiatry, 57, 
960-967. 


Pinel, J. P. J., Assanand, S., & Lehman, D. R. (2000). 
Hunger, eating and ill health. American 
Psychologist, 55, 1105-1116. 


Pinker, S. (1994). The language instinct. New York: 
William Morrow. 


Pinker, S. (2004). Clarifying the logical problem of 
language acquisition. Journal of Child Language, 
31, 949-953. 

Pinker, S., & Jackendoff, R. (2005). The faculty of 
language: What’s special about it? Cognition, 
96, 201-236. 


Pinkerton, S. D., Bogart, L. M., Cecil, H., & 
Abramson, P. R. (2002). Factors associated 
with masturbation in a collegiate sample. 
Journal of Psychology and Human Sexuality, 14, 
103-121. 


Pinkerton, S. D., Cecil, H., Bogart, L. M., & 
Abramson, P. R. (2003). The pleasures of sex: 
An empirical investigation. Cognition and 
Emotion, 17, 341-353. 


Pinquart, M., Duberstein, P. R., & Lyness J. M. 
(2006). Treatments for later-life depressive 
conditions: A meta-analytic comparison of 
pharmacotherapy and psychotherapy. American 
Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 1493-1501. 


Pi-Sunyer, X. (2003). A clinical view of the obesity 
problem. Science, 299, 859-860. 


Pitman, R. K., & Delahanty, D. L. (2005). Con- 
ceptually driven pharmacologic approaches 
to acute trauma. CNS Spectrums, 10, 99-106. 


Platek, S., & Kemp, S. (2009, February). Is family 
special to the brain? An event-related fMRI 
study of familiar, familial, and self-face recog- 
nition. Neuropsychologia, 47, 849-858. 


Plomin, R. (2003). 50 years of DNA: What it has 
meant to psychological science. American 
Psychological Society, 16, 7-8. 


Plomin, R. (2005). Finding genes in child psychol- 
ogy and psychiatry: When are we going to 
be there? Journal of Child Psychology and 
Psychiatry, 46, 1030-1038. 

Plomin, R. (2009). The nature of nurture. In 
K. McCartney & R. A. Weinberg, (Eds.). 
Experience and development: A festschrift in 
honor of Sandra Wood Scarr. New York: 
Psychology Press. 

Plomin, R., & Caspi, R. (1999). Behavioral genet- 
ics and personality. In L. A. Pervin & O. P. 
John (Eds.), Handbook of personality: Theory and 
research. (2nd ed.). New York: Guilford. 

Plomin, R., & Davis, O. (2009, January). The 
future of genetics in psychology and psychia- 


try: Microarrays, genome-wide association, 
and non-coding RNA. Journal of Child Psy- 
chology and Psychiatry, 50, 63-71. 


Plomin, R., & McGuffin, P. (2003). 
Psychopathology in the postgenomic era. 
Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 205-228. 


Plowright, C. M. S., Simonds, V. M., & Butler, 
M. A. (2006). How bumblebees first find flow- 
ers: Habituation of visual pattern preferences, 
spontaneous recovery, and dishabituation. 
Learning and Motivation, 37, 66-78. 


Pluess, M., & Belsky, J. (2009). Differential suscep- 
tibility to rearing experience: The case of 
childcare. Journal of Child Psychology and 
Psychiatry, 50, 396-404. 


Pogarksy, G., & Piquero, A. R. (2003). Can pun- 
ishment encourage offending? Investigating 
the ‘resetting’ effect. Journal of Research in 
Crime and Delinquency, 40, 95-120. 


Pole, N. (2007).The psychophysiology of post- 
traumatic stress disorder: A meta-analysis. 
Psychological Bulletin, 133, 34-45. 


Polivy, J., & Herman, C. P. (2002). Causes of 
eating disorders. Annual Review of Psychology, 
53, 187-213. 


Polivy, J., Herman, C. P., & Boivin, M. (2005). 
Eating disorders. In J. E. Maddux and B. A. 
Winstead, Psychopathology: Foundations for a 
contemporary understanding. Mahwah, NJ: 
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 


Pollack, A. (2006, July 13). Paralyzed man uses 
thoughts to move a cursor. The New York 
Times, p. B5. 


Polonsky, D. C. (2006). Review of the big book of 
masturbation: From angst to zeal. Journal of 
Sex & Marital Therapy, 32, 75-78. 


Pomerlau, O. F. (1995). Individual differences in 
sensitivity to nicotine: Implications of genetic 
research on nicotine dependence [Special 
issue: Genetic, environmental, and situational 
factors mediating the effects of nicotine]. 
Behavior Genetics, 25, 161-177. 


Ponterotto, J. G., Gretchen, D., Chauhan, R. V. 
(2001). Cultural identity and multicultural 
assessment: Quantitative and qualitative tools 
for the clinician. In L. A. Suzuki, & J. G. 
Ponterotto (Eds.), Handbook of multicultural 
assessment: Clinical, psychological, and educa- 
tional applications (2nd ed.). San Francisco: 
Jossey-Bass / Pfeiffer. 


Ponterotto, J. G., Utsey, S. O., & Pedersen, P. B. 
(2006). Preventing prejudice: A guide for counsel- 
ors, educators, and parents. Thousand Oaks, 
CA: Sage Publications. 


Poo, C. & Isaacson, J. S. (2007). An early critical 
period for long-term plasticity and structural 
modification of sensory synapses in olfactory 
cortex. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 7553-7558. 


Popa, D., Léna, C., Alexandre, C., & Adrien, J. 
(2008). Lasting syndrome of depression 
produced by reduction in serotonin uptake 
during postnatal development: Evidence 
from sleep, stress, and behavior. The Journal of 
Neuroscience, 28, 88-97. 


Popp, D., Donovan, R. A., & Crawford, M. (2003). 
Gender, race, and speech style stereotypes. 
Sex Roles, 48, 317-325. 


Porte, H. S., & Hobson, J. A. (1996). Physical 
motion in dreams: One measure of three 


References R-41 


theories. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 105, 
329-335. 


Posner, M. I., & DiGirolamo, G. J. (2000). Cognitive 
neuroscience: Origins and promise. Psycho- 
logical Bulletin, 126, 873-889. 


Post, J., Ali, F., Henderson, S., Shanfield, S., 
Victoroff, J., & Weine, S. (2009, Spring). The 
psychology of suicide terrorism. Psychiatry: 
Interpersonal and Biological Processes, 72, 13-31. 


Poteat, V. P., & Espelage, D. L., (2007, May). Pre- 
dicting psychosocial consequences of homo- 
phobic victimization in middle school students. 
Journal of Early Adolescence, 27(2), 175-191. 


Pottick, K. J., Kirk, S. A., Hsieh, D. K., & Tian, X. 
(2007). Judging mental disorder in youths: 
Effects of client, clinician, and contextual 
differences. Journal of Consulting Clinical 
Psychology, 75, 1-8. 


Powell, L. H. (2006). Review of marital and sex- 
ual lifestyles in the United States: Attitudes, 
behaviors, and relationships in social context. 
Family Relations, 55, 149. 


Powell, L. H, Shahabi, L., & Thoresen, C. E. (2003). 
Religion and spirituality: Linkages to physical 
health. American Psychology, 58, 36-52. 

Powers, K. D. (2006). An analysis of Kohlbergian 
moral development in relationship to biblical 
factors of morality in seminary students 
(Lawrence Kohlberg). Dissertation Abstracts 
International: Section B: The Sciences and 
Engineering, 67(6-B), 3485. 


Powers, M., & Emmelkamp, P. (2008). Virtual real- 
ity exposure therapy for anxiety disorders: A 
meta-analysis. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 22, 
561-569. 


Praisner, C. L. (2003). Attitudes of elementary 
school principals toward the inclusion of 
students with disabilities. Exceptional Children, 
69, 135-145. 


Prasad, B. (2006). Recent advances in artificial 
intelligence [Special issue: Recent advances in 
AJ]. Journal of Experimental & Theoretical 
Artificial Intelligence, 18, 433-434. 


Pratkanis, A. R. (2007). Social influence analysis: 
An index of tactics. In A. R. Pratkanis, The 
science of social influence: Advances and future 
progress. New York: Psychology Press. 


Pratkanis, A. R., Epley, N., & Savitsky, K. (2007). 
Issue 12: Is subliminal persuasion a myth? 
InJ. A. Nier, Taking sides: Clashing views 
in social psychology (2nd ed.) New York: 
McGraw-Hill. 


Pratt, H. D., Phillips, E. L., Greydanus, D. E., & 
Patel, D. R. (2003). Eating disorders in the 
adolescent population: Future directions 
[Special issue: Eating disorders in adoles- 
cents]. Journal of Adolescent Research, 18, 
297-317. 


Pressley, M. P., & Harris., K. R. (2006). Cognitive 
strategies instruction: From basic research to 
classroom instruction. In P. A. Alexander & 
P. H. Winne, Handbook of educational psychol- 
ogy. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum 
Associates. 


Pretzer, J. L., & Beck, A. T. (2005). A cognitive 
theory of personality disorders. In M. F. 
Lenzenweger & J. F. Clarkin (Eds.), Major 
theories of personality disorder (2nd ed.). New 
York: Guilford Press. 


R-42 References 


Price, D. D. (2000, June 9). Psychological and 
neural mechanisms of the affective dimension 
of pain. Science, 288, 1769-1772. 


Price, M. (2008. September). Against doctors’ 
orders. Monitor on Psychology, pp. 34-36. 


Prince, C. V. (2005). Homosexuality, transvestism 
and transsexuality: Reflections on their 
etymology and differentiation. International 
Journal of Transgenderism, 8, 15-18. 


Prinz, J. J. (2007). Emotion: Competing theories 
and philosophical issues. In P. Thagard, 
Philosophy of psychology and cognitive science. 
Amsterdam, Netherlands: North Holland/ 
Elsevier. 


Prislin, R., Brewer, M., & Wilson, D. J. (2002). 
Changing majority and minority positions 
within a group versus an aggregate. Personality 
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 640-647. 


Proffitt, D. R. (2006). Distance perception. Current 
Directions in Psychological Science, 15, 131-139. 


Prohovnik, I., Skudlarski, P., Fulbright, R. K., Gore, 
J. C., & Wexler, B. E. (2004). Functional MRI 
changes before and after onset of reported 


emotions. Psychiatry Research: Neuroimaging, 
132, 239-250. 


Proudfoot, D. (2009). Meaning and mind: 
Wittgenstein’s relevance for the ‘does lan- 
guage shape thought?’ debate. New Ideas in 
Psychology, 27, 163-183. 


Puca, R. M. (2005). The influence of the achieve- 
ment motive on probability estimates in pre- 
and post-decisional action phases. Journal of 
Research in Personality, 39, 245-262. 


Puhl, R., & Latner, J. (2007). Stigma, obesity, and 
the health of the nation’s children. 
Psychological Bulletin, 133, 557-580. 


Putnam, F. W. (2000). Dissociative disorders. In 
A. J. Sameroff & M. Lewis (Eds.), Handbook of 
developmental psychopathology (2nd ed.). 
Dordrecht, Netherlands: Kluwer Academic 
Publishers. 


Quartana, P. J., & Burns, J. W. (2007). Painful con- 
sequences of anger suppression. Emotion, 7, 
400-414. 


Quas, J. A., Malloy, L. C., & Melinder, A. (2007). 
Developmental differences in the effects of 
repeated interviews and interviewer bias on 
young children’s event memory and false 
reports. Developmental Psychology, 43, 823-837. 


Quenot, J. P., Boichot, C., Petit, A., Falcon-Eicher, 
S., d’Athis, P., Bonnet, C., et al. (2005). Use- 
fulness of MRI in the follow-up of patients 
with repaired aortic coarctation and bicuspid 
aortic valve. International Journal of Cardiology, 
103, 312-316. 


Quinn, D. M., Kahng, S. K., & Crocker, J. (2004). 
Discreditable: Stigma effects of revealing a 
mental illness history on test performance. 
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 
803-815. 


Quinn, T. C., & Overbaugh, J. (2005, June 10). 
HIV/AIDS in women: An expanding 
epidemic. Science, 308, 1582-1583. 


Quintana, S. M., Aboud, F. E., & Chao, R. K. 
(2006). Race, ethnicity, and culture in child 
development: Contemporary research and 
future directions. Child Development, 77, 
1129-1141. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Rabin, J. (2004). Quantification of color vision 
with cone contrast sensitivity. Visual Neuro- 
science, 21, 483-485. 


Rachman, S. (1990). Learned resourcefulness in 
the performance of hazardous tasks. In 
Rosenbaum, M. (Ed.), Learned resourcefulness: 
On coping skills, self-control, and adaptive behav- 
ior. New York: Springer. 


Rachman, S., & deSilva, P. (2004). Panic disorders: 
The facts. Oxford, England: Oxford University 
Press. 


Rado, J., Dowd, S., & Janicak, P. (2008). The 
emerging role of transcranial magnetic stimu- 
lation (TMS) for treatment of psychiatric dis- 
orders. Directions in Psychiatry, 28, 315-332. 


Raffaele, P. (2006, November). Speaking Bonobo. 
Smithsonian Magazine. 


Rahman, Q., Kumari, V., & Wilson, G. D. (2003). 
Sexual orientation-related differences in pre- 
pulse inhibition of the human startle response. 
Behavioral Neuroscience, 117, 1096-1102. 


Rajagopal, S. (2006). The placebo effect. Psychiatric 
Bulletin, 30, 185-188. 


Ramachandra, V. (2009, February). On whether 
mirror neurons play a significant role in 
processing affective prosody. Perceptual and 
Motor Skills, 108, 30-36. 


Ramachandran, V. S. (2004). A brief tour of human 
consciousness: From impostor poodles to purple 
numbers. New York: Pi Press. 


Ramachandran, V. S., & Hubbard, E. M. (2001). 
Synesthesia—a window into perception, 
thought and language. Journal of Consciousness 
Studies, 8, 3-34. 

Rampon, C., Jiang, C. H., Dong, H., Tang, Y., 
Lockhart, D. J., Schultaz, P. G., et al. (2000). 
Effects of environmental enrichment on gene 
expression in the brain. Proceedings of the 
National Academy of Sciences, 97, 12880-12884. 


Ramsay, M. C., Reynolds, C. R., & Kamphaus, 
R. W. (2002). Essentials of behavioral assessment. 
New York: Wiley. 


Ramos, R. T. (2006). Antidepressants and dizzi- 
ness. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 20, 708-713. 


Ramus, F. (2006). Genes, brain, and cognition: A 
roadmap for the cognitive scientist. Cognition, 
101, 247-269. 


Randolph-Seng, B., & Nielsen, M. E. (2009). Opening 
the doors of perception: Priming altered states 
of consciousness outside of conscious aware- 
ness. Archiv fiir Religionspsychologie/Archive for 
the Psychology of Religions, 31, 237-260. 


Rangell, L. (2007). The road to unity in psychoana- 
lytic theory. Lanham, MD: Jason Aronson. 


Rapport, R. L. (2005). Nerve endings: The discovery 
of the synapse. New York: W. W. Norton. 


Raskin, N. J., & Rogers, C. R. (1989). Person- 
centered therapy. In R. J. Corsini & D. Wedding 
(Eds.), Current psychotherapies (Ath ed.). Itasca, 
IL: F. E. Peacock. 


Rassin, E. (2008). Individual differences in the 
susceptibility to confirmation bias. Netherlands 
Journal of Psychology, 64, 87-93. 

Rassin, E., & Muris, P. (2007). Abnormal and nor- 
mal obsessions: A reconsideration. Behaviour 
Research and Therapy, 45, 1065-1070. 

Ravindran, A. V., Matheson, K., Griffiths, J., 
Merali, Z., & Anisman, H. (2002). Stress, 


coping, uplifts, and quality of life in subtypes 
of depression: A conceptual framework and 
emerging data. Journal of Affective Disorders, 
71, 121-130. 


Ray, L., Bryan, A., MacKillop, J., McGeary, J., 
Hesterberg, K., & Hutchison, K. (2009). The 
dopamine D4 receptor gene exon III polymor- 
phism, problematic alcohol use and novelty 
seeking: Direct and mediated genetic effects. 
Addiction Biology, 14, 238-244. 


Ray, L. A., & Hutchison, K. E. (2007). Effects of 
naltrexone on alcohol sensitivity and genetic 
moderators of medication response: a 
double-blind placebo-controlled study. 
Archives of General Psychiatry, 64, 1069-1077. 


Ray, R., et al. (2008). Neuroimaging, genetics and 
the treatment of nicotine addiction. Behavioural 
Brain Research, 193, 159-169. 


Raz, A. (2007). Suggestibility and hypnotizability: 
Mind the gap. American Journal of Clinical 
Hypnosis, 49, 205-210. 


Read, J., Beattie, M., Chamberlain, R., & Merrill, J. 
(2008). Beyond the ‘binge’ threshold: Heavy 
drinking patterns and their association with 
alcohol involvement indices in college 
students. Addictive Behaviors, 33, 225-234. 


Redding, G. M. (2002). A test of size-scaling and 
relative-size hypotheses for the moon illusion. 
Perception and Psychophysics, 64, 1281-1289. 


Redding, G. M., & Hawley, E. (1993). Length illu- 
sion in fractional MiillerLyer stimuli: An 
object-perception approach. Perception, 22, 
819-828. 


Redish, A. D. (2004). Addiction as a computa- 
tional process gone awry. Science, 306, 
1944-1947. 


Reece, M., Herbenick, D., Sanders, S., Dodge, B., 
Ghassemi, A., & Fortenberry, J. (2009, July). 
Prevalence and characteristics of vibrator use 
by men in the United States. Journal of Sexual 
Medicine, 6, 1867-1874. 


Reed, P. (2007). Response rate and sensitivity to 
the molar feedback function relating response 
and reinforcement rate on VI+ schedules of 
reinforcement. Journal of Experimental 
Psychology: Animal Behavior Processes, 33, 
428-439. 


Reed, P., & Morgan, T. (2008). Effect on subse- 
quent fixed-interval schedule performance of 
prior exposure to ratio and interval schedules 
of reinforcement. Learning & Behavior, 36, 
82-91. 


Reed, S. K. (1996). Cognition: Theory and applications 
(4th ed.). Pacific Grove, CA: Brooks/ Cole. 


Reese, R. J., Conoley, C. W., & Brossart, D. F. (2002). 
Effectiveness of telephone counseling: A 
field-based investigation. Journal of Counseling 
Psychology, 49, 233-242. 


Regan, P. C. (2006). Love. In R. D. McAnulty, & 
M. M. Burnette, Sex and sexuality, Vol 2: Sexual 
function and dysfunction. Westport, CT: Praeger 
Publishers/Greenwood Publishing. 


Reichenberg, A., & Harvey, P. D. (2007). Neuro- 
psychological impairments in schizophrenia: 
Integration of performance-based and brain 
imaging findings. Psychological Bulletin, 133, 
212-223. 


Reichenberg, A., Harvey, P., Bowie, C., Mojtabai, 
R., Rabinowitz, J., Heaton, R., et al. (2009). 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Neuropsychological function and dysfunction 
in schizophrenia and psychotic affective dis- 
orders. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 35, 1022-1029. 


Reif, A., & Lesch, K. P. (2003). Toward a molecu- 
lar architecture of personality. Behavioural 
Brain Research, 139, 1-20. 


Reijonen, J. H., Pratt, H. D., Patel, D. R., & 
Greydanus, D. E. (2003). Eating disorders in 
the adolescent population: An overview 
[Special issue: Eating disorders in adolescents]. 
Journal of Adolescent Research, 18, 209-222. 


Reilly, T., & Waterhouse, J. (2007). Altered sleep- 
wake cycles and food intake: The Ramadan 
model. Physiology & Behavior, 90, 219-228. 


Reiner, R. (2008, March). Integrating a portable 
biofeedback device into clinical practice for 
patients with anxiety disorders: Results of a 
pilot study. Applied Psychophysiology and 
Biofeedback, 33, 55-61. 


Reisberg, D. (1997). Cognition: Exploring the science 
of the mind. New York: Norton. 


Reisberg, D. (2009). Cognition: Exploring the science 
of the mind. New York: Norton. 


Reiss, S., & Havercamp, S. M. (2005). Motivation 
in developmental context: A new method for 
studying self-actualization. Journal of Human- 
istic Psychology, 45, 41-53. 


Relier, J. P. (2001). Influence of maternal stress on 
fetal behavior and brain development. Biology 
of the Neonate, 79, 168-171. 


Rende, R. (2007). Thinking inside and outside the 
(black) box: Behavioral genetics and human 
development. Human Development, 49, 
343-346. 


Renshaw, D. C. (2006). Male and female circumci- 
sion today. The Family Journal, 14, 283-285. 


Repp, B. H., & Knoblich, G. (2007). Action can 
affect auditory perception. Psychological 
Science, 18, 6-7. 


Rescorla, R. A. (1988). Pavlovian conditioning: 
It’s not what you think it is. American 
Psychologist, 43, 151-160. 


Reynolds, C. R., & Ramsay, M. C. (2003). Bias in 
psychological assessment: An empirical 
review and recommendations. In J. R. 
Graham & J. A. Naglieri (Eds.), Handbook of 
psychology: Assessment psychology (Vol. 10). 
New York: Wiley. 


Reynolds, R. I., & Takooshian, H. (1988, January). 
Where were you August 8, 1985? Bulletin of 
the Psychonomic Society, 26, 23-25. 


Rice, C. (2009, December 18). Prevalence of 
Autism Spectrum Disorders—Autism and 
Developmental Disabilities Monitoring 
Network, United States, 2006. MMWR, 
58(SS10), 1-20. 


Rice, M. L., Tomblin, J. B., Hoffman, L., Richman, 
W. A., & Marquis, J. (2004). Grammatical 
tense deficits in children with SLI and non- 
specific language impairment: Relationships 
with non-verbal IQ over time. Journal of 
Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 47, 
816-834. 


Rich, E. L., & Shapiro, M. L. (2007). Prelimbic/ 
infralimbic inactivation impairs memory for 
multiple task switches, but not flexible selection 
of familiar tasks. Journal of Neuroscience, 27, 
4747—4755. 


Richard, D. C. S., & Lauterbach, D. (Eds). (2006). 
Handbook of exposure therapies. New York: 
Academic Press. 


Richards, R. (2006). Frank Barron and the study 
of creativity: A voice that lives on. Journal of 
Humanistic Psychology, 46, 352-370. 


Richardson, A. S., Bergen, H. A., Martin, G., Roeger, 
L., & Allison, S. (2005). Perceived academic 
performance as an indicator of risk of 
attempted suicide in young adolescents. 
Archives of Suicide Research, 9, 163-176. 


Richardson, B. (2002, September 30). Light-bulb 
moments. The Wall Street Journal, p. R7. 

Richgels, D. J. (2004). Paying attention to 
language. Reading Research Quarterly, 39, 
470—477. 


Rieber, R. W., & Robinson, D. K. (2006). Review 
of the essential Vygotsky. Journal of the History 
of the Behavioral Sciences, 42, 178-180. 


Riedel, G., Platt, B., & Micheau, J. (2003). 
Glutamate receptor function in learning and 
memory. Behavioural Brain Research, 140, 1-47. 


Rigby, L., & Waite, S. (2007). Group therapy for 
self-esteem: Using creative approaches and 
metaphor as clinical tools. Behavioural and 
Cognitive Psychotherapy, 35, 361-364. 


Riley, A., Valdez, C, Barrueco, S., Mills, C., 
Beardslee, W., Sandler, I., et al. (2008). 
Development of a family-based program to 
reduce risk and promote resilience among 
families affected by maternal depression: 
Theoretical basis and program description. 
Clinical Child and Family Psychology Review, 11, 
12-29. 


Riniolo, T. C., Koledin, M., Drakulic, G. M., & 
Payne, R. A. (2003). An archival study of 
eyewitness memory of the Titanic’s final 
plunge. Journal of General Psychology, 130, 
89-95. 


Rinn, W. E. (1984). The neuropsychology of facial 
expression: A review of neurological and 
psychological mechanisms for producing 
facial expressions. Psychological Bulletin, 95, 
52-77. 


Rinn, W. E. (1991). Neuropsychology of facial 
expression. In R. S. Feldman & B. Rimé (Eds.), 
Fundamentals of non-verbal behavior. Cambridge, 
England: Cambridge University Press. 


Riolo, F. (2007). Ricordare, ripetere e rielaborare: 
Un lascito di Freud alia psicoanalisi futura. 
Remembering, repeating, and working 
through: Freud’s legacy to the psychoanalysis 
of the future. Rivista di Psicoanalisi, 53, 439-446. 


Ripley, A. (2005, March 7). Who says a woman 
can’t be Einstein? Time, 51-60. 


Rivera-Gaxiola, M., Klarman, L., Garcia-Sierra, 
A., & Kuhl, P. K. (2005). Neural patterns to 
speech and vocabulary growth in American 
infants. Neuroreport: For Rapid Communication 
of Neuroscience Research, 16, 495-498. 


Robbins, B. (2008). What is the good life? Positive 
psychology and the renaissance of humanistic 
psychology. The Humanistic Psychologist, 36, 
96-112. 


Robert, S. (2006). Deictic space in Wolof: Discourse, 
syntax and the importance of absence. In M. 
Hickman & S. Robert (Eds.), Space in languages: 
Linguistic systems and cognitive categories. 
Amsterdam, Netherlands: John Benjamins. 


References R-43 


Roberts, M. E., Moore, S. D., & Beckham, J. C. 
(2007). Post-traumatic stress disorder and 
substance use disorders. In M. Al’bsi, Stress 
and addiction: Biological and psychological 
mechanisms. San Diego, CA: Elsevier 
Academic Press. 


Robins, R. W. (2005, October 7). The nature of 
personality: Genes, culture, and national 
character. Science, 310, 62-63. 


Robinson, D. N. (2007). Theoretical psychology: 
What is it and who needs it? Theory & 
Psychology, 17, 187-198. 


Robinson, N. M. (2003). Two wrongs do not 
make a right: Sacrificing the needs of gifted 
students does not solve society’s unsolved 
problems. Journal for the Education of the Gifted, 
26, 251-273. 


Rock, A. (1999, January). Quitting time for 
smokers. Money, pp. 139-141. 


Rodd, Z. A., Bell, R. L., Sable, H. J. K., Murphy, 
J. M., & McBride, W. J. (2004). Recent advances 
in animal models of alcohol craving and 
relapse. Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 
79, 439-450. 


Roediger, H. L, HI, & McCabe, D. P. (2007). 
Evaluating experimental research: Critical 
issues. In R. J. Sternberg, H. L. Roediger, III, & 
D. F. Halpern, Critical thinking in psychology. 
New York: Cambridge University Press. 


Roesch, S. C., Adams, L., Hines, A., Palmores, A., 
Vyas, P., Tran, C., et al. (2005). Coping with 
prostate cancer: A meta-analytic review. 
Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 28, 281-293. 


Rogers, C. (1980). A way of being. Boston: 
Houghton Mifflin. 


Rogers, C. R. (1951). Client-centered therapy. 
Boston: Houghton-Mifflin. 


Rogers, C. R. (1971). A theory of personality. In 
S. Maddi (Ed.), Perspectives on personality. 
Boston: Little, Brown. 


Rogers, C. R. (1995). A way of being. Boston: 
Houghton Mifflin. 


Rogers, J. M. (2009). Tobacco and pregnancy: 
Overview of exposures and effects. Birth 
Defects Res. C. Embryo Today, 84, 152-160. 


Rogers, P. (2002, August 2). Too much, too soon. 
People, pp. 79-82. 


Rogers, P., & Eftimiades, M. (1995, July 24). 
Bearing witness. People Weekly, pp. 42-43. 


Rogers, S. (2007). The underlying mechanisms of 
semantic memory loss in Alzheimer’s disease 
and semantic dementia. Dissertation Abstracts 
International: Section B: The Sciences and 
Engineering, 67(10-B), 5591. 

Rohan, K. J., Roecklein, K. A., & Tierney Lindsey, 
K. (2007). A randomized controlled trial of 
cognitive-behavioral therapy, light therapy, 
and their combination for seasonal affective 
disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical 
Psychology, 75, 489-500. 


Roid, G., Nellis, L. & McLellan, M. (2003). 
Assessment with the Leiter International 
Performance Scale—Revised and the S-BIT. In 
R. S. McCallum & R. Steve (Eds.), Handbook of 
nonverbal assessment. New York: Kluwer 
Academic/Plenum Publishers. 


Roisman, G. I., Collins, W. A. Sroufe, L. A., & 
Egeland, B. (2005). Predictors of young adults’ 


R-44 References 


representations of and behavior in their cur- 
rent romantic relationship: Prospective tests 
of the prototype hypothesis. Attachment and 
Human Development, 7, 105-121. 


Roizen, N. J., & Patterson, D. (2003). Down’s syn- 
drome. Lancet, 361, 1281-1289. 


Rollman, G. B. (2004). Ethnocultural variations in 
the experience of pain. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence 
Erlbaum Associates. 


Rom, S. A., Miller, L., & Peluso, J. (2009). Playing 
the game: Psychological factors in surviving 
cancer. International Journal of Emergency 
Mental Health, 11, 25-36. 


Romano, E., Tremblay, R. E., Vitaro, E., Zoccolillo, 
M., & Pagani, L. (2001.) Prevalence of psychi- 
atric diagnoses and the role of perceived 
impairment: Findings from an adolescent 
community sample. Journal of Child Psychology 
and Psychiatry and Allied Disciplines, 42, 
451-461. 


Romeu, P. F. (2006). Memories of the terrorist 
attacks of September 11, 2001: A study of the 
consistency and phenomenal characteristics 
of flashbulb memories. The Spanish Journal of 
Psychology, 9, 52-60. 


Rorschach, H. (1924). Psychodiagnosis: A diagnostic 
test based on perception. New York: Grune & 
Stratton. 


Rosch, E. & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resem- 
blances: Studies in the internal structure of 
categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573-605. 


Rose, N., & Blackmore, S. (2002). Horses for 
courses: Tests of a psychic claimant. Journal of 
the Society for Psychical Research, 66, 29-40. 


Rosen, H. (2000). The creative evolution of the 
theoretical foundations for cognitive therapy 
[Special issue: Creativity in the context of 
cognitive therapy]. Journal of Cognitive 
Psychotherapy, 14, 123-134. 


Rosen, J. (2005, August 28.) The future v. Roberts. 
The New York Times Magazine, pp. 24-29, 44, 
50-51. 


Rosenberg, L., & Park, S. (2002). Verbal and spa- 
tial functions across the menstrual cycle in 
healthy young women. Psychoneuroendo- 
crinology, 27, 834-841. 


Rosenbloom, T., & Wolf, Y. (2002). Sensation 
seeking and detection of risky road signals: A 
developmental perspective. Accident Analysis 
and Prevention, 34, 569-580. 


Rosenhan, D. L. (1973). On being sane in insane 
places. Science, 179, 250-258. 


Rosenman, R. H., Grand R. J., Jenkins, C. D., 
Friedman, M, Straus, R., & Wurm, M. (1994). 
Coronary heart disease in the Western 
Collaborative Group Study: Final follow-up 
experience of 8% years. In A. Steptoe & J. 
Wardle, (Eds). Psychosocial processes and health: A 
reader. New York: Cambridge University Press. 


Rosenstein, D. S., & Horowitz, H. A. (1996). 
Adolescent attachment and psychopathology. 
Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 
64, 244-253. 


Rosenthal, A. M. (2008) Thirty-eight witnesses: The 
Kitty Genovese case. Hoboken, NJ: Melville 
House Publishing. 


Rosenthal, R. (2002). Covert communication in 
classrooms, clinics, courtrooms and cubicles. 
American Psychologist, 57, 838-849. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Rosenthal, R. (2003). Covert communication in 
laboratories, classrooms, and the truly real 
world. Current Directions in Psychological 
Science, 12, 151-154. 


Rosner, H. (2001, April 30). The science of O. New 
York Magazine, pp. 25-31. 


Ross, H. E. (2000). Sensation and perception. In 
D. S. Gupta, et al. (Eds.), Psychology for 
psychiatrists. London: Whurr Publishers. 


Ross, H. E., & Plug, C. (2002). The mystery of the 
moon illusion: Exploring size perception. Oxford: 
University Press. 


Ross, J. (2006). Sleep on a problem . . . It works 
like a dream. The Psychologist, 19, 738-740. 


Ross, P. E. (2004, April). Draining the language 
out of color. Scientific American, pp. 46-51. 


Rossato, M., Pagano, C., & Vettor, R. (2008). The 
cannabinoid system and male reproductive 
functions. Journal of Neuroendocrinology, 20, 
90-93. 


Rossell, S. L., Bullmore, E. T., Williams, S. C. R., & 
David, A. S. (2002). Sex differences in func- 
tional brain activation during a lexical visual 
field task. Brain and Language, 80, 97-105. 


Rossi, J. J., June, C. H., & Kohn, D. B. (2007). 
Genetic therapies against HIV. Natural Biotech- 
nology, 25, 1444-1454. 


Rossouw, J. E., Prentice, R. L., Manson, J. E., Wu, L., 
Barad, D., Barnabei, V. M., et al. (2007). 
Postmenopausal hormone therapy and risk of 
cardiovascular disease by age and years since 
menopause. Journal of the American Medical 
Association, 297, 1465-1477. 


Rotan, L. W., & Ospina-Kammerer, V. (2007). 
Mindbody medicine: Foundations and practical 
applications. New York: Routledge/Taylor & 
Francis Group. 


Roter, D. L., Hall, J. A., & Aoki, Y. (2002). Physician 
gender effects in medical communication: A 
meta-analytic review. Journal of the American 
Medical Association, 288, 756-764. 


Rothblum, E. D. (1990). Women and weight: Fad 
and fiction. Journal of Psychology, 124, 5-24. 


Roughton, R. E. (2002). Rethinking homosexual- 
ity: What it teaches us about psychoanalysis. 
Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association, 
50, 733-763. 


Routtenberg, A., & Lindy, J. (1965). Effects of the 
availability of rewarding septal and hypotha- 
lamic stimulation on bar pressing for food 
under conditions of deprivation. Journal of 
Comparative and Physiological Psychology, 60, 
158-161. 


Rowe, J. B., Toni, I., Josephs, O., Frackowiak, R. S. 
J., & Passingham, R. E. (2000, June 2). The pre- 
frontal cortex: Response selection or mainte- 
nance within working memory? Science, 288, 
1656-1660. 


Royzman, E. B., Cassidy, K. W., & Baron, J. (2003). 
“I know, you know”: Epistemic egocentrism 
in children and adults. Review of General 
Psychology, 7, 38-65. 


Rozencwajg, P., Cherfi, M., Ferrandez, A. M., 
Lautrey, J., Lemoine, C., & Loarer, E. (2005). 
Age-related differences in the strategies used 
by middle aged adults to solve a block design 
task. International Journal of Aging and Human 
Development, 60, 159-182. 


Rozin, P., Kabnick, K., Pete, E., Fischler, C., & 
Shields, C. (2003). The ecology of eating: 
Smaller portion sizes in France than in the 
United States help explain the French para- 
dox. Psychological Science, 14, 450-454. 


Rubichi, S., Ricci, F., Padovani, R., & Scaglietti, L. 
(2005). Hypnotic susceptibility, baseline 
attentional functioning, and the Stroop task. 
Consciousness and Cognition: An International 
Journal, 14, 296-303. 


Rubin, B. D., & Katz, L. C. (1999). Optical imag- 
ing of odorant representations in the mamma- 
lian olfactory bulb. Neuron 23, 499-511. 


Rubin, D. C. (1985, September). The subtle 
deceiver: Recalling our past. Psychology Today, 
pp. 39-46. 


Rubin, D. C., Schrauf, R. W., Gulgoz, S., & Naka, 
M. (2007). Cross-cultural variability of com- 
ponent processes in autobiographical 
remembering: Japan, Turkey, and the USA. 
Memory, 15, 536-547. 


Rudman, L. A. & Ashmore, R. D. (2007). Discri- 
mination and the Implicit Association Test. 
Group Processes & Intergroup Relations, 10, 
359-372. 


Rudner, M., & Rönnberg, J. (2008). The role of 
the episodic buffer in working memory for 
language processing. Cognitive Processing, 9, 
19-28. 


Rule, N. O., Ambady, N., & Hallett, K. C. (2009). 
Female sexual orientation is perceived 
accurately, rapidly, and automatically from 
the face and its features. Journal of Experimental 
Social Psychology, 45, 1245-1251. 


Rulison, K. L., & Loken, E. (2009). I’ve fallen and 
I can’t get up: Can high-ability students 
recover from early mistakes in CAT? Applied 
Psychological Measurement, 33, 83-101. 


Runco, M. A. (2006). Introduction to the special 
issue: Divergent thinking. Creativity Research 
Journal, 18, 249-250. 


Rusche, B. (2003) The 3Rs and animal welfare— 
conflict or the way forward? ALTEX, 20, 
(Suppl. 1), 63-76. 


Ruscher, J. B., Fiske, S. T., & Schnake, S. B. (2000). 
The motivated tactician’s juggling act: 
Compatible vs. incompatible impression 
goals. British Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 
241-256. 


Rushton, J.P. & Jensen, A.R. (2006). The totality of 
available evidence shows the race IQ gap still 
remains. Psychological Science, 17, 921-922. 


Russell, J. A., & Sato, K. (1995). Comparing emo- 
tion words between languages. Journal of 
Cross Cultural Psychology, 26, 384-391. 


Russell, R., Duchaine, B., & Nakayama, K. (2009). 
Super-recognizers: People with extraordinary 
face recognition ability. Psychonomic Bulletin & 
Review, 16, 252-257. 

Russo, N. (1981). Women in psychology. In L. T. 
Benjamin, Jr. & K. D. Lowman (Eds.), Activities 
handbook for the teaching of psychology. 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 

Rustin, M. (2006). Infant observation research: 
What have we learned so far? Infant 
Observation, 9, 35-52. 

Rutherford, B., Rose, S., Sneed, J., & Roose, S. 
(2009, April). Study design affects participant 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


expectations: A survey. Journal of Clinical 
Psychopharmacology, 29, 179-181. 


Rutter, M. (2002). Nature, nurture, and develop- 
ment: From evangelism through science 
toward policy and practice. Child Development, 
73, 1-21. 


Rutter, M. (2006). Genes and behavior: Nature- 
nurture interplay explained. Malden, MA: 
Blackwell Publishing. 


Rychlak, J. (1997). In defense of human conscious- 
ness. Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


Rydell, R., McConnell, A., & Mackie, D. (2008). 
Consequences of discrepant explicit and 
implicit attitudes: Cognitive dissonance and 
increased information processing. Journal of 
Experimental Social Psychology, 44, 1526-1532. 


Rymer, R. (1994). Genie: A scientific tragedy. New 
York: Penguin. 


Saarni, C. (1999). Developing emotional competence. 
New York: Guilford. 


Sabater, J., & Sierra, C. (2005). Review on compu- 
tational trust and reputation models. Artificial 
Intelligence Review, 24, 33-60. 


Sachs-Ericsson, N., Joiner, T., Plant, E. A., & 
Blazer, D. G. (2005). The influence of depres- 
sion on cognitive decline in community- 
dwelling elderly persons. American Journal of 
Geriatric Psychiatry, 13, 402-408. 


Sackeim, H. A., Haskett, R. F., Mulsant, B. H., Thase, 
M. E., Mann, J. J., Pettinati, H. M., et al. (2001). 
Continuation pharmaco-therapy in the 
prevention of relapse following electrocon- 
vulsive therapy: A randomized controlled 
trial. Journal of the American Medical Association, 
285, 1299-1307. 


Sacks, O. (2003, July 28). The mind’s eye. The New 
Yorker, pp. 48-59. 


Saczynski, J., Willis, S., & Schaie, K. (2002). Strategy 
use in reasoning training with older adults. 
Aging, Neuropsychology, & Cognition, 9, 48-60. 


Sadeghi-Nejad, H., & Watson, R. (2008). Prema- 
ture ejaculation: Current medical treatment 
and new directions. Journal of Sexual Medicine, 
5, 1037-1050. 


Sadker, M., & Sadker, D. (1994). Failing at fairness: 
How America’s schools cheat girls. New York: 
Scribners. 


Saggino, A., Perfetti, B., & Spitoni, G. (2006). Fluid 
intelligence and executive functions: New 
perspectives. In L. V. Wesley, Intelligence: New 
research. Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science 
Publishers. 


Sahin, N. T., Pinker, S., & Halgren, E. (2006). 
Abstract grammatical processing of nouns 
and verbs in Broca’s area: Evidence from 
fMRI. Cortex, 42, 540-562. 


Sakai, K. L. (2005, November 4). Language acqui- 
sition and brain development. Science, 310, 
815-817. 


Salgado, D. M., Quinlin, K. J., & Zlotnick, C. (2007). 
The relationship of lifetime polysubstance 
dependence to trauma exposure, symptom- 
atology, and psychosocial functioning in 
incarcerated women with comorbid PTSD 
and substance use disorder. Journal of Trauma 
Dissociation, 8, 9-26. 


Sallquist, J., Eisenberg, N., Spinrad, T. L., Eggum, 
N. D.. & Gaertner, B. (2009). Assessment of 
preschoolers’ positive empathy: Concurrent 
and longitudinal relations with positive 
emotion, social competence, and sympathy. 
The Journal of Positive Psychology, 4, 223-233. 


Salmela-Aro, K., & Nurmi, J-E. (2007). Self- 
esteem during university studies predicts 
career characteristics 10 years later. Journal of 
Vocational Behavior, 70, 463-477. 


Salsman, N. L. (2006). Interpersonal change as 
an outcome of Time-Limited Interpersonal 
Therapy. Dissertation Abstracts International: 
Section B: The Sciences and Engineering, 
66(9-B), 5103. 

Salvi, V., Fagiolini, A., Swartz, H., Maina, G., & 
Frank, E. (2008). The use of antidepressants in 
bipolar disorder. Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 
69, 1307-1318. 


Samantaray, S. K., Srivastava, M., & Mishra, P. K. 
(2002). Fostering self concept and self actual- 
ization as bases for empowering women in 
national development: A challenge for the 
new millennium. Social Science International, 
18, 58-63. 


Samoilov, V., & Zayas, V. (2007). Ivan Petrovich 
Pavlov (1849-1936). Journal of the History of the 
Neurosciences, 16, 74-89. 


Sampson, S., & Moore, L. (2008). Is there a glass 
ceiling for women in development? Nonprofit 
Management and Leadership, 18, 321-339. 

Sams, M., Hari, R., Rif, J., & Knuutila, J. (1993). 
The human auditory memory trace persists 
about 10 sec: Neuromagnetic evidence. 
Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 5, 363-370. 


Samuel, D. B., & Widiger, T. A. (2006). Differen- 
tiating normal and abnormal personality from 
the perspective of the DSM. In S. Strack, 
Differentiating normal and abnormal personality 
(2nd ed.). New York: Springer Publishing. 


Sanders, C., Sadoski, M., van Walsum, K., 
Bramson, R., Wiprud, R., & Fossum, T. (2008). 
Learning basic surgical skills with mental 
imagery: Using the simulation centre in the 
mind. Medical Education, 42, 607—612. 


Sanderson, M. (2007). Assessment of manic 
symptoms in different cultures. British Journal 
of Psychiatry, 190, 178. 


Sandomir, R. (2007, July 17). W. W. E.’s testing is 
examined after Bennoit murder-suicide. The 
New York Times, p. S3. 


Sandoval, J., Frisby, C. L., Geisinger, K. F., 
Scheuneman, J. D., & Grenier, J. R. (Eds.). 
(1998). Test interpretation and diversity: 
Achieving equity in assessment. Washington, 
DC: American Psychological Association. 


Santel, S., Baving, L., Krauel, K., Munte, T. F. & 
Rotte, M. (2006, October 9). Hunger and 
satiety in anorexia nervosa: fMRI during 
cognitive processing of food pictures. Brain 
Research, 1114, 138-148. 


Santelli, J., Carter, M., Orr, M., & Dittus, P. (2009, 
April). Trends in sexual risk behaviors, by 
nonsexual risk behavior involvement, U.S. 
high school students, 1991-2007. Journal of 
Adolescent Health, 44, 372-379. 


Santtila, P., Wager, I., Witting, K., Harlaar, N., 
Jern, P., Johansson, A., et al. (2008). 
Discrepancies between sexual desire and 
sexual activity: Gender differences and asso- 


References R-45 


ciations with relationship satisfaction. Journal 
of Sex & Marital Therapy, 34, 31—44. 


Saper, C. B., Lu, J., Chou, T. C., & Gooley, J. (2005). 
The hypothalamic integrator for circadian 
rhythms. Trends in Neuroscience, 28, 152-157. 


Sapolsky, R. M. (2003). Gene therapy for psychi- 
atric disorders. American Journal of Psychiatry, 
160, 208-220. 


Sargent, J. D., Stoolmiller, M., Worth, K. A., Cal, 
C. S., Wills, T. A., Gibbons, F. X., et al. (2007). 
Exposure to smoking depictions in movies: Its 
association with established adolescent smok- 
ing. Archives of Pediatric Adolescent Medicine, 
161, 849-856. 


Sato, N., Shimamura, M., & Takeuchi, D. (2007). 
Gene therapy for ischemic brain disease 
with special reference to vascular dementia. 
Geriatrics & Gerontology International, 7, 1-14. 


Saucier, D. A., & Cain, M. E. (2006). The founda- 
tions of attitudes about animal research. 
Ethics & Behavior, 16, 117-133. 


Savage, J., & Yancey, C. (2008). The effects of 
media violence exposure on criminal 
aggression: A meta-analysis. Criminal Justice 
and Behavior, 35, 772-791. 


Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S., Toth, N., & Schick, K. 
(2007). Kanzi learns to knap stone tools. In 
D. A. Washburn, Primate perspectives on behav- 
ior and cognition. Washington, DC: American 
Psychological Association. 


Savas, H. A., Yumru, M., & Kaya, M. C. (2007). 
Atypical antipsychotics as ‘mood stabilizers’: 
A retrospective chart review. Progress in 
Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psy- 
chiatry, 31, 1064-1067. 


Savazzi, S., Fabri, M., Rubboli, G., Paggi, A., 
Tassinari, C. A., & Marzi, C. A. (2007). Inter- 
hemispheric transfer following callosotomy in 
humans: Role of the superior colliculus. 
Neuropsychologia, 45, 2417-2427. 


Saville, B. (2009). Performance under competitive 
and self-competitive fixed-interval schedules 
of reinforcement. The Psychological Record, 59, 
21-38. 


Sawa, A., & Snyder, S. H. (2002, April 26). 
Schizophrenia: Diverse approaches to a 
complex disease. Science, 296, 692-695. 


Sayette, M. A. (1993). An appraisal disruption 
model of alcohol’s effects on stress responses 
in social drinkers. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 
459-476. 

Saywitz, K., & Goodman, G. (1990). Unpublished 
study reported in Goleman, D. (1990, No- 
vember 6). Doubts rise on children as 
witnesses. The New York Times, pp. C1, C6. 

Scarr, S. (1998). American child care today. 
American Psychologist, 53, 95-108. 

Scarr, S., & Weinberg, R. A. (1976). I.Q. test 
performance of black children adopted by 
white families. American Psychologist, 31, 
726-739. 

Scaturo, D. J. (2004). Fundamental clinical dilem- 
mas in contemporary group psychotherapy. 
Group Analysis, 37, 201-217. 

Scelfo, J. (2007, February 26). Men & depression: 
Facing darkness. Newsweek, p. 43-50. 

Schachter, S., & Singer, J. E. (1962). Cognitive, 
social, and physiological determinants of 


R-46 References 


emotional state. Psychological Review, 69, 
379-399. 


Schacter, D. L., & Badgaiyan, R. D. (2001). 
Neuroimaging of priming: New perspectives 
on implicit and explicit memory. Current 
Directions in Psychological Science, 10, 1-4. 


Schacter, D. L., Dobbins, I. G., & Schnyer, D. M. 
(2004). Specificity of priming: A cognitive 
neuroscience perspective. Nature Reviews 
Neuroscience, 5, 853-862. 


Schaefer, R. T. (2000). Sociology: A brief introduction 
(3rd ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill. 


Schaer, M., & Eliez, S. (2007). From genes to 
brain: Understanding brain development in 
neurogenetic disorders using neuroimaging 
techniques. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric 
Clinics of North America, 16, 557-579. 


Schaie, K. W. (2005a). Longitudinal studies. In 
Developmental influences on adult intelligence: 
The Seattle Longitudinal Study. New York: 
Oxford University Press. 


Schaie, K. W. (2005b). What can we learn from 
longitudinal studies of adult development? 
Research in Human Development, 2, 133-158. 


Schaller, M., & Crandall, C. S. (Eds.) (2004). The 
psychological foundations of culture. Mahwah, 
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 


Schechter, T., Finkelstein, Y., & Koren, G. (2005). 
Pregnant “DES daughters” and their off- 
spring. Canadian Family Physician, 51, 493-494. 


Schedlowski, M., & Tewes, U. (Eds.) (1999). 
Psychoneuroimmunology: An interdisciplinary 
introduction. New York: Plenum. 


Scheele, B., & DuBois, F. (2006). Catharsis as a 
moral form of entertainment. In J. Bryant, & 
P. Vorderer, Psychology of entertainment. 
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates 
Publishers. 


Scheff, T. J. (1998). Shame in the labeling of 
mental illness. In P. Gilbert & B Andrews, 
(Eds.), Shame: Interpersonal behavior, psychopa- 
thology, and culture. New York: Oxford 
University Press. 


Scheier, M. F., Carver, C. S., & Bridges, M. W. 
(1994). Distinguishing optimism from neuroti- 
cism (and trait anxiety, self-mastery, and self- 
esteem): A reevaluation of the Life Orientation 
Test. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 
67, 1063-1078. 


Schenone, M. H., Aquin, E., Li, Y., Lee, C., Kruger, 
M., & Bahado-Singh, R. O. (2010). Prenatal 
prediction of neonatal survival at the border- 
line viability. Journal of Maternal-Fetal Neonatal 
Medicine, 12, 31-38. 


Schepers, P., & van den Berg, P. T. (2007). Social 
factors of work-environment creativity. 
Journal of Business and Psychology, 21, 407-428. 


Schieber, F. (2006). Vision and aging. In J. E. Birren 
& K. W. Schaire, Handbook of the psychology of 
aging (6th ed.). Amsterdam, Netherlands: 
Elsevier. 


Schiffer, A. A., Pedersen, S. S., Widdershoven, 
J. W., Hendriks, E. H., Winter, J. B., & Denollet, 
J. (2005). The distressed (type D) personality 
is independently associated with impaired 
health status and increased depressive symp- 
toms in chronic heart failure. European Journal 
of Cardiovascular Prevention and Rehabilitation, 
12, 341-346. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Schillinger, D., Bindman, A., Wang, F., Stewart, 
A., & Piette, J. (2004). Functional health liter- 
acy and the quality of physician-patient com- 
munication among diabetes patients. Patient 
Education and Counseling, 52, 315-323. 


Schmidt, J. P. (2006). The Discovery of neuro- 
transmitters: A fascinating story and a 
scientific object lesson. PsycCRITIQUES, 61, 
101-115. 


Schmidt, N. B., Kotov, R., & Joiner, T. E., Jr. (2004). 
Taxometrics: Toward a new diagnostic scheme for 
psychopathology. Washington, DC: American 
Psychological Association. 


Schmitt, D. P., Allik, J., & Mccrae, R. R. (2007). 
The geographic distribution of Big Five per- 
sonality traits: Patterns and profiles of human 
self-description across 56 nations. Journal of 
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 38, 173-212. 


Schmitt, D., Realo, A., Voracek, M., & Allik, J. 
(2008). Why can’t a man be more like a 
woman? Sex differences in Big Five personal- 
ity traits across 55 cultures. Journal of Per- 
sonality and Social Psychology, 94, 168-182. 


Schnabel, K., Asendorpf, J., & Greenwald, A. 
(2008). Assessment of individual differences 
in implicit cognition: A review of IAT 
measures. European Journal of Psychological 
Assessment, 24, 210-217. 


Schnall, S., Haidt, J., Clore, G. L., & Jordan, A. H. 
(2008). Disgust as embodied moral judgment. 
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34, 
1096-1109. 


Schnatz, P. F, Murphy, J. L., O'Sullivan, D. M., & 
Sorosky, J. I. (2007). Patient choice: Com- 
paring criteria for selecting an obstetrician- 
gynecologist based on image, gender, and 
professional attributes. American Journal of 
Obstetrics and Gynecology, 197, 548-561. 


Schneider, A. & Domhoff, G. W. (2002). The quanti- 
tative study of dreams. www.dreamresearch.net. 


Schnell, K., & Herpertz, S. C. (2007). Effects of 
dialectic-behavioral-therapy on the neural 
correlates of affective hyperarousal in border- 
line personality disorder. Journal of Psychiatric 
Research, 41, 837-847. 


Schofield, W., & Vaughan-Jackson, P. (1913). What 
a boy should know. New York: Cassell. 


Schredl, M., Fricke-Oerkermann, L., Mitschke, A., 
Wiater, A., & Lehmkuhl, G. (2009, September). 
Longitudinal study of nightmares in children: 
Stability and effect of emotional symptoms. 
Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 40, 
439-449, 


Schredl, M., & Piel, E. (2005). Gender differences 
in dreaming: Are they stable over time? 
Personality and Individual Differences, 39, 
309-316. 


Schroers, M., Prigot, J., & Fagen, J. (2007, 
December). The effect of a salient odor 
context on memory retrieval in young infants. 
Infant Behavior & Development, 30, 685-689. 


Schubert, T., & Koole, S. (2009). The embodied 
self: Making a fist enhances men’s power- 
related self-conceptions. Journal of Experimental 
Social Psychology, 45, 828-834. 

Schulte-Ruther, M., Markowitsch, J. J., Fink, G. R., 
& Piefke, M. (2007). Mirror neuron and theory 
of mind mechanisms involved in face-to-face 
interactions: A functional magnetic resonance 


imaging approach to empathy. Journal of 
Cognitive Neuroscience, 19, 1354-1372. 


Schutt, R. K. (2001). Investigating the social world: 
The process and practice of research. Thousand 
Oaks, CA: Sage. 


Schwartz, B. (2008). Working memory load differ- 
entially affects tip-of-the-tongue states and 
feeling-of-knowing judgments. Memory & 
Cognition, 36, 9-19. 


Schwartz, B. L. (2001). The relation of tip-of-the- 
tongue states and retrieval time. Memory & 
Cognition, 29, 117-126. 


Schwartz, B. L. (2002). The phenomenology of nat- 
urally-occurring tip-of-the-tongue states: A 
diary study. In S. P. Shohov (Ed.), Advances in 
psychology research (Vol. 8). Huntington, NY: 
Nova. 


Schwartz, J. M., & Begley, S. (2002). The mind and 
the brain: Neuroplasticity and the power of mental 
force. (2002). New York: Regan Books/Harper 
Collins. 


Schwartz, J., & Wald, M. L. (2003). NASA’s curse? 
“Groupthink” is 30 years old, and still going 
strong. The New York Times, p. C1. 


Schwartz, P., Maynard, A., & Uzelac, S. (2008). 
Adolescent egocentrism: A contemporary 
view. Adolescence, 43(171), 441-448. 


Schwartz, S. J., Côté, J. E., & Arnett, J. J. (2005). 
Identity and agency in emerging adulthood: 
Two developmental routes in the individual- 
ization process. Youth & Society, 37, 201-229. 


Schwenkreis, P., El Tom, S., Ragert, P., Pleger, B., 
Tegenthoff, M., & Dinse, H. (2007, December). 
Assessment of sensorimotor cortical represen- 
tation asymmetries and motor skills in violin 
players. European Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 
3291-3302. 


Sciutto, M., & Eisenberg, M. (2007). Evaluating 
the evidence for and against the overdiagno- 
sis of ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 11, 
106-113. 


Scott, L. S., Tanaka, J. W., Sheinberg, D. L., & 
Curran, T. (2008). The role of category 
learning in the acquisition and retention of 
perceptual expertise: A behavioral and 
neurophysiological study. Brain Research, 
1210, 204-215. 


Scullin, M. H., Kanaya, T., & Ceci, S. J. (2002). 
Measurement of individual differences in 
children’s suggestibility across situations. 
Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 8, 
233-246. 


Seamon, M. J., Fass, J. A., Maniscalco-Feichtl, M., 
& Abu-Shraie, N. A. (2007). Medical marijuana 
and the developing role of the pharmacist. 
American Journal of Health System Pharmacy, 64, 
1037-1044. 


Sears, D. O. (1986). College sophomores in the 
laboratory: Influences of a narrow data base 
on social psychology’s view of human nature. 
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 
515-530. 


Sebel, P. S., Bonke, B., & Winograd, E. (Eds.). 
(1993). Memory and awareness in anesthesia. 
Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall. 


Sebire, S. J., Standage, M., & Vansteenkiste, M. 
(2009). Examining intrinsic versus extrinsic 
exercise goals: cognitive, affective, and 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


behavioral outcomes. Journal of Sport Exercise 
Psychology, 31, 189-210. 


Seeley, R., Stephens, T., & Tate, P. (2000). Anatomy 
& Physiology (5th ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill. 


Sefcek, J. A., Brumbach, B. H., & Vasquez, G. 
(2007). The evolutionary psychology of 
human mate choice: How ecology, genes, 
fertility, and fashion influence mating strate- 
gies. Journal of Psychology & Human Sexuality, 
18, 125-182. 


Segall, M. H., Campbell, D. T., & Herskovits, M. J. 


(1966). The influence of culture on visual percep- 
tion. New York: Bobbs-Merrill. 


Segerstrom, S. C., & Miller, G. E. (2004). Psy- 
chological stress and the human immune 
system: A meta-analytic study of 30 years of 
inquiry. Psychological Bulletin, 130, 601-630. 


Seibt, B., & Förster, J. (2005). Stereotype threat 
and performance: How self-stereotypes influ- 
ence processing by inducing regulatory foci. 
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 87, 
38-56. 


Seli, H. (2007). Self in self-worth protection: The 
relationship of possible selves to achievement 
motives and self-worth protective strategies. 
Dissertation Abstracts International Section A: 
Humanities and Social Sciences, 67(9-A), 3302. 


Seligman, M. E. (2007). What you can change... 
and what you can’t: The complete guide to suc- 
cessful self-improvement. New York: Vintage. 


Seligman, M. E. P. (1975). Helplessness: On depres- 
sion, development, and death. San Francisco: 
Freeman. 


Seligman, M. E. P. (1995, December). The effective- 
ness of psychotherapy: The Consumer Reports 
study. American Psychologist, 50, 965-974. 


Seligman, M. E. P. (1996, October). Science as an 
ally of practice. American Psychologist, 51, 
1072-1079. 


Selkoe, D. (2008). Soluble oligomers of the amy- 
loid B-protein impair synaptic plasticity and 
behavior. Behavioural Brain Research, 192, 
106-113. 


Selkoe, D. J. (1997, January 31). Alzheimer’s dis- 
ease: Genotypes, phenotype, and treatments. 
Science, 275, 630-631. 


Selkoe, D. J. (2002). Alzheimer’s disease is a 
synaptic failure. Science, 298, 789-791. 


Sellbom, M., & Ben-Porath, Y. S. (2006). The 
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality 
Inventory-2. In R. P. Archer, Forensic uses of 
clinical assessment instruments. Mahwah, NJ: 
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 

Sellbom, M., Fischler, G., & Ben-Porath, Y. (2007). 
Identifying MMPI-2 Predictors of police 
officer integrity and misconduct. Criminal 
Justice and Behavior, 34, 985-1004. 

Sells, R. (1994, August). Homosexuality study. 
Paper presented at the annual meeting of the 
American Statistical Association, Toronto. 

Selove, R. (2007). The glass is half full: Current 
knowledge about pediatric cancer and 
sickle cell anemia. PsycCRITIQUES, 52, 
88-99. 

Selsky, A. (1997, February 16). African males face 
circumcision rite. The Boston Globe, p. C7. 

Selye, H. (1976). The stress of life. New York: 
McGraw-Hill. 





Selye, H. (1993). History of the stress concept. In 
L. Goldberger & S. Breznitz (Eds.), Handbook 
of stress: Theoretical and clinical aspects (2nd 
ed.). New York: Free Press. 


Semler, C. N., & Harvey, A. G. (2005). Misperce- 
ption of sleep can adversely affect daytime 
functioning in insomnia. Behaviour Research 
and Therapy, 43, 843-856. 


Semykina, A., & Linz, S. J. (2007). Gender differ- 
ences in personality and earnings: Evidence 
from Russia. Journal of Economic Psychology, 
28, 387-410. 


Sen, B., Sinha, S., Ahmed, S., Ghosh, S. Gango- 
padhyay, P. K., & Usha, R. (2007). Lack of 
association of HOXA1 and HOXB1 variants 
with autism in the Indian population. 
Psychiatric Genetics, 17, 22-36. 


Seroczynski, A. D., Jacquez, F. M., & Cole, D. A. 
(2003). Depression and suicide during adoles- 
cence. In G. R. Adams & M. D. Berzonsky 
(Eds.), Blackwell handbook of adolescence. 
Malden, MA: Blackwell Publishers. 


Serpell, R. (2000). Intelligence and culture. In 
R. Sternberg (Ed.), Handbook of intelligence. 
Cambridge, England: Cambridge University 
Press. 


Seymour, B. (2006). Carry on eating: Neural path- 
ways mediating conditioned potentiation of 
feeding. Journal of Neuroscience, 26, 1061-1062. 


Shafer, V. L. & Garrido-Nag, K. (2007). The 
neurodevelopmental bases of language. In 
E. Hoff & M. Shatz, Blackwell handbook of 
language development. Malden, MA: Blackwell 
Publishing. 


Shah, D. B., Pesiridou, A., Baltuch, G. H., Malone, 
D. A. & O’Reardon, J. P. (2008). Functional 
neurosurgery in the treatment of severe 
obsessive compulsive disorder and major 
depression: Overview of disease circuits and 
therapeutic targeting for the clinician. 
Psychiatry, 5, 24-33. 


Shaikholeslami, R., & Khayyer, M. (2006). 
Intrinsic motivation, extrinsic motivation, 
and learning English as a foreign language. 
Psychological Reports, 99, 813-818. 


Shankar, G., & Simmons, A. (2009, January). 
Understanding ethics guidelines using an 
internet-based expert system. Journal of 
Medical Ethics, 35, 65—68. 


Shapiro, L. R. (2006). Remembering September 
11th: The role of retention interval and 
rehearsal on flashbulb and event memory. 
Memory, 14, 129-147. 


Sharma, H. S., Sjoquist, P. O., & Ali, S. F. (2007). 
Drugs of abuse-induced hyperthermia, blood- 
brain barrier dysfunction and neurotoxicity: 
neuroprotective effects of a new antioxidant 
compound h-290/51. Current Pharmaceutical 
Design, 13, 1903-1923. 


Shea, A., & Steiner, M. (2008). Cigarette smoking 
during pregnancy. Nicotine & Tobacco Research, 
10, 267-278. 


Shelton, R. C., Keller, M. B., Gelenberg, A., Dunner, 
D. L., Hirschfeld, R. M. A., Thase, M. E., et al. 
(2002). The effectiveness of St. John’s wort in 
major depression: A multi-center, randomized 
placebo-controlled trial. Journal of the American 
Medical Association, 285, 1978-1986. 


References R-47 


Shepard, R. N., & Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rota- 
tion of three-dimensional objects. Science, 
171(3972), 701-703. 


Sheppard, L. D., & Vernon, P. A. (2008). Intelligence 
and speed of information-processing: A review 
of 50 years of research. Personality and Individual 
Differences, 44, 535-551. 


Shepperd, J., Malone, W., & Sweeny, K. (2008). 
Exploring causes of the self-serving bias. 
Social and Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 
895-908. 


Sherblom, S. (2008). The legacy of the ‘care 
challenge’: Re-envisioning the outcome of the 
justice-care debate. Journal of Moral Education, 
37, 81-98. 


Sherman, S. L., Allen, E. G., Bean, L. H., & 
Freeman, S. B. (2007). Epidemiology of Down 
syndrome [Special issue: Down syndrome]. 
Mental Retardation and Developmental 
Disabilities Research Reviews, 13, 221-227. 


Shier, D., Butler, J., & Lewis, R. (2000). Hole’s 
essentials of human anatomy and physiology 
(7th ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill. 


Shiffman, S. (2007). Use of more nicotine lozenges 
leads to better success in quitting smoking. 
Addiction, 102, 809-814. 


Shimono, K., & Wade N. J. (2002). Monocular 
alignment in different depth planes. Vision 
Research, 42, 1127-1135. 


Shin, A., Zheng, H., & Berthoud, H. (2009). An 
expanded view of energy homeostasis: 
Neural integration of metabolic, cognitive, 
and emotional drives to eat. Physiology & 
Behavior, 97, 572-580. 


Shinn, M., Gottlieb, J., Wett, J. L., Bahl, A., Cohen, 
A., & Baron, E. D. (2007). Predictors of home- 
lessness among older adults in New York 
City: Disability, economic, human and social 
capital and stressful events. Journal of Health 
Psychology, 12, 696-708. 


Shmuel, A., Chaimow, D., Raddatz, G., Ugurbil, 
K., & Yacoub, E. (2010). Mechanisms underly- 
ing decoding at 7 T: Ocular dominance 
columns, broad structures, and macroscopic 
blood vessels in V1 convey information on 
the stimulated eye. Neurolmage, 49, 1957-1964. 


Shoda, Y., & Mischel, W. (2006). Applying meta- 
theory to achieve generalisability and preci- 
sion in personality science. Applied Psychology: 
An International Review, 55, 439-452. 


Shors, T. J. (2009, March). Saving new brain cells. 
Scientific American, pp. 47-54. 


Shulman, J. L., & Horne, S. G. (2006). Guilty or 
not? A path model of women’s sexual force 
fantasies. Journal of Sex Research, 43, 368-377. 


Shultz, S. K., Scherman, A., & Marshall, L. J. (2000). 
Evaluation of a university-based date rape 
prevention program: Effect on attitudes and 
behavior related to rape. Journal of College 
Student Development, 41, 193-201. 


Shurkin, J. N. (1992). Terman’s kids: The ground- 
breaking study of how the gifted grow up. Boston: 
Little, Brown. 

Shweder, R. A. (1994). You’re not sick, you're just 
in love: Emotion as an interpretive system. In 
P. Ekman & R. J. Davidson (Eds.), The nature of 
emotion: Fundamental questions. New York: 
Oxford. 


R-48 References 


Shynkaruk, J. M., & Thompson, V. A. (2006). 
Confidence and accuracy in deductive 
reasoning. Memory & Cognition, 34, 619-632. 


Sidman, M. (2006). The distinction between 
positive and negative reinforcement: Some 
additional considerations. Behavior Analyst, 
29, 135-139. 


Siegel, J. M. (2003, November). Why we sleep. 
Scientific American, pp. 92-97. 

Siegel, R. K., (1989). Intoxication: Life in pursuit of 
artificial paradise. New York: E. P. Dutton. 
Siegert, R. J., & Ward, T. (2002). Clinical psychol- 
ogy and evolutionary psychology: Toward a 

dialogue. Review of General Psychology, 6, 
235-259. 


Siemer, M., Mauss I., & Gross, J. J. (2007). Same 
situation—different emotions: How apprais- 
als shape our emotions. Emotion, 7, 592-600. 


Sifrit, K. J. (2006). The effects of aging and 
cognitive decrements on simulated driving 
performance. Dissertation abstracts interna- 
tional: Section B: The sciences and engineering, 
67, 2863. 


Silva, M. T. A., Goncalves, F. L., & Garcia-Mijares, 
M. (2007). Neural events in the reinforcement 
contingency. Behavior Analyst, 30, 17-30. 


Silverman, K., Roll, J., & Higgins, S. (2008). 
Introduction to the special issue on the 
behavior analysis and treatment of drug 
addiction. Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 
41, 471-480. 


Silverstein, M. L. (2007). Rorschach test findings 
at the beginning of treatment and 2 years 
later, with a 30-year follow-up. Journal of 
Personality Assessment, 88, 131-143. 


Simcock, G., & Hayne, H. (2002). Breaking the 
barrier? Children fail to translate their pre- 
verbal memories into language. Psychological 
Science, 13, 225-231. 


Simon, G., Ludman, E., Uniitzer, J., Operskalski, 
B., & Bauer, M. (2008). Severity of mood 
symptoms and work productivity in people 
treated for bipolar disorder. Bipolar Disorders, 
10, 718-725. 

Simon, S., & Hoyt, C. (2008). Exploring the 
gender gap in support for a woman for 
president. Analyses of Social Issues and Public 
Policy (ASAP), 8, 157-181. 


Simonton, D. K. (2000). Archival research. In A. E. 


Kazdin (Ed.), Encyclopedia of psychology (Vol. 1). 
Washington, DC: American Psychological 
Association. 


Simonton, D. K. (2009). Varieties of (scientific) 
creativity: A hierarchical model of domain- 
specific disposition, development, and 
achievement. Perspectives on Psychological 
Science, 4, 441-452. 


Singer, J. L. (2006). Why imagery, personal mem- 
ories, and daydreams matter. In J. L. Singer, 
Imagery in psychotherapy. Washington, DC: 
American Psychological Association. 

Singh, S., Wulf, D., Samara, R, & Cuca, Y. P. (2000). 
Gender differences in the timing of first 
intercourse: Data from 14 countries. 
International Family Planning Perspectives, 26, 
21-28, 43. 

Sininger, Y. S., & Cone-Wesson, B. (2004, 
September 10). Asymmetric cochlear process- 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


ing mimics hemispheric specialization. 
Science, 305, 1581. 

Sininger, Y. S., & Cone-Wesson, B. (2006). Lateral 
asymmetry in the ABR of neonates: evidence 
and mechanisms. Hearing Research, 212, 
203-211. 


Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal behavior. New York: 
Appleton-Century-Crofts. 


Skinner, B. F. (1975). The steep and thorny road to 
a science of behavior. American Psychologist, 
30, 42-49. 


Skipp, C., & Campo-Flores, A. (2007, September 
3). Taking on Tourette’s. Newsweek, p. 53. 


Skolnick, P., Popik, P., Trullas, R. (2009). 
Glutamate-based antidepressants: 20 years 
on. Trends in Pharmacological Science, 30, 
563-569. 


Slater, E., & Meyer, A. (1959). Contributions to a 
pathography of the musicians: Robert 
Schumann. Confinia Psychiatrica. Reprinted 
in K. R. Jamison, Touched with fire: Manic- 
depressive illness and the artistic temperament. 
New York: Free Press. 


Sleek, S. (1997 June). Can “emotional intelligence” 
be taught in today’s schools? APA Monitor, 
p. 25. 


Sloan, E. P., et al. (1993). The nuts and bolts of 
behavioral therapy for insomnia. Journal of 
Psychosomatic Research, 37 (Suppl.), 19-37. 


Smart, R. G. (2007). Review of introduction to 
addictive behaviours. Addiction, 102, 831. 


Smetana, J. B. (2007). Strategies for understand- 
ing archetypes and the collective unconscious 
of an organization. Dissertation Abstracts 
International Section A: Humanities and Social 
Sciences, 67(12-A), 4714. 


Smetana, J., Daddis, C., & Chuang, S. (2003). 
“Clean your room!” A longitudinal investiga- 
tion of adolescent-parent conflict and conflict 
resolution in middle-class African American 
families. Journal of Adolescent Research, 18, 
631-650. 


Smetana, J. G. (2005). Adolescent-parent conflict: 
Resistance and subversion as developmental 
process. In L. Nucci (Ed.), Conflict, contradic- 
tion, and contrarian elements in moral develop- 
ment and education. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence 
Erlbaum Associates. 


Smith, B. H., Barkley, R. A., & Shapiro, C. J. 
(2006). Attention-Deficit/ Hyperactivity 
Disorder. In E. J. Mash & R. A. Barkley, 
Treatment of childhood disorders (3rd. ed). New 
York: Guilford Press. 


Smith, C. (2006). Symposium V—Sleep and learn- 
ing: New developments [Special issue: 
Methods and learning in functional MRI]. 
Brain and Cognition, 60, 331-332. 


Smith, C. A., & Lazarus, R. S. (2001). Appraisal 
components, core relational themes, and the 
emotions. In W. G. Parrott (Ed.), Emotions in 
social psychology: Essential readings (pp. 
94-114). Philadelphia: Psychology Press. 


Smith, C. D., Chebrolu, J., Wekstein, D. R., 
Schmitt, F. A., & Markesbery, W. R. (2007). 
Age and gender effects on human brain 
anatomy: a voxel-based morphometric study 
in healthy elderly. Neurobiology of Aging, 28, 
1057-1087. 


Smith, E. (1988, May). Fighting cancerous 
feelings. Psychology Today, pp. 22-23. 


Smith, E. R., & Semin, G. R. (2007). Situated social 
cognition. Current Directions in Psychological 
Science, 16, 132-135. 


Smith, L., Cornelius, V., Warnock, A., Bell, A., & 
Young, A. (2007). Effectiveness of mood 
stabilizers and antipsychotics in the mainte- 
nance phase of bipolar disorder: A systematic 
review of randomized controlled trials. 
Bipolar Disorders, 9, 394-412. 


Smith, M. B. (2003). Moral foundations in 
research with human participants. In A. E. 
Kazdin (Ed.), Methodological issues & strategies 
in clinical research (3rd ed.). Washington, DC: 
American Psychological Association. 


Smith, M. L., Glass, G. V., & Miller, T. I. (1980). 
The benefits of psychotherapy. Baltimore: The 
Johns Hopkins University Press. 


Smith, R. A., & Weber, A. L. (2005). Applying 
social psychology in everyday life. In F. W. 
Schneider, J. A. Gruman, & L. M. Coutts, 
Applied social psychology: Understanding and 
addressing social and practical. Thousand Oaks, 
CA: Sage Publications. 


Smith, W. B. (2007). Karen Horney and psycho- 
therapy in the 21st century. Clinical Social 
Work Journal, 35, 57-66. 


Smolowe, J. (2008, June 23). Medical miracle 
surgery for an unborn child. People, p. 96. 


Snyder, D. J., Fast, K., & Bartoshuk, L. M. (2004). 
Valid comparisons of suprathreshold sensa- 
tions. Journal of Consciousness Studies, 11, 
96-112. 


Snyder, J., Cramer, A., & Afrank, J. (2005). The 
contributions of ineffective discipline and 
parental hostile attributions of child misbe- 
havior to the development of conduct prob- 
lems at home and school. Developmental 
Psychology, 41, 30-41. 


Snyder, M. (2002). Applications of Carl Rogers’ 
theory and practice to couple and family 
therapy: A response to Harlene Anderson and 
David Bott. Journal of Family Therapy, 24, 
317-325. 


Sobel, K., Gerrie, M., Poole, B., & Kane, M. (2007, 
October). Individual differences in working 
memory capacity and visual search: The roles 
of top-down and bottom-up processing. 
Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 840-845. 


Society for Personality Assessment. (2005). The 
status of Rorschach in clinical and forensic 
practice: An official statement by the board of 
trustees of the Society for Personality Assess- 
ment. Journal of Personality Assessment, 85, 
219-237. 


Sohr-Preston, S. L., & Scaramella, L. V. (2006). 
Implications of timing of maternal depressive 
symptoms for early cognitive and language 
development. Clinical Child and Family 
Psychology Review, 9, 65-83. 


Sokolove, M. (2003, November 16). Should John 
Hinckley go free? The New York Times Magazine, 
pp. 52-54, 92. 


Soler, J., Pascual, J., Tiana, T., Cebria, A., 
Barrachina, J., Campins, M., et al. (2009). 
Dialectical behaviour therapy skills training 
compared to standard group therapy in bor- 
derline personality disorder: A 3-month 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


randomised controlled clinical trial. Behaviour 
Research and Therapy, 47, 353-358. 


Solomon, M., & Herman, J. (2009). Sex differences 
in psychopathology: Of gonads, adrenals 
and mental illness. Physiology & Behavior, 97, 
250-258. 


Sommer, R., & Sommer, B. (2001). A practical guide 
to behavioral research: Tools and techniques (5th 
ed.). New York: Oxford University Press. 

Sori, C. F. (Ed.). (2006). Engaging children in family 
therapy: Creative approaches to integrating theory 
and research in clinical practice. New York: 
Routledge/Taylor & Francis Group. 


Soussignan, R. (2002). Duchenne smile, emotional 
experience, and automatic reactivity: A test of 
the facial feedback hypothesis. Emotion, 2, 
52-74. 


South, S., & Krueger, R. (2008). An interactionist 
perspective on genetic and environmental 
contributions to personality. Social and 
Personality Psychology Compass, 2, 929-948. 


Spackman, M. P., Fujiki, M., & Brinton, B. (2006). 
Understanding emotions in context: The 
effects of language impairment on children’s 
ability to infer emotional reactions. International 
Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 
41, 173-188. 


Spangler, W. D. (1992). Validity of questionnaire 
and TAT measures of need for achievement: 
Two meta-analyses. Psychological Bulletin, 112, 
140-154. 


Spanos, N. P., Barber, T. X., & Lang, G. (2005). 
Cognition and self-control: Cognitive control 
of painful sensory input. Integrative Phy- 
siological & Behavioral Science, 40, 119-128. 


Sparks, S. D. (2007, October 2). Foundation: 
Gifted poor students given short shrift. 
Education Daily, 40, 3. 


Spearman, C. (1927). The abilities of man. London: 
Macmillan. 


Speirs-Neumeister, K. L., & Finch, H. (2006). 
Perfectionism in high-ability students: 
Relational precursors and influences on 
achievement motivation. Gifted Child 
Quarterly, 50, 238-251. 


Spence-Cochran, K., & Pearl, C. (2006). Moving 
toward full inclusion. In P. Wehman, Life 
beyond the classroom: Transition strategies for 
young people with disabilities (4th ed.). 
Baltimore: Paul H. Brookes Publishing. 


Spence, M. J., & DeCasper, A. J. (1982, March). 
Human fetuses perceive maternal speech. Paper 
presented at the meeting of the International 
Conference on Infant Studies, Austin, TX. 


Spencer, S. J., Fein, S., Zanna, M. P., & Olson, J. M. 
(Eds.) (2003). Motivated social perception: The 
Ontario Symposium (Vol. 9). Mahwah, NJ: 
Erlbaum. 


Spencer-Rodgers, J., Peng, K., Wang, L., & Hou, 
Y. (2004). Dialectical self-esteem and Fast- 
West differences in psychological well-being. 
Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 
1416-1432. 


Sperling, G. (1960). The information available in 
brief visual presentation. Psychological 
Monographs, 74, pp. 29. 


Sperry, R. (1982). Some effects of disconnecting the 
cerebral hemispheres. Science, 217, 1223-1226. 


Spiegel, D. (1996). Hypnosis. In R. E. Hales & 
S. C. Yudofsky (Eds.), The American Psychiatric 
Press synopsis of psychiatry. Washington, DC: 
American Psychiatric Press. 

Spiegel, D. (Ed.). (1999). Efficacy and cost- 
effectiveness of psychotherapy. New York: 
American Psychiatric Press. 


Spielberger, C. D. (2006). Cross-cultural assess- 
ment of emotional states and personality 
traits. European Psychologist, 11, 297-303. 


Spiers, H. J., & Maguire, E. A. (2007). Decoding 
human brain activity during real-world 
experiences. Trends in Cognitive Science, 11, 
356-365. 


Spindler, H., Kruse, C., Zwisler, A., & Pedersen, 
S. (2009). Increased anxiety and depression in 
Danish cardiac patients with a type D person- 
ality: Cross-validation of the Type D Scale 
(DS14). International Journal of Behavioral 
Medicine, 16, 98-107. 


Spinella, M., & Lester, D. (2006). Can money buy 
happiness? Psychological Reports, 99, 992. 


Spitz, H. H. (1987). Problem-solving processes in 
special populations. In J. G. Borkowski & J. D. 
Day (Eds.), Cognition in special children: 
Comparative approaches to retardation, learning 
disabilities, and giftedness. Norwood, NJ: Ablex. 


Spitzer, R. L., Skodol, A. E., Gibbon, M., & 
Williams, J. B. W. (1983). Psychopathology: A 
case book. New York: McGraw-Hill. 


Sprecher, S., & Hatfield, E. (1996). Premarital 
sexual standards among U.S. college students: 
Comparison with Russian and Japanese 
students. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 25, 
261-288. 


Sprecher, S., & Regan, P. C. (2002). Liking some 
things (in some people) more than others: 
Partner preferences in romantic relationships 
and friendships. Journal of Social and Personal 
Relationships, 19, 436-481. 


Sprenger, M. (2007). Memory 101 for educators. 
Thousand Oaks, CA: Corwin Press. 


Sprenkle, D. H., & Moon, S. M. (Eds.). (1996). 
Research methods in family therapy. New York: 
Guilford Press. 

Springen, K. (2004, August 9). Anxiety: Sweet 
and elusive sleep. Newsweek, p. 21. 

Squire, L. R., Clark, R. E., & Bayley, P. J. (2004). 
Medial temporal lobe function and memory. 


In M. S. Gazzaniga (Ed.), Cognitive neurosci- 
ences (3rd ed.). Cambridge, MA: MIT. 


Staddon, J. E. R., & Cerutti, D. T. (2003). Operant 
conditioning. Annual Review of Psychology, 54, 
115-144. 


Staley, J. K., & Sanacora, G., & Tamagnan, G. 
(2006). Sex differences in diencephalon 
serotonin transporter availability in major 
depression. Biological Psychiatry, 59, 40-47. 


Stankov, L. (2003). Complexity in human intelli- 
gence. In R. J. Sternberg, J. Lautrey, et al. 
(Eds.), Models of intelligence: International 
perspectives. Washington, DC: American 
Psychological Association. 

Stanojevic, S., Mitic, K., & Vujic, V. (2007). 
Exposure to acute physical and psychological 
stress alters the response of rat macrophages 
to corticosterone, neuropeptide Y and beta- 


References R-49 


endorphin. International Journal on the Biology 
of Stress, 10, 65-73. 


Stanton, A. L., Danoff-Burg, S., Cameron, C. L., 
Bishop, M., Collins, C. A., Kirk, S. B., et al. 
(2000). Emotionally expressive coping 
predicts psychological and physical adjust- 
ment to breast cancer. Journal of Consulting and 
Clinical Psychology, 68, 875-882. 


Stapel, D. A., & Semin, G. R. (2007). The magic 
spell of language: Linguistic categories and 
their perceptual consequences. Journal of 
Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 23-33. 


Starcevic, V., Berle, D., Milicevic, D., Hannan, 
A., Pamplugh, C., & Eslick, G. D. (2007). 
Pathological worry, anxiety disorders and 
the impact of co-occurrence with depressive 
and other anxiety disorders. Journal of 
Anxiety Disorders, 21, 1016-1027. 


Startup, M., Bucci, S., & Langdon, R. (2009). 
Delusions of reference: A new theoretical 
model. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 14, 110-126. 


St. Dennis, C., Hendryx, M., Henriksen, A. L., Setter, 
S. M., & Singer, B. (2006). Postdischarge treat- 
ment costs following closure of a state gero- 
psychiatric ward: Comparison of 2 levels of 
community care. Primary Care Companion 
Journal of Clinical Psychiatry, 8, 279-284. 


Steblay, N., Dysart, J., Fulero, S., & Lindsay, 
R. C. L. (2003). Eyewitness accuracy rates in 
police showup and lineup presentations: A 
meta-analytic comparison. Law & Human 
Behavior, 27, 523-540. 


Steele, C. M. (1997). A threat in the air: How 
stereotypes shape intellectual identity and 
performance. American Psychologist, 52, 
613-629. 


Steele, C. M., & Josephs, R. A. (1990). Alcohol 
myopia: Its prized and dangerous effects. 
American Psychologist, 45, 921-933. 


Steele, C. M., Spencer, S. J., & Aronson, J. (2002). 
Contending with group image. The psychol- 
ogy of stereotype and social identity threat. In 
M. P. Zanna (Ed.), Advances in experimental 
social psychology (Vol. 34). San Diego: Academic 
Press. 


Steele, J. D., Christmas, D., Eljamel, M. S., & 
Matthews, K. (2007). Anterior cingulotomy 
for major depression: clinical outcome and 
relationship to lesion characteristics. Biological 
Psychiatry, 12, 127-134. 


Stegerwald, F., & Janson, G. R. (2003). Conversion 
therapy: Ethical considerations in family 
counseling. Family Journal—Counseling and 
Therapy for Couples and Families, 11, 55-59. 


Steiger, A. (2007). Neurochemical regulation of 
sleep. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 41, 
537-552. 


Stein, L. A. R., & Graham, J. R. (2005). Ability of 
substance abusers to escape detection on the 
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality Inventory- 
Adolescent (MMPI-A) in a juvenile correc- 
tional facility. Assessment, 12, 28-39. 


Steinberg, L. (2007). Risk taking in adolescence: 
New perspectives from brain and behavioral 
science. Current Directions in Psychological 
Science, 16, 55-59. 


Steiner, J. (2008). Transference to the analyst as an 
excluded observer. The International Journal of 
Psychoanalysis, 89, 39-54. 


R-50 References 


Steinfeld, C., Ellison, N., & Lampe, C. (2008). 
Social capital, self-esteem, and use of online 
social network sites: A longitudinal analysis. 
Journal of Applied Developmental Psychology, 29, 
434—445. 


Stemler, S. E., & Sternberg, R. J. (2006). Using sit- 
uational judgment tests to measure practical 
intelligence. In J. A. Weekley & R. E. Ployhart 
(Eds.), Situational judgment tests: Theory, mea- 
surement, and application. Mahwah, NJ: 
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 


Stemler, S. E., Sternberg, R. J., Grigorenko, E. L., 
Jarvin, L., & Sharpes, K. (2009). Using the the- 
ory of successful intelligence as a framework 
for developing assessments in AP physics. 
Contemporary Educational Psychology, 34, 
195-209. 


Stenbacka, L., & Vanni, S. (2007). fMRI of periph- 
eral visual field representation. Clinical 
Neurophysiology, 108, 1303-1314. 


Stenklev, N. C., & Laukli, E. (2004). Cortical 
cognitive potentials in elderly persons. Journal 
of the American Academy of Audiology, 15, 
401—413. 


Stephenson, R. H., & Banet-Weiser, S. (2007). 
Super-sized kids: Obesity, children, moral panic, 
and the media. In J. A. Bryant, The children’s 
television community. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence 
Erlbaum Associates. 


Stern, E., & Silbersweig, D. A. (2001). Advances in 
functional neuroimaging methodology for the 
study of brain systems underlying human neu- 
ropsychological function and dysfunction. In 
D. A. Silbersweig & E. Stern (Eds.), Neuropsy- 
chology and functional neuroimaging: Convergence, 
advances and new directions. Amsterdam, 
Netherlands: Swets and Zeitlinger. 


Stern, R. M., & Koch, K. L. (1996). Motion sickness 
and differential susceptibility. Current Directions 
in Psychological Science, 5, 115-120. 


Sternberg, R. J. (1990). Metaphors of mind: Concep- 
tions of the nature of intelligence. New York: 
Cambridge University Press. 


Sternberg, R. J. (1998). Successful intelligence: How 
practical and creative intelligence determine 
success in life. New York: Plume. 


Sternberg, R. J. (2000). The Holey Grail of general 
intelligence. Science, 289, 399-401. 


Sternberg, R. J. (2000). Intelligence and wisdom. 
In R. J. Sternberg et al. (Eds.), Handbook of 
intelligence. New York: Cambridge University 
Press. 


Sternberg, R. J. (2002). Individual differences in 
cognitive development. In U. Goswami (Ed.), 
Blackwell handbook of childhood cognitive devel- 
opment. Blackwell handbooks of developmental 
psychology. Malden, MA: Blackwell. 


Sternberg, R. J. (2004). A triangular theory of love. 
In H. T. Reis & C. E. Rusbult (Eds.), Close 
relationships: Key readings. Philadelphia, PA: 
Taylor & Francis. 

Sternberg, R. J. (2005). Culture and measurement. 
Measurement: Interdisciplinary Research and 
Perspectives, 3, 108-113. 


Sternberg, R. J. (2006). A duplex theory of love. In 
R. J. Sternberg, Ed., The new psychology of love. 
New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. 

Sternberg, R. J. (2007). Who are the bright chil- 
dren? The cultural context of being and 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


acting intelligent. Educational Researcher, 36, 
148-155. 


Sternberg, R. J., & Beall, A. E. (1991). How can we 
know what love is? An epistemological 
analysis. In G. J. O. Fletcher & F. D. Fincham 
(Eds.), Cognition in close relationships. Hillsdale, 
NJ: Erlbaum. 


Sternberg, R. J., Grigorenko, E. L., & Kidd, K. K. 
(2005). Intelligence, race, and genetics. 
American Psychologist, 60, 46-59. 


Sternberg, R. J., & Hedlund, J. (2002). Practical 
intelligence, “g,” and work psychology. 
Human Performance, 15, 143-160. 


Sternberg, R. J., Hojjat, M., & Barnes, M. L. (2001). 
Empirical aspects of a theory of love as a 
story. European Journal of Personality, 15, 
1-20. 


Sternberg, R. J., & Jarvin, L. (2003). Alfred Binet’s 
contributions as a paradigm for impact in 
psychology. In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), The 
anatomy of impact: What makes the great works of 
psychology great. Washington, DC: American 
Psychological Association. 


Sternberg, R. J., Kaufman, J. C., & Pretz, J. E. (2004). 
A propulsion model of creative leadership 
[Special issue: Creativity in the workplace]. 
Creativity and Innovation Management, 13, 
145-153. 


Sternberg, R. J., & O'Hara, L. A. (2000). Intelligence 
and creativity. In R. Sternberg et al. (Eds.), 
Handbook of intelligence. New York: Cambridge 
University Press. 


Sternberg, R. J., & Pretz, J. E. (2005). Cognition and 
intelligence: Identifying the mechanisms of the 
mind. New York: Cambridge University Press. 


Stettler, N., Stallings, V. A., Troxel, A. B., Zhao, 
J. Z., Schinnar, R., Nelson, S. E., et al. (2005). 
Weight gain in the first week of life and 
overweight in adulthood. Circulation, 111, 
1897-1903. 


Stevens, C. F. (1979, September). The neuron. 
Scientific American, p. 56. 


Stevens, C., Lauinger, B., Neville, H. (2009). 
Differences in the neural mechanisms of selec- 
tive attention in children from different socio- 
economic backgrounds: An event-related 
brain potential study. Developmental Science, 12, 
634—646. 


Stevens, G., & Gardner, S. (1982). The women of 
psychology: Pioneers and innovators (Vol. 1). 
Cambridge, MA: Schenkman. 


Stevens, M. C., Skudlarski, P., Pearlson, G. D., & 
Calhoun, V. D. (2009). Age-related cognitive 
gains are mediated by the effects of white 
matter development on brain network 
integration. Neuroimage, 48, 738-746. 


Stevens, M. J., & Gielen, U.P. (Eds). (2007). Toward 
a global psychology: Theory, research, interven- 
tion, and pedagogy. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence 
Erlbaum. 


Stevens, P., & Harper, D. J. (2007). Professional 
accounts of electroconvulsive therapy: A 
discourse analysis. Social Science & Medicine, 
64, 1475-1486. 

Stevens, S. S., & Pashler, H. E. (2002). Steven's 
handbook of experimental psychology: Learning, 
motivation, and emotion. New York: Wiley. 

Stevenson, H. W., Lee, S., & Mu, X. (2000). 
Successful achievement in mathematics: 


China and the United States. In C. F. M. van 
Lieshout & P. G. Heymans (Eds.), Developing 
talent across the life span. New York: 
Psychology Press. 


Stevenson, R. J., & Case, T. I. (2005). Olfactory 
imagery: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin and 
Review, 12, 244-264. 


Stickgold, R., Hobson, J. A., Fosse, R., & Fosse, M. 
(2001, November 2). Sleep, learning, and 
dreams: Off-line memory reprocessing. 
Science, 294, 1052-1057. 


Stickgold, R., & Wehrwein, P. (2009, April 27). Sleep 
now, remember later. Newsweek, pp. 56-57. 


Stickley, T., & Nickeas, R. (2006). Becoming one 
person: Living with dissociative identity dis- 
order. Journal of Psychiatric and Mental Health 
Nursing, 13, 180-187. 


Stix, G. (2008, November). Jacking into the brain. 
Scientific American, pp. 56—61. 


St. Jacques, P. L., & Levine, B. (2007). Ageing and 
autobiographical memory for emotional and 
neutral events. Memory, 15, 129-144. 


Stocks, E., Lishner, D., & Decker, S. (2009). 
Altruism or psychological escape: Why does 
empathy promote prosocial behavior? 
European Journal of Social Psychology, 39, 
649-665. 


Stockton, R., Morran, D. K., & Krieger, K. (2004). 
An overview of current research and best 
practices for training beginning group lead- 
ers. In J. L. DeLucia-Waack, D. A. Gerrity, 

C. R. Kalodner, & M. T. Riva (Eds.), Handbook 
of group counseling and psychotherapy. Thousand 
Oaks, CA: Sage Publications. 


Stone, J. (2002). Battling doubt by avoiding prac- 
tice: The effects of stereotype threat on self- 
handicapping in white athletes. Personality 
and Social Psychology Bulletin, 28, 1667-1678. 


Stone, J., Morrison, P., & Pilowsky, L. (2007). 
Glutamate and dopamine dysregulation in 
schizophrenia—A synthesis and selective 
review. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 21, 
440-452. 


Storm, L., & Ertel, S. (2001). Does psi exist? 
Comments on Milton and Wiseman’s (1999) 
meta-analysis of Ganzfeld’s research. Psycho- 
logical Bulletin, 127, 424—433. 


Stouffer, E. M., & White, N. M. (2006). Neural cir- 
cuits mediating latent learning and condition- 
ing for salt in the rat. Neurobiology of Learning 
and Memory, 86, 91-99. 


Strange, D., Clifasefi, S., & Garry, M. (2007). False 
memories. In M. Garry & H. Hayne, Do justice 
and let the sky fall: Elizabeth Loftus and her con- 
tributions to science, law, and academic freedom. 
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 

Strathern, A., & Stewart, P. J. (2003). Landscape, 
memory and history: Anthropological perspec- 
tives. London: Pluto Press. 

Strauss, E. (1998, May 8). Writing, speech sepa- 
rated in split brain. Science, 280, 287. 

Strayer, D. L., & Drews, F. A. (2007). Cell-phone- 
induced driver distraction. Current Directions 
in Psychological Science, 16, 128-131. 

Strayer, D. L., Drews, F. A., Crouch, D. J., & 
Johnston, W. A. (2005). Why do cell phone 
conversations interfere with driving? In W. R. 
Walker and D. Herrmann (Eds.), Cognitive 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


technology: Transforming thought and society. 
Jefferson, NC: McFarland & Company. 


Striano, T., & Vaish, A. (2006). Seven- to 9-month- 
old infants use facial expressions to interpret 
others’ actions. British Journal of Developmental 
Psychology, 24, 753-760. 


Striegel-Moore, R., & Bulik, C. M. (2007). Risk 
factors for eating disorders. American 
Psychologist, 62, 181-198. 


Stroink, M. (2007). Processes and preconditions 
underlying terrorism in second-generation 
immigrants. Peace and Conflict: Journal of Peace 
Psychology, 13, 293-312. 


Strong, T., & Tomm, K. (2007). Family therapy as 
re-coordinating and moving on together. 
Journal of Systemic Therapies, 26, 42-54. 


Strupp, H. H., & Binder, J. L. (1992). Current 
developments in psychotherapy. The 
Independent Practitioner, 12, 119-124. 


Subrahmanyam, K., Reich, S., Waechter, N., & 
Espinoza, G. (2008). Online and offline social 
networks: Use of social networking sites by 
emerging adults. Journal of Applied Develop- 
mental Psychology, 29, 420-433. 


Sue, D. W., & Sue, D. (1990). Counseling the 
culturally different: Theory and practice (2nd 
ed.). Oxford, England: John Wiley & Sons. 


Sue, D. W., Sue, D., & Sue, S. (1990). Under- 
standing abnormal behavior (3rd ed.). Boston: 
Houghton-Mifflin. 


Suh, E. M. (2002). Culture, identity consistency, 
and subjective well-being. Journal of Personality 
& Social Psychology, 83, 1378-1391. 


Suhail, K., & Chaudhry, H. R. (2004). Predictors 
of subjective well-being in an Eastern Muslim 
culture. Journal of Social and Clinical Psychology, 
23, 359-376. 


Suizzo, M-A., & Bornstein, M. H. (2006). French 
and European American child-mother play: 
Culture and gender considerations. Interna- 
tional Journal of Behavioral Development, 30, 
498-508. 


Sullivan, J., Riccio, C., & Reynolds, C. (2008, 
September). Variations in students’ school- 
and teacher-related attitudes across gender, 
ethnicity, and age. Journal of Instructional 
Psychology, 35, 296-305. 


Summers, M. (2000) Everything in its place. New 
York: Putnam. 


Sunder, J. M., Borren, I., & Tambs, K. (2008). The 
Flynn effect is partly caused by changing fer- 
tility patterns. Intelligence, 36, 183-191. 


Super, C. M. (1980). Cognitive development: 
Looking across at growing up. In C. M. Super 
& S. Harakness (Eds.), New directions for child 
development: Anthropological perspectives on 
child development. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. 


Surette, R. (2002). Self-reported copycat crime 
among a population of serious and violent 
juvenile offenders. Crime & Delinquency, 48, 
46-69. 

Susser, E. S., Herman, D. B., & Aaron, B. (2002, 
August). Combating the terror of terrorism. 
Scientific American, pp. 70-77. 

Sutin, A. R., & Robins, R. W. (2007). Phenome- 
nology of autobiographical memories: The 


Memory Experiences Questionnaire. Memory, 
15, 390-411. 


Svarstad, B. (1976). Physician-patient communi- 
cation and patient conformity with medical 
advice. In D. Mechanic (Ed.), The growth of 
bureaucratic medicine. New York: Wiley. 


Svartdal, F. (2003). Extinction after partial rein- 
forcement: Predicted vs. judged persistence. 
Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 44, 55-64. 


Swain, P. I. (2006). New developments in eating 
disorders research. Hauppauge, NY: Nova 
Science Publishers. 


Swales, M. A., & Heard, H. L. (2007). The therapy 
relationship in dialectical behaviour therapy. 
In P. Gilbert & R. L. Leahy, The therapeutic 
relationship in the cognitive behavioral psycho- 
therapies. New York: Routledge/ Taylor & 
Francis. 


Swann, W. B., Jr., Chang-Schneider, C., & Larsen 
McClarty, K. (2007). Do people’s self-views 
matter? Self-concept and self-esteem in every- 
day life. American Psychologist, 62, 84-94. 


Swanson, H. L., Harris, K. R., & Graham, S. (Eds.). 
(2003). Handbook of learning disabilities. New 
York: Guilford Press. 


Swets, J. A., & Bjork, R.A. (1990). Enhancing 
human performance: An evaluation of “new 
age” techniques considered by the U.S. Army. 
Psychological Science, 1, 85-96. 


Swing, E., & Anderson, C. (2007). The unintended 
negative consequences of exposure to violent 
video games. International Journal of Cognitive 
Technology, 12, 3-13. 


Szasz, T. (2006). The pretense of psychology as 
science: The myth of mental illness in statu 
nascendi. Current Psychology: Developmental, 
Learning, Personality, Social, 25, 42-49. 


Szasz, T. S. (1994). Cruel compassion: Psychiatric 
control of society's unwanted. New York: Wiley. 


Szegedy Maszak, M. (2003, January 13). The 
sound of unsound minds. U.S. News & World 
Report, pp. 45-46. 


Tadmor, C., T. (2007). Biculturalism: The plus 
side of leaving home? The effects of second- 
culture exposure on integrative complexity 
and its consequences for overseas perfor- 
mance. Dissertation Abstracts International 
Section A: Humanities and Social Sciences, 
67(8-A), 3068. 

Taggi, F., Crenca, A., Cedri, C., Giustini, M., Dosi, 
G., & Marturano, P. (2007). Road safety and the 
tsunami of cell phones. Ann Ig, 19, 269-274. 


Tajfel, H., & Turner, J. C. (2004). The social iden- 
tity theory of intergroup behavior. In J. T. Jost 
& J. Sidanius (Eds.), Political psychology: Key 
readings. New York: Psychology Press. 

Takahashi, M., Nakata, A., Haratani, T., Ogawa, 
Y., & Arito, H. (2004). Post-lunch nap as a 
worksite intervention to promote alertness on 
the job. Ergonomics, 47, 1003-1013. 


Takizawa, T., Kondo, T., & Sakihara, S. (2007). 
Stress buffering effects of social support on 
depressive symptoms in middle age: Recipro- 
city and community mental health: Corrigen- 
dum. Psychiatry and Clinical Neurosciences, 61, 
336-337. 


Talarico, J. (2009). Freshman flashbulbs: Memories 
of unique and first-time events in starting 
college. Memory, 17, 256-265. 


References R-51 


Talarico, J., & Rubin, D. (2007). Flashbulb memo- 
ries are special after all; in phenomenology, 
not accuracy. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 21, 
557-578. 


Talmi, D., Anderson, A., Riggs, L., Caplan, J., & 
Moscovitch, M. (2008). Immediate memory 
consequences of the effect of emotion on 
attention to pictures. Learning & Memory, 15, 
172-182. 


Tal-Or, N., & Papirman, Y. (2007). The fundamen- 
tal attribution error in attributing fictional 
figures’ characteristics to the actors. Media 
Psychology, 9, 331-345. 


Talukdar, S., & Shastri, J. (2006). Contributory 
and adverse factors in social development of 
young children. Psychological Studies, 51, 
294-303. 


Tan, L., Chan, A., Kay, P., Khong, P., Yip, L., & 
Luke, K. (2008). Language affects patterns of 
brain activation associated with perceptual 
decision. PNAS Proceedings of the National 
Academy of Sciences of the United States of 
America, 105(10), 4004-4009. 


Tan, L., & Ward, G. (2008). Rehearsal in immedi- 
ate serial recall. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 
15, 535-542. 


Tanner, J. M. (1978). Education and physical growth 
(2nd ed.). New York: International Universities 
Press. 


Tanner, J. M. (1990). Foetus into man: Physical 
growth from conception to maturity (rev. ed.). 
Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 


Taras, H., & Potts-Datema, W. (2005). Chronic 
health conditions and student performance at 
school. Journal of School Health, 75, 255-266. 


Tasker, F. (2005). Lesbian mothers, gay fathers, 
and their children: A review. Journal of Develop- 
mental and Behavioral Pediatrics, 26, 224—240. 


Taylor, F., & Bryant, R. A. (2007). The tendency to 
suppress, inhibiting thoughts, and dream 
rebound. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 45, 
163-168. 

Taylor, S. (2003). Anxiety sensitivity and its impli- 
cations for understanding and treating PTSD. 
Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 17, 179-186. 


Taylor, S. E. (1995). Quandary at the crossroads: 
Paternalism versus advocacy surrounding end- 
of-treatment decisions. American Journal of 
Hospital Palliatory Care, 12, 43-46. 


Taylor, S. E., Kemeny, M. E., Reed, G. M., Bower, 
J. E., & Gruenewald, T. L. (2000). Psycholo- 
gical resources, positive illusions, and health. 
American Psychologist, 55, 99-109. 


Teff, K. L., Petrova, M., & Havel, P. J. (2007). 48-h 
Glucose infusion in humans: Effect on hor- 
monal responses, hunger and food intake. 
Physiology & Behavior, 90, 733-743. 


Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., 
Wilcox, K. J., Segal, N. L., & Rich, S. (1988). 
Personality similarity in twins reared apart 
and together. Journal of Personality and Social 
Psychology, 54, 1031-1039. 


Templer, D. I., & Arkawa, H. (2006). Association 
of race and color with mean IQ across 
nations. Psychological Reports, 99, 191-196. 


Tenenbaum, H. R., & Leaper, C. (2002). Are 
parents’ gender schemas related to their 
children’s gender-related cognitions? A 


R-52 References 


meta-analysis. Developmental Psychology, 38, 
615-630. 


Tenenbaum, H. R., & Ruck, M. D. (2007). Are 
teachers’ expectations different for racial 
minority than for European American 
students? A meta-analysis. Journal of 
Educational Psychology, 99, 253-273. 


Tenopyr, M. L. (2002). Theory versus reality: 
Evaluation of ‘g’ in the workplace. Human 
Performance, 15, 107-122. 


Teodorov, E., Salzgerber, S. A., Felicio, L. F., 
Varolli, F. M. F., & Bernardi, M. M. (2002). 
Effects of perinatal picrotoxin and sexual 
experience on heterosexual and homosexual 
behavior in male rats. Neurotoxicology and 
Teratology, 24, 235-245. 


Terry, W. S. (2003). Learning and memory: Basic 
principles, processes, and procedures (2nd ed.). 
Boston: Allyn & Bacon. 


Thachil, A. F., Mohan, R., & Bhugra, D. (2007). 
The evidence base of complementary and 
alternative therapies in depression. Journal of 
Affective Disorders, 97, 23-35. 


Tharp, R. G. (1989). Psychocultural variables and 
constants: Effects on teaching and learning in 
schools [Special issue: Children and their 
development: Knowledge base, research 
agenda, and social policy application]. 
American Psychologist, 44, 349-359. 

Thatcher, D. L., & Clark, D. B. (2006). Adolescent 
alcohol abuse and dependence: Development, 
diagnosis, treatment and outcomes. Current 
Psychiatry Reviews, 2, 159-177. 


Thomas, P., Mathur, P., Gottesman, I. I., Nagpal, 
R., Nimgaonkar, V. L., & Deshpande, S. N. 
(2007). Correlates of hallucinations in 
schizophrenia: A cross-cultural evaluation. 
Schizophrenia Research, 92, 41—49. 


Thompson, J. (2000, June 18). “I was certain, but I 
was wrong.” The New York Times, p. E14. 


Thorkildsen, T. A. (2006). An empirical explora- 
tion of language and thought. 
PsycCRITIQUES, 51, no pagination specified. 


Thorndike, E. L. (1932). The fundamentals of 
learning. New York: Teachers College. 


Thornton, A., & McAuliffe, K. (2006, July 14). 
Teaching in wild meerkats. Science, 313, 
227-229. 


Thornton, A., & Young-DeMarco, L. (2001). Four 
decades of trends in attitudes toward family 
issues in the United States: The 1960s through 
the 1990s. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 
63, 1009-1017. 


Thrash, T. M., & Elliot, A. J. (2002). Implicit and 
self-attributed achievement motives: Concor- 
dance and predictive validity. Journal of 
Personality, 70, 729-755. 


Time. (1966, July 22). Less pain, more gain. Time. 


Tippin, J., Sparks, J., & Rizzo, M. (2009, August). 
Visual vigilance in drivers with obstructive 
sleep apnea. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 
67, 143-151. 


Tirri, K., & Nokelainen, P. (2008). Identification of 
multiple intelligences with the Multiple 
Intelligence Profiling Questionnaire III 
[Special issue: High-ability assessment]. 
Psychology Science, 50, 206-221. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Titone, D. A. (2002). Memories bound: The neuro- 
science of dreams. Trends in Cognitive Science, 
6,4-5. 

Todorov, A., & Duchaine, B. (2008). Reading 
trustworthiness in faces without recognizing 
faces. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 25, 1-16. 


Tolman, E. C., & Honzik, C. H. (1930). Intro- 
duction and removal of reward and maze 
performance in rats. University of California 
Publications in Psychology, 4, 257-275. 


Tommasi, L. (2009). Mechanisms and functions 
of brain and behavioural asymmetries. 
Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B, 
364, 855-859. 


Tonidandel, S., Quinones, M. A., & Adams, A. A. 
(2002). Computer-adaptive testing: The 
impact of test characteristics on perceived 
performance and test takers’ reactions. Journal 
of Applied Psychology, 87, 320-332. 


Tononi, G., & Koch, C. (2008). The neural 
correlates of consciousness: An update. In 
A. Kingstone & M. B. Miller (Eds.), The year in 
cognitive neuroscience. Malden, MA: Blackwell 
Publishing. 


Toole, L. M., DeLeon, I. G., Kahng, S., Ruffin, 
G. E., Pletcher, C. A., & Bowman, L. G. (2004). 
Re-evaluation of constant versus varied 
punishers using empirically derived conse- 
quences. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 
25, 577-586. 


Toth, J. P., & Daniels, K. A. (2002). Effects of prior 
experience on judgments of normative word 
frequency: Automatic bias and correction. 
Journal of Memory and Language, 46, 845-874. 


Touhara, K. (2007). Molecular biology of peptide 
pheromone production and reception in mice. 
Advanced Genetics, 59, 147-171. 


Tracy, J. L., & Robins, R. W. (2004). Show your 
pride: Evidence for a discrete emotion 
expression. Psychological Science, 15, 194-197. 


Tranter, L. J., Koutstaal, W. (2008). Age and flexi- 
ble thinking: An experimental demonstration 
of the beneficial effects of increased cogni- 
tively stimulating activity on fluid intelli- 
gence in healthy older adults. Neuropsychology 
and Cognition, 15, 184-207. 


Travis, F. (2006). From I to I: Concepts of self on 
a object-referral/self-referral continuum. In 
A. P. Prescott, The concept of self in psychology. 
Hauppauge, NY: Nova Science Publishers. 


Travis, F., et al. (2009, February). Effects of 
transcendental meditation practice on brain 
functioning and stress reactivity in college 
students. International Journal of Psychophy- 
siology, 71, 170-176. 


Tremblay, A. (2004). Dietary fat and body weight 
set point. Nutrition Review, 62(7, Pt 2), S75-S77. 


Triesch, J., Jasso, H., & Deak, G. O. (2007). 
Emergence of mirror neurons in a model of 
gaze following. Adaptive Behavior, 15, 149-165. 


Tropp, L. R., & Bianchi, R. A. (2006). Valuing 
diversity and interest in intergroup contact. 
Journal of Social Issues, 62, 533-551. 


Tropp, L. R., & Pettigrew, T. F. (2005). Differential 
relationships between intergroup contact and 
affective and cognitive dimensions of preju- 
dice. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 
31, 1145-1158. 


Troyer, A. K., Hafliger, A., & Cadieux, M. J. 
(2006). Name and face learning in older 
adults: Effects of level of processing, self- 
generation, and intention to learn. Journals of 
Gerontology: Series B: Psychological Sciences and 
Social Sciences, 61, P67—P74. 


Trudel, G. (2002). Sexuality and marital life: 
Results of a survey. Journal of Sex and Marital 
Therapy, 28, 229-249. 


Trull, T. J., & Widiger, T. A. (2003). Personality 
disorders. In. G. Stricker, T. A. Widiger, et al. 
(Eds.), Handbook of psychology: Clinical psychol- 
ogy (Vol. 8). New York: Wiley. 


Tsai, A., Yang, M., Lan, C., & Chen, C. (2008). 
Evaluation of effect of cognitive intervention 
programs for the community-dwelling 
elderly with subjective memory complaints. 
International Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 23, 
1172-1174. 


Tsai, K. J., Tsai, Y. C., & Shen, C. K. (2007). GCSF 
rescues the memory impairment of animal 
models of Alzheimer’s disease. Journal of 
Experimental Medicine, 11, 1273-1289. 


Tsaousis, I., Nikolaou, I., & Serdaris, N. (2007). 
Do the core self-evaluations moderate the 
relationship between subjective well-being 
and physical and psychological health? 
Personality and Individual Differences, 42, 
1441-1452. 


Tseng, W. S. (2003). Clinician’s guide to cultural 
psychiatry. San Diego, CA: Elsevier 
Publishing. 

Tsukasaki, T., & Ishii, K. (2004). Linguistic-cultural 
relativity of cognition: Rethinking the Sapir- 
Whorf hypothesis. Japanese Psychological 
Review, 47, 173-186. 


Tsunoda, T. (1985). The Japanese brain: Uniqueness 
and universality. Tokyo: Taishukan Publishing. 


Tucker Blackwell, V.G. (2006). Factors which 
influence the academic motivation and disen- 
gagement of adolescent, African American 
males within a social-historical and psycho- 
logical context. Dissertation Abstracts Inter- 
national, 67, 1654A. 


Tucker, C. M., & Herman, K. C. (2002). Using 
culturally sensitive theories and research to 
meet the academic needs of low-income 
African American children. American Psycho- 
logist, 57, 762-773. 

Tuerlinckx, F., De Boeck, P., & Lens, W. (2002). 
Measuring needs with the Thematic Apper- 
ception Test: A psychometric study. Journal 
of Personality and Social Psychology, 82, 
448-461. 


Tugay, N., et al. (2007). Effectiveness of transcuta- 
neous electrical nerve stimulation and 
interferential current in primary dysmenor- 
thea. Pain Medicine, 8, 295-300. 


Tulving, E. (2000). Concepts of memory. In E. 
Tulving, F. I. M. Craik, et al. (Eds.). The Oxford 
handbook of memory. New York: Oxford 
University Press. 

Tulving, E. (2002). Episodic memory and common 
sense: How far apart? In A. Baddeley & J. P. 
Aggleton (Eds.), Episodic memory: New directions 
in research. London: Oxford University Press. 

Tulving, E., & Psotka, J. (1971). Retroactive 
inhibition in free recall: Inaccessibility of 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


information available in the memory store. 
Journal of Experimental Psychology, 87, 1-8. 


Tulving, E., & Thompson, D. M. (1983). Encoding 
specificity and retrieval processes in episodic 
memory. Psychological Review, 80, 352-373. 


Turk, D. C. (1994). Perspectives on chronic pain: 
The role of psychological factors. Current 
Directions in Psychological Science, 3, 45-49. 


Turkel, R. A. (2002). From victim to heroine: 
Children’s stories revisited. Journal of the 
American Academy of Psychoanalysis, 30, 
71-81. 


Turkewitz, G. (1993). The origins of differential 
hemispheric strategies for information 
processing in the relationships between voice 
and face perception. In B. de Boysson-Bardies, 
S. de Schonen, P. W. Jusczyk, P. McNeilage, & 
J. Morton (Eds.), Developmental neurocognition: 
Speech and face processing in the first year of life. 
NATO ASI series D: Behavioural and social 
sciences (Vol. 69). Dordrecht, Netherlands: 
Kluwer Academic. 


Turnbull, O., & Solms, M. (2007). Awareness, 
desire, and false beliefs: Freud in the light 
of modern neuropsychology. Cortex, 43, 
1083-1090. 


Turner, M. E., Pratkanis, A. R., & Struckman, 
C. K. (2007). Groupthink as social identity 
maintenance. In C. K. Struckman, The science 
of social influence: Advances and future progress. 
New York: Psychology Press. 


Turner, M., Tamborini, R., Limon, M., & Zuckerman- 
Hyman, C. (2007). The moderators and 
mediators of door-in-the-face requests: Is it 
a negotiation or a helping experience? 
Communication Monographs, 74, 333-356. 


Turner, W. J. (1995). Homosexuality, Type 1: An 
Xq28 phenomenon. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 
24, 109-134. 


Tuszynski, M. H. (2007). Nerve growth factor gene 
therapy in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimer’s 
Disease and Associated Disorders, 21, 179-189. 


Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1987). Rational 
choice and the framing of decisions. In 
R. Hogarth & M. Reder (Eds.), Rational choice: 
The contrast between economics and psychology. 
Chicago: University of Chicago Press. 


Tydgat, I., & Grainger, J. (2009). Serial position 
effects in the identification of letters, digits, 
and symbols. Journal of Experimental Psychology: 
Human Perception and Performance, 35, 480-498. 


Ubell, E. (1993, January 10). Could you use more 
sleep? Parade, pp. 16-18. 


Ullman, S. E., & Townsend, S. M. (2007). Barriers 
to working with sexual assault survivors: A 
qualitative study of rape crisis center work- 
ers. Violence Against Women, 13, 412-443. 


Umphress, E. E., Smith-Crowe, K., & Brief, A. P. 
(2007). When birds of a feather flock together 
and when they do not: Status composition, 
social dominance orientation, and organiza- 
tional attractiveness. Journal of Applied 
Psychology, 92, 396-409. 


UNAIDS. (December, 2007). 07 AIDS epidemic 
update. Geneva, Switzerland: United Nations 
Programme on HIV/AIDS. 


Unal-Karagiiven, M. (2009). Psychological impact 
of an economic crisis: A Conservation of 
Resources approach. International Journal of 
Stress Management, 16, 177-194. 


Underwood, A. (2005, October 3). The good 
heart. Newsweek, p. 49. 


University of Chicago. (2009). MSTP recent PhD 
theses. Retrieved from http://pritzker 
-uchicago.edu/jointdegrees /mstp /theses 
shtml 


Unsworth, N., & Engle, R. W. (2005). Individual 
differences in working memory capacity and 
learning: Evidence from the serial reaction 
time task. Memory and Cognition, 33, 213-220. 


Updegraff, K. A., Helms, H. M., McHale, S. M., 
Crouter, A. C., Thayer, S. M., & Sales, L. H. 
(2004). Who’s the boss? Patterns of perceived 
control in adolescents’ friendships. Journal of 
Youth & Adolescence, 33, 403—420. 


Ursprung, W. W., Sanouri, A., & DiFranza, J. R. 
(2009). The loss of autonomy over smoking in 
relation to lifetime cigarette consumption. 
Addictive Behaviors, 22, 12-19. 


U.S. Bureau of the Census. (2000). Census 2000. 
Retrieved from American Fact Finder 
http://factfinder.census.gov/servlet/ 
BasicFactsServlet 


U.S. Bureau of the Census. (2001). Living arrange- 
ments of children. Washington, DC: Author. 


U.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics. (2009). Women’s 
weekly earnings as a percentage of men’s earnings. 
Washington, DC: U.S. Bureau of Labor 
Statistics. 


U.S. Senate Select Committee on Intelligence 
(2004, July 9). Report of the U.S. intelligence 
community's prewar intelligence assessments on 
Iraq. Retrieved from http:/ /www.gpoaccess. 
gov /serialset/creports /iraq.html 


Uttl, B., Graf, P., & Cosentino, S. (2003). Implicit 
memory for new associations: Types of con- 
ceptual representations. In J. S. Bowers & C. J. 
Marsolek (Eds.), Rethinking implicit memory. 
London: Oxford University Press. 


Uylings, H. B. M. (2006). Development of the 
human cortex and the concept of ‘critical’ or 
‘sensitive’ periods. Language Learning, 56, 
59-90. 


Vaillant, G. E., & Vaillant, C. O. (1990). Natural 
history of male psychological health: XII. A 
46-year study of predictors of successful 
aging at age 65. American Journal of Psychiatry, 
147, 31-37. 


Vaitl, D., Schienle, A., & Stark, R. (2005). Neuro- 
biology of fear and disgust. International 
Journal of Psychophysiology, 57, 1-4. 


Valencia, R. R., & Suzuki, L. A. (2003). Intelligence 
testing and minority students: Foundations, 
performance factors, and assessment issues. 
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 


Valenzuela, S., Park, N., & Kee, K. F. (2009). Is 
there social capital in a social network site? 
Facebook use and college students’ life 
satisfaction, trust, and participation. Journal of 
Computer-Mediated Communications, 14, 
875-901. 


Valsiner, J., Diriwachter, R., & Sauck, C. (2005). 
Diversity in unity: Standard questions and 
nonstandard interpretations. In Science and 


References R-53 


medicine in dialogue: Thinking through particu- 
lars and universals. Westport, CT: Praeger 
Publishers/Greenwood Publishing Group. 


Vanasse, A., Niyonsenga, T., & Courteau, J. (2004). 
Smoking cessation within the context of 
family medicine: Which smokers take action? 
Preventive Medicine: An International Journal 
Devoted to Practice and Theory, 38, 330-337. 


Van Beekum, S. (2005). The therapist as a new 
object. Transactional Analysis Journal, 35, 
187-191. 


Van De Graaff, K. (2000). Human anatomy (5th 
ed.). Boston: McGraw-Hill. 


Vandell, D. L., Burchinal, M. R., Belsky, J., Owen, 
M. T., Friedman, S. L., Clarke-Stewart, et al. 
(2005). Early child care and children’s develop- 
ment in the primary grades: Follow-up results 
from the NICHD Study of Early Child Care. 
Paper presented at the biennial meeting of the 
Society for Research in Child Development, 
Atlanta, GA. 


Van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., & Van der Molen, 
M. W. (2004). Developmental trends in simple 
and selective inhibition of compatible and 
incompatible responses. Journal of Experi- 
mental Child Psychology, 87, 201-220. 


van der Helm, P. A. (2006). Review of perceptual 
dynamics: Theoretical foundations and philo- 
sophical implications of gestalt psychology. 
Philosophical Psychology, 19, 274-279. 


Vandervert, L. R., Schimpf, P. H., & Liu, H. (2007). 
How working memory and the cerebellum 
collaborate to produce creativity and innova- 
tion. Creativity Research Journal, 19, 1-18. 


Vanheule, S., Desmet, M., Rosseel, Y., & Meganck, 
R. (2006). Core transference themes in depres- 
sion. Journal of Affective Disorders, 91, 71-75. 


van Hooren, S. A. H., Valentijn, A. M., & Bosma, 
H. (2007). Cognitive functioning in healthy 
older adults aged 64-81: A cohort study into 
the effects of age, sex, and education. Aging, 
Neuropsychology, and Cognition, 14, 40-54. 


van Marle, K., & Wynn, K. (2009). Infants’ 
auditory enumeration: Evidence for analog 
magnitudes in the small number range. 
Cognition, 111, 302-316. 


van Oort, R., & Kessels, R. (2009). Executive dys- 
function in Korsakoff’s syndrome: Time to 
revise the DSM criteria for alcohol-induced 
persisting amnestic disorder? International 
Journal of Psychiatry in Clinical Practice, 13, 78-81. 


Van Overwalle, F., & Siebler, F. (2005). A connec- 
tionist model of attitude formation and 
change. Personality and Social Psychology 
Review, 9, 231-274. 


Vansteenkiste, M., Simons, J., Lens, W., Soenens, 
B., & Matos, L. (2005). Examining the motiva- 
tional impact of intrinsic versus extrinsic goal 
framing and autonomy-supportive versus 
internally controlling communication style on 
early adolescents’ academic achievement. 
Child Development, 76, 483-501. 

van Wel, F., Linssen, H., & Abma, R. (2000). The 
parental bond and the well-being of adoles- 


cents and young adults. Journal of Youth & 
Adolescence, 29, 307-318. 


Varma, S. (2007). A computational model of 
Tower of Hanoi problem solving. Dissertation 


R-54 References 


Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences 
and Engineering, 67(8-B), 4736. 


Vartanian, O. (2009). Variable attention facilitates 
creative problem solving. Psychology of 
Aesthetics, Creativity, and the Arts, 3, 57-59. 


Vassalli, A., & Dijk, D. (2009). Sleep function: 
Current questions and new approaches. 
European Journal of Neuroscience, 29, 1830-1841. 


Vedantam, S. (2005, January 23). See no bias. 
Washington Post, p. W12. 


Vega, C. P. (2006). The effects of therapeutic 
components on at-risk middle school chil- 
dren’s grades and attendance: An archival 
study of an after-school prevention program. 
Dissertation Abstracts International: Section B: 
The Sciences and Engineering, 66, 4504. 


Vellacott, J. (2007). Resilience: A psychoanalytic 
exploration. British Journal of Psychotherapy, 
23, 163-170. 


Velliste, M., Perel, S., Spalding, M., Whitford, S., 
& Schwartz, A. (2008). Cortical control of a 
prosthetic arm for self-feeding. Nature, 453, 
1098-1101. 


Veltman, M. W. M., & Browne, K. D. (2001). Three 
decades of child mal-treatment research: 
Implications for the school years. Trauma 
Violence and Abuse, 2, 215-239. 


Veniegas, R. C. (2000). Biological research on 
women’s sexual orientations: Evaluating the 
scientific evidence. Journal of Social Issues, 56, 
267-282. 


Verdejo, A., Toribio, I., & Orozco, C. (2005). 
Neuropsychological functioning in metha- 
done maintenance patients versus abstinent 
heroin abusers. Drug and Alcohol Dependence, 
78, 283-288. 


Verfaellie, M., & Keane, M. M. (2002). Impaired 
and preserved memory processes in amnesia. 
In L. R. Squire & D. L. Schacter (Eds.), Neuro- 
psychology of memory (3rd ed.). New York: 
Guilford Press. 


Verleger, R., Schuknecht, S., Jaskowski, P., & 
Wagner, U. (2008). Changes in processing of 
masked stimuli across early- and late-night 
sleep: A study on behavior and brain poten- 
tials. Brain and Cognition, 68, 180-192. 


Vernon, P., Villani, V., Vickers, L., & Harris, J. 
(2008, January). A behavioral genetic 
investigation of the Dark Triad and the Big 5. 
Personality and Individual Differences, 44, 
445-452. 


Verona, E., & Sullivan, E. (2008). Emotional 
catharsis and aggression revisited: Heart rate 
reduction following aggressive responding. 
Emotion, 8, 331-340. 


Victor, S. B., & Fish, M. C. (1995). Lesbian moth- 
ers and the children: A review for school 
psychologists. School Psychology Review, 24, 
456-479. 


Vieira, E. M., & Freire, J. C. (2006). Alteridade e 
psicologia humanista: Uma leitura ética da 
abordagem centrada na pessoa. Alterity and 
humanistic psychology: An ethical reading of 
the person-centered approach. Estudos de 
Psicologia, 23, 425-432. 


Villarosa, L. (2002, December 3). To prevent sex- 
ual abuse, abusers step forward. The New York 
Times, p. B1. 


Villemure, C., Slotnick, B. M., & Bushnell, M. C. 
(2003). Effects of odors on pain perception: 
Deciphering the roles of emotion and 
attention. Pain, 106, 101-108. 


Virginia Tech Transportation Institute. (2009). 
New data from VTTI provides insight into 
cell phone use and driving distraction. 
Retrieved from http://www.vtti.vt.edu/ 
PDF/7-22-09-VTTI-Press_Release_Cell 
_phones_and_Driver_Distraction.pdf 


Vitaro, F., Brendgen, M., & Arseneault, L. (2009). 
Methods and measures: The discordant 
MZ-twin method: One step closer to the holy 
grail of causality. International Journal of 
Behavioral Development, 33, 376-382. 


Vitiello, A. L., Bonello, R. P., & Pollard, H. P. (2007). 
The effectiveness of ENAR® for the treatment 
of chronic neck pain in Australian adults: A 
preliminary single-blind, randomised con- 
trolled trial. Chiropractic Osteopathology, 9, 9. 


Vitello, P. (2006, June 12). A ring tone meant to 
fall on deaf ears. The New York Times, A1. 


Vleioras, G., & Bosma, H. A. (2005). Are identity 
styles important for psychological well- 
being? Journal of Adolescence, 28, 397-409. 


Vogt, D., Rizvi, S., Shipherd, J., & Resick, P. (2008). 
Longitudinal investigation of reciprocal 
relationship between stress reactions and 
hardiness. Personality and Social Psychology 
Bulletin, 34, 61-73. 


Volterra, V., Caselli, M. C., Capirci, O., Tonucci, F., 
& Vicari, S. (2003). Early linguistic abilities of 
Italian children with Williams syndrome 
[Special issue: Williams syndrome]. 
Developmental Neuropsychology, 23, 33-58. 


Voruganti, L. P., Awad, A. G., Parker, B., Forrest, 
C., Usmani, Y., Fernando, M. L. D., et al. 
(2007). Cognition, functioning and quality of 
life in schizophrenia treatment: Results of a 
one-year randomized controlled trial of 
olanzapine and quetiapine. Schizophrenia 
Research, 96, 146-155. 


Voss, J., & Paller, K. (2008). Brain substrates of 
implicit and explicit memory: The importance 
of concurrently acquired neural signals of 
both memory types. Neuropsychologia, 

46(13), 3021-3029. 


Vygotsky, L. S. (1926/1997). Educational psychol- 
ogy. Delray Beach, FL: St. Lucie Press. 


Wachs, T. D., Pollitt, E., Cueto, S., & Jacoby, E. 
(2004). Structure and cross-contextual stability 
of neonatal temperament. Infant Behavior and 
Development, 27, 382-396. 

Waddell, J., & Shors, T.J. (2008). Neurogenesis, 
learning and associative strength. European 
Journal of Neurosciences, 27, 3020-3028. 

Wadden, T. A., Crerand, C. E., & Brock, J. (2005). 
Behavioral treatment of obesity. Psychiatric 
Clinics of North America, 28, 151-170. 

Wade, K. A., Sharman, S. J., & Garry, M. (2007). 
False claims about false memory research. 
Consciousness and Cognition: An International 
Journal, 16, 18-28. 


Wager, T. D (2005). The neural bases of placebo 
effects in pain. Current Directions in Psycho- 
logical Science, 14, 175-180. 


Wagner, A. W, Rizvi, S. L., & Hamed, M. S. (2007). 
Applications of dialectical behavior therapy 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


to the treatment of complex trauma-related 
problems: When one case formulation does 
not fit all. Journal of Trauma Stress, 20, 391—400. 


Wagner, H. J., et al. (2004). A strategy for treat- 
ment of Epstein Barr virus-positive Hodgkin’s 
disease by targeting interleukin 12 to the 
tumor environment using tumor antigen- 
specific T cells. Cancer Gene Therapy, 2, 81-91. 


Wagner, R. K. (2002). Smart people doing dumb 
things: The case of managerial incompetence. 
In R. J. Sternberg (Ed.), Why smart people can be 
so stupid. New Haven, CT: Yale University 
Press. 


Wagstaff, G. (2009, January). Is there a future for 
investigative hypnosis? Journal of Investigative 
Psychology and Offender Profiling, 6, 43-57. 


Wain, H. J., Grammer, G. G., & Stasinos, J. (2006). 
Psychiatric intervention for medical and 
surgical patients following traumatic injuries. 
In E. C. Ritchie, P. J. Watson, & M. J. Friedman, 
Interventions following mass violence and 
disasters: Strategies for mental health practice. 
New York: Guilford Press. 


Walker, L., & Frimer, J. (2009). The song remains the 
same: Rebuttal to Sherblom’s re-envisioning 
of the legacy of the care challenge. Journal of 
Moral Education, 38, 53—68. 


Walker, L. J., & Frimer, J. A. (2007). Moral person- 
ality of brave and caring exemplars. Journal of 
Personality and Social Psychology, 93, 845-860. 


Walker, M. P., & van der Helm, E. (2009). 
Overnight therapy? The role of sleep in emo- 
tional brain processing. Psychological Bulletin, 
135, 731-748. 


Walker, W. (2008, May). Introducing hypnosis for 
pain management to your practice. Australian 
Journal of Clinical & Experimental Hypnosis, 36, 
23-29. 


Walker, W. R., Skowronski, J. J., & Thompson, 
C. P. (2003). Consolidation of long-term mem- 
ory: Evidence and alternatives. Review of 
General Psychology, 7, 203-210. 


Waller, B., Cray, J., & Burrows, A. (2008, June). 
Selection for universal facial emotion. 
Emotion, 8, 435—439. 


Wallerstein, J. S., Lewis, J., Blakeslee, S., & Lewis, 
J. (2000). The unexpected legacy of divorce. New 
York: Hyperion. 


Walsh, B. T., Kaplan, A. S., Attia, E., Olmstead, 
M., Parides, M., Carter, J. C., et al. (2006). 
Fluoxetine after weight restoration in 
anorexia nervosa: A randomized controlled 
trial. JAMA: Journal of the American Medical 
Association, 295, 2605-2612. 


Walsh, R., & Shapiro, S. L. (2006). The meeting of 
meditative disciplines and western psychology. 
American Psychologist, 61, 227-239. 


Wang, A., & Clark, D. A. (2002). Haunting 
thoughts: The problem of obsessive mental 
intrusions [Special issue: Intrusions in cogni- 
tive behavioral therapy]. Journal of Cognitive 
Psychotherapy, 16, 193-208. 


Wang, O. (2003). Infantile amnesia reconsidered: 
A cross-cultural analysis. Memory, 11, 
65-80. 

Wang, P. S., Aguilar-Gaxiola, S., Alonso, J., 
Angermeyer, M. C., Borges, G., Bromet, E. J., 
et al. (2007, September 8). Use of mental 
health services for anxiety, mood, and 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


substance disorders in 17 countries in the 
WHO world mental health surveys. Lancet, 
370, 841-850. 


Wang, Q. (2004). The emergence of cultural self- 
constructs: Autobiographical memory and 
self-description in European American and 
Chinese children. Developmental Psychology, 
40, 3-15. 


Wang, Q., & Conway, M. A. (2006). Autobiogra- 
phical memory, self, and culture. In L-G. 
Nilsson & N. Ohta, Memory and society: Psyc- 
hological perspectives. New York: Psychology 
Press. 


Wang, V. O., & Sue, S. (2005). In the eye of the 
storm: Race and genomics in research and 
practice. American Psychologist, 60, 37-45. 


Wang, X., Lu, T., Snider, R. K., & Liang, L. (2005). 
Sustained firing in auditory cortex evoked by 
preferred stimuli. Nature, 435, 341-346. 


Ward, L. M. (2004). Wading through the stereo- 
types: Positive and negative associations 
between media use and Black adolescents’ 
conceptions of self. Developmental Psychology, 
40, 284-294. 


Ward-Baker, P. D. (2007). The remarkable oldest 
old: A new vision of aging. Dissertation 
Abstracts International Section A: Humanities 
and Social Sciences, 67(8-A), 3115. 


Warden, C. A., Wu, W-Y., & Tsai, D. (2006). Online 
shopping interface components: Relative 
importance as peripheral and central cues. 
CyberPsychology & Behavior, 9, 285-296. 


Ward, W. C., Kogan, N., & Pankove, E. (1972). 
Incentive effects in children’s creativity. Child 
Development, 43, 669-677. 


Wark, B., Lundstrom, B., & Fairhall, A. (2007, 
August). Sensory adaptation. Current Opinion 
in Neurobiology, 17, 423-429. 


Wasserman, E. A., & Miller, R. R. (1997). What’s 
elementary about associative learning? 
Annual Review of Psychology, 48, 573—607. 


Watkins, C. (2007). Date rape. Farmington Hills, 
MI: Greenhaven Press. 


Watson, J. B. (1924). Behaviorism. New York: 
Norton. 


Watson, M., Haviland, J. S., Greer, S., Davidson, 
J., & Bliss, J. M. (1999). Influence of psycho- 
logical response on survival in breast cancer: 
A population-based cohort study. Lancet, 354, 
1331-1336. 


Watters, E. (2010, January 10). The Americanization 
of mental illness. The New York Times, p. C2. 


Waxman, S. (2009). Learning from infants’ first 
verbs. Monographs of the Society for Research in 
Child Development, 74, 127-132. 


Weber, R., Ritterfeld, U., & Kostygina, A. (2006). 
Aggression and violence as effects of playing 
violent video games? In P. Vorderer, & 

J. Bryant, (Eds.), Playing video games: Motives, 
responses, and consequences. Mahwah, NJ: 
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 

Wechsler, H., Kuo, M., Lee, H., & Dowdall, G. W. 
(2000). Environmental correlates of underage 
alcohol use and related problems of college 
students. Cambridge, MA: Harvard School of 
Public Health. 

Wechsler, H., Lee, J. E., Nelson, T. F., & Kuo, M. 
(2002). Underage college students’ drinking 
behavior, access to alcohol, and the influence 


of deterrence policies. Journal of American 
College Health, 50, 223-236. 


Weeks, M., & Lupfer, M. B. (2004). Complicating 
race: The relationship between prejudice, 
race, and social class categorizations. Persona- 
lity and Social Psychology Bulletin, 30, 972-984. 


Wegener, D. T., Petty, R. E., Smoak, N. D., & 
Fabrigar, L. R. (2004). Multiple routes to 
resisting attitude change. In E. S. Knowles & 
J. A. Linn (Eds.), Resistance and persuasion. 
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 


Wehrle, R., Kaufmann, C., Wetter, T. C., Holsboer, 
F., Auer, D. P., Pollmacher, T., & Czisch, M. 
(2007). Functional microstates within human 
REM sleep: First evidence from fMRI of a 
thalamocortical network specific for phasic 
REM periods. European Journal of Neuroscience, 
25, 863-871. 


Weinberg, M. S., Williams, C. J., & Pryor, D. W. 
(1991, February 27). Personal communication. 
Indiana University, Bloomington. 


Weiner, B. A., & Wettstein, R. (1993). Legal issues 
in mental health care. New York: Plenum Press. 


Weiner, I. B. (2004a). Monitoring psychotherapy 
with performance-based measures of person- 
ality functioning. Journal of Personality 
Assessment, 83, 323-331. 


Weiner, I. B. (2004b). Rorschach Inkblot method. 
In M. E. Maruish (Ed.), Use of psychological 
testing for treatment planning and outcomes 
assessment, Vol. 3: Instruments for adults (3rd 
ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum 
Associates. 


Weinstein, L. (2007). Selected genetic disorders 
affecting Ashkenazi Jewish families. Family & 
Community Health, 30, 50-62. 


Weinstein, M., Glei, D. A., Yamazaki, A., & Ming- 
Cheng, C. (2004). The role of intergenerational 
relations in the association between life 
stressors and depressive symptoms. Research 
on Aging, 26, 511-530. 


Weiss, A., Bates, T., & Luciano, M. (2008). 
Happiness is a personal(ity) thing: The 
genetics of personality and well-being in a 
representative sample. Psychological Science, 
19, 205-210. 


Weiss, W. M., & Weiss, M. R. (2003). Attraction- 
and entrapment-based commitment among 
competitive female gymnasts. Journal of Sport 
& Exercise Psychology, 25, 229-247. 


Weissman, M., Markowitz, J., & Klerman, G. L. 
(2007). Clinician’s quick guide to interpersonal 
psychotherapy. New York: Oxford University 
Press. 


Weissman, M. M., Bland, R. C., Canino, G. J., 
Faravelli, C., Greenwald, S., Hwu, H. G., et 
al. (1997, July 24-31). Cross-national 
epidemiology of major depression and 
bipolar disorder. Journal of the American 
Medical Association, 276, 293-299. 


Welkowitz, L. A., Struening, E. L., Pittman, J., 
Guardino, M., & Welkowitz, J. (2000). 
Obsessive-compulsive disorder and comorbid 
anxiety problems in a national anxiety screen- 
ing sample. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 14, 
471-482. 

Wells, G. L., Olson, E. A., & Charman, S. D. 
(2002). The confidence of eyewitnesses in 
their identifications from lineups. Current 
Directions in Psychological Science, 11, 151-154. 


References R-55 


Wenar, C. (1994). Developmental psychopathology: 
From infancy through adolescence (3rd ed.). New 
York: McGraw-Hill. 


Wensley, D., & King, M. (2008, June). Scientific 
responsibility for the dissemination and inter- 
pretation of genetic research: Lessons from 
the ‘warrior gene’ controversy. Journal of 
Medical Ethics, 34, 507-509. 


Wenzel, A., Zetocha, K., & Ferraro, R. F. (2007). 
Depth of processing and recall of threat 
material in fearful and nonfearful individuals. 
Anxiety, Stress & Coping: An International 
Journal, 20, 223-237. 


Werblin, F., & Roska, B. (2007, April). The movies 
in our eyes. Scientific American, pp. 73-77. 


Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (2005). Speech percep- 
tion as a window for understanding plasticity 
and commitment in language systems of the 
brain. Developmental Psychobiology, 46, 233-234. 


Werner, J. S., Pinna, B., & Spillmann, L. (2007, 
March). Illusory color and the brain. Scientific 
American, 90-96. 


Wertheimer, M. (1923). Untersuchungen zur 
Lehre von der Gestalt, II. Psychol. Forsch., 5, 
301-350. In R. Beardsley & M. Wertheimer 
(Eds.). (1958), Readings in perception. New 
York: Van Nostrand. 


West, D. S., Harvey-Berino, J., & Raczynski, J. M. 
(2004). Behavioral aspects of obesity, dietary 
intake, and chronic disease. In J. M. Raczynski 
and L. C. Leviton (Eds.), Handbook of clinical 
health psychology: Vol. 2. Disorders of behavior 
and health. Washington, DC: American 
Psychological Association. 


West, J. R., & Blake, C. A. (2005). Fetal alcohol 
syndrome: An assessment of the field. Experi- 
mental Biological Medicine, 6, 354-356. 


West, R. L., Bagwell, D. K., & Dark-Freudeman, 
A. (2007). Self-efficacy and memory aging: 
The impact of a memory intervention based 
on self-efficacy. Neuropsychological Development 
and Cognition, B, Aging and Neuropsychological 
Cognition, 14, 1-28. 

West, S. L., & O'Neal, K. K. (2004). Project 
D.A.R.E. outcome effectiveness revisited. 
American Journal of Public Health, 94, 1027-1029. 


Westen, D., Novotny, C. M., & Thompson- 
Brenner, H. (2004). The empirical status of 
empirically supported psychotherapies: 
Assumptions, findings, and reporting in 
controlled clinical trials. Psychological Bulletin, 
130, 631-663. 


Westerberg, H., Brehmer, Y., D'Hondt, N., 
Söderman, D., & Bäckman, L. (2008, April). 
Computerized training of working memory: A 
controlled, randomized trial. Paper presented at 
the 2008 annual meeting of the Cognitive 
Neuroscience Society. 


Westerhausen, R., Moosmann, M., Alho, K., 
Medvedev, S., Hämäläinen, H., & Hugdahl, K. 
(2009, January). Top-down and bottom-up 
interaction: Manipulating the dichotic listening 
ear advantage. Brain Research, 1250, 183-189. 


Westerterp, K. R. (2006). Perception, passive over- 
feeding and energy metabolism. Physiology & 
Behavior, 89, 62-65. 

Wetter, D. W., Fiore, M. C., Gritz, E. R., Lando, 

H. A., Stitzer, M. L., Hasselblad, V., et al. 
(1998). The Agency for Health Care Policy 
and Research. Smoking cessation clinical 


R-56 References 


practice guideline: Findings and implications 
for psychologists. American Psychologist, 53, 
657-669. 


Whaley, B. B. (Ed.). (2000). Explaining illness: 
Research, theory, and strategies. Mahwah, NJ: 
Erlbaum. 


Whisman, M., & Snyder, D. (2007). Sexual 
infidelity in a national survey of American 
women: Differences in prevalence and 
correlates as a function of method of assess- 
ment. Journal of Family Psychology, 21, 14-154. 


Whitbourne, S. (2010). The search for fulfillment. 
New York: Ballantine. 


Whitbourne, S. K. (2000). The normal aging pro- 
cess. In S. K. Whitbourne & S. Krauss (Eds.), 
Psychopathology in later adulthood. New York: 
Wiley. 

Whitbourne, S. K., & Wills, K (1993). Psychological 
issues in institutional care of the aged. In S. B. 
Goldsmith (Ed.), Long-term care. Gaithersburg, 
MD: Aspen Press. 


Whitbourne, S. K., Zuschlag, M. K., Elliot, L. B., 
& Waterman, A. S. (1992). Psychosocial 
development in adulthood: A 22-year 
sequential study. Journal of Personality and 
Social Psychology, 63, 260-271. 


Whitebread, D., Coltman, P., Jameson, H., & 
Lander, R. (2009). Play, cognition and self- 
regulation: What exactly are children learning 
when they learn through play? Educational 
and Child Psychology, 26, 40-52. 


Whitehouse, W. G., Orne, E. C., Dinges, D. F., 
Bates, B. L., Nadon, R., & Orne, M. T. (2005). 
The cognitive interview: Does it successfully 
avoid the dangers of forensic hypnosis? 
American Journal of Psychology, 118, 213-234. 


White, L. (2007). Linguistic theory, universal 
grammar, and second language acquisition. 
In B. Van Patten & J. Williams, Theories in 
second language acquisition: An introduction. 
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 


Whitfield, J. B., Zhu, G., Madden, P. A., Neale, 
M. C., Heath, A. C., & Martin, N. G. (2004). The 
genetics of alcohol intake and of alcohol 
dependence. Alcoholism: Clinical and 
Experimental Research, 28, 1153-1160. 


WHO World Mental Health Survey Consortium. 
(2004). Prevalence, severity, and unmet need 
for treatment of mental disorders in the World 
Health Organization World Mental Health 
Surveys. Journal of the American Medical 
Association, 291, 2581-2590. 


Whorf, B. L. (1956). Language, thought, and reality. 
New York: Wiley. 


Wickelgren, E. A. (2004). Perspective distortion of 
trajectory forms and perceptual constancy in 
visual event identification. Perception and 
Psychophysics, 66, 629-641. 

Widaman, K. (2009). Phenylketonuria in children 
and mothers: Genes, environments, behavior. 
Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18, 
48-52. 

Widiger, T. A., & Clark, L. A. (2000). Toward 
DSM-V and the classification of psychopa- 
thology. Psychological Bulletin, 126, 946-963. 


Widmeyer, W. N., & Loy, J. W. (1988). When 
you're hot, you're hot! Warm-cold effects in 
first impressions of persons and teaching 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


effectiveness. Journal of Educational Psychology, 
80, 118-121. 


Wiehe, V. R., & Richards, A. L. (1995). Intimate 
betrayal: Understanding and responding to the 
trauma of acquaintance rape. Thousand Oaks, 
CA: Sage Publications. 


Wielgosz, A. T., & Nolan, R. P. (2000). Biobehav- 
ioral factors in the context of ischemic cardio- 
vascular disease. Journal of Psychosomatic 
Research, 48, 339-345. 


Wiggins, J. S. (2003). Paradigms of personality 
assessment. New York: Guilford Press. 


Wildavsky, B. (2000, September 4). A blow to 
bilingual education. U.S. News & World 
Report, pp. 22-28. 


Wiley, C., & Sisson, M. (2006, November). Ethics, 
accuracy, and assumption: The use of Facebook by 
students and employers. Paper presented at the 
Southwestern Ohio Council for Higher 
Education Special Topics Forum, Dayton, OH. 


Wilgoren, J. (1999, October 22). Quality day care, 
early, is tied to achievements as an adult. The 
New York Times, p. A16. 


Wilkinson, H. A. (2009). Cingulotomy. Journal of 
Neurosurgery, 110, 607-611. 


Wilkinson, L., & Olliver-Gray, Y. (2006). The sig- 
nificance of silence: Differences in meaning, 
learning styles, and teaching strategies in 
cross-cultural settings [Special issue: Child 
language]. Psychologia: An International Journal 
of Psychology in the Orient, 49, 74-88. 


Willander, J., & Larsson, M. (2006). Smell your 
way back to childhood: Autobiographical 
odor memory. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 
13, 240-244. 


Willems, R. M., & Hagoort, P. (2007). Neural evi- 
dence for the interplay between language, 
gesture, and action: a review. Brain Language, 
101, 278-289. 


Williams, J. E., & Bennett, S. M. (1975). The defi- 
nition of sex stereotypes via the Adjective 
Check List. Sex Roles, 1, 327-337. 


Williams, J. E., & Best, D. L. (1990). Measuring sex 
stereotypes: A multinational study. Newbury 
Park, CA: Sage. 


Williams, J. E., Paton, C. C., Siegler, I. C., 
Eigenbrodt, M. L., Nieto, F. J., & Tyroler, H. A. 
(2000). Anger proneness predicts coronary 
heart disease risk: Prospective analysis from 
the Atherosclerosis Risk in Communities 
(ARIC) Study. Circulation, 101, 2034-2039. 


Williams, J. W., Mulrow, C. D., Chiquette, E., 
Noel, P. H., Aguilar, C., & Cornell, J. (2000). A 
systematic review of newer pharmacothera- 
pies for depression in adults: Evidence report 
summary. Annals of Internal Medicine, 132, 
743-756. 


Williamson, P., McLeskey, J., & Hoppey, D. (2006). 
Educating students with mental retardation 
in general education classrooms. Exceptional 
Children, 72, 347-361. 


Willis, G. (2008, June). Intraocular microinjections 


repair experimental Parkinson’s disease. Brain 
Research, 1217, 119-131. 


Willis, G. L. (2005). The therapeutic effects of 
dopamine replacement therapy and its 
psychiatric side effects are mediated by pineal 


function. Behavioural Brain Research, 160, 
148-160. 


Willis, J., & Todorov, A. (2006). First impressions: 
Making up your mind after a 100-ms exposure 
to a face. Psychological Science, 17, 592-598. 


Willis, S. L., & Schaie, K. W. (1994). In C. B. Fisher 
& R. M. Lerner (Eds.), Applied developmental 
psychology. New York: McGraw-Hill. 


Willness, C. R., Steel, P., & Lee, K. (2007). A meta- 
analysis of the antecedents and consequences 
of workplace sexual harassment. Personnel 
Psychology, 60, 127-162. 


Wills, T., Sargent, J., Stoolmiller, M., Gibbons, F., 
& Gerrard, M. (2008). Movie smoking expo- 
sure and smoking onset: A longitudinal study 
of mediation processes in a representative 
sample of U.S. adolescents. Psychology of 
Addictive Behaviors, 22, 269-277. 


Wilson, T. D. (2006, September 1). The power of 
social psychological interventions. Science, 
313, 1251-1252. 


Wilson, T. G., Grilo, C. M., & Vitousek, K. M. 
(2007). Psychological treatment of eating 
disorders [Special issue: Eating disorders]. 
American Psychologist, 62, 199-216. 


Windholz, G., & Lamal, P. A. (2002). Koehler’s 
insight revisited. In R. A. Griggs (Ed.), 
Handbook for teaching introductory psychology, 
Vol. 3: With an emphasis on assessment. 
Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. 


Winerman, L. (2005, June). ACTing up. Monitor 
on Psychology, pp. 44-45. 


Winik, L. W. (2006, October 1). The true cost of 
depression. Parade, p. 7. 


Winner, E. (2003). Creativity and talent. In M. H. 
Bornstein & L. Davidson (Eds.), Well-being: 
Positive development across the life course. 
Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. 


Winsler, A., Madigan, A. L., & Aquilino, S. A. 
(2005). Correspondence between maternal and 
paternal parenting styles in early childhood. 
Early Childhood Research Quarterly, 20, 1-12. 


Winson, J. (1990, November). The meaning of 
dreams. Scientific American, pp. 86-96. 


Winstead, B. A., & Sanchez, A. (2005). Gender 
and psychopathology. In J. E. Maddux & B. A. 
Winstead, Psychopathology: Foundations for a 
contemporary understanding. Mahwah, NJ: 
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 


Winston, J. S., O'Doherty, J., & Kilner, J. M. (2006). 
Brain systems for assessing facial attractive- 
ness. Neuropsychologia, 45, 195-206. 


Winter, D. G. (1988). The power motive in 
women—and men. Journal of Personality and 
Social Psychology, 54, 510-519. 


Winter, D. G. (1995). Personality: Analysis and 
interpretation of lives. New York: McGraw-Hill. 


Winter, D. G. (2007). The role of motivation, 
responsibility, and integrative complexity in 
crisis escalation: Comparative studies of war 
and peace crises. Journal of Personality and 
Social Psychology, 92, 920-937. 


Winters, B. D., & Bussey, T. J. (2005). Glutamate 
receptors in perirhinal cortex mediate encod- 
ing, retrieval, and consolidation of object rec- 
ognition memory. Journal of Neuroscience, 25, 
4243-4251. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Wiseman, R., & Greening, E. (2002). The mind 
machine: A mass participation experiment into 
the possible existence of extra-sensory percep- 
tion. British Journal of Psychology, 93, 487-499. 


Witelson, S., Kigar, D., Scamvougeras, A., 
Kideckel, D., Buck, B., Stanchev, P., et al. 
(2008). Corpus callosum anatomy in right- 
handed homosexual and heterosexual men. 
Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37, 857-863. 


Witt, C. M., Jena, S., & Brinkhaus, B. (2006). 
Acupuncture for patients with chronic neck 
pain. Pain, 125, 98-106. 


Wittchen, H., Nocon, A., Beesdo, K., Pine, D., 
Höfler, M., Lieb, R., et al. (2008). Agoraphobia 
and panic. Psychotherapy and Psychosomatics, 
77, 147-157. 


Wittenbrink, B., & Schwarz, N. (Eds.). (2007). 
Implicit measures of attitudes. New York: 
Guilford Press. 


Wixted, J. T., & Carpenter, S. K. (2007). The 
Wickelgren Power Law and the Ebbinghaus 
Savings Function. Psychological Science, 18, 
133-134. 


Wolfe, D. A. (1999). Child abuse: Implications for 
child development and psychopathology. 
Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. 


Wolfe, M. S. (2006, May). Shutting down 
Alzheimer’s. Scientific American, 73-79. 


Wolff, N. (2002). Risk, response, and mental 
health policy: Learning from the experience of 
the United Kingdom. Journal of Health Politic 
and Policy Law, 27, 801-802. 


Wolitzky, D. L. (2006). Psychodynamic theories. 
In J. C. Thomas, D. L. Segal, & M. Hersen, 
Comprehensive handbook of personality and 
psychopathology, Vol. 1: Personality and everyday 
functioning. Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley & Sons. 


Woller, K., Buboltz, W., & Loveland, J. (2007). 
Psychological reactance: Examination across 
age, ethnicity, and gender. American Journal of 
Psychology, 120, 15-24. 


Wood, E., Desmarais, S., & Gugula, S. (2002). The 
impact of parenting experience on gender 
stereotyped toy play of children. Sex Roles, 47, 
39-49. 


Wood, J. M., Nezworski, M. T., Lilienfeld, S. O., & 
Garb, H. N. (2003). What’s wrong with the 
Rorschach? Science confronts the controversial 
inkblot test. New York: Wiley. 


Wood, J. V., Perunovic, W. Q. E., & Lee, J. W. (2009). 
Positive self-statements: Power for some, peril 
for others. Psychological Science, 20, 860-866. 


Wood, W. (2000). Attitude change: Persuasion 
and social influence. Annual Review of Psycho- 
logy, 51, 539-570. 


Wood, W., & Eagly, A. H. (2002). A cross-cultural 
analysis of the behavior of women and men: 
Implications for the origins of sex differences. 
Psychological Bulletin, 128, 699-727. 


Woodruff, S. I., Conway, T. L., & Edwards, C. C. 
(2007). Sociodemographic and smoking- 
related psychosocial predictors of smoking 
behavior change among high school smokers. 
Addictive Behaviors, 33, 354-358. 

Woods, S. C., Schwartz, M. W., Baskin, D. G., & 
Seeley, R. J. (2000). Food intake and the 
regulation of body weight. Annual Review of 
Psychology, 51, 255-277. 


Woods, S. C., & Seeley, R. J. (2002). Hunger and 
energy homeostasis. In H. Pashler & R. 
Gallistel (Eds.). Steven’s handbook of experimen- 
tal psychology (3rd ed.), Vol. 3: Learning, 
motivation, and emotion. New York: Wiley. 


Woodson, S. R. J. (2006). Relationships between 
sleepiness and emotion experience: An 
experimental investigation of the role of 
subjective sleepiness in the generation of 
positive and negative emotions. Dissertation 
Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences 
and Engineering, 67(5-B), 2849. 


Woollett, K., & Maguire, E. (2009). Navigational 
expertise may compromise anterograde 
associative memory. Neuropsychologia, 47, 
1088-1095. 


Wren, A. M., & Bloom, S. R. (2007). Gut hormones 
and appetite control. Gastroenterology, 132, 
2116-2130. 


Wright, K. (2002, September). Times of our lives. 
Scientific American, pp. 59-65. 


Wrosch, C., Bauer, I., & Scheier, M. (2005, 
December). Regret and quality of life across 
the adult life span: The influence of 
disengagement and available future goals. 
Psychology and Aging, 20, 657—670. 


Wrzesniewski, K., & Chylinska, J. (2007). 
Assessment of coping styles and strategies 
with school-related stress. School Psychology 
International, 28, 179-194. 


Wu, L-T., Schlenger, W. E., & Galvin, D. M. 
(2006). Concurrent use of methamphetamine, 
MDMA, LSD, ketamine, GHB, and flunitraze- 
pam among American youths. Drug and 
Alcohol Dependence, 84, 102-113. 


Wuethrich, B. (2001, March 16). Does alcohol 
damage female brains more? Science, 291, 
2077-2079. 


Wurtz, R. H., & Kandel, E. R. (2000). Central visual 
pathways. In E. R. Kandel, J. H. Schwartz, & 
T. M. Jessell (Eds.), Principles of neural science 
(4th ed.). New York: McGraw-Hill. 


Wynn, K., Bloom, P., & Chiang, W. C. (2002). 
Enumeration of collective entities by 
5-month-old infants. Cognition, 83, B55-B62. 


Wyra, M., Lawson, M. J., & Hungi, N. (2007). The 
mnemonic keyword method: The effects of 
bidirectional retrieval training and of ability 
to image on foreign language vocabulary 
recall. Learning and Instruction, 17, 360-371. 


Xiao, Z., Yan, H., Wang, Z., Zou, Z., Xu, Y., Chen, 
J., et al. (2006). Trauma and dissociation in 
China. American Journal of Psychiatry, 163, 
1388-1391. 


Yamawaki, N., Darby, R, & Queiroz, A. (2007). 
The moderating role of ambivalent sexism: 
The influence of power status on perception 
of rape victim and rapist. Journal of Social 
Psychology, 147, 41-56. 


Yang, J., Hou, C., Ma, N., Liu, J., Zhang, Y., Zhou, 
J., et al. (2007). Enriched environment treat- 
ment restores impaired hippocampal synaptic 
plasticity and cognitive deficits induced by 
prenatal chronic stress. Neurobiology of 
Learning and Memory, 87, 257-263. 


Yao, S-Q., Zhour, Y-H., & Jiang, L. (2006). The 
intelligence scale for Chinese adults: Item 


References R-57 


analysis, reliability and validity. Chinese 
Journal of Clinical Psychology, 14, 441-445. 


Yao, Y., Kelly, M., Sajikumar, S., Serrano, P., 
Tian, D., Bergold, P., et al. (2008). PKM¢ 
maintains late long-term potentiation by 
n-ethylmaleimide-sensitive factor /gluR2- 
dependent trafficking of postsynaptic 
AMPA receptors. Journal of Neuroscience, 28, 
7820-7827. 


Yapko, M. D. (2006). Utilizing hypnosis in 
addressing ruminative depression-related 
insomnia. In M. D. Yapko, Hypnosis and 
treating depression: Applications in clinical 
practice. New York: Routledge/Taylor & 
Francis Group. 


Yardley, L., & Moss-Morris, R. (2009, January). 
Current issues and new directions in 
psychology and health: Increasing the 
quantity and quality of health psychology 
research. Psychology & Health, 24, 1-4. 


Yeomans, M. R., Tepper, B. J., & Ritezschel, J. 
(2007). Human hedonic responses to 
sweetness: Role of taste genetics and anatomy. 
Physiology & Behavior, 91, 264-273. 


Yesilyaprak, B., Kisac, I., & Sanlier, N. (2007). 
Stress symptoms and nutritional status 
among survivors of the Marmara region 
earthquakes in Turkey. Journal of Loss & 
Trauma, 12, 1-8. 


Yordanova, J., Kolev, V., Verleger, R., Bataghva, Z., 
Born, J., & Wagner, U. (2008). Shifting from 
implicit to explicit knowledge: Different roles 
of early- and late-night sleep. Learning & 
Memory, 15, 508-515. 


Yost, M. R., & Zurbiggen, E. L. (2006). Gender 
differences in the enactment of sociosexuality: 
An examination of implicit social motives, 
sexual fantasies, coercive sexual attitudes, 
and aggressive sexual behavior. Journal of Sex 
Research, 43, 163-173. 


Zacks, J. (2008). Neuroimaging studies of mental 
rotation: A meta-analysis and review. Journal 
of Cognitive Neuroscience, 20, 1-19. 

Zaitsu, W. (2007). The effect of fear on eyewitness’ 
retrieval in recognition memory. Japanese 
Journal of Psychology, 77, 504-511. 


Zajonc, R. B. (2001). Mere exposure: A gateway to 
the subliminal. Current Directions in 
Psychological Science, 10, 224-228. 


Zaragoza, M. S., Belli, R. F., & Payment, K. E. 
(2007). Misinformation effects and the sug- 
gestibility of eyewitness memory. In M. Garry 
& H. Hayne, Do justice and let the sky fall: 
Elizabeth Loftus and her contributions to science, 
law, and academic freedom. Mahwah, NJ: 
Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 

Zarren, J. I., & Eimer, B. N. (2002). Brief cognitive 
hypnosis: Facilitating the change of dysfunctional 
behavior. New York: Springer. 

Zaslow, J. (2003, May 1). Going on after the 
unthinkable: A rape victim shares her story. 
The Wall Street Journal, p. A2. 

Zaslow, M., Halle, T., & Martin, L. (2006). Child 
outcome measures in the study of child care 
quality. Evaluation Review, 30, 577—610. 

Zebrowitz, L. A., & Montepare, J. M. (2005, June 
10). Appearance DOES matter. Science, 308, 
1565-1566. 


R-58 References 


Zebrowitz-McArthur, L. (1988). Person perception 
in cross-cultural perspective. In M. H. Bond 
(Ed.), The cross-cultural challenge to social 
psychology. Newbury Park, CA: Sage. 


Zeigler, D. W., et al. (2005). The neurocognitive 
effects of alcohol on adolescents and college 
students. Preventive Medicine: An International 
Journal Devoted to Practice and Theory, 40, 
23-32. 


Zevon, M., & Corn, B. (1990). Paper presented 
at the annual meeting of the American 
Psychological Association, Boston, MA. 


Zhang, F., Chen, Y., Heiman, M., & Dimarchi, R. 
(2005). Leptin: Structure, function and 
biology. Vitamins and Hormones: Advances in 
Research and Applications, 71, 345-372. 


Zhou, Z., & Buck, L. B. (2006, March 10). 
Combinatorial effects of odorant mixes in 
olfactory cortex. Science, 1477-1481. 


Zhou, Z., Liu, Q., & Davis, R. L. (2005). Complex 
regulation of spiral ganglion neuron firing 
patterns by neurotrophin-3. Journal of 
Neuroscience, 25, 7558-7566. 


Zians, J. (2007). A comparison of trait anger and 
depression on several variables: Attribution 
style, dominance, submissiveness, need for 
power, efficacy and dependency. Dissertation 
Abstracts International: Section B: The Sciences 
and Engineering, 67(7-B), 4124. 


Zigler, E., Bennett-Gates, D., Hodapp, R., & 
Henrich, C. (2002). Assessing personality 
traits of individuals with mental retardation. 


American Journal on Mental Retardation, 107, 
181-193. 


Zigler, E. F., Finn-Stevenson, M., & Hall, N. W. 
(2002). The first three years and beyond: Brain 
development and social policy. In E. F. Zigler, 
M. Finn-Stevenson, & N. W. Hall, Current 
perspectives in psychology. New Haven, CT: 
Yale University Press. 


Zilbergeld, B., & Ellison, C. R. (1980). Desire 
discrepancies and arousal problems in sex 
therapy. In S. R. Leiblum & L. A. Pervin 
(Eds.), Principles and practices of sex therapy. 
New York: Guilford. 


Zimbardo, P. (2007). The Lucifer effect: Under- 
standing how good people turn evil. New York: 
Random House. 


Zimbardo, P. G. (1973). On the ethics of interven- 
tion in human psychological research: With 
special reference to the Stanford Prison 
Experiment. Cognition, 2, 243-256. 


Zimbardo, P. G. (2004). Does psychology make a 
significant difference in our lives? American 
Psychologist, 59, 339-351. 


Zimbardo, P. G., Maslach, C., & Haney, C. (2000). 
Reflections on the Stanford Prison Experiment: 
Genesis, transformations, consequences. In 
T. Blass (Ed.), Obedience to authority: Current 
perspectives on the Milgram Paradigm. Mahwah, 
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. 


Zimmerman, U. S., Blomeyer, D., & Laucht, M. 


(2007). How gene-stress-behavior interactions 
can promote adolescent alcohol use: The roles 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


of predrinking allostatic load and childhood 
behavior disorders [Special issue: Adole- 
scents, drug abuse and mental disorders]. 
Pharmacology, Biochemistry and Behavior, 86, 
246-262. 


Zito, J. M. (1993). Psychotherapeutic drug manual 
(3rd ed., rev.). New York: Wiley. 


Zolotor, A., Theodore, A., Chang, J., Berkoff, M., 
& Runyan, D. (2008). Speak softly—and forget 
the stick: Corporal punishment and child 
physical abuse. American Journal of Preventive 
Medicine, 35, 364-369. 


Zuckerman, M. (1978, February). The search for 
high sensation. Psychology Today, pp. 30-46. 


Zuckerman, M. (2002). Genetics of sensation 
seeking. In J. Benjamin, R. P. Ebstein, et al. 
(Eds.), Molecular genetics and the human 
personality. Washington, DC: American 
Psychiatric Publishing. 

Zuckerman, M., & Kuhlman, D. M. (2000). 
Personality and risk-taking: Common 
biosocial factors [Special issue: Personality 
processes and problem behavior]. Journal of 
Personality, 68, 999-1029. 


Zuger, A. (2005, November 10). Doctors learn 
how to say what no one wants to hear. The 
New York Times, p. S1. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Credits 


Text and Line Art Credits 


Chapter 2 


Module 5: Figure 4: From Darley, J. M., & Latané, B. 
(1968). Bystander intervention in emergencies: 
Diffusion of responsibility. Journal of Personality and 
Social Psychology, 8, 377-383. Published by The 
American Psychological Association, adapted with 
permission. Module 6: Figure 1: From Stevens, C., 
Lauinger, B., Neville, H. (2009). Differences in the 
neural mechanisms of selective attention in children 
from different socioeconomic backgrounds: an 
event-related brain potential study. Developmental 
Science 12(4): 634-646. Reprinted by permission of 
John Wiley & Sons, Inc. 


Chapter 3 


Module 7: Figure 1: Line art from Kent Van De 
Graaff, Human Anatomy, updated 5th ed. Copyright 
© 2000 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. 
Reprinted with permission. Figure 3: From Sylvia 
S. Mader, Human Biology, 6th ed., p. 250. Copyright 
© 2000 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. 
Reprinted with permission of The McGraw-Hill 
Companies. Figure 4a: From Sylvia S. Mader, 
Human Biology, 6th ed., p. 250. Copyright © 2000 by 
The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Reprinted with 
permission of The McGraw-Hill Companies. Figure 
4b: From George B. Johnson, The Living World, 2nd 
ed., p. 600. Copyright © 2000 by The McGraw-Hill 
Companies, Inc. Reprinted with permission of The 
McGraw-Hill Companies. Module 8: Figure 2: 
From Michael W. Passer and Ronald E. Smith, 
Psychology. Copyright © 2001 by The McGraw-Hill 
Companies, Inc. Reprinted with permission of The 
McGraw-Hill Companies. Figure 3: From Michael 
W. Passer and Ronald E. Smith, Psychology, p. 91. 
Copyright © 2001 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, 
Inc. Reprinted with permission of The McGraw- 
Hill Companies. Figure 4: Adapted from Robert J. 
Brooker, Eric P. Widmaier, Linda Graham, and Peter 
Stiling, Biology, p. 1062. Copyright © 2008 by The 
McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Reprinted with per- 
mission of The McGraw-Hill Companies. Module 
9: Figure 2: From Rod R. Seeley, Trent D. Stephens, 
and Philip Tate, Anatomy & Physiology, 5th ed., 

p. 384. Copyright © 2000 by The McGraw-Hill 
Companies, Inc. Reprinted with permission of The 
McGraw-Hill Companies. Figure 3: Adapted from 
Robert J. Brooker, Eric P. Widmaier, Linda Graham, 
and Peter Stiling, Biology, p. 940. Copyright © 2008 
by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Reprinted 
with permission of The McGraw-Hill Companies. 
Figure 4: Adapted from Allen M. Schneider and 
Barry Tarshis, Elements of Physiological Psychology, 

p. 87. Copyright © 1995 by The McGraw-Hill 
Companies, Inc. Reprinted with permission of The 
McGraw-Hill Companies. Figure 8a: From Robert J. 
Brooker, Eric P. Widmaier, Linda Graham, and Peter 
Stiling, Biology, p. 943. Copyright © 2008 by The 
McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Reprinted with per- 
mission of The McGraw-Hill Companies. 


Chapter 4 


Module 11: Figure 1: From Camille B. Wortman, 
Elizabeth F. Loftus, and Charles Weaver, 
Psychology, 5th ed., p. 113. Copyright © 1999 by 
The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Reprinted with 
permission of The McGraw-Hill Companies. 
Figure 3: From David Shier, Jackie Butler, and Ricki 
Lewis, Hole’s Essentials of Human Anatomy and 
Physiology, 7th ed., p. 283. Copyright © 2000 by The 
McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Reprinted with per- 
mission of The McGraw-Hill Companies. Figure 5: 
From Sylvia S. Mader, Human Biology, 6th ed., 

p. 250. Copyright © 2000 by The McGraw-Hill 
Companies, Inc. Reprinted with permission of The 
McGraw-Hill Companies. Module 12: Figure 1: 
From Robert J. Brooker, Eric P. Widmaier, Linda 
Graham, and Peter Stiling, Biology, p. 956. Copyright 
© 2008 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. 
Reprinted with permission of The McGraw-Hill 
Companies. Figure 2: From Rod R. Seeley, Trent D. 
Stephens, and Philip Tate, Anatomy & Physiology, 
5th ed., p. 384. Copyright © 2000 by The McGraw- 
Hill Companies, Inc. Reprinted with permission of 
The McGraw-Hill Companies. Figure 3: From Paul 
Vitello, “A Ring Tone Meant to Fall on Deaf Ears,” 
New York Times, June 12, 2006, p. A1. Copyright © 
2006 New York Times. All rights reserved. Reprinted 
by permission. Figure 4: Adapted from Bartoshuk, 
L., & Lucchina, L., “Take a taste test” from 
Brownlee, S., & Watson, T. (1997, January 13). The 
senses. U.S. News & World Report, pp. 51-59. 
Reprinted by permission of Linda Bartoshuk. 
Module 13: Figure 4: From Stanley Coren and 
Lawrence M. Ward, Sensation & Perception, 3rd ed. 
(1989), p. 329. Reprinted with permission of John 
Wiley & Sons, Inc. Figure 5b, c: From Stanley 
Coren and Lawrence M. Ward, Sensation & Per- 
ception, 3rd ed. (1989), p. 5. Reprinted with permis- 
sion of John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Figure 8: Adapted 
from Picture 2 from Figure 1 (p. 186) from 
Hudson, W. (1960). Pictorial depth perception in 
sub-cultural groups in Africa. Journal of Social 
Psychology, 52, 183-208. Reprinted by permission 
of the publisher (Taylor & Francis, http://www. 
informaworld.com). 


Chapter 5 


Module 14: Figure 1: From Palladino, J. J., & 
Carducci, B. J. (1984). Students’ knowledge of sleep 
and dreams. Teaching of Psychology, 11, 189-191. 
Reprinted by permission of the publisher (Taylor & 
Francis, http://www.informaworld.com). Figure 2: 
Figure on p. 16 from the book Sleep by J. Allan 
Hobson. Copyright © 1989 by J. Allan Hobson. 
Reprinted by permission of Henry Holt and 
Company, LLC. Figure 3: From Ernest Hartmann, 
The Biology of Dreaming (1967), p. 6. Courtesy of 
Charles C Thomas Publisher, Ltd., Springfield, 
Illinois. Figure 4: From Secrets of Sleep by Alexander 
Borbély, p. 43. Copyright © 1988 Alexander 
Borbély. Reprinted by permission of Basic Books, a 
member of the Perseus Books Group. Figure 6: 
From Adam Schneider and G. William Domhoff, 


The Quantitative Study of Dreams (2002). Reprinted 
by permission of G. William Domhoff, www. 
dreamresearch.net. Module 16: Figure 1: From 
Johnston, L. D., O'Malley, P. M., Bachman, J. G., & 
Schulenberg, J. E. (2009). Monitoring the future 
national survey results on drug use: 1975-2008. Volume 
I: Secondary school students (NIH Publication No. 
09-7402). Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug 
Abuse. Figure 2: From Sylvia S. Mader, Human 
Biology, 6th ed., p. 250. Copyright © 2000 by The 
McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Reprinted with per- 
mission of The McGraw-Hill Companies. Figure 3: 
New York Times graphic “Levels of caffeine in vari- 
ous foods” from Blakeslee, S. (1991, August 7). The 
secrets of caffeine: America’s favorite drug. New 
York Times. Copyright © 1991 by The New York 
Times Co. Reprinted with permission. Figure 7: 
From Johnston, L. D., O'Malley, P. M., Bachman, 

J. G., & Schulenberg, J. E. (2007). Monitoring the 
future national results on adolescent drug use: Overview 
of key findings, 2006 (NIH Publication No. 07-6202). 
Bethesda, MD: National Institute on Drug Abuse. 


Chapter 6 


Module 19: Figure 1: From Tolman, E. C., & 
Honzik, C. H. (1930). Introduction and removal of 
reward and maze performance in rats. University 
of California Publications in Psychology, 4, 257-275. 
Figure 3: From Anderson, J. A., & Adams, M. (1992). 
Acknowledging the learning styles of diverse 
student populations: Implications for instructional 
design. New Directions for Teaching and Learning, 49, 
19-33. Reprinted with permission of John Wiley & 
Sons, Inc. 


Chapter 7 


Module 20: Figure 6: From Collins, A. M., & 
Loftus, E. F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory 
of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 
407—428. Published by The American Psychological 
Association, adapted with permission. Figure 7: 
From Kent Van De Graaff, Human Anatomy, 
updated 5th ed. Copyright © 2000 by The 
McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Reprinted with per- 
mission. Figure 8a, b: graphs only: Maguire, E. A., 
Woollett, K., and Spiers, H. J. (2006). London Tax 
Drivers and Bus Drivers: A Structural MRI and 
Neuropsychological Analysis. Hippocampus, 16, 
1091-1101. Reprinted by permission of John Wiley 
& Sons, Inc. Module 21: Figure 4: Reprinted from 
Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, vol. 
13, Loftus, E. F., & Palmer, J. C., “Reconstruction of 
automobile destruction: An example of the inter- 
face between language and memory,” pp. 585-589, 
Copyright 1974, with permission from Elsevier. 
Figure 5: Data from Table 2 (p. 266) from Bahrick, 
H. P., Hall, L. K., & Berger, S. A. (1996). Accuracy 
and distortion in memory for high school grades. 
Psychological Science, 7, 265-269. Copyright © 1996, 
Association for Psychological Science. Reprinted by 
permission of Sage Publications, Inc. Module 22: 
Figure 2: Reprinted from Cognitive Psychology, vol. 
11, Nickerson, R. S., & Adams, M. J., “Long-term 


C-1 


C-2 Credit 


memory for a common object,” pp. 287-307, 
Copyright 1979, with permission from Elsevier. 


Chapter 8 


Module 23: Figure 1: From Shepard, R. N., & 
Metzler, J. (1971). Mental rotation of three- 
dimensional objects. Science, 171, no. 3972, 701-703 
(Figure. 1, p. 702). Reprinted with permission 
from AAAS. Figure 3: Reprinted from Cognitive 
Psychology, vol. 7, Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B., 
“Family resemblances: Studies in the internal 
structure of categories,” pp. 573-605, Copyright 
1975, with permission from Elsevier. http://www 
.sciencedirect.com/science/journal/00100285. 
Figure 5: From Barry F. Anderson, The Complete 
Thinker. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice-Hall, 1980. 
Reprinted by permission of the author. Module 25: 
Figure 1: MLA Language Map, 2005. Reprinted by 
permission of the Modern Language Association. 
© 2005 Modern Language Association. 


Chapter 9 


Module 26: Figure 1: From Multiple Intelligences: 
New Horizons in Theory and Practice by Howard 
Gardner. Copyright © 2006 by Howard Gardner. 
Reprinted by permission of Basic Books, a member 
of the Perseus Books Group. Figure 3: From 
Sternberg, R. J. (2000). The Holey Grail of general 
intelligence. Science, 289, no. 5478, 399-401. 
Reprinted with permission from AAAS. Figure 7: 
New York Times graphic “A New Approach in 
Testing” from Winerip, M. (1993, November 15). 
No. 2 pencil fades as graduate exam moves to 
computer. New York Times, p. B9. Copyright © 
1993 by The New York Times Co. Reprinted with 
permission. Module 28: Figure 1: Adapted from 
Table 1 (p. 410) from Henderson, N. D. (1982). 
Human behavior genetics. Reprinted, with per- 
mission, from the Annual Review of Psychology, 
Volume 33 © 1982 by Annual Reviews. www 
-annualreviews.org. 


Chapter 10 


Module 30: Figure 2: Line art from Rod R. Seeley, 
Trent D. Stephens, and Philip Tate, Anatomy & 
Physiology, 5th ed., p. 384. Copyright © 2000 by The 
McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Reprinted with per- 
mission. Module 31: Figure 1: Adapted from Figure 
1 (p. 1067) from Shaver, P., Schwartz, J., Kirson, 

D., & O’Connor, C. (1987). Emotion knowledge: 
Further exploration of a prototype approach. 
Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52, 1061- 
1086. Published by The American Psychological 
Association, adapted with permission. 


Chapter 11 


Module 32 Figure 1: Originally published in 
Williams, J. E., & Bennett, S. M. (1975). The defini- 
tion of sex stereotypes via the Adjective Check List. 
Sex Roles, 1, 327-337 (adapted from pp. 330-331). 
Copyright © 1975 by Plenum Publishing Cor- 
poration. With kind permission of Springer Science 
and Business Media. Figure 4: From Hostile hall- 
ways: Bullying, teasing, and sexual harassment in 
school. Washington, DC: American Association of 
University Women Educational Foundation, 2001, 
from Figure 11 (p. 22) and Figure 12 (p. 23). 
Reprinted by permission of AAUW Educational 
Foundation. Module 34: Figure 1: From Table 2 

(p. 300) from Arafat, I., & Cotton, W. L. (1974). 
Masturbation practices of males and females. Journal 
of Sex Research, 10, 293-307. Reprinted by permission 
of The Society for the Scientific Study of Sexuality. 
Figure 2: Gallup poll. Reprinted by permission of 
Gallup, Inc. Figure 5: From Finkelhor, D. (2002). 
Sexual abuse in the United States. Durham, NH: 
University of New Hampshire. Reprinted by per- 
mission of David Finkelhor, Ph.D. 


Chapter 12 


Module 37: Figure 3: Figure 2 (p. 239) from 
Leppänen, J. M., Moulson, M. C., Vogel-Farley, 

V. K., & Nelson, C. A. (2007). An ERP study of 
emotional face processing in the adult and infant 
brain. Child Development, 78, 232-245. Reprinted by 
permission of John Wiley & Sons, Inc. Figure 10: 
Judith A. Schickedanz, et al., Understanding 
Children and Adolescents, 4th edition, p. 440, Figure 
13.1, © 2001 Pearson Education, Inc. Reproduced 
by permission of Pearson Education, Inc. All rights 
reserved. Figure 11: From Dempster, F. N. (1981). 
Memory span: Sources for individual and develop- 
mental differences. Psychological Bulletin, 89, 
63-100. Published by The American Psychological 
Association, adapted with permission. Module 38: 
Figure 2: Rest, J. (1968). Developmental hierarchy in 
preference and comprehension of moral judgment. 
Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of 
Chicago. Figure 4: Graph adapted from Table 2 

(p. 64) from Boehm, K. E., & Campbell, N. B. 
(1995). Suicide: A review of calls to an adolescent 
peer listening phone service. Child Psychiatry and 
Human Development, 26, 61-66. Copyright © 1995 
Springer Netherlands. With kind permission of 
Springer Science and Business Media. Module 39: 
Figure 1: From Arnett, S. (2001). Conceptions of the 
transitions to adulthood: perspectives from adoles- 
cence to midlife. Journal of Adult Development, 8, 
133-144. Reprinted by permission of Springer 
Science and Business Media. 


Chapter 13 


Module 41: Figure 1: From Eysenck, H. J. (1990). 
Biological dimensions of personality. In L. A. Pervin 
(Ed.), Handbook of personality: Theory and research 

(p. 246). New York: Guilford. Reprinted by permis- 
sion of Guilford Press. Figure 2: From Pervin, L. A. 
(Ed.). (1990). Handbook of personality: Theory and 
research (chapter 3). New York: Guilford. Reprinted 
by permission of Guilford Press. Figure 4: From 
Tellegen, A., Lykken, D. T., Bouchard, T. J., Jr., 
Wilcox, K. J., Segal, N. L., & Rich, S. (1988). 
Personality similarity in twins reared apart and 
together. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 
54, 1031-1039. Published by The American 
Psychological Association, Reprinted with permis- 
sion. Module 42: Figure 2: From Richard P. Halgin 
and Susan Krauss Whitbourne, Abnormal Psychology, 
p. 72. Copyright © 1994 by The McGraw-Hill 
Companies, Inc. Reprinted with permission. 


Chapter 14 


Module 43: Figure 1: Map by Cleo Vilett, from 
Susser, E. S., Herman, D. B., & Aaron, B. (2002, 
August). Combating the terror of terrorism. 
Scientific American, p. 74. Reprinted by permission 
of Cleo Vilett. Figure 2 top: Adapted from Table 2 
(p. 475) from Chamberlain, K., & Zika, S. (1990). 
The minor events approach to stress: Support for 
the use of daily hassles. Reproduced with permis- 
sion from the British Journal of Psychology, 81, 469- 
481, © The British Psychological Society. Figure 2 
bottom: Adapted from Table II (p. 14) from 
Kanner, A. D., Coyne, J. C., Schaefer, C., & Lazarus, 
R. S. (1981). Comparison of two modes of stress 
measurement: Daily hassles and uplifts versus 
major life events. Journal of Behavioral Medicine, 4, 
1-39. © 1981 Plenum Publishing Corporation. 
With kind permission of Springer Science and 
Business Media. Figure 3: “Perceived Stress Scale” 
(pp. 394-395) adapted from Cohen, S., Kamarck, T., 
& Mermelstein, R. (1983). A global measure of per- 
ceived stress. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 
24, 385-396. Reprinted by permission of the 
American Sociological Association. Figure 4: From 
Hans Selye, The Stress of Life. Copyright © 1976 by 
The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc. Reprinted with 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


permission of The McGraw-Hill Companies. 
Module 45: Figure 2: From Andrews, F. M., & 
Withey, S. B. (1976). Social indicators of well-being: 
Americans’ perceptions of life quality, p. 376. © 1976 
Plenum Press, New York. With kind permission of 
Springer Science and Business Media. 


Chapter 15 


Module 47: Figure 1: Adapted from Susan Nolen- 
Hoeksema, Abnormal Psychology, 4th ed., p. 232. 
Copyright © 2007 by The McGraw-Hill Companies, 
Inc. Reprinted with permission of The McGraw-Hill 
Companies. Figure 2: From Anxiety Disorders and 
Phobias: A Cognitive Perspective by Aaron T. Beck 
and Gary Emery, with Ruth L. Greenberg, Tables 
6.1 and 6.2 (pp. 87-88). Copyright © 1985 by 
Aaron T. Beck, Gary Emery, Ruth L. Greenberg. 
Reprinted by permission of Basic Books, a 
member of Perseus Books Group. Figure 5: From 
Table 3 (p. 65) from Zung, W. W. K. (1965). 

A self-rating depression scale. Archives of General 
Psychiatry, 12, 63-70. Copyright © 1965, American 
Medical Association. All rights reserved. 
Reprinted with permission. Figure 6: Adapted 
from Slater, E., & Meyer, A. (1959). Contributions 
to a pathography of the musicians: Robert 
Schumann. Confinia Psychiatrica, 2, 65-94, Table II. 
Reprinted by permission of S. Karger AG, Basel. 
Figure 9: Table showing genetic probability of 
inheriting schizophrenia from the book 
Schizophrenia Genesis by Irving I. Gottesman. 
Copyright © 1990 by Irving I. Gottesman. 
Reprinted by permission of Henry Holt and 
Company, LLC. Module 48: Figure 1: From 
Benton, S. A., et al. (2003). Changes in counseling 
center client problems across 13 years. Professional 
Psychology: Research and Practice, 34, 66-72. 
Published by The American Psychological 
Association, adapted with permission. 


Chapter 16 


Module 49: Figure 2: Text reprinted by permission 
of Herbert Benson, M.D., Benson-Henry Institute 
for Mind Body Medicine, Massachusetts General 
Hospital, Chestnut Hill, MA. Module 50: Figure 1: 
Smith, Mary Lee, Gene V. Glass and Thomas I. 
Miller. The Benefits of Psychotherapy, p. 89, Table 5-1. 
© 1980 The Johns Hopkins University Press. 
Reprinted with permission of The Johns Hopkins 
University Press. 


Chapter 17 


Module 52: Figure 2: From Cacioppo, J. T., 
Berntson, G. G., & Crites, S. L., Jr. (1996). Social 
neuroscience: Principles of psychophysiological 
arousal and response. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. 
Kruglanski (Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of 
basic principles. © 1996 The Guilford Press. 
Reprinted by permission of the publisher. Figure 4: 
Adapted from Anderson, C. A., Krull, D. S., & 
Weiner, B. (1996). Explanations: Processes and con- 
sequences. In E. T. Higgins & A. W. Kruglanski 
(Eds.), Social psychology: Handbook of basic principles 
(p. 274). © 1996 The Guilford Press. Reprinted by 
permission of the publisher. Module 55: Figure 1: 
From Sternberg, R. J. (1986). A triangular theory of 
love. Psychological Review, 93, 119-135. Published 
by The American Psychological Association, 
adapted with permission. Figure 2: From Buss, 
D. M,, et al. (1990). International preferences in 
selecting mates: A study of 37 cultures. Journal of 
Cross-Cultural Psychology, 21(1), 5-47, copyright © 
1990 by Sage Publications. Reprinted by permis- 
sion of Sage Publications, Inc. Figure 3: From 
Table 1 (p. 41) from Benjamin, L. T., Jr. (1985). 
Defining aggression. An exercise for classroom 
discussion, 40-42. Reprinted by permission of the 
publisher. 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Photo Credits 


Front Matter 


p. xi (top): © Comstock/PictureQuest; p. xi 
(bottom): © Keith Brofsky /Getty Images; p. xii: 

© Alexander Tsiaras/Stock Boston; p. xiii (top): 

© Studio Photogram/Alamy RF; p. xiii (bottom): 
© Nicholas Devore II /Photographers/Aspen/Still 
Media; p. xiv: © Getty Images; p. xv (top): © Steve 
Raymer/Corbis; p. xv (bottom): © Ariel Skelley / 
Blend Images/Alamy; p. xvi (top): © Michael J. 
Doolittle/Image Works; p. xvi (bottom): © Michael 
Schwarz; p. xvii: © Somos/Veer/Getty Images; 

p. xviii: © Image Source /Superstock; p. xix (top): 
© Inspirestock Inc./Alamy; p. xix (bottom): © Orbit/ 
Masterfile; p. xx (top): © Zave Smith/Corbis; p. xx 
(bottom): © Blend Images /Superstock RF; p. xxi: 
© AFP/Getty Images; p. xxii: © Jose Fuste Raga/ 
Corbis; p. xxxviii: © Comstock/PunchStock; p. xl: 
© Stockbyte/Getty Images; p. xli: © Photodisc/ 
PunchStock; p. xlii : © Digital Vision/Getty 
Images; p. xliii: © Photodisc; p. xliv: © Stockbyte / 
Getty Images; p. xlvii: © Stockbyte /PunchStock 


Chapter 1 


Opener: © Comstock/PictureQuest; p. 3 (top): 

© David Buffington/Getty Images; p. 3 (center): 

© Bettmann/Corbis; p. 4 (top): © AP Images/Bill 
Haber; p. 4 (center): © Comstock/PictureQuest; p. 7 
(top): © Jeff Greenberg /Photo Researchers; p. 7 
(center): © Chuck Keeler /Getty Images; p. 7 (bot- 
tom): © David Buffington/Getty Images; p. 10: 

© Veronique Burger/Photo Researchers; p. 15: 

© Bettmann/Corbis; p. 16 (top left): © Corbis; 

p. 16 (center): © The Granger Collection; p. 16 (top 
right): Courtesy, Wellesley College Archives. Photo- 
graphed by Notman; p. 16 (bottom left): © Photo 
Researchers; p. 16 (bottom right): © Bettmann/ 
Corbis; p. 17 (top left): © Culver Pictures; p. 17 (top 
right): © The Granger Collection; p. 17 (bottom 
left): © The Granger Collection; p. 17 (bottom right): 
Courtesy, Elizabeth Loftus; p. 19: © Bettmann/ 
Corbis; p. 21: © AP Images/Hadi Mizban; p. 25 
(left): © Terry Vine/Blend Images/Corbis; p. 25 
(tight): © Frank Herholdt/Getty Images; p. 29: 

© Comstock /PictureQuest 


Chapter 2 


Opener: © Keith Brofsky /Getty Images; p. 31 (top): 
© Bill Aron/PhotoEdit; p. 31 (center): © Thomas 
Pflaum/Visum/Image Works; p. 31 (bottom): © 
Douglas Faulkner /Photo Researchers; p. 32: © Keith 
Brofsky /Getty Images; p. 38: © Robert I. M. 
Campbell / National Geographic Image Collection; 
p- 40: © Bill Aron/PhotoEdit; p. 42: © James Wilson/ 
Woodfin Camp; p. 45 (top): © Bill Aron/PhotoEdit; 
p- 45 (bottom): © Marc Steinmetz /VISUM/Image 
Works; p. 47: © Kris Timken/Blend Images /Getty 
Images; p. 50: © Thomas Pflaum/Visum/Image 
Works; p. 52: © Douglas Faulkner /Photo 
Researchers; p. 57: © Keith Brofsky /Getty Images 


Chapter 3 


Opener: © Alexander Tsiaras/Stock Boston; p. 59 
(top): © Dennis Kunkel/Visuals Unlimited; p. 59 
(center): © Passieka/SPL/Photo Researchers; p. 59 
(bottom): © Martin Rotker/Photo Researchers; p. 
60 (top): © Living Art Enterprises, LLC/Photo 
Researchers; p. 60 (center): © Alexander Tsiaras/ 
Stock Boston; p. 62: © Dennis Kunkel/ Visuals 
Unlimited; p. 67 (top, center): © Moonrunner 
Design Ltd.; p. 67 (bottom): © Michael Buckner/ 
Getty Images for Michael J. Fox Foundation for 
Parkinson's Research; p. 77: © AP Images/ 

Jeff Roberson; p. 79: © Martin Rotker/Photo 
Researchers; p. 80 (a): © SPL/Photo Researchers; 

p- 80 (b): © Volker Steger/Peter Arnold Photolibrary; 


p- 80 (c): © Roger Ressmeyer/Corbis; p. 80 (d): © 
Bryan Christie Design; p. 82: © Stephane de 
Sakutin/ AFP/Getty Images; p. 86: © AP Images/ 
Chris O'Meara; p. 87: Courtesy, Trustees of the 
British Museum, Natural History; p. 89 (a-c): 
From Kato, R., Yokoi, H. Arieta, A. H., Yu, W., 
Arai, T. (2009). Mutual adaptation among man 
and machine by using f-MRI analysis. Robotics 
and Autonomous Systems, 57, 161-166. © 2009 with 
permission from Elsevier. Figures. 3, 7a, 7b; p. 95: 
© Alexander Tsiaras/Stock Boston 


Chapter 4 


Opener: © Studio Photogram/Alamy RF; p. 97 
(top): © Curtis Myers/Stock Connection; p. 97 
(center left): © Joe Epstein /Design Conceptions; 
p. 97 (center right): © Digital Vision/Getty; p. 98: 
© Studio Photogram/Alamy RF; p. 100: © Curtis 
Myers/Stock Connection; p. 101: © Marty 
Heitner/ Image Works; p. 105 (left): © Biophoto 
Associates / Photo Researchers; p. 105 (right): 

© Biophoto Associates/Photo Researchers; p. 110 
(all): Shmuel, A., Chaimow, D., Raddatz, G., 
Ugerbil, K., Yacoub, E. (2010). Mechanisms under- 
lying decoding at 7T: Ocular dominance columns, 
broad structures, and macroscopic blood vessels in 
V1 convey information on the stimulated eye. 
Neurolmage, 49, 1957-1964. © 2010 with permission 
from Elsevier; p. 111 (a-c): © Joe Epstein/Design 
Conceptions; p. 118 (top): © NASA; p. 118 (bottom): 
© Prof. P. Motta/Dept. of Anatomy /University “La 
Sapienza,” Rome/SPL/Photo Researchers; p. 119: 

© Omikron/Photo Researchers; p. 122: © Liu Yang/ 
Redlinek/Corbis RF; p. 129: © Jeff Greenberg /Stock 
Boston; p. 130: © Cary Wolinsky /Stock Boston; 

p. 132: © John G. Ross/Photo Researchers; p. 137: 

© Studio Photogram/Alamy RF 


Chapter 5 


Opener: © Nicholas Devore III/Photographers/ 
Aspen/Still Media; p. 139 (top): © Ted Spagna/ 
Photo Researchers; p. 139 (center): © liquidlibrary / 
PictureQuest; p. 139 (bottom): © Bob Daemmrich/ 
Image Works; p. 140: © Nicholas Devore III/ 
Photographers/Aspen/Still Media; p. 143 (all): 

© Ted Spagna/Photo Researchers; p. 144: © CC 
Studio/Photo Researchers; p. 146: Walker, M. P., van 
der Helm, E. (2009). Overnight therapy? The role of 
sleep in emotional brain processing. Psychological 
Bulletin, 135 (5), 731-748. Reprinted with permission 
from the American Psychological Association; 

p- 148: © J. Luke/PhotoLink/Getty Images; p. 150 
(top): © Philippe Garo/Photo Researchers; p. 150 
(bottom): © Don Smith/ Alamy; p. 151 (top): © B. 
Boissonnet/BSIP/Corbis; p. 151 (bottom): © Jose 
Carrillo/Stock Boston; p. 155: © AP Images/Midland 
Daily News/Erin Painter; p. 157: © Liquidlibrary / 
PictureQuest; p. 159: © Suzanne Opton; p. 165 (left): 
© Bob Daemmrich/Image Works; p. 165 (right): 

© Jim Arbogast/Getty Images; p. 167: © Andrew 
Brookes/Corbis; p. 171: © Nicholas Devore III/ 
Photographers/Aspen/Still Media 


Chapter 6 


Opener: © Getty Images; p. 173 (top): © Scott T. 
Baxter /Getty Images; p. 173 (bottom): © Spencer 
Grant/Stock Boston; p. 174: © Getty Images; p. 176: 
© Culver Pictures; p. 178: © Scott T. Baxter/Getty 
Images; p. 185: © Nina Leen/Time & Life Pictures / 
Getty Images; p. 187 (top left): © PhotoDisc/Getty 
Images; p. 187 (top right): © Stockbyte /Corbis; p. 
187 (center left): © BananaStock/PunchStock; p. 
187 (center right): © Amy Etra/PhotoEdit; p. 188: 
© NYC Center for Economic Opportunity, Office of 
the Mayor, City of New York; p. 192: © Stephen 
Chernin/Getty Images; p. 193: Courtesy, Dr. 
Marian Bailey; p. 194 (left): © The McGraw-Hill 
Companies, Inc./Jill Braaten, photographer; p. 194 


Credit c-3 


(right): © Robin Nelson/PhotoEdit; p. 200 (top): 
Courtesy, Albert Bandura; p. 200 (bottom): © 
Spencer Grant/Stock Boston; p. 201 (a-d): From 
Montgomery, K. J. Isenberg, N., Haxby, J. V. (2007). 
Communicative hand gestures and object-directed 
hand movements activated the mirror neuron sys- 
tem. Social, Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience, 2, 
114-122 by permission of Oxford University Press; 
p. 202 (both): From Meltzhoff, A. N. (1988). 
Imitation of Televised Models by Infants. Child 
Development, 59, 1221—1229. Photo Courtesy of A. 
N. Meltzhoff & M. Hanak; p. 203: © Design Pics 
Inc./Alamy; p. 205: © Getty Images 


Chapter 7 


Opener: © Steve Raymer/Corbis; p. 207 (top): 

© Bob Wallace/Stock Boston; p. 207 (center): 

© Disney Enterprises, Inc; p. 207 (bottom): © Paul 
Thompson, UCLA Laboratory of Neuroimaging, 
2003; p. 208: © Steve Raymer/Corbis; p. 211: 

© Bob Wallace/Stock Boston; p. 219 (a, b): 
Maguire, E. A., Woollett, K., Spiers, H. J. (2006) 
London Taxi Drivers and Bus Drivers: A Structural 
MRI and Neuropsychological Analysis, Hippo- 
campus 16, 1091-1101. Reproduced with permis- 
sion from John Wiley and Sons; p. 220: © Marty 
Bahamonde/FEMA,; p. 222: © Disney Enterprises, 
Inc.; p. 226 (top): © St PetersburgTimes/Steve J. 
Coddington/Image Works; p. 226 (center): 

© Syracuse Newspapers/H. Bragman/Image 
Works; p. 226 (bottom): © David Young-Wolff/ 
PhotoEdit; p. 228: © AP Images/Paul Sakuma; 

p. 230: © Albert Moldvay / National Geographic/ 
Getty Images; p. 236 (a-c): Reprinted from Maguire, 
E. A., et al. Autobiographical memory in semantic 
dementia: A longitudinal fMRI study. Neuropsy- 
chologia. 2010 Jan, 48 (1):123-136. © 2010 with 
permission from Elsevier; p. 239: © Steve Raymer/ 
Corbis 


Chapter 8 


Opener: © Ariel Skelley/Blend Images/ Alamy; 

p. 241 (top): © Kevin C. Cox/Getty Images; p. 241 

(center): © Roberto Otero/Black Star; p. 241 (bot- 
tom): Courtesy, Dr. Laura Ann Petitto @1991/ 
photo by Robert LaMarche; p. 242: © Ariel Skelley/ 
Blend Images/Alamy; p. 245 (top): © Kevin C. 
Cox/Getty Images; p. 245 (center): © Tom 
McHugh/Photo Researchers; p. 245 (bottom left): 
© Greg Girard /Contact Press Images; p. 245 (bot- 
tom right): © Stephen Studd/Getty Images; p. 246: 
From Scott, L. S., Tanaka, J. W., Sheinberg, D. L., 
Curran, T. (2008). The role of category learning in 
the acquisition and retention of perceptual exper- 
tise: A behavioral and neurophysiological study. 
Brain Research, 1210, Fig. 5, 204-215. © 2008 with 
permission from Elsevier; p. 247 (top left): © 
Stockbyte /PunchStock; p. 247 (top right): © 
Isabelle Rozenbaum & Frederic; p. 247 (center left): 
© McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc./Gary He, photo- 
grapher; p. 247 (center right): © Radlund & 
Associates /Getty Images; Cirou/PhotoAlto/ 
PunchStock; p. 249: © AKG/Photo Researchers; 

p. 255 (left): © Hoby Finn/Getty Images; p. 255 
(right): © Stockbyte/PunchStock; p. 257 (a-c): © 
Superstock; p. 261: © Roberto Otero/Black Star; 

p- 262: © Jan Moline/Photo Researchers; p. 266: 
Courtesy, Dr. Laura Ann Petitto @1991/photo by 
Robert LaMarche; p. 268: © AP Images; p. 271: 
Courtesy, Great Ape Trust of lowa; p. 273: From 
Kovelman, I., Shalinsky, M. H., Berens, M. S., &, 
Petitto, L. A. (2008). Shining new light on the brain’s 

“bilingual signature”: A functional Near Infrared 
Spectroscopy investigation of semantic processing. 
Neuroimage, 39, 1457-1471. © 2008 with permission 
from Elsevier. Photo: © Laura Ann Petitto, 
University of Toronto; p. 275: © Ariel Skelley/Blend 
Images/ Alamy 


C-4 Credit 


Chapter 9 


Opener: © Michael J. Doolittle/Image Works; 

p- 277 (top): © David Hiser/Network Aspen; 

p. 277 (center): © Harold Holt/Hulton Archive/ 
Getty Images; p. 277 (bottom): © Alamy; p. 278 
(top left): © Scott Wintrow /Getty Images; p. 278 
(top right): Courtesy Sho Yano; p. 278 (bottom): 

© Michael J. Doolittle /Image Works; p. 279: © 
David Hiser/Photographers/ Aspen/Still Media; 
p- 280: © Paul Souders/Corbis; p. 282 (1): © Harold 
Holt/Hulton Archive/Getty Images; p. 282 (2): 

© Bettmann/Corbis; p. 282 (3): © Cold Spring 
Harbor Laboratory; p. 282 (4): © Bettmann/Corbis; 
p- 282 (5): © David Hiser/Photographers/Aspen/ 
Still Media; p. 282 (6): © Bettmann/Corbis; p. 282 
(7): © George C. Beresford/Getty Images; p. 282 
(8): © Royalty-Free / Corbis; p. 284: From Roberto 
Colom, Richard J. Haier b, Kevin Head, Juan 
Alvarez-Linera, Maria Angeles Quiroga, Pei Chun 
Shih, Rex E. Jung. (2009) Gray matter correlates of 
fluid, crystallized, and spatial intelligence: Testing 
the P-FIT model. Intelligence, 37, 124-135. © 2009 
with permission from Elsevier; p. 286: © Douglas 
McFadd/Getty Images; p. 287: © Roger Viollet/ 
Image Works; p. 288: © M. Siluk/Image Works; 

p. 297: © Ingram Publishing /Alamy; p. 303: 

© Creatas/PunchStock; p. 305: © Michael J. 
Doolittle /Image Works 


Chapter 10 


Opener: © Michael Schwarz; p. 307 (center): © 
Ryan McVay /Getty Images; p. 307 (bottom): Used 
by permission of Dr. Don Dutton; p. 308: © Michael 
Schwarz; p. 309: © AP Images/E. Pablo Kosmicki; 
p- 315 (top left): © Digital Vision/PunchStock; 

p- 315 (top right): © BananaStock /JupiterImages; 
p- 315 (center): © Digital Vision; p. 315 (bottom 
left): © LCPL Casey N. Thurston, USMC/DoD 
Media; p. 315 (bottom right): © Royalty-Free/ 
Corbis; p. 319: © Ryan McVay/Getty Images; p. 320: 
© JupiterImages /ImageSource; p. 321: © Karen 
Kasmauski/Corbis; p. 322: © Ed Quinn/Corbis; 

p- 323 (a, b): Santel, S., Baving, L., Krauel, K., 
Muente, T. F., Rotte, M. Hunger and satiety in 
anorexia nervosa: fMRI during cognitive process- 
ing of food pictures. Brain Research, V. 1114, (1) 9 
Oct. 2006, pp. 138-148. Fig. 4. © 2006 With permis- 
sion from Elsevier; p. 324 (top): © Image Source/ 
Getty Images; p. 324 (bottom): © Royalty-Free / 
Corbis; p. 325: Reprinted by permission of the 
publishers from Henry A. Murray, Thematic 
Apperception Test, Plate 12F, Cambridge, Mass.: 
Harvard University Press, Copyright © 1943 by 
the Presidents and Fellows of Harvard College, 

© 1971 by Henry A. Murray; p. 332: © Eric Fowke/ 
PhotoEdit; p. 334: © Art Resource, NYC; p. 335 (all): 
Matsumoto & Ekman, 1988; p. 337: © AP Images/ 
Petros Karadjias; p. 339: © Michael Schwarz 


Chapter 11 


Opener: © Somos/Veer/Getty Images; p. 341 
(top): © David Young-Wolff/PhotoEdit; p. 341 
(center): © Everett Collection; p. 341 (bottom): 

© AP Images/Noah Berger; p. 342 (left): © Arnaldo 
Magnan/Getty Images; p. 342 (center): © Somos/ 
Veer /Getty Images; p. 342 (right): Ida Mae 
Astute/© ABC/Getty Images; p. 343: © Pete 
Winkel/Focus Group; p. 346: © Digital Vision/ 
Getty Images; p. 348: © Ellis Herwig /Stock Boston; 
p- 350 (a, b): From Cheryl L. Garn, Mark D. Allen 
and James D. Larsen (2009) An fMRI study of sex 
differences in brain activation during object nam- 
ing. Cortex, 45, Figure 4, 610-618. © 2009 with per- 
mission from Elsevier; p. 352 (left): © Stockdisc/ 
Getty Images ; p. 352 (right): © The McGraw-Hill 
Companies Inc. /Ken Cavanagh, Photographer; 

p- 354 (left): © Laura Dwight; p. 354 (right): © 
Andy Sotiriou/Getty Images; p. 362 (left): © Blue 
Lantern Studio/Corbis; p. 362 (right): © Michael 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Newman/PhotoEdit; p. 363: © AP Images/ Noah 
Berger; p. 369: © Rachel Epstein/Image Works; 
p- 377: © Somos/Veer/Getty Images 


Chapter 12 


Opener: © Image Source/Superstock; p. 379 (top 
left): © Lennart Nilsson/ Albert Bonniers Forlag 
AB/A Child Is Born/Dell Publishing; p. 379 (cen- 
ter right): © Look GMBH/eStock Photography; 

p. 379 (center left): © Purestock /Punchstock; 

p. 379 (bottom): © Deborah Davis/PhotoEdit; p. 380: 
© Image Source/Superstock; p. 381: © Peter Byron; 
p- 387 (left): © D. W. Fawcett/Photo Researchers; p. 
387 (center left): © L. Willatt, East Anglian Regional 
Genetics Service /SPL/Photo Researchers; p. 387 
(center right): © Kenneth Eward/Photo Researchers; 
p- 387 (right): © Biophoto Associates /Science 
Source/Photo Researchers; p. 388: © Peter Menzel / 
Photo Researchers; p. 389 (left): © Lennart Nilsson/ 
Albert Bonniers Forlag AB/A Child Is Born/Dell 
Publishing /Scanpix; p. 389 (right): © Petit Format/ 
Science Source /Photo Researchers; p. 393: © Charles 
Gupton/Stock Boston; p. 395 (all): From: A. N. 
Meltzoff & M. K. Moore. 1977. “Imitation of facial 
and manual gestures by human neonates.” Science, 
198: 75-78. © 1977 American Association for the 
Advancement of Science; p. 399: Harlow Primate 
Laboratory, University of Wisconsin, p. 402: © SW 
Productions/Getty Images; p. 403: © Banana Stock/ 
Punchstock; p. 405: © Farrell Grehan/Corbis; p. 406: 
© Laura Dwight/Peter Arnold Photolibrary; p. 414: 
© Purestock/Punchstock; p. 415: © David Young- 
Wolff/Getty Images; p. 416: © Olive Pierce /Black 
Star; p. 418: © Photodisc/Superstock; p. 419: © Mary 
Kate Denny /PhotoEdit; p. 424: © image100/Corbis; 
p- 430: © Deborah Davis/PhotoEdit; p. 431: © AP 
Images /Gary Malerba; p. 435: © Image Source/ 
Superstock 


Chapter 13 


Opener: © Inspirestock Inc./Alamy; p. 437 (top): 
© Guy Gillette /Photo Researchers; p. 437 (center): 
© Meritt Vincent/PhotoEdit; p. 437 (bottom): 

© Hogrefe Publishing; p. 438: © Inspirestock Inc./ 
Alamy; p. 440: © Corbis Premium RF/Alamy; 

p. 441: © Bettmann/Corbis ; p. 442: © Guy 

Gillette / Photo Researchers; p. 444: © Andy Sacks/ 
Getty Images; p. 445 (left): © Warner Brothers 
Pictures /PhotoFest; p. 445 (right): © Warner Brothers 
Pictures/PhotoFest; p. 446: © Bettmann/Corbis; 
p. 451: © Bob Daemmrich/Image Works; p. 452: 

© The McGraw-Hill Companies, Inc./Lars A. Niki, 
photographer; p. 454: © BananaStock /Punchstock; 
p. 456: © Brand X Pictures/Punchstock; p. 457: 
Gardini, S., Cloninger, C. R., Venneri, A. (2009) 
Individual differences in personality traits reflect 
structural variance in specific brain regions, Brain 
Research Bulletin, 79 (5), 265-270. © 2009 with per- 
mission from Elsevier; p. 465: © Psychological 
Corporation; p. 467: © BananaStock ; p. 469: © 
Inspirestock Inc./Alamy 


Chapter 14 


Opener: © Orbit/Masterfile; p. 471 (top): © Ariel 
Skelley/Blend Images/Corbis; p. 471 (center): 

© Reza/Webistan/Corbis; p. 471 (bottom): © Jose 
Luis Pelaez/Corbis; p. 472: © Orbit /Masterfile; 

p. 473: © Ariel Skelley /Blend Images /Corbis; 

p. 475: © Royalty-Free /Corbis; p. 480: © Dr. David 
Phillips/ Visuals Unlimited; p. 482: From Coan, J., 
Schaefer, H. S., Davidson, R. J. Lending a Hand: 
Social Regulation of the Neural Response to Threat. 
Psychological Science, 17, 1032-1039. Reprinted with 
permission from John Wiley and Sons.; p. 487: 

© Lucas Jackson/Reuters; p. 488: Janes, A. C., et al. 
(2009) Brain fMRI Reactivity to Smoking-Related 
Images Before and During Extended Smoking 
Abstinence. Experimental and Clinical 


Psychopharmacology. December 2009, 368. Reprinted 
with permission from the American Psychological 
Association ; p. 490: © Reza/Webistan/Corbis; 

p- 495: © Jose Luis Pelaez/Corbis; p. 496: © Kevin 
Dodge/Corbis; p. 499: © Orbit/Masterfile 


Chapter 15 


Opener: © Zave Smith/Corbis; p. 501 (top): © AP 
Images/Brett Coomer, Pool; p. 501 (center): © 
Victoria and Albert Museum; p. 501 (bottom): 

© Phyllis Picardi; p. 502: © Zave Smith/Corbis; 

p. 504: © AP Images/Brett Coomer, Pool; p. 509: 
Reprinted with permission from the Diagnostic and 
Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, Text Revision, 
Copyright 2000. American Psychiatric Association; 
p. 515: © Owen Franken/Corbis; p. 517 (a, b): 
Christian, C. J., et al. (2008) Gray matter structural 
alterations in obsessive-compulsive disorder: 
Relationship to neuropsychological functions. 
Neuroimaging,164 (2), 123-131. © 2008 with permis- 
sion from Elsevier; p. 526 (all): © Victoria and 
Albert Museum; p. 528 (a, b): Courtesy, Nancy 
Andreasen, University of lowa Hospitals & 
Clinics; p. 531: © Tony Freeman/PhotoEdit; 

p. 537: © Sharon Gekoski-Kimmel/MCT/Landov; 
p. 541: © Zave Smith/Corbis 


Chapter 16 


Opener: © Blend Images/Superstock RF; p. 543 
(top): © Bruce Ayres/Getty Images; p. 543 
(center): © Jon Bradley /Getty Images; p. 543 
(bottom): © Will & Deni McIntyre/Photo 
Researchers; p. 544: © Blend Images/Superstock 
RF; p. 547: © Getty RF; p. 548: © Jonathan Nourok/ 
PhotoEdit; p. 549: © Bill Aron/PhotoEdit; p. 551: 

© Michael Newman/PhotoEdit; p. 552 (a, b): 
Schnell., K., Herpertz, S. C. (2007). Effects of dialectic- 
behavioral-therapy on the neural correlates of 
affective hyperarousal in borderline personality 
disorder. Journal of Psychiatric Research, 41, 837-847, 
Figure 3. © 2007 with permission from Elsevier; 

p. 556: © Don Hammond/ Design Pics/Corbis RF; 
p. 557: © Royalty-Free / Corbis; p. 559: © Jon 
Bradley /Getty Images; p. 562: © Image Source/ 
Alamy RF; p. 566: © Custom Medical Stock Photo; 
p. 570: © Phyllis Picardi; p. 572: © Michael 
Newman/PhotoEdit; p. 575: © Blend Images/ 
Superstock RF 


Chapter 17 


Opener: © AFP/Getty Images; p. 577 (top): © Paul 
Barton/Corbis; p. 577 (center left): © Superstock/ 
PictureQuest/ JupiterImages; p. 577 (center right): 
© Barbara Burnes/Photo Researchers; p. 577 
(bottom): © Bob Daemmrich/Image Works; p. 578: 
© AFP/Getty Images; p. 579: © AP Images/Elise 
Amendola; p. 581: © Michael Peuckert/ 
Imagebroker/Alamy; p. 586: © Paul Barton/ 
Corbis; p. 592: © Jim Sugar/Corbis; p. 594: © 
Superstock/PictureQuest/JupiterImages; p. 596 
(both): From the film Obedience © 1965 by Stanley 
Milgram and distributed by Penn. State Media 
Sales. Permission granted by Alexandra Milgram; 
p. 599: © Barbara Burnes/Photo Researchers; 

p. 600: From Lieberman, M.D., et al. (2005) An 
fMRI investigation of race-related amygdala activ- 
ity in African-American and Caucasian-American 
individuals. Nature Neuroscience, 8(6), 720-722. 
Reprinted by permission from Macmillan 
Publishers Ltd.; p. 606: © E. D. Torial/ Alamy; 

p. 610: © Bob Daemmrich/Image Works; p. 612: 

© Reuters NewMedia Inc./Corbis; p. 613: Greene, 
Joshua D., Paxton, Joseph M. (2009) Patterns of 
neural activity associated with honest and dishon- 
est moral decisions. PNAS, 106 (30), 12506-12511; 
p. 615: © AFP/Getty ImagesAppendixOpener: 

© Jose Fuste Raga/Corbis; pp. A-2, A-4: © Jose 
Fuste Raga/Corbis; p. A-17: © Geri Engberg/ 
Image Works 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Name Index 


Page numbers preceded by an A refer to 
the appendix. 


A 

AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety, 101 

AAUW. See American Association of 
University Women 

Aaker, J. L., 495 

Aaron, B., 475 

Aarts, H., 134 

Aartsen, M. J., 281 

Aazh, H., 100 

Abbott, M., 607 

Abe, J. A., 399 

Abel, J. R., 90 

Abell, S. C., 314 

Abelman, R., 353 

Ablon, J. S., 548 

Abma, R., 418 

Aboitiz, F., 271 

Abraham, H. D., 169 

Abraham, P. F., 566 

Abramowitz, J. S., 518 

Abrams, R. L., 134 

Abramson, L. Y., 343, 348, 366, 369, 
370, 524 

Accardi, M., 123, 156 

Ackard, D. M., 370 

Acocella, J., 465 

Adams, A. A., 292 

Adams, G., 538 

Adams, K. B., 431 

Adams, M., 202, 203 

Adams-Byers, J., 298 

Adarves-Yorno, I., 590 

Addus, A. A., 38 

Adlam, A., 214 

Adler, A., 439, 446, 459 

Adler, J., 514 

Adolphs, R., 219 

Adonu, J., 591 

Affeltranger, M. A., 124 

Afrank, J., 402 

Aftanas, L., 157 

Agargum, M. Y., 148 

Aguilar, J., 286 

Ahiima, R. S., 321 

Ahn, S. H., 88 

Aiello, R. J., 370 

Aiken, L., 432 

Aiken, L. R., 291, 300, 461 

Ainsworth, M. D. S., 399 

zen, I., 582 

kil, H., 475 

kins, S., 140, 161 

kirav, I., 67 

berts, A., 418 

brektsen, G., 483 

exander, G., 351 

lho, K., 117 

li, S. F., 163 

lik, J., 450 

lison, D. B., 322 

loy, L. B., 465 

Ilport, G. W., 449, 459, 460 


DPPP 





Allwood, M. A., 202 

Aloia, M. S., 150 

Alon, I., 158, 587 

Alonso, A., 558 

Alonso, L., 395 

Alonso, S., 558 

Alpers, G., 193 

Altman, L. K., 489 

Altman, N., 444 

Amabady, N., 451 

Amano, K., 112 

Amarel, D., 482 

Amato, L., 168 

American Anorexia Bulimia 
Association, 323 

American Association of Mental 
Retardation (AAMR), 295 

American Association of University 
Women (AAUW), 347, 354 

American College Health 
Association, 375 

American Psychiatric Association, 33, 
508-509 

American Psychological Association 
(APA), 9, 10, 12, 18, 21, 49, 463 

American Psychological Association 
Task Force on Intelligence, 302 

Amos, A., 488 

Amsel, L., 26 

Anastasi, A., 290 

Ancoli-Israel, S., 149 

Anderson, C., 146, 202, 609, 610 

Anderson, C. A., 585 

Anderson, J. A., 202, 203 

Anderson, J. R., 235 

Anderson, K. B., 371 

Andrasik, F., 93, 477 

Andreasen, N. C., 74, 527 

Andrew, M., 482 

Andrews, B., 326 

Andrews, F. M., 498 

Angelopoulos, N., 77 

Angier, N., 350 

Angleitner, A., 607 

Angoff, W. H., 302 

Ansaldo, I., 90 

Ansley, T. N., 292 

Antonini, A., 67 

Antonioni, D., 594 

Antony, M. M., 513 

Anwo, J., 360 

Aoki, Y., 493 

APA Presidential Task Force, 562 

Apanovich, A. M., 495 

Aponte, J. F., 562 

Aquilino, S. A., 402 

Arafat, I., 365 

Arasteh, K., 164 

Arbuthnott, A., 494 

Archer, J., 353 

Archer, R., 465 

Archer, T., 482 

Arena, J. M., 366 

Arenas, E., 89 

Argibay, J. C., 135 


Arguin, M., 90 
Ariely, D., 587 
Arikawa, H., 300 
Ariyanto, A., 579 
Armenta, B. E., 598 
Armstrong, R. D., 292 
Arnedt, J. J., 150, 422, 423 
Aronson, J., 349, 602 
Arseneault, L., 383 
Arvey, R. D., 74, 382 
Asch, S., 590-591 
Asendorpf, J., 601 
Ashford, L., 348 
Ashmore, R. D., 601 
Aspinwall, L. G., 483 
Assanand, S., 313 
Associated Press, 537 
Association for the Advancement of 
Artificial Intelligence, 286 
Astbury-Ward, E., 374 
Astin, A. W., 349 
Atkinson, H., 493, 494 
Atkinson, R. C., 209, 210 
Auer, J. A., 52 
Aujoulat, I., 481 
Auld, F., 546 
Aussilloux, C., 296 
Auyeung, B., 352 
Averill, J. R., 329 
Avery, D., 598 
Ayres, M., 349 


B 

Baars, B., 140 
Babson, K., 145 
Bacchiochi, J. R., 464 
Bachman, J. G., 489 
Bacue, A. E., 349 
Baddeley, A., 211, 214 
Badgaiyan, R. D., 224 
Baer, J., 262 

Bagby, R. M., 486 
Bagdadli, A., 296 
Bagge, C., 419 
Bagwell, D. K., 430 
Bahrick, H. P., 229 
Bai, L., 400 

Bailey, J. M., 368 
Baillargeon, R., 408 
Bains, O. S., 149 

Bair, D., 446 

Bakay, R., 89 

Baker, J., 181, 480 
Baker, S. A., 272 
Balaban, C. D., 124, 517 
Baldwin, C. K., 480 
Baler, R. D., 159 

Ball, D., 67 

Ball, H., 74 

Ball, L. J., 262 
Ballesteros, S., 119 
Baltes, P. B., 425 
Banaji, M. R., 224 
Bancroft, J., 524 
Bandura, A., 200, 452, 453, 459, 460, 551 


Banet-Weiser, S., 317 
Banich, T., 90 

Banks, J. A., 602 
Bannon, L., 353 
Baraas, R. C., 112 
Barandiaran, A. A., 415 
Barber, T. X., 123 
Bard, P, 332 

Bargh, J. A., 326, 583 
Barker, J., 156 
Barkley, R., 531 
Barlow, D. H., 513, 551 
Barmeyer, C. I., 202 
Barnes, M. L., 607 
Barnes, T., 374 
Barnes, V. A., 157 
Barnett, J. E., 50 
Barnett, R. C., 345 
Baron, J., 400 

Baron, R. S., 592 
Barone, P., 67 
Barressi, J., 140 
Barrett, L. F., 286, 329 
Barron, F., 261 
Barron, G., 611 
Barrowclough, C., 528 
Bartecchi, C. E., 490 
Bartholow, B., 610 
Bartholow, B. D., 202 
Bartlett, F., 227 
Bartlett, M. Y., 611 
Bartocci, G., 157 
Bartolomeo, J., 342 
Bartone, P., 482 
Bartoshuk, L., 119 
Bartoshuk, L. M., 119 
Bartress-Faz, D., 252 
Bartzokis, G., 527 
Baruss, I., 150, 169 
Basak, C., 429 
Bassotti, G., 61 

Bates, E., 266 

Bates, P. E., 296 
Bates, R., 605 

Bates, T., 497 

Batson, C. D., 612 
Bauer, I., 431 

Bauer, P., 215 

Baum, A., 479, 480 
Baum, M. J., 70 
Bauman, S., 459 
Baumeister, R. F., 357, 590 
Baumgartner, F., 481 
Baumrind, D., 402 
Bayley, P. J., 220 
Baylis, F., 89 

Bayliss, D. M., 213, 408 
Bayne, R., 446 
Bazalakova, M. H., 67 
Beach, F. A., 362 
Beall, A., 343 
Beardslee, W., 572 
Bearman, C. R., 262 
Beaton, R. J., 210 
Beatty, J., 60 


l-2 Name Index 


Beatty, W. W., 283 
Bechara, A., 87 

Beck, A., 523 

Beck, A. P., 559 

Beck, A. T., 516, 553, 554, 561 
Beck, H. P., 178 
Becker, B. E., 402, 413 
Becker, T., 286 
Beckham, J. C., 179 
Beckmann, H., 527 
Beckmann, S. C., 256 
Bedard, W. W., 84 
Beersma, D. G. M., 151 
Begeny, J. C., 297 
Begg, D., 310 

Begley, S., 9, 88, 174, 225 
Behar, L., 45 

Behrens, M., 430 
Beidel, D. C., 517 
Beilock, S.L., 214 
Beirut, L. J., 165, 167 
Bekkering, H., 131 
Bélanger, C., 515 
Belar, C., 8 

Bellezza, F. S., 213 
Belli, R. F., 227 
Belmaker, R. H., 457 
Belov, D. I., 292 
Belsky, J., 381, 401 
Bem, D. J., 135, 369 
Bem, S. L., 354 
Ben-Porath, Y. S., 264, 465 
Benac, N., 537 
Benbow, C. P., 350 
Benca, N., 152 
Benderly, B. L., 285 
Benet-Martinez, V., 272 
Benham, G., 155 
Benight, C. C., 474 
Benjamin, J., 457 
Benjamin, L. T., Jr., 609 
Benoit, C., 77 

Benson, E., 357 
Benson, H., 157 
Benton, S. A., 534, 535, 568 
Berenbaum, H., 480 
Beresnevaité, M., 486 
Bergen, L., 40 

Berger, M. P. F., 292 
Berk, L. E., 402 
Berkowitz, L., 608, 610 
Berle, D., 550 

Berliner, L., 372 
Berman, A. L., 544 
Berman, H., 424 
Bernal, G., 11 

Bernard, L. L., 309 
Bernardi, S., 81 
Bernier, M., 245 
Bernstein, D. M., 21, 229, 409 
Berntsen, D., 225, 229 
Berntson, G. G., 334, 580 
Berridge, K. C., 313 
Berrios, G. E., 505 
Berry, D. C., 224, 604 
Bertakis, K., 493 
Berthoud, H. R., 310, 319 
Berz, E. R., 349 

Best, D. L., 344 

Betz, N., 452 

Beutler, L. E., 561 
Beyene, Y., 424 

Bhar, S., 554 

Bhugra, D., 566 
Bialystok, E., 272 
Bianchi, R. A., 602 
Bianchi, S. M., 418 
Biener, L., 489 
Bieschke, K. J., 369 
Bigler, R. S., 353 
Billiard, M., 150 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Bindemann, M., 109 
Binder, J. L., 561 
Binet, A., 287-288 
Binetti, R., 165 
Bingenheimer, J. B., 202 
Binstock, R., 431 
Birbaumer, N., 82 
Birren, J. E., 427 
Bishop, M., 472 
Biswas-Diener, R., 497 
Bitterman, M. E., 177 
Bittles, A. H., 296 
Bivona, J., 358 
Bizley, J., 87 

Bjork, R.A., 135 
Bjorklund, D. F., 9 
Bjornstad, R., 481, 523 
Black, B. M., 371 
Black, J., 150 

Black, P., 310 
Blackmore, S., 135 
Blair, C. A., 611 
Blake, C. A., 296 
Blakeslee, S., 88 
Blass, T., 596 

Blatter, K., 151 
Blixen, C. E., 318 
Blomeyer, D., 167 
Bloom, P., 408 
Bloom, P. N., 594 
Bloom, S. R., 319 
Blum, D., 399 
Boahen, K., 61, 70 
Boake, C., 9 

Bock, K., 265 

Bode, C., 483 

Boden, J. M., 419 
Bodin, G., 546 
Boehm, K. E., 419 
Bogart, R. K., 123 
Bogenschutz, M. P., 559 
Bohn, A., 225 
Boisvert, C. M., 561 
Boivin, M., 322 
Boles, D. B., 90 
Bolger, N., 482 
Boller, F., 85 

Bollier, T., 347 
Bonanni, R., 215 
Bonanno, G. A., 482 
Bond, J. T., 345 
Bond, M., 548 

Bond, M. H., 453 
Bonello, R. P., 123 
Bonham, V. L., 26 
Bonke, B., 224 
Bonnardel, V., 110 
Bonnie, R. J., 504 
Bono, C., 342 
Bordnick, P. S., 549 
Borisenko, J., 400 
Borkan, J. M., 494 
Born, J., 149 
Bornstein, M. H., 400 
Bornstein, R. F., 506 
Borovecki, A., 179 
Borren, I., 303 
Bosma, H. A., 417, 429, 430 
Botvinick, M., 525 
Bouchard, C., 318 
Bouchard, T. J., Jr., 74, 382, 455, 457 
Bourne, L. E., 253, 256 
Bower, C., 296 
Bower, G. H., 234 
Bower, J. M., 81 
Boxer, P., 202 

Boyce, W. T., 473 
Boyd, J., 129 
Boyd-Wilson, B. M., 496 
Boyle, G. J., 360 
Boyle, S. H., 486 


Bozarth, J. D., 557 
Bradshaw, J. W. S., 189 
Brady, T., 579 

Braff, D. L., 528 
Brambilla, P., 566 
Branigan, C., 329 
Brantley, P. J., 482 
Brasic, J. R., 518 
Braun, A., 148 
Brausch, A. M., 418 
Braver, T. S., 283 
Bray, G. A., 318 
Brazelton, T. B., 393 
Breland, K., 193 
Breland, M., 193 
Brendgen, M., 383 
Brennan, R. T., 202, 489 
Brennen, T., 222 
Bresler, C., 331 
Breslin, M. M., 278 
Brett, J. M., 158, 587 
Brewer, J. B., 220 
Brewer, M., 591 
Brewer, M. B., 583 
Brewer, R. D., 164 
Bridges, M. W., 463 
Brief, A. P., 605 
Bright, P., 236 
Brinkhaus, B., 122 
Brinton, B., 330 
Brislin, R., 280 
Brock, J., 195 

Brock, T. C., 579 
Bröder, A, 214 
Brody, J., 489 
Broidy, L. M., 530 
Broman, C. L., 479 
Bronson, P., 599 
Brooker, R. J., 83, 92, 115 
Broome, K. M., 168 
Brossart, D. F., 571 
Brown, D. L., 32 
Brown, E., 490 
Brown, J., 585 
Brown, L. S., 229 
Brown, P. K., 111 
Brown, R., 215 
Brown, R. J., 520 
Brown, S., 85, 87 
Brown, S. I., 254 
Brown, T. A., 513 
Browne, K. D., 266 
Brownlee, K., 562 
Bruce, V., 110, 130 
Bruehl, S., 613 
Bruggeman, H., 130 
Brule, D., 21 
Brumbach, B. H., 9 
Brunet, A., 220 
Brunetti, E., 271 
Bryan, J., 237 
Bryant, R. A., 26 
Bryant, R. M., 11, 147 
Brydon, L., 476 
Brynhildsen, J., 424 
Brzustowicz, L. M., 527 
Bschor, T., 497 
Buboltz, W., 493 
Bucci, S., 525 
Buchanan, R. W., 526 
Buchanan, T., 452 
Buchanan, T. W., 219 
Buchert, R., 168 
Buck, L. B., 119 
Buckman, J., 236 
Buehner, M., 281 
Bukobza, G., 422 
Bulik, C. M., 320, 322 
Bunge, S. A., 26 
Bunting, M., 234 
Burbach, M. E., 262 


Burchinal, M. R., 401 
Burger, J. M., 594, 596 
Burgoon, J. K., 349, 604 
Burke, C., 278, 296, 305 
Burns, C. W., 109 
Burns, J. W., 613 
Burns, N. R., 237 
Burrows, A., 34 

Burt, D. M., 605 
Burton, K. S., 281 

Bury, A. S., 248 

Busey, T. A., 21 

Bush, J., 550 

Bushman, B. J., 202, 609 
Bushnell, M. C., 122 


Buss, D. M., 9, 351, 352, 382, 450, 


455, 607 
Bussey, T. J., 67 
Butcher, J. N., 464 
Butler, A. C., 553 
Butler, J., 107 
Butler, L. D., 487 
Butler, L. T., 224, 604 
Butler, M. A., 180 
Butt, T., 587 
Buzi, R., 372 
Byne, W., 368 
Byrne, B. M., 300 
Byrne-Davis, L., 472 


C 

Cabanac, M., 319 
Cabioglu, M., 122 
Cachelin, F. M., 323 


Cacioppo, J. T., 26, 580, 581, 600 


Cadenhead, K., 528 
Cadieux, M. J., 223 
Caelian, C. F., 419 
Cahil, L., 90 

Cahill, L., 208, 237 
Cai, D., 144 

Cain, D. J., 459 
Cajochen, C., 151 
Calabrese, J. R., 566 
Caldwell, D. F., 594 
Calin-Jageman, R. J., 102 
Calkins, M., 16, 18 
Calvert, S., 606 
Cameron, O. G., 331 
Campbell, D. T., 133 
Campbell, N. B., 419 
Campbell, R., 371 
Campbell, T. S., 123 
Campbell, W., 587 
Campo-Flores, A., 544 
Canetti-Nisim, D., 26 
Cannon, T. D., 93, 262 
Cannon, W. B., 332 
Cantalupo, C., 89 
Cantwell, R. H., 326 
Capaldi, E. D., 319 
Caplan, D., 88 
Capone, N. C., 382 
Cardemil, E. V., 26 


Carey, B., 82, 214, 220, 258, 568 


Carhart-Harris, K., 163 
Carlin, A., 46 
Carlsmith, J. M., 582 
Carlson, M., 610 
Carnagey, N., 202, 610 
Carney, D., 584 
Carney, R. N., 213, 237 
Carnochan, P., 330 
Carpenter, S., 50 
Carpenter, S. K., 233 
Carr, A., 373 

Carr, P., 349 

Carr, T. H., 214 
Carrera, M., 515 
Carrillo, M., 68 
Carroll, J., 323 


Carter, R. T., 272 
Cartwright, R., 60, 148 
Caruso, D. R., 285 
Caruso, E., 249 
Carvalho, F. M., 67 
Carver, C., 432 
Carver, C. S., 463 
Cary, P., 24 
Casad, B. J., 345 
Casasanto, D., 269 
Case, R., 408 
Case, T. I., 118 
Casey, S. D., 189 
Cashon, C. H., 397, 408 
Casper, L. M., 418 
Caspi, A., 9, 382, 457 
Caspi, R., 457 
Cassells, J. V. S., 445 
Cassidy, K. W., 400 
Castro, C. A., 475 
Catalano, S. M., 370 
Catenacci, V., 317 
Cattell, A. K., 449 
Cattell, H. E. P., 449 
Cattell, R. B., 449, 459 
Cauce, A. M., 571 
Cavallini, E., 429 
Cavenett, T., 311 
Ceccanti, M., 165 
Ceci, S. J., 228 
Centers for Disease Control and 
Prevention (CDC), 372, 
373, 418, 419 
Ceponiene, R., 216 
Cerutti, D. T., 189 
Chabris, C. F., 283 
Chaiken, S., 47 
Chamberlain, K., 475, 476 
Chamberlain, S. R., 517 
Chambless, D. L., 562 
Chan, K. K., 582 
Chandra, P., 268 
Chandran, S., 254 
Chang, J., 26 
Chang, K., 350 
Chang, L., 352 
Chang, S. W., 292 
Chang-Schneider, C., 453 
Chapkis, W., 168 
Chapman, J., 592 
Chapman, J. P., 525 
Chapman, L. J., 525 
Charlton, S., 21 
Charman, S. D., 227 
Chartrand, T. L., 134, 583 
Chaudhry, H. R., 498 
Chauhan, R. V., 562 
Cheakalos, C., 317, 360 
Chechile, R. A., 99 
Chen, A., 112 
Chen, C., 25 
Chen, D., 38, 119 
Chen, T. H., 117 
Cheney, C. D., 494 
Cheng, C., 483 
Cheng, S., 453 
Cheston, S. E., 562 
Cheung, M. L., 483 
Chiang, W. C., 408 
Chincotta, D., 214 
Chiodo, D., 347 
Chivers, M. L., 356 
Cho, A., 115 
Cho; S., 93, 262 
Chodorow, M., 234 
Chomsky, N., 267, 268 
Chou, K., 481 
Choy, Y., 549, 550 
Christ, S. E., 390 
Christakis, N. A., 489, 496 
Christensen, A. J., 492 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Christian, C. J., 517 
Chrysikou, E. G., 256 
Chu, D: Z. J.,; 123 
Chuang, S., 418 
Chylinska, J., 480 
Cialdini, R. B., 580, 594 
Cicchetti, D., 402 
Cirelli, C., 144 

Clair, J. M., 495 

Clark, D., 121 

Clark, D. A., 507, 516, 518 
Clark, D. B., 166 

Clark, J., 557 

Clark, L., 258 

Clark, L. A., 509 

Clark, L. V., 407 

Clark, M., 586 

Clark, M. P., 18 

Clark, M. S., 586 
Clark, R. E., 220 


Clarke-Stewart, K. A., 402, 425 


Clarkin, J. F., 529 
Clay D. L., 548 
Clayton, K., 162 
Cleary, A. M., 222 
Clements, A. M., 90 
Clements, M., 367 
Clementz, B., 587 
Clemons, T. L., 298 
Clifasefi, S., 229 
Clifford, C. W. G., 108 
Clifton, D., 498 
Cloud, J., 168, 572 
Coan, J. A., 482, 483 
Coates, S. L., 224 
Coats, E. J., 349 
Cobos, P., 331 
Cochran, S. D., 369 
Coffman, S. J., 561 
Cohen, A. L., 429 
Cohen, B. H., 45 
Cohen, G. L., 602 
Cohen, J., 35 

Cohen, L. B., 397, 408 
Cohen, P., 168, 538 
Cohen, S., 477, 480, 482 
Cohler, B. J., 546 
Cole, D. A., 530 
Coleman, E., 365 
Coleman, G., 144 
Coleman, J., 413 
Coleman, M., 426 
Coles, R., 356, 415 
Colland, V. T., 492 
Collinge, M. D., 181 
Collins, A. M., 217 
Collins, F., 26 
Collins, L. M., 414 
Colom, R, 280, 283, 284 
Coltheart, M., 525 
Coltraine, S., 599 
Colvin, C., 584 
Colwell, M., 400 
Combrink-Graham, L., 399 
Compagni, A., 60 
Comuzzie, A. G., 322 
Conduit, R., 144 
Cone-Wesson, B., 117 
Conger, R., 21 
Conger, R. D., 508 
Connell, B., 267 
Conner, M., 582 
Connolly, A. C., 245 
Conoley, C. W., 571 
Consentino, S., 224 
Conte, H. R., 443 
Conway, M. A., 224, 230 
Conway, T. L., 489 
Cooke, J. R., 149 
Cooklin, A., 559 
Cooper, H., 46, 371 





Cooper, J., 101, 582 
Cooper, M., 558 
Cooper, Z., 323 
Cooper-Brown, L. J., 189 
Cope, D., 249 
Copeland, J. B., 249 
Coplan, R., 402 
Copolov, D. L., 525 
Cordnoldi, C., 229 
Coren, S., 102, 128, 131, 132 
Corn, B., 487 
Cornelius, M. D., 168 
Cornell, C. B., 288 
Cornell, D. G., 298 
Corp, E. S., 318 
Corpus, J. H., 313 
Corsello, A., 227 
Cortina, L. M., 347 
Cosmides, L., 193 
Costa, P., 591 
Costa, P. T., Jr., 450, 530 
Cété, J. E., 422 
Cotton, P., 537 
Cotton, W. L., 365 
Council of National Psychological 
Associations for the Advancement 
of Ethnic Minority Interests, 26 
Courteau, J., 488 
Couturier, J., 322 
Coventry, K. R., 252 
Cowan, N., 212, 408 
Cowley, B. J., 152 
Cowley, G., 235, 430, 473 
Cox, J., 309 
Cox, R., 181 
Coyle, N., 432 
Coyle, T. R., 291 
Craig, K. D., 122 
Craik, F. I. M., 223 
Cramer, A., 402 
Cramer, D., 35 
Cramer, P., 443 
Crandall, C. S., 410 
Crawford, M., 345, 349 
Cray, J., 34 
Creasey, G. L., 383 
Crerand, C. E., 195 
Crews, F., 444 
Crewther, S. G., 144 
Criswell, H., 67 
Critchfield, T., 186 
Critelli, J., 358 
Crites, S. L., 580 
Crits-Christoph, P., 573 
Crocker, J., 510 
Crockett, E., 584 
Crombag, H. S., 159 
Cropley, A., 261 
Crosnoe, R., 431 
Crouter, A. C., 353 
Crowder, R. G., 210 
Crum, A. J., 53, 68 
Crump, J., 446 
Cruz, A., 124 
Cuddy, A. J. C., 345 
Cuijpers, P., 26, 560 
Culbert, K., 322 
Culhane-Pera, K. A., 494 
Cullinane, C. A., 123 
Cumberland, A., 612 
Cummings, A., 216 
Cunningham, P., 408 
Curran, M., 565 
Cutting, J. C., 265 
Cwikel, J., 45 
Cynkar, A., 11 
Czopp, A. M., 602 


D 


Daddis, C., 418 
Dadds, M., 77 


Name Index 1-3 


Daftary, F., 229 
Dai, D. Y., 581 
Daines, B., 367 
Dalal, A. K., 472 
Dale, A., 38 
Dale, J. W., 74 
Daley, E. M., 38 
Damasio, A., 140 
Damon, W., 415 
Danaei, G., 487 
Dani, J. A., 488 
Daniels, K. A., 224 
Darby, R., 371 
D'Arcy, R. C. N., 80 
Dardenne, B., 347 
Dark-Freudeman, A., 430 
Darley, J. M., 34-35, 37, 41, 42, 44, 
45, 46, 49, 50, 611 
Darroch, J. E., 366 
Darwin, C., 8 
Darwin, C. J., 210 
Das, A., 364, 365 
Daskalakis, Z., 81 
David, D., 554 
Davidson, J. E., 254 
Davidson, R. J., 331, 482, 483, 1994 
Davies, I., 395 
Davies, S., 468, 517 
Davis, D. D., 554 
Davis, J, 336 
Davis, L. J., 462 
Davis, O., 74 
Davis, R., 530 
Davis, R. D., 529 
Davis, R. L., 115 
Davis, S. F., 193 
Day, A. L., 482 
Day, R. D., 400 
Dayan, P., 192 
De Beni, R., 229, 244 
de Gelder, B., 109 
Deacon, B. J., 518 
Deák, G. O., 64 
Dean, C., 65 
Dean-Borenstein, M. T., 476 
DeAngelis, D., 9 
Dearing, F., 401 
Deary, I. J., 283 
Deater-Deckard, K., 383, 402 
DeBoeck, P., 325 
DeBord, K. A., 369 
DeCasper, A. J., 389 
Deccache, A., 481 
DeCesare, K., 594 
Decety, J., 26 
Deci, E., 315 
Decker, S., 612 
Dede, G., 88 
Dediu, D., 268 
DeFrain, J., 425 
DeGaton, J. F., 366 
deGroot, A. D., 212 
del Rosal, E., 395 
Delahanty, D. L., 220 
DeLamater, J. D., 428 
Delcourt, M. A. B., 298 
Delgado, M. R., 549 
Delinsky, S. S., 195 
DeLoache, J., 193 
Demaree, H. A., 486 
Dement, W. C., 148 
DeMichael, T., 92-93 
Dempster, F. N., 409 
Denby, L., 472, 499 
Denmark, G. L., 18 
Dennett, D. C., 24 
Dennis, I., 586 
Dennis, T. A., 403 
Denollet, J., 486 
Dent, P., 348 
d’Entremont, M. L., 402 


1-4 Name Index 


Deouell, L. Y., 210 
Der, G., 283 
Deregowski, J. B., 133, 134 
Deremaux, S., 245 
Derryberry, W. P., 445 
DeRubeis, R., 569 
Des Jarlais, D. C., 161 
Descartes, R., 15, 16 
Deshields, T., 482 
deSilva, P., 515 
Desimone, R., 220 
Desmarais, S., 353 
Dessing, J. C., 86 
DeSteno, D., 353, 611 
Detoledo-Morrell, L., 235 
Detterman, D. K., 296 
Deuser, R., 254 
Devonport, J. J., 452 
Dewey, J., 17 

Dey, E. L., 349 

Dhar, R., 256 

Dhillon, S., 565 

Di Fabio, A., 281 

Di Forti, M., 68 
Diamond, M., 370 
DiCano, P., 180 
Dickens, W. T., 303 
Dickerson, S. S., 472 
Diener, E., 495, 497, 498 
Diener, M., 400 
Diesendruck, G., 272 
DiFranza, J. R., 488 
DiGiorlamo, G. J., 60 
DiGiovanna, A. G., 423 
Dijk, D., 310 
Dijksterhuis, A., 134 
Dill, K., 353 

Dillard, J. P., 329, 493 
Dillon, J., 20 
DiLorenzo, P. M., 119 
Dionne, R., 121 
Diriwachter, R., 465 
Dishion, T., 418 
Ditto, B., 123 

Dixon, R. A., 429, 430 
Do, V. T., 599 
Dobbins, A. C., 130 
Dobbins, I. G., 224 
Dodge, K. A., 382 
Doganis, G, 245 

Doi, T., 444 

Dolan, P., 495 

Dolan, R. J., 334 
Dolan, S. L., 560 
Dolbier, C. L., 473 
Doll, H. A., 524 
Dollinger, S. J., 581 
Domhoff, G. W., 147, 149 
Dominoswski, R. L., 253 
Donahoe, J. W., 193 
Donnellan, M., 21 
Donoghue, J. P., 82 
Donovan, R. A., 349 
Dortch, S., 159 

Doty, R. L., 119 
Dougall, A. L., 480 
Douglas, O., 281 


Dovidio, J. F., 598, 599, 602, 612 


Dowd, S., 81 
Dowling, N., 549 
Downey, J. E., 18 
Doyle, R., 570 
Dozois, D., 518 
Draguns, J. G., 443 
Drain, T., 187 
Draine, S. C., 134 
Dresbach, T., 65 
Drewes, F., 400 
Drews, F., 21 
Drews, F. A., 214 
Driver, J., 109, 124 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Drob, S., 445 
Dryden, W., 554 
Duberstein, P. R., 569 
DuBois, F., 609 
Duchaine, B., 98, 451 
Ducharme, J. M., 187 
Dudek-Singer, J., 551, 610 
Duffy, M., 510 
Dugger, C. W., 360 
Duke, M., 529 

Dulac, C., 119 
Dumont, D., 571 
Dumont, M., 347, 571 
Duncker, K., 258 
Durik, A. M., 344 
Dworkin, R. H., 527 
Dybdahl, R., 222 


E 

Eagly, A., 47 

Eagly, A. H., 343, 353 
Eaker, E. D., 486 
Eardley, I., 374 

Earls, F. J., 202 
Ebbinghaus, H., 11, 17, 231 
Eberhard, J., 564 
Eberhard, K. M., 265 
Eberhardt, J. L., 598 
Eberling, J., 75 
Ebstein, R. P., 457 
Ebster, C., 594 
Ecenbarger, W., 490 
Echemendia, R., 462 
Eckardt, M. H., 446 
Eden, J., 592 
Edinger, J. D., 152, 550 
Edwards, C. C., 489 
Edwards, R. R., 121 
Eftimiades, M., 32, 611 
Egan, K., 408 
Egliston, K., 551 
Ehlers, C. L., 165 
Ehlert, U., 479 
Eimer, B. N., 156 
Einarsson, C., 354 
Einstein, A., 313, 382 
Einstein, G. O., 237 
Eisch, A., 88 
Eisenberg, M., 531 
Eisenberg, N., 612 
Eizenberg, M. M., 257 
Ekman, P., 329, 335, 335-336, 336, 337 
Ekroll, V., 131 
El-Hai, J., 568 
El-Mallakh, R. S., 169 
Elder, G. H., Jr., 431 
Elfhag, K., 320 

Eliez, S., 283 

Eliot, T. S., 282 
Elkind, D., 418 
Elkins, D., 459 
Elkins, G., 156 
Ellason, J. W., 519 
Ellins, E., 476 

Elliot, J., 424 

Elliott, A., 506 
Elliott, A. J., 325 
Elliott, D. M., 372 
Ellis, A., 554, 613 
Ellis, B. J., 473 
Ellison, C. R., 358 
Ellison, N., 606 
Emery, G., 516 
Emick, J., 252 
Emmelkamp, P., 550 
Endres, T., 193 
Engen, T., 118 

Engle, R. W., 213, 281 
Engler, J., 539 

Epley, N., 134 
Epstein, R., 257, 493 


Epstein, W., 108 
Eranti, S. V., 567 
Ergene, N., 122 
Erickson, R., 119 
Eriksen, M., 490 
Erikson, E., 403-404, 416-417, 446 
Ertel, S., 135 

Ervik, S., 150 

Espelag, D. L., 369 
Essau, C. A., 523 
Essén, B., 360 
Etchegary, H., 75 
Evans, A. M., 547 
Evans, D. E., 456 
Evans, J. B. T., 252, 260 
Evcik, D., 123 
Everette, M., 390 
Everhart, D. E., 486 
Everly, G. S., Jr., 507 
Eynan, R., 530 
Eysenck, H. J., 449, 459, 460 


F 

Fagan, J., 426 

Fagan, J. F., 300, 301, 302 
Fagen, J., 118 

Fairhall, A., 102 
Falck-Ytter, T., 64 
Fallon, A., 50 

Fallon, B. A., 518 
Fanselow, M. S., 65 
Farell, B., 109 
Farinelli, L., 34 
Farrell, E. F., 372 
Fassler, O., 155 

Fast, K., 119 

Faust, D., 561 

Fearing, V. G., 557 
Fedeli, A., 319 
Federer, R., 585 

Fee, E., 122 

Feeney, A., 260 
Feeney, B. C., 586 
Feinberg, A. W., 430 
Feingold, A., 348 
Feinstein, S., 518 
Feldhusen, J. F., 215 
Feldman, D. H., 407 
Feldman, R. S., 237, 292, 349, 400, 472 
Feldt, L. S., 291 
Feltmate, S. E., 430 
Fendt, M., 193 

Fenter, V. L., 566 
Feret, A., 388 
Fergusson, D. M., 419 
Fernandez, A. M., 353 
Fernandez, L. C., 18 
Ferraro, R. F., 224 
Feshbach, S., 40 
Festinger, D., 312 
Festinger, L., 17, 582 
Fichtenberg, C. M., 489 
Field, T., 395 

Fields, R. D., 61 
Fields-Meyer, T., 412 
Fillingim, R. B., 121, 313 
Finch, A. J., Jr., 613 
Finch, H., 325 

Fine, L., 255 

Fine, R., 255 
Fingelkurts, A. A., 155 
Fink, A., 283 

Fink, B., 351 

Fink, G., 567 
Finkelhor, D., 371, 372 
Finkelstein, M., 313 
Finkelstein, Y., 391 
Finkler, K., 158 

Finlay, F. O., 413 
Finley, C. L., 152 
Finn-Stevenson, M., 390 


Firestein, B. A., 367 
First, M. B., 509 
Fischer, A. H., 329, 334 
Fischer, K. W., 330 
Fischer, T. M., 102 
Fischler, G., 464 

Fish, M. C., 369 
Fishbach, A., 256 
Fisher, C. B., 49 
Fisher, L., 213 

Fisher, L. A., 156 
Fisk, J. E., 247, 248 
Fiske, S. T., 345, 583 
Fitch, W. T., 268 
Fitzgerald, H., 347 
Fitzgerald, P., 81 
Flam, F., 118 

Flavell, S. W., 66 
Fleck, J. L., 151 
Fleischman, D. A., 430 
Fleming, J., 462 
Fleminger, S., 531 
Flynn, J., 303 

Flynn, P. M., 168 
Fobes, J. L., 84 
Foderaro, L. W., 520 
Fogarty, J. S., 493 
Fogel, A., 395 

Folk, C., 128 
Folkman, S., 474, 480, 483 
Follett, K., 587 
Forbes, G. B., 347 
Forbey, J., 465 

Ford, C. S., 362 

Forer, B., 461 

Forgas, J. P., 584 
Forlenza, M. J., 70 
Förster, J., 598 
Fossey, D., 38 

Foster, D. H, 112 
Foster, K., 286 

Foster, K. M., 203 
Foster, P., 217 

Fotti, S., 551 

Foulds, J., 488 
Fountas, K. N., 84 
Fournier, J., 245 
Fowler, C. A., 117 
Fowler, J. H., 489, 496 
Fox, C. R., 249 

Fox, K., 370 

Fox, M. J., 67 

Fox, S., 286 

Fradera, A., 236 
Frances, A., 509 
Frankenburg, W. K., 394 
Frankham, P., 319 
Franklin, A., 395 
Franklin, M. E., 550 
Franko, D., 414 
Franks, D. D., 333 
Franks, P., 493 
Franzek, E., 527 
Fraser, S. C., 593 
Frasure-Smith, N., 487 
Frawley, T., 354 
Frederickson, B. L., 329 
Freedman, D. S., 321 
Freedman, J. L., 593 
Freeman, A., 554 
Freeman, J. E., 26 
Freeman, K., 474 
Freire, J. C., 557 
Frensch, P. A., 200 
Freud, A., 18, 443, 446 


Freud, S., 16, 18, 19, 39, 147, 368, 
437-445, 447, 459, 469, 518 


Friborg, O., 474, 482 
Friedberg, R. D., 553 
Friederici, A. D., 395 
Friedman, C. K., 353 


Friedman, J. N. W., 529 
Friedman, M. J., 475 
Friesen, W. V., 336 
Frijda, N. H., 329, 334 
Frimer, J., 416 

Frimer, J. A., 612 
Frincke, J. L., 11 
Frings, L., 90 

Frith, C. D., 109 
Fritsch, T., 430 

Fritz, S. M., 262 

Frost, L. E., 504 

Frost, R. O., 516, 518 
Fuchs- Lacelle, S., 122 
Fuerst, D. E., 374 
Fujiki, M., 330 

Fuller, A., 156 
Funder, D. C., 450 
Funke, G. J., 409 
Furnham, A., 358, 446 
Furumoto, L., 18 
Fusari, A., 119 

Fyer, A. J., 549, 550 


G 

Gaab, J., 479 

Gabriel, L. T., 296 
Gaertner, S. L., 598, 602 
Gaffney, K. F., A-15 
Gage, F. H., 90 

Gage, P., 87 

Gagnon, G. H., 358 
Gailliot, M. T., 612 
Gain, M. E., 53 
Galanter, E., 100 
Galanter, M., 559 
Galantucci, B., 117 
Galatzer-Levy, R. M., 546 
Galef, D., 462 

Gall, F. J., 15, 16 

Gallo, P., 267 

Gallois, C., 579 

Gallup News Service, 366 
Gallup Poll, 135 
Galton, Sir F., 287, 291 
Galvin, D. M., 169 
Gambescia, N., 374 
Gamez, E., 247 

Gami, A. S., 150 
Gangestad, S. W., 357 
Gannon, T., 346 
Ganong, L. H., 426 
Garb, H. N., 466 
Garber, J., 530, 572 
Garcia, A., 550 

Garcia, J., 181 

Garcia, R., 271 

Garcia, S. M., 611 
Garcia-Andrade, C., 165 
Garcia-Mijares, M., 193 
Garcia-Palacios, A., 46 
Gardini, S., 244, 457 
Gardner, B., 567 
Gardner, E. P., 121 
Gardner, F., 613 
Gardner, H., 88, 99, 281, 282 
Gardner, S., 18, 162 
Garlow, S. J., 165 

Garn, C. L., 350 
Garretson, M. E., 228 
Garrido-Nag, K., 266 
Garrigan, P., 130 

Garry, M., 229 
Garwick, G. B., 296 
Garwood, C. L., 489 
Garza-Guerrero, C., 605 
Gass, C. S., 38 

Gatchel, R. J., 122, 323 
Gautreaux, G., 551, 610 
Gawronski, B., 518 
Gazzaniga, M. S., 91 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Ge, X., 413 

Geary, D. C., 352 
Gegenfurtner, K. R., 112 
Geitman, L. R., 268 
Gelbard-Sagiv, H., 221 
Gelfand, M. M., 428 
Gelman, R., 394, 408 
General Motors, 467 
Gennaro, R. J., 140 
Genovese, J. E. C., 407 
Genovese, K., 32, 34, 611 
Gentile, B., 348, 453 
Gentner, D., 270 
George, J., 559 

George, L. K., 431 
George, M. S., 333 
George, P., 124 

George, R., 395 

George, S., 164 

George, T. P., 395 
Georgeson, M., 110, 130 
Georgia Tech, 429 
Georgiou, G. A., 193 
Geppert, C. M., 559 
Gepshtein, S., 108 
Gerdes, A., 193 
Gershkoff-Stowe, L., 267 
Gerson, K., 426 

Gerstel, N, 426 

Getner, D., 262 
Giacobbi, P. R., Jr., 474 
Giacomini, M., 89 
Gibb, K., 296 

Gibb, R., 88 

Gibbons, R. D., 566 
Gibbs, N., 412 

Gibbs, W. W., 265 
Gielen, U. P., 11 
Gilbert, D. T., 448 
Gilbert, P. D., 483 
Gilboa, A., 236 
Gilchrist, A., 212 
Gilestro, G. F., 144 
Gilligan, C., 415, 417 
Gilliss, C., 424 
Ginsberg, S., 418 
Giusberti, F., 244 
Gladwell, M., 446, 466, 468 
Glantz, S. A., 489 

Glass, G. V., 560 

Glass, K., 480 

Glick, P., 345, 347 
Glickler, J., 452 
Glicksohn, J., 449, 584 
Glisky, E. L., 430 

Goin, M. K., 544 

Gold, M., 162 

Gold, P. E., 237 
Goldberg, M. D., 298 
Golden, R. N., 151, 156 
Goldfried, M. R., 561 
Goldin, S., 270 
Goldin-Meadow, S., 270 
Goldman, E., 569 
Goldsmith, H. H., 530 
Goldstein, I., 356, 358, 359, 374 
Goldstein, S. N., 417 
Goldstone, R. L., 245 
Goleman, D., 397, 494, 539 
Golimbet, V. E., 74, 456 
Golombok, S., 369 
Golosheykin, S., 157 
Goncalves, F. L., 193 
Gong, H., 112 

Gontier, N., 268 
Gontkovsky, S. T., 84, 283 
Goode, E., 118, 547 
Goodman, G., 228 
Goodman, W. K., 517 
Goodwin, J. E., 395 
Goodwin, R., 591 


Goodwin, R. D., 529 
Gooren, L., 368 
Gordijn, M. C. M., 151 
Gordon, E. F., 47, 412 
Gordon, R., 119 
Gossop, M., 559 
Gotlib, I. H., 523 
Gottesman, I. I., 381, 527 
Gottlieb, D. A., 189 
Gould, E., 90 

Gould, R. L., 424 
Govindarajan, A., 218 
Gowaty, P. A., 371 
Grace, A. A., 219 
Gradinaru, V., 81 
Grady, D., 489 

Graf, P., 224 

Graham, C. A., 507 
Graham, J. R., 464 
Graham, L., 83 
Graham, S., 50, 414, 531 
Grahek, N., 122 
Grainger, L., 215 
Grammer, G. G., 493 
Grange, D. K., 390 
Granic, I., 418 

Grann, J. D., 233 
Granstroem, K., 354 
Grant, S., 466 

Gray, G. C., 611 

Gray, J. R., 283 

Gray, R., 391 
Graziano, M. S., 86 
Graziano, W. G., 612 
Greco, C. M., 93 
Green, B. G., 124 
Green, D. L., 156 
Green, J. S., 181 
Green, M. C., 579 
Green, P. R., 110, 130 
Greenberg, G., 508 
Greenberg, R., 569 
Greenberg, R. M., 567 
Greene, J. D., 334, 612-613 
Greenfield, P. M., 301 
Greenfield, S., 140 
Greening, E., 135 
Greenspan, S., 296 
Greenwald, A., 601 
Greenwald, A. G., 134, 224 
Greer, R. D., 551, 610 
Gregg, J. P., 74 
Gregory, R. L., 131, 132 
Gregory, S., 362 
Gregory, S. S., 360 
Gretchen, D., 562 
Griffin, D. W., 605 
Grigorenko, E., 268 
Grigorenko, E. L., 285, 302 
Grigoriadis, S., 558 
Grilo, C. M., 322 
Grimes, T., 40 
Grindstaff, J. S., 156 
Grinspoon, S., 492 
Groh, K., 422 
Gronholm, P., 80 
Gross, J. J., 329 
Grossmann, T., 395 
Groves, R. M., 38 
Grucza, R., 165 
Grunwald, T., 334 
Grunwald, T, 395 
Grusec, J. E., 200, 551 
Guadagno, R. E., 580 
Guarnaccia, P. J., 508, 538 
Guastella, A., 77 
Guéguen, N., 594 
Guerrero, L., 34 
Guez, J., 234 

Gugula, S., 353 
Guilleminault, C., 150 


Name Index 


Guldemond, H., 297 
Gurin, P., 35 

Guthrie, I. K., 612 
Guthrie, R. V., 50 
Gutierrez, P. M., 418, 419 
Guyll, M., 598 

Gwynn, M. I., 154 


H 

Haas, K. B., 347 
Haberstick, B. C., 162, 387 
Hackam, D. C., 53 
Hadjistavropoulos, T., 122 
Hagoort, P., 268 
Haier, R. J., 280, 283 
Hains, S. C., 591 
Haley, W. E., 495 
Halford, S., 400 
Halgren, E., 268 
Halkitis, P., 163 

Hall, B. J., 26 

Hall, E., 422, 426 

Hall, G. C. N., 371 
Hall, J., 584 

Hall, J. A., 493 

Hall, N. W., 390 

Hall, R. E., 302 

Halle, M., 60 

Halle, T., 401 

Hallett, K. C., 451 
Hallschmid, M., 319 
Halpern, D., 27 
Halpern, D. F., 349, 426 
Halpert, J., 493 
Hamani, Y., 492 
Hamann, S., 219 
Hamann, S. B., 333 
Hambleton, R. K., 461 
Hamed, M. S., 551 
Hamer, D. H., 368 
Hamer, M., 483 
Hamilton, A., 429 
Hamilton, A. C., 211 
Hamilton, S. P., 529 
Hamilton, W. L., 489 
Hammond, C., 156, 162 
Hamrick, N., 480 
Hand, V., 508 

Haney, C., 593 
Hanley, C., 201 
Hanley, S. J., 314 
Hannon, E. E., 395 
Hanson, D. R., 381, 527 
Harding, D. J., 366 
Hardison, D. M., 265 
Hardt, J., 419 

Hardy, L. T., 399 
Harlow, H. F., 399 
Harlow, J. M., 87 
Harmon-Jones, E., 600 
Harold, G. T., 425 
Harper, D. J., 567 
Harper, T., 427 

Harris, J. L., 349 
Harris, K. R., 283, 531 
Harris, R., 67 


Harry Harlow Primate Laboratory 
(University of Wisconsin), 399 


Hart, B., 267 
Hartmann, E., 143 
Hartung, C. M., 537 
Harvard Mental Health Letter 
(HMHL), 573 
Harvey, J. H., 367 
Harvey, P. D., 527 
Harvey-Berino, J., 321 
Haslam, A., 258 
Haslam, C., 391 
Hassold, T. J., 278 
Hastings, R. P., 297 
Hatfield, E., 367 


1-6 Name Index 


Hatsopoulos, N. G., 82 
Hau, K. T., 318 
Hauke, C., 445 
Hauser, M. D., 268, 408 
Hausmann, R., 344 
Havercamp, S. M., 314 
Havermans, R. C., 550 
Haviland-Jones, J. M., 119 
Haw, S., 488 

Hawkes, C. H., 119 
Hawley, E., 132 
Haworth, C., 74 
Hayflick, L., 427 
Hayne, H., 229 
Haynes, P., 158 
Hayes, P. A., 562 

He, S-Z., 425 

Head, K., 284 

Health Pages, 492 
Heard, H. L., 551 
Heath, R. A., 369 
Heatherton, T., 487 
Hecht, J. M., 498 
Hedgepeth, E., 415 
Hedges, D. W., 565 
Hedlund, J., 284 
Hegarty, P., 298, 579 
Heier, M. S., 150 
Heilman, K. M., 261 
Heilman, M. E., 346 
Heim, C., 77, 372 
Heimann, M., 408 
Heinrichs, R. W., 525 
Heisel, M. J., 530 
Heller, S., 444 

Heller, W., 90 
Helmbold, N., 283 
Helms, J. E., 300 
Helmuth, L., 66 
Helstrup, T., 229 
Henderlong, J., 313 
Henderson, F. R., 181 
Henderson, J., 391 
Henderson, N. D., 302 
Henik, A., 123 
Heninger, M., 165 
Hennig-Fast, K., 520 
Henningsen, D. D., 592 
Henningsen, M. L., 592 
Henrich, C., 133 
Henry, D., 149 
Henry, L. L., A-15 
Hentschel, U., 443 
Herbenick, D., 365 
Hering, E., 112 
Herman, C. P., 322 
Herman, D. B., 475 
Herman, J., 524 
Herman, K. C., 26 
Herrán, A., 515 
Herrington, D. M., 77 
Herrmann, A., 256 
Herrnstein, R., 301 
Herskovits, M. J., 133 
Hertlein, K. M., 374 
Hertzog, C., 430 
Herzberg, L., 334 
Heshka, S., 323 

Hess, M. J., 92 

Hess, T. M., 430, 587 
Heward, W. L., 295 
Hewstone, M., 583 
Heyman, G. D., 272 
Heyn, E., 360 
Hibbard, P., 129 
Hiby, E. F., 189 
Hicks, B. M., 612 
Hicks, T. V., 358 
Higgins, S., 20 
Hilarski, C., 529 
Hilgard, E., 155 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Hill, J. O., 317 

Hill, S. S., 50 

Hillix, W. A., 271 
Hines, M., 90, 351, 352 
Hinson, J. T., 430 
Hippocrates, 16 
Hirsch, J., 321 
Hirschler, B., 75 
Hirsh, I. J., 117 

Hirt, E. R., 227 

Ho, S. M. Y., 494 
Hobfoll, S. E., 26, 474 
Hobson, J. A., 149 
Hochschild, A., 426 
Hock, H. S., 99 

Hoff, E., 265, 267, 269 
Hoffer, T. B., 11 
Hoffmann, H., 46 
Hoffmann, M., 424, 550 
Höflinger, A., 223 
Hofmann, W., 224 
Hogan, J., 468 

Hogan, R., 468 

Hoge, C.W., 475 
Hogg, M. A., 591, 599 
Hojjat, M., 607 

Hol, E. M., 89 
Holden, C., 89, 525 
Holden, L. M., 524 
Holden, R., 248 
Holland, C. R., 300, 301, 302 
Holland, J. C., 487 
Hollenstein, T., 418 
Holler, G. D., 151 
Holleran, S., 584 
Hollin, C. R., 371 
Hollingworth, H. L., 18 
Hollingworth, L. S., 17, 18 
Hollins, K., 390 
Hollis, K. L., 181 
Hollon, S., 569 
Hollon, S. D., 569 
Holloway, L., 271 
Holmes, A., 517 
Holmes, J. G., 605 
Holowka, S., 90 

Holt, M., 66 

Holy, T. E., 119 
Holyoak, K. J., 93, 262 
Home, J. A., 146 
Hongchun, W., 155 
Honorton, C., 135 
Honzik, C. H., 198 
Hope, K., 571 
Hopkins, W., 89 
Hoppey, D., 297 
Hopwood, C., 530 
Horinek, D., 235 
Horn, J. L., 302 
Horne, S. G., 358 
Horney, K., 18, 439, 446, 459 
Hornsey, M. J., 579, 590 
Horowitz, H. A., 530 
Horowitz, J. L., 530 
Hort, J.,235 

Horton, K. D., 225 
Horwood, L. J., 419 
Houg, S., 92 
Houghtalen, R. P., 519 
Howard, T. D., 77 
Howe, C. J., 267 
Howes, O., 68, 506, 527 
Howitt, D., 35 
Hoyme, E. H., 296 
Hoyt, C., 579 

Hsu, B., 358 

Hu, F. B., 324 
Hubbard, E. M., 123, 124 
Hubbard, K., 317 
Hubel, D., 17, 108, 109 
Huber, F., 256 


Hudson, W., 133 
Hudspeth, A. J., 87, 117 
Huerta, M., 347 
Hugdahl, K., 351 
Hugenberg, K., 600 
Huijie, T., 383 

Hull, C. L., 310 
Hummer, T. A., 119 
Humphrey, N., 150 
Humphreys, G. W., 127 
Hungi, N., 237 

Hunt, E., 256, 283 
Hunt, J. S., 598 

Hunt, M., 364, 365, 367 
Hurt, C. S., 430 
Hussain, R., 296 
Huston, A. C., 202 
Hutchinson, S. L., 480 
Hutchison, K. E., 161 
Hyde, J., 348, 366, 369, 370, 524 
Hyde, J. S., 343, 344, 350, 364 
Hyde, K., 87 

Hyman, M., 546 
Hyman, R., 135 
Hyman, S. E., 74 


l 

Iachini, T., 244 

Iacoboni, M., 64 

Iaria, G., 200 

Ievers-Landis, C. E., 390 

Iglesias, A., 156 

Igo, S. E., 38 

Thler, E., 493 

Ikonomidou, C., 391 

lies, R., 74, 382 

Imamura, M., 131 

Indiana University-Purdue 
University, 467 

Inhelder, B., 406 

Innocenti, G. M., 389 

Insko, C., 605 

Interlandi, J., 140 

International Human Genome 
Sequencing Consortium, 388 

Irons, G., 178 

Irwin, M., 479 

Irwin, R. R., 157 

Isaac, M., 475 

Isaacson, J. S., 88 

Isacson, O., 75 

Isay, R. A., 368 

Isbell, L.M., 602 

Ishii, K., 270 

Ishikawa, S., 554 

Iversen, L., 67, 506 

Iversen, S., 67, 506 

Iverson, L., 168 

Iverson, P., 265 

Ivry, R. B., 60 

Ivy, L., 402 

Iyengar, S. S., 313 

Izaki, Y., 219 

Izard, C. E., 336, 399 


J 

Jaasma, M. A., 611 
Jackendoff, R., 269 
Jacklin, C., 349 
Jackson, A., 549 
Jackson, J. D., 482 
Jacob, K. S., 534 
Jacobs, J. A., 426 
Jacobs, M., 322 
Jacobson, N. S., 465 
Jacoby, L. L., 234 
Jacquez, F. M., 530 
Jaffé, A., 75 

Jahn, R., 66 

Jain, S., 475 


James, H: S., Jr., 313 
James, W., 15, 16, 331 
Jamieson, G. A., 155 
Jamison, K. R., 520, 522 
Janca, A., 474, 475 
Jang, H., 315 

Jang, K., 74 

Jang, S. J., 88 

Janicak, P., 81 

Janis, I. L., 592 
Janson, G. R., 370 
Jarlais, D. C. D., 164 
Jarvin, L., 288 

Jasso, H., 64 

Jayson, S., 537 
Jefferson, N. C., 163 
Jena, S., 122 

Jencks, C., 366 
Jenkins, J. M., 328 
Jenkins, S. R., 11, 326 
Jensen, A. R., 283, 287, 462 
Jensen, L., 366 
Jequier, E., 319 
Jessell, T. M., 70 
Jessen, S., 124 

Jeter, D., 61, 382 
Jetten, J., 590 

Jha, A., 220 

Ji, D., 108 

Jiang, L., 291 

Jinks, A. L., 119 

Jobes, D. A., 544 

Joe, G. W., 168 
John-Steiner, V., 410 
Johnsdotter, S., 360 
Johnson, C., 131, 429 
Johnson, G., 249 
Johnson, G. B., 65 
Johnson, H. D., 326 
Johnson, J. A., 492 
Johnson, J. G., 202 
Johnson, P. J., 181 
Johnson, R. J., 495 
Johnson, S. P., 395, 396 
Johnson, V. E., 358, 359, 368, 374 
Johnson-Greene, D., 50 
Johnson-Laird, P.N., 247 
Johnston, L. D., 489 
Johnston, M., 164, 168 
Johnston, M. V., 90 
Johnston, W. A., 159 
Joiner, T. E., Jr., 510 
Jones, A. L., 446 
Jones, E. E., 548 
Jones, J. E., 318 

Jones, J. M., 403 
Jones, L. M., 371 
Jones, M., 156, 169 
Jones, R., 413 

Jones, R. K., 366 
Jones, S., 528 
Jorgensen, G., 416 
Josephs, R. A., 165 
Jourdan, A., 557 
Joyce, J., 503 

Julien, R. M., 168 
June, C. H., 388 

Jung, C. G., 439, 446, 447, 459 
Jung, J., 164 

Jung, K., 347 

Jung, R. E., 280 
Juslin, P., 248 

Juster, F. T., 426 

Jylha, M., 431 


K 

Kadosh, R., 123 

Kagan, J., 456 
Kahneman, D., 254, 497 
Kahng, S. K., 510 
Kainz, K. M., 110 


Kalb, C., 121, 123, 161 
Kaller, C. P., 256 
Kallio, S., 155 
Kaminski, P., 322 
Kamphaus, R. W., 466 
Kanaya, T., 228 
Kandel, E. R., 70, 87, 121 
Kane, M. J., 281 
Kangawa, K., 319 
Kanner, A. D., 476 
Kantrowitz, B., 165 
Kaplan, J. R., 44 
Kaplan, H. S., 358 
Kaplan, M. S., 474 
Kaplan, R. M., 461 
Kapur, S., 68, 506, 527 
Kara, P., 129 

Karama, S., 283 
Karaszewski, B., 329 
Karni, A., 149 

Kasof, J., 151 
Kassam, K. S., 226 
Kassel, J. D., 488 
Kassin, S. M., 21 
Kaštelan, A., 179 
Kato, K., 481 

Kato, N., 567 

Kato, R., 89 

Kato, T., 523 
Katsiyannis, A., 296 
Katz, J., 122 

Katz, L., 551 

Katz, M., 310 
Kaufman, A., 131, 429 
Kaufman, J. C., 262, 285 
Kawakami, K., 602 
Kawasaki, C., 403 
Kawashima, H., 219 
Kaya, M. C., 564 
Kazar, D. B., 156 
Kazdin, A., 562 
Keane, M. M., 236 
Kearns, K. P., 88 
Keating, D. P., 407 
Kee, K. F., 606 

Keehn, J. D., 184 
Keillor, J. M., 336 
Kelleher, R. J., 218 
Keller, H., 282 

Keller, J., 349 

Kelley, H., 584 
Kellman, P., 130 
Kellner, C. H., 567 
Kelly, D., 281 

Kelly, J. B., 425 
Keltikangas-Jarvinen, L., 456 
Keltner, D., 328 
Kemeny, M. E., 472 
Kemp, S., 109 
Kempermann, G., 90 
Kemps, E., 229 
Kendler, K., 162 
Kendler, K. S., 336, 521 
Kennedy, J. E., 135 
Kensinger, E. A., 245, 334 
Kenway, L., 148 
Kersten, A., 245 
Kesebir, P., 495 
Kesmodel, U., 391 
Kess, J. F., 91 

Kessels, R., 236 
Kessler, R. C., 534 
Kettenmann, H., 61 
Key, W. B., 134 

Khan, A. S., 38 
Khayyer, M., 313 
Khazaal, Y., 319 
Kidd, E., 267 

Kidd, K. K., 285, 302 
Kiecolt, J. K., 426 
Kiefer, A. K., 343 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Kihlstrom, J. F., 155, 224, 520 
Kilner, J. M., 109 
Kim, D. R., 567 
Kim, H., 121 

Kim, J-J., 336 

Kim, J-W., 336 

Kim, K.H., 272 

Kim, N., 131 

Kim, S., 424 

Kim, S-E., 336 
Kimbrel, N. A., 514 
Kim-Cohen, J., 457 
Kimura, D., 351 
King, M., 75 

King, R. A., 566 
King-Casas, B., 530 
Kinsey, A. C., 364, 367-368 
Kirby, D., 364 

Kirk, E. P., 324 

Kirk, K. M., 368 
Kirkby, J., 148 
Kirsch, I., 181 
Kirsch, L., 155 
Kirschenbaum, H., 557 
Kisac, I., 474 

Kish, S., 163 

Kiss, A., 493 
Kit-Fong Au, T., 394 
Kitayama, S., 588 
Kleiber, D. A., 431 
Kleinman, A., 157, 538 
Kleinman, J., 157, 538 
Klerman, G. L., 558 
Kling, K. C., 348 
Klinger, M. R., 134 
Klötz, F., 77 

Kluck, A., 322 
Kluger, J., 249, 513 
Klump, K., 322 
Knight, S. C., 156 
Knoblich, G., 134 
Knox, J., 155 
Kobayashi, F., 330 
Koch, C., 140 

Koch, J., 354 

Koch, K. L., 118 
Kocur, J., 188 

Koff, W. J., 370 
Kogan, N., 261 
Kohlberg, L., 414-415, 415 
Kohler, C. G., 336 
Köhler, W., 256 
Kohn, D. B., 388 
Kojima, M., 319 
Kolarov, V., 581 
Kolata, G., 68 

Kolb, D., 88 
Konczak, J., 130 
Kondo, T., 483 
Konig, R., 389 
Koocher, G. P., 50 
Koole, S., 326 
Kopelman, M. D., 531 
Koper, R. J., 611 
Koplewicz, H., 530 
Kopp, T. G., 459 
Kordower, J., 75 
Korecka, J. A., 89 
Koren, G., 391 

Korn, J., 475 
Kosambi, D. D., 122 
Kosfeld, M., 77 
Koss, M. P., 370, 371 
Kosslyn, S. M., 60, 90, 244 
Kostygina, A., 202 
Kotov, R., 510 
Kotre, J., 422, 426 
Kotz, S. A., 124 
Koukoulis, G. N., 77 
Kounios, J., 151 
Koutstaal, W., 281 


Kovacs, A. M., 272 
Kovelman, I., 273 
Kowert, P. A., 592 
Koyama, A, 216 
Kozak, A. T., 549 
Kozaric-Kovacic, D., 179 
Kozulin, A., 410 
Kramer, A. F., 429 
Kramer, P., 566 
Kreher, D., 217 
Kreuger, A., 497 
Krijn, M., 548 
Krueger, K., 192 
Krueger, R., 457 
Krueger, R. G., 612 
Krull, D. S., 585 
Krumm, S., 281 
Krusemark, E., 587 
Ku, L; 372 
Kübler-Ross, E., 432 
Kubovy, M., 108 
Kuhlman, D. M., 311 
Kumari, V., 368 
Kunzmann, U., 425 
Kuo, L. J., 272 
Kuppens, P., 330 
Kuriyama, K., 148 
Kuther, T. L., 11, 12, 2003 
Kvavilashvili, L., 213 
Kwan, K., 453 
Kwate, N. O. A., 301 
Kwon, P., 523 
Kwon, S. M., 515 


L 
La Valley, A., 34 
Laas, I., 314 
LaBar, K., 334 
Labouliere, C. D., 549 
Lacerda, F., 408 
Lacey, M., 360 
Ladd, D. R., 268 
Ladewig, J., 192 
Laederach-Hofmann, K., 515 
LaFrance, M., 349 
Lagacé-Séguin, D. G., 402 
Laham, S. M., 584 
Lahti, J., 456 
Laing, D. G., 119 
Laing, R. D., 505 
Laird, J. D., 331 
Lakhan, S., 74 
Lal, S.,18 
Lamal, P. A., 5, 257 
Lamb, M. E., 228, 400 
Lamborn, S. D., 422 
Lampe, C., 606 
Lane, A. M., 452 
Lane, K. A., 168, 601 
Lang, A. J., 122 
Lang, G., 123 
Langan-Fox, J., 466 
Langdon, R., 525 
Langdridge, D., 587 
Lange, C., 331 
Langer, E. J., 53, 68 
Langley, J., 310 
Langreth, R., 483 
Lankov, A., 425 
Lanza, S. T., 414 
Lappin, J., 68 
Laqueur, T. W., 364 
Larcher, V., 75 
Larsen, R. J., 450 
Larson, J., 403 
Larsson, M., 118 
Lascaratos, G., 108 
Latané, B., 34-35, 37, 41, 42, 44, 45, 46, 
49, 50, 611 
Latif, T., 567 
Lating, J. M., 507 


Name Index 


Latner, J., 21 

Latner, J. D., 195 
Laucht, M., 167 
Laugharne, J., 474 
Laukli, E., 427 
Laumann, E. O., 364, 374 
Laurenceau, J. P., 523 
Lauterbach, D., 551 
Lavelli, M., 395 
Laven, A., 537 
Lavenex, P., 218 
Lawrence, J., 348 
Lawrence, W., 391 
Lawson, M. J., 237 
Lazarus, R. S., 328, 483 
Le Maner-Idrissi, G., 354 
Leahy, R. L., 554 
Leaper, C., 349, 353 
LeBow, M. D., 323 
Leckman, J. F., 566 
Lee, A., 157, 475 

Lee, A. Y., 495 

Lee, D., 538 

Lee, F., 272 

Lee, H. J., 515 

Lee, J. W., 47 

Lee, K., 346, 424 

Lee, S., 588 
Lee-Chai, A. Y., 326 
Lee-Chiong, T. I., 150 
Lefaucheur, J. P., 567 
Leffard, S. A., 466 
Lehar, S., 127 
Lehman, D. R., 313, 481 
Lehrman, S., 370 
Leib, J. R., 390 
Leibel, R. L., 321 
Leiblum, S. R., 356 
Leigh, J. H., 223 
Leitenberg, H., 358 
Leiter, S., 462 

Leiter, W. M., 462 
Leitner, L. M., 562 
Lemay, E. P., Jr., 586 
Lemonick, M. D., 161 
Lendon, C., 430 
Lenhart, A., 606 
Lens, W., 325 
Lenzenweger, M. F., 527, 529 
Leo, R. J., 567 
Leonard, E., 308 
Lepage, J. F., 64, 200 
Leppänen, J. M., 396 
Lepper, M. R., 313 
Lepre, A., 473 

Lesch, K. P., 382 
Lesperance, F., 487 
Lester, D., 497 

Leu, J., 272 
Levander, S., 564 
Levant, R. F, 561 
LeVay, S., 368 
Levenson, R. W., 336 
Levey, G., 381 

Levi, A., 582 

Levin, B. E., 147, 321 
Levin, J. R., 213, 237 
Levin, R. J., 365 
Levine, B., 430 
Levine, J. M., 591 
Levine, S. Z., 526 
Levinson, D. J., 424 
Levinson, S., 178 
Levitt, S., 584 

Levy, B., 430 

Levy, B. R., 431 
Lewin, T., 418 
Lewinsohn, P. M., 523 
Lewis, C. M., 559 
Lewis, R., 107 
Lewis, S., 487 


l-8 Name Index 


Li, J., 26, 330 

Li, M. D., 487 

Li, T-K, 159, 258 
Liang, K. A., 365 
Liben, L. S., 353 

Lidz, J., 268 
Lieberman, M. D., 600 
Lien, Y-W., 587, 588 
Lilienfeld, S. O., 27,561 
Lin, C-H., 611 

Lin, H-M., 611 

Lin, Y. Y., 91 

Lin, Z., 25 

Lincoln, A., 313 
Lindahl, M., 479 
Lindblad, F., 479 
Lindemann, O., 131 
Lindh-Astrand, L., 424 
Lindley, L. D., 50 
Lindorff, M., 482 
Lindsay, P., 156 
Lindsay, P. H., 216 
Lindsey, E., 400 
Lindy, J., 84 

Links, P. S., 530 
Linssen, H., 418 
Linz, S. J., 326 

Lippa, R. A., 350 
Lips, H. M., 344 
Lipsitz, J. D., 549, 550 
Lishner, D., 612 
Liszkowski, U, 271 
Litowitz, B. E., 445 
Little, A., 605 

Little, K., 164 

Liu, H., 81 

Liu, J. H., 598 

Liu, L., 425 

Liu, Q., 115 

Liu, X., 131, 429 
Livesley, W., 74 
Livingstone, H. A., 482 
Lloyd, B. B., 353 
Lobato, M. I., 370 
Lobban, F., 528 

Lobo, I., 67 

LoBue, V., 193 
Locicero, A., 21 

Lock, J., 322 

Locke, E. A., 452 
Locke, J., 15, 16 
Locke, J. L., 266 
Lockhart, R., 223 
Lockl, K., 409 
Loehlin, J. C., 303 
Lofholm, N., 309 
Loftus, E. F., 17, 21, 217, 228, 229 
Loftus, G. R., 21, 409 
Lohr, J. M., 27 
Loken, E., 292 

Long, G. M., 210 
Lopes, A. C., 568 
López, S. R., 508, 538 
Lorenz, K., 398-399, 608-609 
Lothane, Z., 445 
Love, G., 348 
Loveland, J., 493 
Loving, T., 584 
LoVullo, S., 187 
Lowe, P., 150 
Lowenstein, J. H., 102 
Lowery, D., 313 

Loy, J. W., 584 

Lu, J., 144 

Lubell, K. M., 419 
Lubinski, D., 297, 350 
Lublin, H., 564 
Lucas, R. E., 495, 497 
Lucas, W., 161 
Lucchina, L., 120 
Luchins, A. S., 259 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Luciano, M., 497 
Lucki, I., 565 

Luders, E., 90, 283 
Ludwig, A. M., 522 
Lum, J., 267 

Luminet, O., 481 

Lun, V. M., 453 
Lundberg-Love, P., 162 
Lundstrom, B., 102 
Lunt, I., 11 

Luo, S., 605 

Lupfer, M. B., 600 
Luria, A. R., 237 

Lurie, K., 292 

Luthar, S. S., 402, 413 
Lutz, C. K., 52 

Lutz, W., 560 

Ly, D. H., 427 

Lykken, D. T., 529 
Lymberis, S. C., 388, 568 
Lynch, Shannon, 297 
Lynch, T. R., 551 
Lyness J. M., 569 
Lyness, K. S., 346 
Lynn, R., 303 

Lynn, S. J., 27, 151, 155, 156 


M 

Macaluso, E., 109, 124 
Maccoby, E., 349 
Macduff, I., 587 
Mack, J., 230 
Mackay, J., 490 
MacKenzie, T. D., 490 
Mackie, D., 582 
MacLean, L., 21 
MacLennan, A., 424 
Macmillian, M., 444 
MacNab, B, 280 
MacNeilage, P. F., 89 
MacPherson, K., 345 
Madden, D. J., 427 
Maddi, S. R., 481 
Mader, S. S., 65, 109 
Madigan, A. L., 402 
Madoff, B., 438, 469 
Madon, S., 598 
Magida, A. J., 420 
Magley, V. J., 347 
Magoni, M., 390 
Magoon, M., 186 
Magruder, K. M., 561 
Magruder, K., 474 
Maguire, E. A., 219, 236 
Maguire, E., 219 
Mahmood, M., 150 
Mahn, H., 410 

Maier, S. F., 237, 523 
Majdandzic, M., 402 
Majeres, R.L., 252 
Malamuth, N. M., 371 
Maldonado, J. R., 519 
Malenka, R. C., 159 
Malle, B. F., 585 
Malloy, L. C., 228 
Malone, D., 567 
Malone, W., 587 
Malouff, J. M., 560 
Malpas, P., 75 
Mamassis, G, 245 
Mamelak, A. N., 123 
Mancinelli, R., 165 
Manderscheid, R. W., 60 
Manenti, C., 370 
Mangun, G. R., 60 
Manley, J. J., 462 
Mann, K., 165 

Mann, R. E., 371 
Manning, M. A., 296 
Manning, S. Y., 551 
Manor, J. K., 612 


Manstead, A. S. R., 329, 330, 334 
Manuck, S. B., 44 
Marcaurelle, R., 515 
March, J. S., 550 
Marchand, A., 515 
Marcus, D., 561 
Marcus, S., 566 
Marcus-Newhall, A., 610 
Mariën, P., 81 
Markoff, J., 286 
Markowitz, J. C., 558, 569 
Marks, A. C., 344 
Marks, I. M., 176 
Markus, H. R., 588 
Marmar, C., 475 
Maroda, K. J., 326 
Maroun, M., 67 
Marrero, H., 247 
Marsden, J., 559 
Marsh, B., 490 

Marsh, H. W., 318, 325 
Marshall, K., 119 
Marshall, L. J., 375 
Marshall, L., 149 
Marshall, M. K., 458 
Marshall, R. D., 26, 475 
Marsiske, M., 300 
Marszalek, J., 292 
Martelle, S., 201 
Martens, B. K., 297 
Martin, A. C., 211 
Martin, A. J., 325 
Martin, A., 123 
Martin, C. E., 368 
Martin, L., 269, 401 
Martin, M. M., 272 
Martin, M., 281 
Martin, N. G., 368 
Martin, P. D., 482 
Martindale, C., 158 
Martinez, C. N., 353 
Martinez, M. J., 85, 87 
Martini, M. I., 409 
Martuza, R. L., 568 
Mascia, K., 578 

Maser, J. D., 517 
Mashour, G. A., 568 
Maslach, C., 593 
Malsow, A. H., 313-314, 458, 459 
Maslow, A., 17, 20, 507 
Mason, M., 362 
Massaro, D. W., 117 
Massey, S., 579 

Mast, F. W., 244 
Masters, W. H., 358, 359, 368, 374 
Masuda, M., 606 
Mataix-Cols, D., 252 
Matkin, G. S., 262 
Matlin, M. W., 349 
Maton, K. I., 11 
Matson, J., 187 
Matsumoto, D., 336, 337 
Matsuo, H., 475 
Matthews, G., 409 
Maurer, D., 394 
Mauss, I., 329 

Mayer, J. D., 285 
Maynard, A. E., 409, 418 
Maynard, L., 156 
Mazard, A., 80 
McAdams, D. P., 417 
McAulifffe, K., 200 
McBride, C. M., 594 
McBurney, D. H., 124 
McCabe, C., 119 
McCabe, M., 374 
McCarthy, D., 495 
McCarthy, J., 562 
McCartney, K., 401 
McCaul, K. D., 495 
McCauley, R. N., 133 


McClarty, L. K., 453 
McClellan, M., 289 
McClelland, D. C., 285, 325 
McClintock, B., 282 
McClintock, M. K., 119 
McClure, J., 496, 579 
McConnell, A., 582 
McCrea, R. R., 450 
McCrink, K., 408 
McDaniel, J., 278 
McDaniel, M. A., 237 
McDonald, C., 529 
McDonald, H. E., 227 
McDougall, W., 309 
McDowell, D. M., 160 
McEwen, B. S., 475 
McGabe, D. P., 27 
McGaugh, J. L., 208, 219 
McGilvray, J., 268 
McGinn, D., 467 
McGregor, K. K., 382 
McGue, M., 612 
McGuffin, P., 387, 506, 523, 527 
McHale, S. M., 353 
McIntyre, K., 475 
McKay, D., 516 
McKay, P., 598 
McKay, R., 525 
McKenna, S. B., 399 
McKinley, M. J., 310 
McLeskey, J., 297 
McLoughlin, D. M., 567 
McMurtray, A. M., 80 
McNamara, P., 145 
McWilliams, N., 444 
Mead, M., 420 
Mealey, L., 351, 352 
Means, M. K., 550 
Medeiros, R., 430 
Meeter, M., 219 
Mehl, M., 584 
Mehl, M. R., 348 
Mehl-Madrona, L. E., 156 
Mehler, J., 272 
Meinlschmidt, G., 77 
Meister, M., 119 
Mel, B. W., 66 
Melinder, A., 228 
Mel’nikov, K. S., 234 
Meltzer, H. Y., 529 
Meltzoff, A. N., 395, 409 
Melzack, R., 122 
Mendelsohn, J., 357, 374 
Menlove, F. L., 200, 551 
Menon, G., 254 
Menuhin, Y., 282 
Meri, J. M., 229 
Merikangas, K. R., 534 
Merryman, A., 599 
Mertz, J. E., 350 
Mervis, C. B., 247 
Messer, S. B., 444 
Messerli-Buergy, N., 515 
Messineo, M., 599 
Messner, M., 579 
Meston, C. M., 374 
Metzler, J., 244 
Meyer, A., 522 
Meyer, I., 192 
Meyer, R. G., 156 
Meyer-Bahlburg, H., 368 
Meyerowitz, J., 369 
Mezulis, A. H., 343, 348, 366, 369, 
370, 524 
Michael, R. T., 364, 365, 367 
Micheau, J., 67 
Microsoft, 467 
Midanik, L. T., 164 
Middlebrooks, J. C., 117 
Miesenbock, G., 81 
Mifflin, L., 202 


Mignon, A., 584 
Mika, V. S., 493 
Miklowitz, D. J., 528 
Mikulincer, M., 399 
Miletic, M. P., 446 
Milgram, S., 595-596 
Miller, A. G., 448 
Miller, G., 538, 558, 612 
Miller, G. E., 480 
Miller, G. F., 301 
Miller, J. A., 466 
Miller, J. G., 587 
Miller, L., 388, 486 
Miller, L. A., 334 
Miller, M. N., 320 
Miller, N. E., 561 
Miller, R. R., 177 
Miller, T. J., 560 
Miller, W. R., 483, 521 
Miller-Jones, D., 300 
Miller-Perrin, C., 188 
Milling, L., 123, 156 
Millon, C., 530 
Millon, T., 529, 530 
Mills, D., 598 

Mills, P. J., 475 
Milner, B., 84, 215, 231 
Milton, J., 135 
Mineka, S., 193, 445 
Miner, J., 402 
Miner-Rubino, K., 347, 425 
Ming, L., 155 

Mintz, A., 21 
Minuchin, S., 559 
Miquel, J., 427 
Mirabile, R., 582 
Mischel, W., 448, 453 
Miserando, M., 223 
Mishra, P. K., 314 
Misra, G., 472 
Mitchell, D. B., 430 
Mitchell, P., 77 
Mitic, K., 68 

Mitte, K., 554 
Miyamoto, T., 91 
Mobasher, B., 286 
Moffitt, T. E., 9, 382, 457 
Mograss, M., 145 
Mohan, R., 566 
Mohapel, P., 67 
Moher, C., 185 
Mokdad, A. H., 164 
Mollaret, P., 584 
Monahan, J., 9 
Monnier, J., 371 
Montague, P., 488 
Monteith, M. J., 602 
Montepare, J. M., 605 
Montgomery, S., 66 
Montgomery, S. A., 156, 169 
Montoya, R., 605 
Moody, H. R., 427 
Moon, S. M., 298, 559 
Moore, B. C. J., 100 
Moore, D. G., 395 
Moore, L., 346 
Moore, M. M., 38 
Moore, S. D., 179 
Moore, T., 86 

Moore, Z., 613 
Moorey, S., 554 
Mora-Giral, M., 322 
Morad, Y., 146 
Moran, A., 245 
Morano, M. I., 475 
Moreland, R. L., 591 
Moreno, R., 395 
Moretz, M., 516 
Morgan, A. A., 300 
Morgan, T., 189 
Morley, S., 122 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Morone, N. E., 93 
Morris, J. F., 369 
Morrison, P., 527 
Morrone, A. S., 325 
Morrow, J., 92 
Moselhy, H., 164 
Mosher, C. J., 140 
Moskowitz, G. B., 583 
Moskowitz, J. T., 474, 480, 483 
Moss-Morris, R., 8 
Motley, M. T., 439 
Mu, X., 588 
Muammar, O. M., 284 
Mueller, C. E., 297 
Mullen, B., 600 
Müller, H., 127 
Munakata, Y., 408 
Munger, D., 98 
Mungus, D., 462 
Munroe, R. L., 348 
Muris, P., 515 
Murphy, D., 68, 119 
Murphy, G. L., 245 
Murphy, R. T., 474 
Murphy, S., 166 
Murphy, S. T., 328 
Murray, B., 12 
Murray, C., 301 
Murray, H. A., 325 
Murray, J. P., 467 
Murray, R., 68 
Murray, R. M., 529 
Murray, S. L., 605 
Murre, J. M. J., 219 
Murthy, P., 296, 391 
Myers, D. G., 495, 498 
Myers, J. E. B., 162 
Myers, L. L., 322 
Myers, L. M., 431 
Myerson, J., 408 
Myrtek, M., 486 


N 

adeem, E., 414 

agai, Y., 93 

agda, B. A., 602 

agin, D. S., 530 

ahari, G., 449, 584 

aik, G., 318, 386 

aimi, T., 164 

ajman, J. M., 390 

akamizo, S., 131 

akamura, Y., 75 

akayama, K., 98 

aldini, L., 388 

arrow, W. E., 534 

asir, N. S., 508 

asrallah, H., 564 

ass, C., 101 

ater, U. M., 479 

athan, P. E., 560 

ational Academy of Sciences, 462 

ational Adolescent Health 
Information Center, 412 

National Association for the Education 

of Young Children, 401 

National Center for Health 

Statistics, 398 

National Depression Screening 

Day, 521 

National Institute of Child Health and 

Development (NICHD) Early 

Child Care Research Network, 400, 

401 

National Institute on Drug Abuse, 169 

National Institute of Mental Health, 50 

National Research Council, 400 

National Research Council and 

Institute of Medicine of the 

National Academies, 572 

National Science Foundation, 50, 429 


N 
N 
N 
N 
N 
N 
N 
N 
N 
N 
N 
N 
N 
N 
N 
N 
N 
N 
N 
N 








Natvig, G. K., 483 
Naveh-Benjamin, M., 212, 226, 234 
Neher, A., 9 

Neitz, J., 110 

Neitz, M., 110 

Nellis, L., 289 

Nelson, P. D., 11 
Nelson, W. M., III, 613 
Nemeroff, C. B., 372 
Neron, S., 123 
Nesheim, S., 390 

Nesse, R. M., 523 
Nestler, E. J., 159 
Nestoriuc, Y., 93, 123 
Nettelbeck, T. 
Neubauer, A. C., 283 
Neumann, N., 82 
Neumark-Sztainer, D., 21, 370 
Neumayr, B., 594 
Newby-Clark, R. I., 227 
Newell, A., 256 
Newman, C. F., 523 
Newman, M., 89, 381 
Newport, F., 323 
Niccols, A., 391 
Nicholson, I. A. M., 449 
Nickeas, R., 519 
Niedenthal, P. M., 329 
Nielsen, C., 121 
Nielsen, M. E., 134 
Nielsen, T., 147 

Nigg, J. T., 530 
Nikolaou, I., 495 
Nilsson, H., 248 
Nimrod, G., 431 
Nisbett, R., 280 

Nisbett, R. E., 302 
Nishida, M., 144, 149 
Nishino, S., 150 

Nissle, S., 497 
Nittrouer, S., 102 
Nixon, J. C., 158 

Nixon, R. D. V., 311 
Niyonsenga, T., 488 
Noble, H. B., 489 
Nokelainen, P., 281 
Nolan, R. P., 486 
Nolen-Hoeksema, S., 504, 514, 524, 530 
Norberg, K., 165 
Norcia, A. M., 395 
Norcross, J. C., 50, 561 
Norlander, T., 482 
Norman, D. A., 216 
North Carolina State University, 429 
Norton, M. I., 587 
Norton, P. J., 548 
Nosek, B. A., 224 
Novak, M. A., 52 
Novello, A., 536-537 
Novotny, C. M., 560, 561 
Nowicki, S., Jr., 529 
Noyes, R., 518 

Ntinas, K. M., 195 
Nucci, L. P., 415 

Nunes, A., 429 

Nurmi, J-E., 453 
Nurnberger, J. I., Jr., 167 
Nuss, C. K., 590 
Nussbaum, A. D., 602 
Nyberg, L., 215 


Oo 

Oakford, S., 297 
Oatley, K., 328 
Oberauer, K., 212 
O’Brien, K. M., 323 
Occhionero, M., 148 
Ochsner, K., 336 
O’Connor, D., 567 
O'Connor, D. B., 320 
O'Connor, R. C., 320 








Name Index 


Odeku, K., 360 
O'Doherty, J., 109 
O'Donohue, W., 347 
Oehman, A., 193, 445 
Offer, D., 229 

Ogbu, J., 203 
Ogletree, S. M., 353 
Ogren, H. A., 330 
Ogren, K., 568 

Oh, S. S., 480 

Ohara, K., 527 
O'Hara, L. A., 261 
Ohira, T., 486 

Oishi, S., 495 

Ojha, H., 314 
Okamoto, Y., 408 
Okamura, L., 371 
O'Keefe, T., 370 
Olatunji, B., 518 
Olatunji, B. O., 518 
Olds, M. E., 84 
O'Leary, O. F., 565 
O'Leary-Kelly, A., 346 
Olfson, M., 566 
Olivardia, R., 414 
Oliver, M. B., 364 
Olliver-Gray Y., 202 
Olson, D. H., 425 
Olson, E. A., 227 
Olsson, H, 248 
Oltmanns, T. F., 529 
O'Malley, P. M., 489 
Omerod, T. C., 262 
O'Neal, K. K., 161 
O'Neill, A., 317 

Ono, H., 426 

Oordt, M. S., 323 
Oosterhof, N. N., 451 
Ophir, E., 101 

Opler, M., 529 
Oppenheimer, D. M., 249 
O’Reardon, J. P., 567 
Orlansky, M. D., 295 
Ormrod, R., 371 
Ornat, S. L., 267 
Orozco, C., 168 

Osei, S. Y., 321 
Oskamp, S., 598 
Ospina-Kammerer, V., 472 
O'Sullivan, M., 335 
Otake, K., 495 
Ouimet, A., 518 
Ovarnstrom, U., 483 
Oveis, C., 451 
Overbaugh, J., 373 
Oviedo-Joekes, E., 168 
Owens, D., 32 


P 

Pääbo, F., 26 
Pachankis, J. E., 561 
Packer, D., 592 
Pagano, C., 168 
Pager, D., 598 
Pagnin, A., 429 
Pagonis, T. A., 77 
Pagoto, S. L., 549 
Paik, A., 374 
Palazzeschi, L., 281 
Pallanti, S., 81 
Paller, K., 224 
Palmer, J. C., 228 
Paluck, E. L., 602 
Pandya, M., 567 
Paniagua, F. A., 508, 562 
Pankove, E., 261 
Papirman, Y., 587 
Paquier, P. F., 81 
Parish, C. L., 89 
Parish, W., 364 
Park, A., 496 


1-10 Name Index 


Park, D. C., 322 
Park, H., 594 

Park, N., 606 

Park, S., 351 

Parke, B. N., 298 
Parke, R. D., 400 
Parker, E. S., 208 
Parker-Pope, T., 323 
Parmley, M. C., 260 
Parnes, A., 210 
Parra, A., 135 
Parsons, L. M., 81, 85 
Pascual, A., 184, 594 
Pascual, A. C., 415 
Pascual-Leone, A., 245 
Pashler, H. E., 193 
Passos, V. L., 292 
Pastalkova, E., 220 
Pasterski, V., 352 
Pasupathi, M., 21 
Patall, E., 46 

Pate, W. E., IL, 11 
Patten, S., 494 
Patterson, D., 278, 390 
Paukert, A., 571 
Paul, A. M., 468 
Paulmann, S., 124 
Paulozzi, L. J., 164 
Pavitt, C., 584 
Pavlov, I., 16 
Paxson, A., 584 
Paxton, J. M., 612-613 
Payment, K. E., 227 
Payne, D. G., 234 
Payne, K., 561 
Pazzaglia, F., 244 
Pearce, J. M. S., 123 
Pearce, R. R., 25 
Pearl, C., 297 
Pearlstein, T., 537 
Pearson, A. R., 598 
Pearson, J., 108 
Pecsenye, M., 292 
Pedersen, P. B., 562, 599, 602 
Pedersen, W. C., 610 
Pederson, N. L., 481 
Pedraza, O., 462 
Peiro, J. M., 11 

Pell, M. D., 117 
Pellegrini, S., 189 
Pelli, D. G., 109 
Pellis, S. M., 400 
Pellis, V. C., 400 
Peluso, J., 486 
Pempek, T., 606 
Pence, D., 582 
Penke, L., 301 
Penley, J. A., 480 
Penn, D. L., 525 
Penney, J. B., Jr., 66 
Penza, K. M., 372 
Penzel, F., 517 
Peretz, I., 87 

Perez, R. M., 369 
Perfetti, B., 281 
Perlis, T., 164 
Perloff, R. M., 579 
Perrett, D. I., 605 
Perrin, R., 188 
Perry, B., 208 
Persinger, M. A., 84 
Perunovic, W. Q. E., 47 
Pervin, L. A., 450, 459 
Pesiridou, A., 567 
Pesman, C, 121, 123 
Peterfi, Z., 145 
Peters, E., 429 
Peters, J., 218 
Peterson, C., 496 
Peterson, F. L., Jr., 374 
Peterson, L. R., 215 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Peterson, M. J., 215 
Petersson, K. M., 90 
Petitto, L. A., 90, 272 
Petrill, S. A., 301, 383 
Pettigrew, T. F., 598, 602 
Pettito, L. A., 266 
Petty, R. E., 580 
Pfeffer, C. R., 523 
Phelps, E. A., 549 
Phelps, R. P., 291 
Philip, P., 146 
Phinney, J. S., 26 
Pi-Sunyer, X., 322 
Piaget, J., 17, 405-408 
Picchioni, D., 148 
Pickard, N., 210 
Pickel, K., 227 
Pickering, G. J., 119 
Pickett, C. L., 600 
Piechowski, M. M., 314 
Piel, E., 147 
Pietarinen, A.-V., 266 
Pillay, S. S., 515 
Pilling, M., 395 
Pilotti, M., 234 
Pilowsky, L., 527 
Pina, A., 346 
Pincus, H. A., 509 
Pincus, T., 122 
Pine, D. S., 517 
Pinel, J. P. J., 313 
Pinker, S., 268, 269, 382 
Pinkerton, S. D., 364 
Pinna, B., 109 
Pinquart, M., 569 
Pintrich, P. R., 325 
Piper, W., 558 
Piquero, A. R., 189 
Pitman, R. K., 220 
Platek, S., 109 
Plato, 15 
Platt, B., 67 
Ploeger, A., 99 
Plomin, R., 60, 74, 296, 301, 387, 457, 
506, 523, 527 
Plowright, C. M. S., 180 
Pluess, M., 381, 401 
Plug, C., 131 
Plutchik, R., 443 
Poe, C., 60 
Pogarsky, G., 189 
Poirier, C., 10 
Pole, N., 474 
Polivy, J., 322 
Pollack, A., 82 
Pollak, K. I., 594 
Pollard, H. P., 123 
Polonsky, D. C., 364 
Pomerlau, O. F., 487 
Pomeroy, W. B., 368 
Ponterotto, J. G., 562, 599, 602 
Poo, C., 88 
Popa, D., 68, 523 
Pope, H., 414 
Pope, K. S., 229 
Popik, P., 565 
Popp, D., 349 
Porte, H. S., 149 
Posner, M. I., 60 
Post, J., 21 
Postl, L., 551 
Poteat, V. P., 369 
Pottick, K. J., 562 
Potts, L. A., 489 
Potts-Datema, W., 390 
Poulos, A. M., 65 
Powell, A. A., 612 
Powell, L. H., 367, 483 
Powers, K. D., 415 
Powers, M., 550 
Pozuelo, L., 567 


Praisner, C. L., 297 
Pramanick, M., 314 
Prasad, S. A., 75, 249 
Pratkanis, A. R., 134, 592, 594 
Pratt, H. D., 531 
Pratto, F., 598 

Press, J., 426 
Pressley, M. P., 283 
Pretz, J. E., 280, 285 
Pretzer, J. L., 561 
Price, D., 121 

Price, D. D., 571 
Price, E. C., 548 
Price, M., 208, 487 
Prigot, J., 118 

Prince, C. V., 370 
Prinz, J. J., 334 
Prislin, R., 591 
Proffitt, D. R., 130 
Prohovnik, I., 333 
Proudfoot, D., 249, 270 
Pryor, D. W., 368 
Psotka, J., 235 

Puca, R. M., 325 
Puhl, R., 21 

Pullum, G. K., 269 
Pumariega, A. J., 320 
Purselle, D. C., 165 
Putnam, F. W., 520 


Q 

Quartana, P. J., 613 
Quas, J. A., 228 
Queiroz, A., 371 
Quenot, J. P., 80 
Quillian, M. R., 216 
Quinlin, K. J., 531 
Quinn, A., 101 
Quinn, D. M., 510 
Quinn, T. C., 373 
Quinones, M. A., 292 
Quintana, S. M., 26 


R 

Rabin, J., 110 
Rabinowitz, J., 526 
Rabson-Hare, J., 45 
Rachman, S., 258, 515 
Raczynski, J. M., 321 
Rado, J., 81 

Raffaele, P., 270 
Rahman, Q., 368 
Raizel, H., 67 
Rajagopal, S., 53, 68 
Ralson, A., 309, 315-316 
Ralston, A., 315 
Ramachandra, V., 64 
Ramachandran, V. S., 123, 124 
Rammsayer, T., 283 
Ramos, R. T., 66 
Rampon, C., 283 
Ramsay, M. C., 300, 466 
Ramus, F., 387 
Randolph-Seng, B., 134 
Rangell, L., 506 
Ransom, B. R., 61 
Rapee, R., 551 

Rapport, R. L., 66 
Raskin, N. J., 557 
Rasmussen, F., 320 
Rassin, E., 260, 515 
Ravindran, A. V., 475 
Ravitz, P., 558 

Ray, L., 162 

Ray, L. A., 161, 456 
Ray, R., 579 

Raz, R., 154 

Read, J., 165 

Rector, N. A., 553, 554 
Redding, G. M., 130, 132 


Redish, A. D., 164 
Reece, M., 38 

Reed, M., 594 

Reed, P., 189 

Reed, S. K., 256 
Reese, R. J., 571 
Reese-Durham, N., 281 
Regan, P. C., 323, 605, 606 
Reichel, M., 402 
Reichenberg, A., 527 
Reif, A., 382 
Reijonen, J. H., 531 
Reilly, T., 151 

Reiner, R., 93 
Reinhard, M., 579 
Reisberg, D., 265 
Reiss, S., 314 

Relier, J. P., 390 
Rembe, S., 360 
Remington, R., 128 
Renault, L., 354 
Rende, R., 9 
Renshaw, D. C., 360 
Repp, B. H., 134 
Rescorla R. A., 181 
Revonsuo, A., 155 
Reynolds, C., 37 
Reynolds, C. R., 300, 466 
Reynolds, R. I., 216 
Riccio, C., 37 

Rice, C., 531 

Rice, D. R., 600 

Rice, M. L., 267 

Rich, E. L., 84 
Richard, D. C. S., 551 
Richards, A. L., 370 
Richards, R., 261 
Richardson, A. S., 419 
Richardson, B., 242 
Richgels, D. J., 266 
Ridge, Tom, 473 
Rieber, R. W., 409 
Riedel, G., 67 

Rigby, L., 558 
Riggins, R., Jr., 401 
Riggio, H., 27 

Riley, A., 572 
Rimrodt, S. L., 90 
Riniolo, T. C., 37 
Rinn, W. E., 336 
Riolo, F., 444 

Ripley, A., 346, 350 
Risley, T. R., 267 
Ritezschel, J., 119 
Ritterfeld, U., 202 
Rivera-Gaxiola, M., 397 
Rizvi, 5. L., 551 
Rizzo, M., 150 
Robbins, B., 20 
Robbins, R. W., 229 
Robbins, W. J., 398 
Robert, J., 89 

Robert, S., 265 
Roberts, J. E., 401 
Roberts, M. E., 179 
Robertson, E., 215 
Robins, L. N., 538 
Robins, R. W., 329, 456 
Robinson, A., 292 
Robinson, D. K., 409 
Robinson, D. N., 112, 565 
Robinson, N. M., 298 
Robinson, R. E., 159 
Robinson, T. E., 88 
Roch-Locours, L. A., 90 
Rock, A., 489 

Rodd, Z. A., 180 
Rodgers, C. S., 122 
Rodriguez, M., 480 
Rodriguez, M. A., 184 
Roe, C., 430 


Roecklin, K. A., 151 
Roediger, H. L., M, 27 
Roesch, S. C., 480 
Rogers, C., 17, 20, 507 
Rogers, C. R., 458, 459, 557 
Rogers, J. M., 391 
Rogers, L. J., 89 
Rogers, P., 32, 159, 611 
Rogers, S., 430 
Rohan, K. J., 151 
Rohleder, N., 479 
Roid, G., 289 
Roisman, G. I., 399 
Roizen, N. J., 390 
Roll, J., 20 

Rollman, G. B., 122 
Rolls, E. T., 119 

Rom, S. A., 486 
Romano, E., 530 
Romeu, P. F., 225 
Rönnberg, J., 214 
Rooney, N. J., 189 
Roosevelt, E., 313 
Rorschach, H., 465, 466 
Rosch, E., 247 

Rose, N., 135 

Rosen, H., 554 

Rosen, J., 89 

Rosen, R. C., 374 
Rosenbaum, M., 321 
Rosenbaum, R. S., 236 
Rosenberg, L., 351 
Rosenbloom, T., 310 
Rosenhan, D., 510 
Rosenheck, R., 508 
Rosenman, R. H., 486 
Rosenstein, D. S., 530 
Rosenthal, A. M., 611 
Rosenthal, R., 53 
Roska, B., 109 

Rosner, H., 374 

Ross, C. A., 270, 519 
Ross, D., 200 

Ross, H. E., 123, 131 
Ross, J., 148 

Ross, L., 448 

Ross, M., 227 

Ross, S., 200 

Rossato, M., 168 
Rossell, S. L., 91 
Rossi, J. J., 388 
Rossouw, J. E., 424 
Rotan, L. W., 472 
Roter, D. L., 493 
Rothbart, M. K., 456 
Rothblum, E. D., 358, 369 
Rothman, A. J., 495 
Roughton, R. E., 368 
Routtenberg, A., 84 
Rowan, K., 402 
Rowe, J. B., 87 
Royzman, E. B., 400 
Rozencwajg, P., 429 
Rozin, P., 320 
Rubichi, S., 155 


Rubin, D. C., 225, 226, 229, 230 


Ruck, M. D., 46 
Rudman, L. A., 601 
Rudner, M., 214 
Rudorfer, M. V., 517 
Rudzinski, D., 546 
Rule, N. O., 451 
Rulison, K. L., 292 
Runco, M. A., 261 
Rünger, D., 200 
Rupert, G., 320 
Rusche, B., 52 
Ruscher, J. B., 583 
Rushton, J. P., 462 
Russell, J. A., 330 
Russell, R., 98 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Russo, N., 461 
Rust, T. B., 430 
Rustin, M., 38 
Ruth, B., 282 
Rutherford, B., 53 
Ruthsatz, J. M., 296 
Rutter, M., 24, 381, 382 
Ryan, R., 315 
Rychlak, J., 140 
Rydell, R., 582 
Ryff, C. D., 348 
Rymer, R., 266 
Ryncarz, R. A., 415 


S 


Saarni, C., 402 
Sabater, J., 249 

Sacco, D., 600 
Saccuzzo, D. P., 461 
Sachs-Ericsson, N., 430 
Sackeim, H. A., 567 
Sacks, O., 85 
Saczynski, J., 429 
Sadeghi-Nejad, H., 374 
Sadker, D., 354 
Sadker, M., 354 
Sagarin, B. J., 594 
Saggino, A., 281 
Sahin, N. T., 268 

St. Dennis, C., 570 

St. Jacques, P. L., 430 
Sakai, K. L., 268 
Sakihara, S., 483 
Salgado, D. M., 531 
Sallquist, J., 612 
Salmela-Aro, K., 453 
Salovey, P., 285, 286, 495 
Salsman, N. L., 558 
Salvi, V., 566 
Salzmann, H.-P., 82 
Samaniego, C. M., 415 
Samantaray, S. K., 314 
Samoilov, V., 176 
Sampson, S., 346 
Sams, M., 210 
Samuel, D. B., 510 
Sanacora, G., 521 
Sanchez, A., 419 
Sanders, S, 245 
Sanderson, M., 504 
Sandlund, M., 568 
Sandomir, R., 77 
Sandoval, J., 301 
Sanjuan, E., 187 
Sanlier, N., 474 
Sanouri, A., 488 
Santel, S., 322, 323 
Santelli, J., 38 

Santtila, P., 358 

Saper, C. P., 151 
Sapolsky, R. M., 479, 568 
Sargent, J. D., 487 
Sato, K., 330 

Sato, N., 388 

Saucier, D. A., 53 
Sauck, C., 465 
Saulsberry, K., 495 
Saunders, B., 346 
Savage, J., 202 


Savage-Rambaugh, E. S., 270 


Savas, H. A., 564 
Savazzi, S., 91 
Saville, B., 191 
Savitsky, K., 134 
Sawa, A., 526 
Sayette, M. A., 165 
Saywitz, K., 228 
Scaramella, L. V., 389 
Scarborough, E., 18 
Scarr, S., 302, 457 
Scaturo, D. J., 558 


Scelfo, J., 521 
Schachter, S., 333 
Schacter, D. L., 224, 245 
Schaefer, H. S., 482, 483 
Schaefer, R. T., 425 
Schaer, M., 283 
Schaie, K. W., 427, 428, 429 
Schaller, M., 410 
Schallert, D. L., 330 
Schechter, T., 391 
Schedlowski, M., 487 
Scheele, B., 609 
Scheff, T. J., 504 
Scheier, M. F., 431, 432, 463 
Schenone, M. H., 389 
Schepers, P., 261 
Scher, S. J., 582 
Scherman, A., 375 
Scherzer, T. R., 131 
Schick, K., 270 
Schieber, F, 427 
Schienle, A., 333 
Schiffer, A. A., 486 
Schiffer, S., 214 
Schillinger, D., 493 
Schimpf, P. H., 81 
Schlenger, W. E., 169 
Schmidt, J. P., 66 
Schmidt, N. B., 510 
Schmitt, D. P., 450 
Schmitt, F. A., 430 
Schnabel, K., 601 
Schnake, S. B., 583 
Schnall, S., 612 
Schnatz, P. F., 493 
Schneider, A., 149 
Schneider, W., 409 
Schnyer, D. M., 224 
Schofield, W., 362 
Schredl, M., 147 
Schrier, R. W., 490 
Schroers, M., 118 
Schubert, T., 326 
Schulte-Ruther, M., 64, 200 
Schumann, R., 522 
Schutt, R. K., 38 
Schutte, N. S., 560 
Schwartz, B. L., 222 
Schwartz, J., 592 
Schwartz, J. H., 70 
Schwartz, J. M., 88 
Schwartz, P., 418 
Schwartz, S. J., 422 
Schwarz, N., 497, 601 
Schwenkreis, P., 86 
Sciutto, M., 531 
Scollon, C. N., 497 
Scott, L. S., 246 
Scullin, M. H., 228 
Seamon, M. J., 168 
Sears, D. O., 50 
Sebanz, N., 134 
Sebel, P. S., 224 
Seeley, R., 81, 116 
Seeley, R. J., 319 
Sefcek, J. A., 9 

Segall, M. H., 133 
Segerstrom, S. C., 480 
Seibt, B., 598 

Seifert, A. L., 598 
Seli, H., 310 
Seligman, M., 523 
Seligman, M. E., 481 


Seligman, M. E. P., 495, 498, 560, 561 


Selkoe, D. J., 235, 430 
Sellbom, M., 464 
Sells, R., 367 
Selmon, N., 452 
Selove, R., 390 
Selsky, A., 420 

Selye, H., 478 





Name Index 


Semin, G. R., 265, 583 
Semler, C. N., 149 
Semykina, A., 326 
Sen, B., 75 

Senghas, A., 336 
Serdaris, N., 495 
Seroczynski, A. D., 530 
Serpell, R., 301 
Servis, R., 578 

Sestir, M. A., 202 
Seth, A. K., 140 
Seymour, B., 319 
hafer, V. L., 266 
hafran, R., 323 

hah, D. B., 568 
hahabi, L., 483 
haikholeslami, R., 313 
hankar, G., 53 
hapiro, C. J., 531 
hapiro, L. R., 157, 225 
hapiro, M. L., 84 
harma, H. S., 163 
harman, S. J., 229 
harp, C., 247 

harpe, D., 494 
hastri, J., 400 

haver, P. R., 330, 399 
hea, A., 391 

helton, R., 569 
helton, R. C., 566 
hen, C. K., 88 

hen, L., 329, 493 
hepard, R. N., 244 
hepherd, H., 598 
heppard, L. D., 283 
hepperd, J., 587 

her, K., 419 
herblom, S., 416 
herman, S. L., 296, 390 
hier, D., 107 
hiffman, S., 489 
hiffrin, R. M., 209, 210 
himai, S., 495 
himamura, M., 388 
himono, K., 130 

hin, A., 310 

hinn, M., 571 

hmuel, A., 110 

hoda, Y., 453 

hono, Y., 234 

hors, T. J., 88 
hulman, J. L., 358 
hultz, S. K., 375 
hurkin, J. N., 298 
hweder, R. A., 329 
hynkaruk, J. M., 247 
Sibley, C. G., 579 
Sidman, M., 189 
Siebler, F., 581 

Siegel, J. M., 145 
Siegel, R. K., 158 
Siegert, R. J., 523 
Siemer, M., 329 
Sierra, C., 249 

Sierra, J. C., 353 
Sierra-Biddle, D., 515 
Sifrit, K. J., 214 
Silbersweig, D. A., 527 
Sill, M., 428 

Silva, M. T. A., 193 
Silverman, K., 20 
Silverman, M. M., 544 
Silverstein, M. L., 466 
Simcock, G., 229 
Simmons, A., 53 
Simon, G., 521 
Simon, H., 256 
Simon, S., 579 

Simon, T., 288 
Simonds, V. M., 180 
Simonton, D. K., 37, 261 


NNANNNNNNNNNNNNNnNHN 


NNANNnNNN 


nn 





NNANNNNNNNNNNNNNKHHNNH 


1-12 Name Index 


Sinclair, S., 21 

Singer, J. E., 333 
Singer, J. L., 151 
Singh, A., 318 

Singh, S., 366, 367 
Sininger, Y. S., 117 
Sipes, B., 422 

Sisson, M., 606 
Sjoquist, H. S., 163 
Skandara, Daniel, 295 
Skinner, B. F., 17, 20, 267, 452, 454, 459 
Skipp, C., 544 
Skolnick, P., 565 
Skowronski, J. J., 229 
Slater, D., 181 

Slater, E., 522 

Sleek, S., 286 

Sloan, E. P., 152 
Slomkowski, C., 538 
Slotnick, B. M., 122 
Smart, R. G., 159 
Smetana, J., 418 
Smetana, J. G., 446 
Smith, B. H., 148, 531 
Smith, C. A., 152, 483 
Smith, E., 487 

Smith, E. R., 583 
Smith, J., 402 

Smith, J. R., 84 

Smith, L., 90, 267, 566 
Smith, M. B., 49 
Smith, M. L., 560 
Smith, P., 372 

Smith, R. A., 604 
Smith, S. E., 473 
Smith, T. S., 333 
Smith, W. B., 446 
Smith-Crowe, K., 605 
Smolowe, J., 380 
Snyder, D., 367 
Snyder, D. J., 119 
Snyder, J., 402 
Snyder, M., 458 
Snyder, S. H., 526 
Sobel, K., 128 
Sohr-Preston, S. L., 389 
Sokolove, M., 504 
Soler, J.,551 

Solms, M., 445 
Solomon, M., 524 
Sommer, B., 38 
Sommer, R., 38 

Sorek, S., 234 

Sori, C. E, 559 

Sorrell, J. T., 122 
Soussignan, R., 336 
South, S., 457 
Spackman, M. P., 330 
Spangler, W. D., 325 
Spanos, N. P., 123, 154 
Sparks, J., 150 

Sparks, S. D., 298 
Spates, C. R, 549 
Spearman, C., 280 
Spector, P. E., 286 
Speirs-Neumeister, K. L., 325 
Spence, M. J., 389 
Spence-Cochran, K., 297 
Spencer, S. J., 349, 482, 483 
Spencer-Rodgers, J., 453 
Sperry, R., 91 

Spiegel, D., 487, 519, 561 
Spielberger, C. D., 444 
Spiers, H. J., 219 
Spillmann, L., 109 
Spindler, H., 486 
Spinella, M., 497 
Spitoni, G., 281 

Spitz, H. I., 160 
Spitzer, L., 426 
Spitzer, R. L., 522 





www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Sporer, S., 579 
Sprecher, S., 367, 605 
Sprenger, M., 213 
Sprenkle, D. H., 559 
Squilkr, S., 298 
Squire, L. R., 220 
Sriram, N., 224 
Srivastava, M., 314 
Staddon, J. F. R., 189 
Stafford, F., 426 
Stagner, B., 571 
Staley, J. K., 521 
Stankov, L, 280 
Stanojevic, S., 68 
Stanton, A. L., 480 
Stapel, D. A., 265 
Starcevic, V., 515 
Stark, J., 88 
Stark, R., 333 
Stark, S., 88 
Startup, M., 525 
Stasinos, J., 493 
Statham, J. A., 430 
Staud, R., 121 
Steblay, N., 227 
Steel, P., 346 
Steele, C. M., 165, 349, 602 
Steele, J. D., 568 
Stegerwald, F., 370 
Steiger, A., 145 
Stein, L. A. R., 464 
Steinberg, L., 418 
Steiner, M., 391, 537, 547 
Steiner, R. D., 390 
Steinfeld, C., 606 
Steinhardt, M. A., 473 
Steketee, G., 516, 518 
Stemler, S. E., 285 
Stenbacka, L., 87 
Stenklev, N. C., 427 
Stenstrom, P., 147 
Stephens, T., 81, 116 
Stephenson, R., 123 
Stephenson, R. H., 317 
Steptoe, A., 483 
Stern, E., 527 
Stern, R. M., 118 
Sternberg, R., 606-607 
Sternberg, R. J., 72, 254, 261, 280, 283, 
284, 285, 288, 302, 383, 472 
Sternberg, R. S., 343 
Stettler, N., 321 
Stevens, C., 51 
Stevens, C. F., 63 
Stevens, G., 18 
Stevens, M. C., 51 
Stevens, M. J., 11 
Stevens, P., 567 
Stevens, S. S., 193 
Stevenson, H. W., 25, 588 
Stevenson, R. J., 118 
Stewart, A. J., 425 
Stewart, D., 559 
Stewart, P. J., 230 
Stickgold, R., 144, 148 
Stickley, T., 519 
Stiffman, A., 286 
Stiling, P., 83 
Stillman, T., 357 
Stix, G., 82 
Stocks, E., 612 
Stokes, G., 356 
Stone, J., 527, 602 
Storm, L., 135 
Stoub, T. R., 235 
Stouffer, E. M., 200 
Strange, D., 229 
Strathern, A., 230 
Strauss, E., 91 
Strayer, D., 21, 101 
Strayer, D. L., 21, 214 


Striano, T., 395, 396 
Striegel-Moore, R., 322, 414 
Strong, T., 559 
Stronink, M., 21 
Struckman, C., 592 
Strupp, H. H., 561 
Stuart, S. P., 560 
Subrahmanyam, K., 606 
Sue, D. W., 547, 562 
Sue, S., 26, 547 

Suh, E. M., 498 
Suhail, K., 498 
Suizzo, M-A., 400 
Sullivan, A., 282 
Sullivan, E., 609 
Sullivan, J., 37 
Summers, M., 517 
Sunder, J. M., 303 
Sung, R. Y. T., 318 
Super, C. M., 407 
Surette, R., 202 
Susser, E. S., 475 
Sutin, A. R., 229 
Sutton, R. M., 579 
Suzuki, L. A., 301 
Svarstad, B., 493 
Svartdal, F., 189 
Swain, P. I., 322 
Swales, M. A., 551 
Swami, V., 358 
Swaminathan, V., 223 
Swann, W. B., Jr., 453 
Swanson, H. L., 531 
Sweeny, K., 587 
Swets, J. A., 135 
Swing, E., 202 

Szasz, T. S., 505, 510 
Szegedy Maszak, M., 522 


T 

Tadmor, C. T., 272 
Taggi, F., 21 

Tajfel, H., 599, 600 
Takahashi, M., 151 
Takeuchi, D., 388 
Takizawa, T., 483 
Takooshian, H., 216 
Tal-Or, N., 587 
Talajic, M., 487 
Talarico, J., 225 
Talbot, N., 519 
Talmi, D., 219 
Talukdar, S., 400 
Tam, T. W., 164 
Tamagnan, G., 521 
Tambs, K., 303 
Tammaro, E., 92 
Tan, F. E., 292 

Tan, L., 215, 269 
Tan, U, 122 
Tanaka-Matsumi, J., 495 
Tangney, B., 281 
Tangney, J., 40 
Tanner, J. M., 413 
Taras, H., 390 
Tasker, F., 369 

Tate, P., 81, 116 
Tausch, R., 557 
Taylor, A., 483 
Taylor, B., 401 
Taylor, C. S., 86 
Taylor, G. J., 486 
Taylor, P. M., 147 
Taylor, S. E., 479, 481, 483, 492, 496 
Tees, R. C., 389 
Teff, K. L., 319 
Tellegen, A., 455, 459 
Templer, D. I., 300 
Tenenbaum, H. R., 46, 353 
Tenopyr, M. L., 280 
Teodorov, E., 368 


Tepper, B. J., 119 
Terman, L., 297-298 
Terry, W. S., 193 
Tewes, U., 487 
Thachil, A. F., 566 
Tharp, R. S., 202 
Thase, M. E., 569 
Thatcher, D. L., 166 
Theorell, T., 479 
Theoret, H., 64, 200 
Thill, K., 353 

Thomas, O., 156 
Thomas, P., 525 
Thomas, S., 549 
Thompson, C. P., 229 
Thompson, J., 227 
Thompson, L. F., 611 
Thompson, M. C., 528 
Thompson, S. S., 234 
Thompson, V. A., 247 
Thompson-Brenner, H., 560, 561 
Thomsen, D. K., 225 
Thoreson, C. E., 483 
Thorkildsen, T. A., 270 
Thornton, A., 200, 366 
Thorsteinsson, E. B., 560 
Thrash, T. M., 325 
Tierney, K., 151 
Tierney Lindsey, K., 151 
Tiggemann, M., 229 
Tippin, J., 150 

Tirri, K., 281 

Titone, D. A., 144 
Todorov, A., 451 
Tolman, E. C., 198 
Tomaka, J., 480 
Tomm, K., 559 
Tommasi, L., 89 
Tompson, T., 537 
Tonegawa, S., 218 
Tonidandel, S., 292 
Tononi, G., 140, 144 
Tooby, J., 193 

Toole, L. M., 187 
Toribio, L., 168 
Torres, A. A., 32, 57 
Toth, J. P., 224 

Toth, N., 270 
Touhara, K., 119 
Townsend, S. M., 371 
Tracy, J. L., 329 
Tranter, L. J., 281 
Travis, F., 157, 158 
Tremaine, M. J., 86 
Tremblay, A., 321 
Tremblay, R. E., 530 
Triesch, J., 64 

Troche, S., 283 

Tropp, L. R., 602 
Troyer, A. K., 223 
Trudel, G., 358 

Trull, T. J., 530 
Trullas, R., 565 

Tsai, A., 429 

Tsai, D., 580 

Tsai, K. J., 88 

Tsai, Y. C., 88 
Tsaousis, I., 495 
Tseng, W. S., 534, 538, 562 
Tsukasaki, T., 270 
Tsunoda, T., 91 
Tucker Blackwell, V. G., 418 
Tucker, C. J., 353 
Tucker, C. M., 26 
Tuerlinckx, F., 325 
Tugay, N., 123 
Tulving, E., 215, 224, 234, 235 
Turk, D. C., 122 
Turkel, R. A., 353 
Turkewitz, G., 90 
Turkheimer, E., 529 


Turnbull, O., 445 
Turner, H., 371 
Turner, J. C., 599, 600 
Turner, M. E., 592, 594 
Turner, S. M., 517 
Turner, T. R., 353 
Turner, W. J., 368 
Turvey, M. T., 210 
Tuszynski, M. H., 568 
Tuzhilin, A., 286 
Tversky, A., 254 
Twenge, J. M., 590 
Tydgat, I., 215 

Tyler, J. M., 602 
Tynelius, P., 320 
Tyson, L. D., 344 


U 

Ubell, E., 152 

Uhl, G., 193 

llman, S. E., 371 

mphress, E. E., 605 

nal-Karagüven, M., 21 

nderwood, A., 123, 165 

nger, R., 345 

nited Nations Programme on 
HIV/AIDS (UNAIDS), 373 

niversity of Chicago, 278 

niversity of Minnesota, 455 

nsworth, N., 213 

pdegraff, K. A., 417 

rbina, S., 290 

rsprung, W. W., 488 

.S. Bureau of the Census, 425, 426 

.S. Bureau of Labor Statistics, 
345, 426 

.S. Senate Select Committee on 
Intelligence, 260 

tsey, S. O., 599, 602 

ttl, B., 224 

ylings, H. B. M., 389 

zelac, S., 418 


QAC OOEC 


Geeecece 


Ge 





aces 


< 


Vaccaro, D., 61 

Vaillant, C. O., 424 
Vaillant, G. E., 424 

Vaish, A., 396 

Vaitl, D., 333 

Valencia, R. R., 301 
Valentijn, A. M., 429 
Valenzuela, S., 606 
Vallortigara, G., 89 
Valsiner, J., 465 

Van Beekum, S., 547 

van den Berg, P. T., 261 
van den Boom, D. C., 402 
Van den Wildenberg, W. P. M., 408 
van der Helm, P. A., 127, 144 
Van der Molen, M. W., 408 
van Hooren, S. A. H., 429 
van Marle, K., 408 

van Oort, R., 236 

Van Overwalle, F., 581 
van Wel, F., 418 

Vanasse, A., 488 

Vandell, D. L., 401 
Vandervert, L. R., 81 
Vanheule, S., 523 

Vanni, S., 87 
Varjassyova, A., 235 
Varma, S., 257 

Vartanian, O., 252 
Vasquez, G., 9 

Vassalli, A., 310 
Vaughan-Jackson, P., 362 
Vecchi, T., 429 

Vedhara, K., 472 

Vega, C. P., 37 

Vellacott, J., 402 

Velliste, M., 82 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Veltman, M. W. M., 266 
Veniegas, R. C., 368 
Vera-Villarroel, P., 353 
Verdejo, A., 168 
Verfaellie, M., 236 
Vergas, R., 193 
Verhaagen, J., 89 
Verleger, R., 144 
Vernon, P., 74 

Vernon, P. A., 283 
Verona, E., 609 

Vettor, R., 168 

Victor, S. B., 369 
Vieira, E. M., 557 
Vieira, K., 74 

Vikan, A., 222 
Villarosa, L., 371 
Villemure, C., 122 


Virginia Tech Transportation 


Institute, 101 
Visser, P. S., 600 
Vitaro, F., 383 
Vitello, P., 116, 117 
Vitiello, A. L., 123 
Vitousek, K. M., 323 
Vleioras, G., 417 
Vogt, D., 482 
Volkow, N. D., 159 
Volterra, V., 267 
von Helmholtz, H., 111 
von Hofsten, C., 408 
Von Känel, R., 475 
Von Schantz, M., 74 
Von Schedvin, H., 482 
Voruganti, L. P., 565 
Voss, J., 224 
Vujic, V., 68 
Vygotsky, L., 409 


WwW 

Wacher, D. P., 189 
Wachs, T. D., 456 
Waddell, J., 88 
Wadden, T. A., 195 
Wade, K. A., 229 
Wade, N. J., 130 
Wager, T. D., 68, 329, 330 
Wagner, A. D., 101 
Wagner, A. W., 551 
Wagner, H. J., 75 
Wagner, R. K., 284 
Wagstaff, G., 155 
Wain, H. J., 493 
Waite, S., 558 
Wald, G., 111 
Wald, M. L., 592 
Waldo, C. R., 369 
Walker, E. E., 568 
Walker, L., 416 
Walker, L. J., 612 
Walker, M. P., 144, 148 
Walker, W., 123 
Walker, W. R., 229 
Walkey, F. H., 496 
Wall, T. L., 165 
Waller, B., 34 
Wallerstein, J. S., 425 
Wallis, J. D., 26 
Walsh, B. T., 157, 565 
Walsh, V., 123 
Walter, M. I., 254 
Wang, A., 518 
Wang, C., 235 
Wang, L., 330 
Wang, M. C., 609 
Wang, O., 229 
Wang, P. S., 534 
Wang, Q., 230, 588 
Wang, V. O., 26 
Wang, X., 117, 581 
Ward, G., 215 


Ward, L. M., 128, 132, 599 
Ward, T., 523 

Ward, W. C., 261 
Ward-Baker, P. D., 425 
Warden, C. A., 580 
Wark, B., 102 
Warshauer-Baker, E., 26 
Washburn, M. F., 18 
Wasserman, E. A., 177 
Waterhouse, J., 151 
Waters, G., 88 
Watkins, C., 375 
Watkins, D., 300 
Watkins, L. R., 523 
Watson, C. S., 117 
Watson, J. B., 17, 19-20 
Watson, M., 486 
Watson, R., 374 
Watters, E., 538 
Waxman, S., 269 
Webb, R., 168 

Weber, A. L., 604 
Weber, R., 202 
Wechsler, D., 290 
Wechsler, H., 165 
Weed, S., 366 

Weeks, G. R., 374 
Weeks, M., 600 
Wegencer, D. T., 580 
Wehrle, R., 148 
Wehrwein, P., 144 
Weinberg, M. S., 368 
Weinberg, R. A., 302 
Weiner, B. A., 504 
Weiner, I. B., 466 
Weinman, M., 372 
Weinstein, L., 390 
Weinstein, M., 475 
Weisberg, J. N., 122 
Weisner, C., 164 
Weiss, A., 497 

Weiss, M. R., 592 
Weiss, W. M., 592 
Weissman, M. M., 534, 558 
Weisz, A. N., 371 
Welkowitz, L.A., 534 
Wells, G. A., 227 
Welsh, M., 252 

Wenar, C., 530 

Wenk, G. L., 237 
Wensley, D., 75 
Wenzel, A., 224, 367 
Werblin, F., 109 
Werker, J. F., 389 
Werner, J. S., 109 
Wertheimer, M., 11, 17, 126 
West, D. S., 161, 321 
West, J. R., 296 

West, R. L., 158, 430 
Westen, D., 560, 561 
Westerberg, H., 429 
Westerhausen, R., 128 
Westerterp, K. R., 319 
Wetter, D. W., 487 
Wettstein, R., 504 
Whaley, B. B., 494 
Whisman, M., 367 
Whitbourne, S. K., 417, 424, 493 
White, M. P., 268, 495 
White, N. M., 200 
Whitebread, D., 400 
Whitehouse, W. G., 156 
Whitfield, J. B., 167 
Whitfield, K. E., 300 
Whorf, B. L., 269 
Wickelgren, E. A., 130 
Widaman, K., 390 
Widiger, T., 474 
Widiger, T. A., 509, 510, 530, 537 
Widmaier, E. P., 83 
Widmeyer, W. N., 584 


Name Index 


Wiebe, J. S., 480 
Wiehe, V. R., 370 
Wielgosz, A. T., 486 
Wiesel, D., 17 
Wiesel, T., 108, 109 
Wiggins, J. S., 450 
Wildavsky, B., 271 
Wiley, C., 606 
Wilgoren, J., 401 
Wilkinson, H. A., 568 
Wilkinson, L., 202 
Willander, J., 118 
Willard, J., 598 
Willems, R. M., 268 
Williams, C. J., 368 
Williams, D., 380 


Williams, J. E., 344, 486, 566 


Williams, J. H. G., 150 
Williams, S., 61 
Williamson, P., 297 
Willingham, B., 337 
Willis, G., 6 

Willis, G. L., 67 
Willis, J., 451 

Willis, S. L., 429 
Willness, C. R., 346 
Wills, K., 493 

Wills, T., 487 
Wilson, B., 211 
Wilson, D., 598 
Wilson, D. J., 591 
Wilson, G. D., 368 
Wilson, G. T., 195 
Wilson, P. J., 119 
Wilson, T. D., 602 
Wilson, T. G., 323 
Wiltshire, S., 488 
Windholz, G., 257 
Winerman, L., 610 
Winik, L. W., 521 
Winkielman, P., 600 
Winner, E., 298 
Winocur, G., 236 
Winogard, E., 224 
Winsler, A., 402 
Winson, J., 148 
Winstead, B. A., 419 
Winston, J. S., 109 
Winter, D. G., 326, 425 
Winters, B. D., 67 
Wise, E. H., 50 
Wiseman, R., 135 
Wismar, K., 474 
Witelson, S., 368 
Withey, S. B., 498 
Witt, C. M., 122 
Wittchen, H., 515 
Wittenbrink, B., 601 
Wixted, J. T., 233 
Wohl, J., 562 

Wolf, Y., 310 

Wolfe, D. A., 371 
Wolfe, M. S., 430 
Wolff, N., 570 
Wolff, R., 92 
Wolitzky, D. L., 547 
Woller, K., 493 
Wolpert, E. A., 148 
Wood, E., 353 
Wood, J. M., 466 
Wood, J. P., 108 
Wood, J. V., 47 
Wood, W., 353, 580 
Woodruff, N., 296 
Woodruff, S. I., 489 
Woods, S. C., 318, 319 
Woodson, S. R. J., 333 
Woody, E. Z., 155 
Woolf, V., 282 
Woollett, K., 219 





l-13 


1-14 Name Index 


World Health Organization 
(WHO), 317 

World Mental Health Survey 
Consortium (WHO), 534, 536 

Worthley, R., 280 

Wren, A. M., 319 

Wright, K., 151 

Wright, R. A., 313 

Wrosch, C., 431 

Wrzesniewski, 480 

Wu, L-T., 169 

Wu, W-Y., 580 

Wu, Y., 425 

Wuensch, K. L., 611 

Wuethrich B., 165 

Wundt, W., 15-16 

Wurtz, R. H., 87 

Wyatt, H. R., 317 

Wynn, K., 408 

Wyra, M., 237 


X 
Xiao, Z., 520 
Xu, M., 318 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Y 

Yamawaki, N., 371 
Yancey, C., 202 
Yang, J., 283 

Yang, L., 565 

Yano, S., 278 

Yao, S-Q., 291 

Yao, Y., 220 

Yapko, M. D., 149 
Yardley, L., 8 

Yates, A., 504 
Yeager, D., 474 
Yechiam, E., 611 
Yeomans, M. R., 119 
Yermolayeva, Y., 606 
Yesilyaprak, B., 474 
Yonas, A., 130 
Yordanova, J., 144 
Yoshino, K., 201 
Yost, M. R., 371 

You, S. H., 88 
Yougentob, S. L., 119 
Young, G. A., Jr., 493 
Young, T., 111 


Young-DeMarco, L., 366 
Yumru, M., 564 


Z 

Zacks, J., 244 

Zahidi, S., 344 
Zaitsu, W., 227 
Zajonc, R. B., 328, 604 
Zaragoza, M. S., 227 
Zarren, J. L., 156 
Zaslavsky, O., 257 
Zaslow, J., 371 
Zaslow, M., 401 
Zatorre, R., 87 

Zayas V., 176 
Zebrowitz, L. A., 605 
Zebrowitz-McArthur, L., 587 
Zeigler, D. W., 166 
Zetocha, K., 224 
Zevon, M., 487 
Zhang, D., 296 
Zhang, F., 321 
Zhang, G., 605 
Zhang, Y., 256 
Zheng, H., 310 


Zhou, Z., 112, 115, 119 
Zhour, Y-H., 291 

Zians, J., 326 

Ziegler, M., 281 

Zigler, E., 296 

Zigler, E. F., 390 

Zika, S., 475, 476 
Zilbergeld, B, 358 
Zimbardo, P. G., 26, 593 
Zimmerman, R. R., 399 
Zimmermann, U. S., 167 
Zimprich, D., 281 
Zimring, F. M., 557 
Zinkham, G. M., 223 
Zito, J. M., 567 
Zlotnick, C., 531 
Zolotor, A., 188 
Zuckerman, M., 311 
Zurbriggen, E. L., 371 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Subject Index 


Page numbers followed by f refer to fig- 
ures. Page numbers preceded by an A 
refer to the appendix. 


A 
A-B-C model of rational-emotive 
behavior therapy, 553f-554 
AAA Foundation for Traffic Safety, 101 
Abnormal behavior, 504 
Abnormality 
controversies underlining our 
understanding of, 535-537 
defining, 503-504 
different perspectives on, 505f-508 
DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria for, 
508-510 
Absolute thresholds, 100-101 
Acetylcholine (ACh), 67f 
Acrophobia, 515 
Action potential, 67f 
Activation information modulation 
(AIM) theory, 149 
Activation-synthesis dream theory, 
147f, 149 
Activity theory of aging, 431 
Acupuncture, 122 
Adaptation, 102 
Adaptive testing, 292 
Addictive drugs, 159-161 
ADHD (attention-deficit hyperactivity 
disorder), 506, 530-531 
Adipose tissue, 76f 
Adolescence 
average heights and weights 
during, 398f 
definition of, 412 
drug use by, 160f 
egocentrism of, 418 
marijuana use by, 168f 
myth versus reality of stormy, 
417-418 
rites of passage around the 
world, 420 
smoking as “rite of passage” 
during, 487, 489f 
Adolescence development 
moral and cognitive, 414-416 
physical, 412-414 
puberty and, 412-414, 417-418 
social, 416-420 
Adolescence egocentrism, 418 
Adolescent suicide, 418-420 
Adrenal glands, 76f 
Adulthood development 
aging body and physical changes, 
427-428f 
changing gender roles, 426 
cognitive changes in late adulthood, 
428-432 
emerging adulthood, 422-423f 
growing old, 426—427f 
physical, 423-424 
social, 424-425 
Advertising agency creator, 581 
Afferent (sensory) neurons, 72 


African Americans. See also Racial/ 
ethnicity differences 
adolescent suicide and, 418-419 
psychological tests using norms 
of, 462 
sickle-cell anemia and, 390 
stereotype vulnerability of, 602 
Age differences 
cross-sectional research on, 
383-384 
masturbation and, 365f 
memory span changes and, 409f 
sound range by, 117f 
Age of viability, 388-389 
Aggression 
catharsis of, 609 
definition of, 608 
frustration-aggression approach 
to, 609-610 
as instinctual drive, 608-609 
Is This Aggression? quiz, 609f 
observational learning approach 
to, 610 
Aging 
adjusting to death, 432 
cognitive changes, 428f-430 
growing old, 426—427 
physical changes and, 427-428 
social relationships and, 431-432 
Aging theories 
activity theory of aging, 431 
disengagement theory of 
aging, 431 
genetic preprogramming, 427, 428 
wear-and-tear, 427-428 
AIDS (acquired immune deficiency 
syndrome), 373, 390, 391f 
Alcohol 
effects by consumption, 166f 
overview of, 162f 
Alcohol abuse. See also Drug use/ 
abuse 
alcohol use disorders and, 531 
as avoidant coping strategy, 480 
binge drinking, 164—-165f 
prenatal development and 
mother’s, 391 
Alcohol use disorders, 531 
Alcoholics, 166-167 
Alcoholics Anonymous (AA), 559 
Algorithms, 248 
All-or-none law, 62 
Altruism, 612 
Alzheimer’s disease. See also Organic 
mental disorders 
ACH production relationship 
to, 67 
biological basis of, 531 
brain deterioration due to, 236f 
description and progress of, 430 
memory dysfunctions of, 
235-236 
neuroplasticity relationship to, 88 
new treatment approaches to, 
88-89 


American Psychological Association 
“Help Center,” 573 
Amnesia 
definition of, 236 
dissociative, 520 
dissociative fugue, 520 
Amphetamines, 162-163 
Amygdala. See also Brain 
description of, 83f, 84 
experience of emotions and role 
of, 333-334f 
memory and role of, 218f, 219f 
prejudice and activation of the, 600 
Analytical learning style, 203f 
Androgens, 356 
Androgynous individuals, 354 
Anemia (sickle-cell), 390 
Anger 
aggression and, 608-610 
dealing effectively with, 613 
Animals 
ethics of research on, 52-53 
language used by, 270-271 
Anorexia nervosa, 322-323f, 531 
Anorgasmia, 374 
Anterior pituitary gland, 76f 
Anterograde amnesia, 236 
Antianxiety drugs, 567 
Antidepressant drugs, 565f-566 
Antipsychotic drugs, 564-565f 
Antisocial (sociopathic) personality, 
529-530 
Anxiety disorders 
causes of, 517-518 
definition of, 513 
generalized anxiety disorder, 
515, 516f 
OCD (obsessive-compulsive dis- 
order), 515-517, 517f 
panic disorder, 514-515 
phobic disorder, 513-514f 
Aphasia, 87-88 
Apparent movement, 131 
Applying psychology. See also 
Psychology 
contagion of emotions and 
happiness, 496 
Facebook online friends, 606 
facial expressions of smiling 
athletes, 337 
fetal exposure to testosterone and 
gender behavior, 352 
to forget traumatic memories, 220 
judged at first sight, 451 
Opportunity NYC paying for good 
behavior, 188 
preventing psychological dis- 
orders, 572 
robot artificial intelligence, 286 
role of sleep in memory and 
thinking, 144 
stress and depression by college 
students, 537 
testing value of self- 
affirmations, 47f 


texting while driving distrac- 
tion, 101 
thought-based interface technol- 
ogy helping disabled 
individuals, 82 
understanding modern 
problems, 21 
video games improving cognitive 
functioning in older 
adults, 429 
when hunches save lives, 258 
Archetypes, 445-446 
Archival research 
compared to other methods, 45f 
description of, 37 
Arousal approaches to motivation 
compared to other 
approaches, 315f 
definition of, 311 
Arrangement problems, 252, 
253f, 254f 
Artificial intelligence 
building smarter robot, 286 
computer, 249 
Asian Americans. See also Racial/ 
ethnicity differences 
adolescent suicide and, 419 
alcohol consumption by, 165 
Assessment. See also Evaluation 
attachment, 339 
behavioral, 466-468 
biomedical therapy, 568-569 
intelligence, 287-292 
personality, 461-468, 601 
Association areas of cortex, 87-88 
Assumed-similarity bias, 586 
Ataque de nervios disorder, 538 
Attachment 
assessing, 399 
definition of, 398-399 
terry-cloth “monkey” experiment 
on, 399f 
Attitudes 
cognitive dissonance of conflicting, 
582f-583 
definition of, 579 
factors leading to change in, 
579-580 
link between behavior and, 
581-583 
routes to persuading change 
in, 580-581 
Attribution processes, 584-586 
Attribution theory, 585 
Atypical antipsychotics, 564, 565f 
Authoritarian parents, 401, 402f 
Authoritative parents, 402f 
Autism, 531 
Autobiographical memory, 229f 
Automatic division, 71f, 72 
Autonomic division, 71f, 72 
Autonomic nervous system 
parasympathetic division of, 
72-73f 
sympathetic division of, 72, 73f 


1-15 


1-16 Subject Index 


Autonomy-versus-shame-and-doubt 
stage, 404 
Availability heuristic strategy, 249 
Aversive conditioning, 549 
Avoidant coping, 480 
Awa tribe (New Guinea), 420 
Axons 
description of, 61, 62f 
ganglion (optic nerve), 107f-108 
movement of action potential 
along, 67f 
synapse between dendrite and, 65f 


B 
Babbling, 266 
Background stressors, 475, 476f 
Balance sense, 117-118 
Barbiturates, 162f, 167 
Basilar membrane, 114, 115f 
Beck’s cognitive therapy, 554 
Behavior. See also Sexual behavior 
abnormal, 504 
altruism, 612 
applying conditioning principles 
to, 178-179 
attribution processes for under- 
standing causes of, 584-586 
compliant and obedient, 593-596f 
conscious vs. unconscious causes 
of, 23f, 24 
deterministic view of, 454 
developmental psychology study 
of, 380 
dispositional cause of, 586 
free will versus determinism cause 
of, 20 
gender differences in aggres- 
sive, 348 
gender differences in nonver- 
bal, 349 
how culture, ethnicity, and race 
influence, 25f-26 
instincts and, 309-310 
internal mental processes vs. 
observable, 23f, 24 
link between attitudes and, 
581-583 
neutrotransmitter functions and 
role in, 66-68 
Opportunity NYC paying for 
good, 188 
prosocial, 611f-612 
self-fulfilling prophecy of, 598 
shaping complex, 192 
situational causes of, 585-586 
Type A, B, D patterns of, 485-486 
variables of, 39-40f 
Behavior analysis, 194-195 
Behavior economics, 587 
Behavior modification 
basic steps for, 195 
description of, 194 
Behavioral assessment, 466-468 
Behavioral genetics. See also Genetic 
factors 
definition of, 6f, 9, 74 
new therapeutic treatments 
from, 75 
psychological disorders and, 
74-75 


Behavioral neuroscience. See also 
Neuroscience in your life 
definition of, 6, 7f 
on emotions, 333-334 
key psychology issues and posi- 
tion of, 23f 
memory and the brain, 217-221 
perspective of, 18, 19 
Behavioral neuroscientists, 60 
Behavioral perspective 
of abnormality, 505f, 506 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


description of, 18, 19-20 
key psychology issues and 
position of, 23f 
Behavioral theories of depression, 523 
Behavioral treatment approaches 
brain research on, 552f 
classical conditioning, 548-550 
definition of, 548 
dialectical behavior therapy, 551 
evaluating, 551-552 
operant conditioning, 550-551 
The Bell Curve (Hernstein and 
Murray), 301 
Benevolent sexism, 347 
Bereavement support group, 559 
Biases 
assumed-similarity, 586 
attribution, 586-587 
avoiding experimental, 53-55 
confirmation, 260 
self-service, 587 
Biculturalism, 262 
Big Five personality traits, 449-450f 
Bilingual education 
the brain and, 275 
debate over immersion vs., 
271-272 
Bimodal distribution, A-7 
Binge drinking, 164—165f 
Binge-eating disorder, 531 
Binocular disparity, 129 
Biofeedback 
definition of, 93 
pain management using, 123 
recovery from nervous system 
damage using, 92-93 
Biological constraints on learning, 193 
Biological and evolutionary approaches 
to personality theories, 
454—-457f 
Biological factors. See also 
Evolutionary psychology 
basic biology of sexual behavior, 
356-357f 
gender differences related to, 
351-353 
schizophrenia, 526-528f 
Biologically prepared for 
learning, 181 
Biomedical therapy 
assessing the use of, 568-569 
definition of, 544 
drug therapy, 564-567 
ECT (electroconvulsive 
therapy), 567 
psychosurgery, 568 
Bipolar cells, 107f, 108 
Bipolar disorder 
description of, 522f-523 
mood stabilizers treatment of, 
566-567 
Bisexuals 
definition of, 367 
research findings on, 367-369 
Blackberry addict, 174 
Blame-the-victim phenomenon, 536 
Blind spot, 107-108f 
BMI (body mass index) quiz, 318f 
Bodily kinesthetic intelligence, 282f 
Body mass index (BMI), 317 
Body setting point, 322 
Boomerang children, 418 
Borderline personality disorder, 530 
Bottom-up processing, 128 
Brain. See also Amygdala; 
Neuroscience in your life 
Alzheimer’s disease and deteriora- 
tion of, 236f 
bilingual education and the, 275 
central core (“old brain”) of 
the, 81f, 83 


cerebral cortex of the, 81f, 83f, 
84-88 
cross-section of the, 79f 
daydreaming activity of the, 151 
different kinds of intelligence and 
areas of the, 284f 
hemispheres of the, 89-92f 
hippocampus of the, 83t, 84, 217, 
218f-219f, 334f 
how behavioral therapy changes 
your, 552f 
hypothalamus of the, 83, 319f 
illustrated diagram of major struc- 
tures, 83f 
neuroplasticity of the, 88-89f 
neuroscience of memory and, 
217-221 
processing the visual message, 
108-109f 
psychosurgery of the, 568 
research on personality traits and 
related neurons in the, 457f 
reticular formation in the, 81f 
schizophrenia and changes in 
the, 528f 
sending message from eyes to the, 
106-108 
social support research and activa- 
tion in the, 482f 
technology and techniques used to 
study the, 79-81 
thalamus of the, 83 
Broca’s aphasia, 87 
Bulimia, 322-323, 531 
Bystander diffusion of responsibility 
study 
description of, 41—46f 
Kitty Genovese case explained 
through, 32, 34-35, 41—46f, 611 


C 


Caffeine consumption, 161f-162 
Calorie expenditures, 324f 
Cancer 
psychological aspects of, 486-487 
research on personality type 
and, 487 
Cannon-Bard theory of 
emotions, 332f 
Carbamazepine (Tegretol), 565f, 566 
Cardinal traits, 449 
Caring morality, 415-416 
Case managers, 557 
Case study 
compared to other methods, 45f 
description of, 38-39 
Cataclysmic events, 474, 476f 
Catharsis, 609 
Cell body, 62 
Cell phone ring frequency, 117f 
Cells 
bipolar, 107f, 108 
ganglion, 107f, 108 
hair, 115, 118 
obesity related to fat, 321 
olfactory, 118, 119 
Central core (“old brain”) 
cerebellum in the, 81 
description of, 81f 
Central nervous system (CNS), 
70-71f 
Central route processing, 580 
Central tendency, A-7 
Central traits, 449, 584 
Cerebellum, 81 
Cerebral cortex (“new brain”) 
association areas of the, 87-88 
description of the, 81f, 83f, 84 
four lobes of the, 85f 
motor area of the, 86 
sensory area of the, 86-87 


Changing attitudes 
factors which may lead to, 579-580 
routes to persuasion for, 580f-581 
Chilcotin Indian tribe, 202 
Child abuse 
child protection caseworkers role 
in treating, 403 
sexual, 371-372f 
Child protection caseworker, 403 
Child-care centers, 400-401f 
Childhood development 
attachment, 398-399 
child care outside the home and, 
400-401f 
Erikson’s theory of psychosocial 
development, 403-404 
father’s role in, 399-400 
information-processing 
approaches, 408-410 
parenting styles and social, 
401-403 
physical, 397 
social behavior, 397-404 
Childhood disorders 
ADHD, 506, 530-531 
autism, 531 
Childhood sexual abuse, 371-372f 
Children 
adulthood development and ties 
to, 425 
bilingual education of, 271-272 
cognitive development of, 404—408 
fetal exposure to testosterone and 
gender behavior of, 352 
gender differences in social environ- 
ment of, 353-354 
impact of divorce on, 425 
infancy through childhood develop- 
ment, 397-410 
integrating individuals with men- 
tal retardation, 296-297 
language development in, 266-269 
newborns, 393-397 
Opportunity NYC program paying 
for good behavior, 188 
principle of conservation learning 
by, 406, 407f 
reliability as eyewitnesses, 228 
Terman’s study on intellectually 
gifted, 297-298 
Chlamydia, 372 
Chromosomes, 386, 387f 
Chunk 
definition of, 211 
short-term memory and, 211-212 
Cingulotomy, 568 
Circadian rhythms, 151 
Circumcision 
female, 360 
male, 360 
Classical conditioning 
basic process of, 177f 
the basics of, 176-178 
challenging basic assumptions 
of, 180-181 
comparing operant and, 193-194f 
definition of, 176 
“Little Albert” case study on, 178 
Pavlov’s experiments on, 16f, 
176-177f, 180 
as type of learning, 175 
Classical conditioning treatments 
aversive, 549 
description of, 548-549 
exposure, 550 
operant, 183-195, 550-551 
systematic, 549-550 
Clinical mental health counselors, 545f 
Clinical neuropsychology, 6f, 9 
Clinical psychologists, 545f 
Clinical psychology, 7f, 8 


Clinical social workers, 545f 
Closure gestalt law, 126-127f 
Cocaine, 164 
Cochlea, 114, 115f 
Cognitive abilities. See also Reasoning; 
Social cognition; Thinking 
gender differences in, 349-350 
interaction of brain and socializa- 
tion in gender, 350f 
video games improving older 
adult functioning, 429 
Cognitive appraisal, 554 
Cognitive approaches to motivation 
compared to other 
approaches, 315f 
definition of, 313 
Cognitive development 
adolescence, 414-416 
childhood, 404-408 
definition of, 404 
Piaget's theory of, 405f—408 
Vygotsky’s zone of Proximal 
development (ZPD), 409f-410 
Cognitive dissonance, 582f-583 
Cognitive learning theory 
description of, 198 
on latent learning, 198-200 
on learning styles, 202-203 
on observational learning, 200-202 
Cognitive perspective 
of abnormality, 505f, 506-507 
on anorexic patients, 323f 
description of, 18, 20 
key psychology issues and posi- 
tion of, 23f 
pain management through cogni- 
tive restructuring, 123 
Cognitive psychology 
definition of, 6, 7f, 242 
on problem solving, 251-263 
on thinking and reasoning, 
243-249 
Cognitive theory 
of depression, 523 
of schizophrenia, 528 
Cognitive thinking 
metacognition of, 408-409 
The Need for Cognition quiz, 580f 
Cognitive treatment approaches 
definition of, 552 
evaluating, 554 
psychotherapy using, 552-554 
Cognitive-affective processing system 
(CAPS) theory, 453 
Cognitive-behavioral approach, 553 
Collective unconscious, 445-446 
Collectivistic orientation, 588 
College students. See also Education 
binge drinking by, 164-165f 
problems reported by, 535f 
pros and cons as research 
subjects, 50f 
stress and depression of, 537 
Color blindness, 110-111 
Color vision, 111-112f 
Columbia shuttle disaster (2003), 
591-592 
Communication. See also Language 
advertising agency creator use 
of, 581 
effective health-care provider and 
patient, 
493—494f 
overgeneralization of language 
rules during, 267 
persuasion to change attitudes 
through, 579-581 
positively and negatively framed 
messages, 495 
telegraphic speech, 267 
Community psychology, 570f-571 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Companionate love, 605-606 
Compliance 
definition of, 593 
effective communication to pro- 
mote, 493-494f 
increasing medical advice, 494-495 
obedience through, 595-596f 
techniques used to gain, 593-595 
Compulsion, 516 
Computers 
adaptive testing using, 292 
artificial intelligence of, 249 
Conception 
alternative paths to, 391-392 
illustration of, 387f 
Concepts 
categorizing the world through, 
245-246 
definition of, 245 
prototype representations of, 
245, 247f 
Concrete operational stage, 405f, 406 
Conditional positive regard, 458 
Conditioned response (CR), 176-177, 
178 
Conditioned stimulus (CS), 176, 
177f, 178 


Conditioning. See Classical conditioning 


Cones 
retina, 106 
trichromatic theory of color vision 
on, 111 
Confirmation bias, 260 
Conformity 
definition of, 590-591 
groupthink, 591-592 
research findings on nature of, 591 
to social roles, 592-593 
Conscious vs. unconscious issue, 
23f, 24 
Consciousness 
cross-cultural routes to altered 
states of, 157 
definition of, 140 
divided, 155 
hypnosis and, 155-156 
meditation for regulating, 156-158 
Consolidation of memory, 220 
Constructive memory processes, 
226-227 
Contingency contracting, 551 
Continuous reinforcement sched- 
ule, 189 
Control groups, 41 
Convergent thinking, 261-262 
Conversion disorder, 518, 519f 
Coping with stress 
avoidant coping, 480 
defense mechanisms used for, 
480-481 
effective strategies for, 483 
emotion-focused, 480 
GAS (general adaptation syn- 
drome) on course of, 478f-479 
learned helplessness for, 
481, 523 
problem-focused, 480 
social support research on brain 
activation when, 482f 
Coping styles 
hardy personality, 481-482 
social support by turning to others, 
482-483 
Coronary heart disease, 485-486 
Corpus callosum 
description of, 83f, 90 
split-brain patients and the, 92f 
Correlation coefficient 
definition of, A-16-A-17 
IQ scores of mothers and 
daughters, A-17f 


negative relationship, A-15, A-16f 
positive relationship, A-15, A-16f 
Cortex, 76f 
Counseling 
genetic, 75 
rehabilitation, 86 
Counseling psychologists, 545f 
Counseling psychology, 7f, 8 
Creative nonadherence, 492 
Creativity 
Dr. Matson’s Cork-EZ invention 
as, 242 
problem solving and, 261 
thinking critically and, 262-263 
Critical (or sensitive) periods, 389 
Cross-cultural psychology, 7f, 8 
Cross-sectional research, 383-384 
Crystallized intelligence, 281, 287f, 429 
Cue-dependent forgetting, 234 
Cultural differences 
on academic success, 25f 
alcohol consumption and, 165 
in attribution biases, 587-588 
depth perception influenced 
by, 133-134f 
in expression of emotions, 335f-336 
in memory, 229-230 
physician-patient communication 
and, 494 
rites of passage around the 
world, 420 
Culture. See also Exploring diversity 
attribution biases in context 
of, 587-588 
brain lateralization impacted by, 91 
of DSM categorization, 538-539 
female circumcision debate 
over, 360 
gender division of labor and, 
351-352 
Horney’s stress on personality 
and, 446 
how behavior is influenced 
by, 25f-26 
how learning is influenced by, 
202-203 
information framing influenced 
by, 230 
language and biculturalism, 272 
Culture-fair IQ test, 301 


D 
Daily hassles, 475, 476f 
“Danger feeling,” 258 
DARE (Drug Abuse Resistance 
Education), 161 
Dark adaptation phenomenon, 106 
Date rape 
definition of, 370-371 
drugs used for, 162f, 167, 375 
lowering risks of, 375 
Death 
Kiibler-Ross stages of grief, 432 
older adults and adjusting to, 432 
Decay (memory), 233 
Decision making 
brain research on moral, 613 
groupthink, 591-592 
Declarative memory, 215, 216f 
Deepwater Horizon oil rig explosion 
(2010), 4 
Defense mechanisms 
coping with stress using, 480-481 
Freud’s psychoanalysis approach 
on, 442-444, 546-547 
Deinstitutionalization, 570f 
Delusions, 525 
Dendrite 
description of, 61, 62f 
synapse between axon and, 65f 
Dependent variables, 42 


Subject Index 1-17 


Depressants 
alcohol, 162f, 164-167 
barbiturates, 162f, 167 
overview of, 162f 
rohypnol (“date rape drug”), 
162f, 167 
Depression 
college students suffering 
from, 537 
preventing onset of, 572 
Depth perception, 129-130, 133-134f 
DES (diethylstilbestrol), 368, 391f 
Descriptive and correlational research 
compared to other methods, 45f 
description of, 39 
positive and negative correlation 
findings in, 39-40f 
Descriptive statistics 
central tendency, A-7 
comparing the mean, median, 
mode, A-8 
definition of, A-5-A-6 
the mean, A-6—-A-7 
the median, A-7, A-8 
the mode, A-7 
normal and skewed distributions, 
A-8f-A-9f 
Determinism 
definition of, 20, 24 
free will versus, 20, 23f, 24 
Developmental psychology. See also 
Prenatal development; 
Psychology 
adolescence, 412-420 
adulthood, 422—432 
definition of, 6, 7f, 380, 381 
infancy through childhood, 
397-410 
newborns, 393-397 
stages versus continuous, 407-408 
Devil’s tuning fork, 133f 
Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of 
Mental Disorders, Fourth 
Edition (DSM-IV-TR) 
categories of disorders represented 
in the, 509f 
description of, 508-509 
five Axis of information categories 
included in the, 508 
Rosenhan’s study showing the 
shortcomings of the, 510 
Dialectical behavior therapy, 551 
Dieting strategies, 323-324 
Difference thresholds, 101-102 
Diffusion of responsibility theory, 35, 
41-46f, 611 
Discrimination 
definition of, 598 
measuring, 601 
reducing consequences of, 601-602 
Discriminative stimulus, 191 
Disease. See also Wellness 
AIDS (acquired immune defi- 
ciency syndrome), 373, 
390, 391f 
cancer, 486-487 
coronary heart disease, 485-486 
STIs (sexually transmitted infec- 
tions), 372-373 
Disengagement theory of aging, 431 
Dispositional cause of behavior, 
586 
Dissociation, 155 
Dissociative amnesia, 520 
Dissociative disorders 
definition of, 518-519 
DID (dissociative identity dis- 
order), 519-520 
dissociative amnesia, 520 
dissociative fugue, 520 
Dissociative fugue, 520 


1-18 Subject Index 


Dissociative identity disorder (DID), 
519 
Distractions 
multitasking, 101 
texting while driving, 21, 101 
Divalproex sodium (Depakote), 
565f, 566 
Divergent thinking, 261-262 
Diversity. See Exploring diversity 
Divided consciousness, 155 
Divorce. See also Marriage, 425 
Do You Seek Out Sensation? quiz, 312f 
Doom (video game), 202 
Door-in-the-face technique, 594 
Dopamine (DA), 67f, 68 
Dopamine hypothesis, 527 
Double standard, 365-367 
Down syndrome, 278, 296, 390 
Dream interpretation, 546 
Dream theories 
activation-synthesis, 147f, 149 
dreams-for-survival, 147f, 148-149 
unconscious wish fulfill- 
ment, 147f-148 
Dreams. See also Sleep 
daydreams, 151 
function and meaning of, 146f-149 
latent content of, 147 
manifest content of, 147, 148f 
three theories of, 147f 
Dreams-for-survival theory, 147f, 
148-149 
Drive-reduction approaches to 
motivation 
compared to other approaches, 
315f 
definition of, 310 
homeostasis and, 310-311f 
Drives, 310 
Drug reduction programs, 161 
Drug therapy 
antianxiety drugs, 565f, 567 
antidepressant drugs, 565f-566 
antipsychotic drugs, 564—565f 
definition of, 564 
mood stabilizers, 565f, 566-567 
Drug use/abuse. See also Alcohol 
abuse; Substance abuse 
counselors 
addictive, 159-161 
as avoidant coping strategy, 480 
depressants, 162f, 164-167 
hallucinogens, 163f, 168-169 
marijuana, 163f, 168f 
narcotics, 162f-163f, 167-168 
nervous system impacted by, 160f 
prenatal development and 
mother’s, 390-391f 
psychoactive, 159 
psychoactive substance use dis- 
order, 531 
steroid, 77f 
stimulants, 161/-164 
Drugs 
date-rape, 162f, 167, 375 
pain management, 123 
therapeutic, 564-567 


E 
Ear 
balance and semicircular canals 
of, 115f, 117-118 
major parts of the, 114—115f 
sensing sound, 114-118 
Eardrum, 114, 115f 
Eating. See also Hunger 
as avoidant coping strategy, 480 
motivation behind, 317-319f 
social factors in, 320-322 
Eating disorders 
anorexia nervosa, 322-323f, 531 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


as avoidant coping strategy, 480 
binge-eating disorder, 531 
bulimia, 322-323, 531 
description of, 531 
Ebbinghaus’ memory experi- 
ment, 232-233 
Eclectic approach to therapy, 562 
Economic crisis coping, 21 
Ecstasy (MDMA), 163f, 168-169 
Education. See also College stu- 
dents; Schools 
bilingual vs. immersion, 271 
gender differences in the environ- 
ment of, 353-354 
reducing prejudice and discrimi- 
nation through, 602 
special, 296-297 
Education for All Handicapped 
Children Act, 296 
EEG (electroencephalogram), 79-80f 
Efferent (motor) neurons, 72 
Ego, 440 
Ego-integrity-versus-despair 
stage, 417 
Egocentric thought, 405-406 
Ejaculation, 359 
Elaborative rehearsal, 213 
Electroconvulsive therapy (ECT), 567 
Electrode pain management, 123 
Embryo, 388 
Emerging adulthood, 422-423f 
“EMI” Bach composition experiment, 
249 
Emotion-focused coping, 408 
Emotional disturbances, 526 
Emotional insulation, 481 
Emotional intelligence, 285-286, 
287f 
Emotions. See also Feelings 
aggression and, 608-610 
amygdala role in experience of, 
333-334f 
anger, 608-610, 613 
Cannon-Bard theory of, 332f 
CAPS (cognitive-affective process- 
ing system) theory on, 453 
contagion of happiness and, 496 
contemporary perspectives on 
neuroscience of, 333-334 
cultural differences in expression 
of, 335f-336 
definition of, 328-329 
determining the range of, 329-330f 
expressed, 528 
functions of, 329 
infant recognition of, 396f-397 
James-Lange theory on gut reac- 
tions and, 331-332f 
lack of sleep impact on, 146 
making sense of multiple perspec- 
tives on, 334-335 
prenatal development and moth- 
er’s, 390 
roots of, 330-335 
Schachter-Singer theory of, 
332f-333 
Employer personality testing, 467-468 
Encoding, 209f 
Endocrine system. See also Hormones 
description of, 75 
location and function of major 
glands in, 76f 
pituitary gland of the, 76f-77 
Endorphins, 67f, 68 
Entrapment, 592 
Environmental factors. See also 
Socialization 
intelligence determinant of genet- 
ics versus, 300-303 
language and verbal skills and, 
90-91 


nature-nurture debate over, 
381-383 
prenatal development and, 
390-39 1f 
schizophrenia, 528 
Episodic memory, 215-216f 
Erectile dysfunction, 374 
Erikson’s theory of psychosocial 
development, 403-404, 
416f-417, 446 
Erogenous zones, 358 
Estrogens 
definition of, 356 
studies on task abilities and levels 
of, 351 
Ethical issues 
animal research, 52-53 
informed consent, 49-50 
research and related, 49-51f 
Ethnicity. See also Race 
definition of, 26 
divisions on acceptable names 
for, 26 
how behavior is influenced 
by, 25f-26 
Evaluation. See also Assessment 
behavioral treatment approaches, 
551-552 
cognitive treatment approaches, 
554 
of different psychotherapy 
approaches, 559-562 
humanistic approaches to person- 
ality, 458-459 
humanistic therapy, 558 
personality learning approaches, 
454 
psychodynamic therapy, 547-548 
solutions, 257, 260 
trait theory used for personal- 
ity, 450-451 
Evidence-based psychotherapy, 
561-562 
Evolutionary psychology. See also 
Biological factors 
on causes of depression, 523 
definition of, 7f, 8, 73 
nervous system evolutionary 
foundations studied by, 
73-75 
Excitatory message, 66 
Excitement phase, 359 
Experiment, definition of, 41 
Experimental bias, 53-55 
Experimental groups 
definition of, 41 
depiction of drug propranolol 
testing, 44f 
Experimental manipulation, 41 
Experimental psychologist, 6 
Experimental psychology 
definition of, 6, 7f 
research approach of, 41-46 
Experimental research 
compared to other methods, 45f 
description of, 41 
experimental and control groups 
used in, 41-42 
independent and dependent vari- 
ables studied, in, 42 
Latané and Darley’s bystander, 
41-46f 
random assignment of participants 
in, 42-44 
replication of, 46 
treatment during, 41-42 
Experimenter expectations, 53 
Explicit memory, 224-225 
Exploring diversity. See also 
Culture; Racial/ethnicity 
differences 


attribution biases in cultural con- 
text, 587-588 
bilingual education, 271-272 
cross-cultural altered states of 
consciousness, 157 
cross-cultural differences in mem- 
ory, 229-230 
culture of DSM, 538-539 
culture-fair IQ test, 30 
expression of emotions and, 
335f-336 
female circumcision, 360 
how learning is influenced by cul- 
ture, 202-203 
international promotion of smok- 
ing, 490 
interplay of biology and environ- 
ment, 90-91 
psychotherapy consideration of 
race and ethnic factors, 562 
rites of passage around the 
world, 420 
understanding how culture, 
ethnicity, and race influence 
behavior, 25-26 
Exposure treatments, 550 
Expressed emotion, 528 
Extinction, 179f-180 
Extramarital sex, 367 
Extrasensory perception (ESP), 135 
Extraversion, 449f 
Extrinsic motivation, 313 
Eyes. See also Vision 
accommodation by, 106 
basic cells of, 107f 
binocular disparity of left and 
right, 129 
light response by, 105f-106 
optic nerve of, 107f-108, 109f 
retina of the, 106 
sending message to the brain 
from, 106-108 
Eyewitnesses memory 
children’s reliability as, 228 
memory-related errors of, 227-228 
recollection of speed in car acci- 
dent, 228f 
repressed and false memo- 
ries, 228-229 


F 
Faceblindness, 98 
Facebook, 606 
Facial expressions 
facial-feedback hypothesis on, 
336 
smiling athletes, 337 
Facial-affect program, 336 
Facial-feedback hypothesis, 336 
Factor analysis, 449 
Fallingwater (Frank Lloyd 
Wright), 263 
False memories, 228-229 
Familial retardation, 296 
Family therapy, 559 
Fathers, 399-400 
“Fearless Peer” model, 551 
Feature detection, 109 
Feelings. See also Emotions 
angry, 608-610, 613 
happiness, 495-498f 
labeling our, 329-330f 
love, 605-607f 
Female circumcision, 360 
Fertilization 
genetics of, 387f 
IVF (In vitro fertilization), 392 
Fetal alcohol syndrome (FAS), 296, 391 
Fetus 
age of viability, 388-389 
definition of, 388 


photos of live, 389 
sensitive periods of, 389 
Fight-or-flight response, 329 
First impressions, 451 
Fixations, 441 
Fixed-interval reinforcement schedule, 
190f-191 
Fixed-ratio reinforcement schedule, 
189-190f 
Flashbulb memories, 225—226f 
Fluid intelligence, 280-281, 287f, 429 
Fluoxetine (Prozac), 565f, 566 
Flynn effect, 302-303 
fMRI (functional magnetic resonance 
imaging) scans, 80f 
Foot-in-the-door technique, 593-594 
Forebrain, 81 
Forgetting 
Ebbinghaus’ experiment on, 
232-233 
memory dysfunctions and afflic- 
tions of, 235-237 
proactive and retroactive interfer- 
ence of, 234-235f 
strategies for coping with, 237 
why we experience, 233f-234 
Formal operational stage, 405f, 
406-407 
Free association, 546 
Free will 
definition of, 24 
determinism versus, 20, 23f, 24 
humanistic emphasis on, 20 
Frequency distribution, A-5, A-6f 
Frequency theory of hearing, 116-117 
Freud, Sigmund 
on aggression as instinctual 
drive, 608-609 
contributions and influence of, 16f, 
18, 19f 
defense mechanisms identified by, 
442-444, 546-547 
evaluating legacy of, 444-445 
latent content of dreams theory 
of, 147 
manifest content of dreams theory 
by, 147, 148f 
neo-Freudian psychoanalysts 
influenced by, 445-446 
psychoanalysis approach of, 
46-547 


psychoanalytic theory of, 439-444 
unconscious wish fulfillment 
dream theory of, 147f-148 
Freudian slip, 439 
Friendship. See also Social relationships 
childhood relationships with 
peers, 400 
need for affiliation and, 325-326 
social support of, 482 
Frontal lobes, 85f 
Frustration-aggression theory, 609-610 
Functional fixedness, 259 
Functionalism, 16-17 
Fundamental attribution error, 587 


G 


g or g-factor, 280 
Gamete intrafallopian transfer 
(GIFT), 392 
Gamma knife surgery, 568 
Gamma-amino butyric acid 
(GABA), 67f 
Ganglion cells, 107f, 108 
Gardner’s multiple intelligences, 
281-282f, 287f 
Gate-control theory of pain, 122 
Gender, 343 
Gender differences. See also Men; 
Women 
adolescent suicide and, 418-419 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


average heights and weights dur- 
ing childhood, 398f 
binge drinking and, 164-165f 
biological and evolutionary factors 
of, 351-353 
brain lateralization and, 90-91 
cognitive abilities, 349f-351 
color blindness and, 110-111 
happiness and, 497 
learning styles and, 203 
major depression and, 521 
masturbation and, 364-365f 
need for power and, 326 
pain sensitivity and, 121 
personality factors, 348-349 
processing information and, 350f 
range of ages for major sexual 
changes, 413f 
sense of smell and, 118-119 
sense of taste and, 119 
social environment impact 
on, 353-354 
World Economic Forum gender 
equality rankings, 344f 
Gender division of labor, 351-352 
Gender roles 
definition of, 343 
family life and changes in, 426 
society expectations for, 343-344 
Gender schema, 354 
Gender stereotypes 
androgynous individuals defy- 
ing, 354 
research findings on, 344f 
Gene therapy, 75, 388 
General adaptation syndrome (GAS), 
478f-479 
Generalization (stimulus), 191 
Generalized anxiety disorder 
definition of, 515 
frequency of symptoms in cases 
of, 516f 
OCD (obsessive-compulsive 
disorder), 515-517 
Generativity-versus-stagnation 
stage, 417 
Genes, 386-387 
Genetic counseling, 75 
Genetic factors. See also Behavioral 
genetics 
body setting point and, 322 
characteristics influenced by, 382f 
fetus influenced by, 389-390 
inherited roots of personal- 
ity, 455f-456 
IQ (intelligence quotient) heritability, 
301-302f, 303 
Genetic preprogramming theories of 
aging, 427, 428 
Genital herpes, 372 
Genital warts, 373 
Genitals (sex organs) 
cutaway side views of, 357f 
definition of, 356 
Genovese, Kitty, 32, 34-35 
Gestalt psychology 
description of, 17 
early development of, 16f 
Gestalt (or perceptual) whole, 128f 
laws of organization, 126f-127 
Glial cells, 61 
Glutamate, 67f, 565 
Gonorrhea, 373 
Graded exposure, 550 
Grammar 
parts of, 265-266 
universal, 268 
Group IQ testing, 291 
Group therapy, 558-559 
Groups 
conformity in, 590-593 


definition of, 590 
foundations of prejudice by, 599-600 
norms formed by, 590 
social supporter in, 591 
Groupthink, 591-592 
Gut reactions (hunches), 258, 331-332f 


Habit-reversal training (HRT), 544 
Habituation, 395 
Hair cells, 115, 118 
Hallucinations, 525, 527 
Hallucinogens, 163f, 168-169 
Halo effect, 586 
Happiness 
characteristics of happy 
people, 495-497 
contagion of emotions and, 496 
money and relationship to, 
497-498 
percentage in U.S. rating their 
level of, 498f 
subjective well-being and, 495 
Hardiness, 481-482 
Health promotion 
following medical advice, 492-495 
health-care providers and patient 
communication, 493-494f 
increasing compliance with medi- 
cal advice, 494-495 
Health psychology 
definition of, 7f, 8, 472 
promoting health and well- 
ness, 492-498f 
psychological aspects of illness 
and well-being, 485-490 
stress and coping, 473-483 
Hearing 
major parts of the ear, 114—-115f 
range by age group, 117f 
sensing sound, 114-118 
speech perception and, 117 
Heart, 76f 
“Help Center” (American 
Psychological Association), 573 
Helping behavior 
altruism, 612 
basic steps of, 611f-612 
Hemispheres 
cultural and gender differences in 
use of, 90-91 
definition of brain, 89-90 
lateralization of, 90 
split-brain patients and function 
of, 91-92f 
Heritability 
definition of, 301 
IQ (intelligence quotient), 
301-302f, 303 
Hermaphrodite, 370 
Heterosexuality 
definition of, 365 
marital sex, 367f 
premarital sex and double stan- 
dard, 365-367 
Heuristics, 248-249 
Hippocampus 
description and function of, 83t, 84 
experience of emotion and role 
of, 334f 
memory and role of, 217, 218f-219f 
Hispanic test norms, 462 
Histogram, A-5 
Homeostasis, 83, 310-311f 
Homosexuals 
definition of, 367 
research findings on, 367-369 
Hormone therapy (HT), 423-424 
Hormones. See also Endocrine system 
androgens, 356 
description of, 75-76 


Subject Index 1-19 


estrogens, 351, 356 

ingestion of artificial, 77f 

progesterone, 356 

as sexual orientation factor, 368 

testosterone, 168, 352 
“Hot lines,” 571 
How Sensitive Are You? quiz, 100f 
How Stressful Is Your Life? quiz, 477f 
HOXA1 gene, 74 
Human Genome Project, 387-388 
Human resources manager, 467 
Humanistic approaches to personality 

description of, 457-458 

evaluating, 458-459 

Rogers’ self-actualization, 458f 
Humanistic perspective 

of abnormality, 505f, 507 

description of, 18, 20 

key psychology issues and posi- 

tion of, 23f 

Humanistic therapy 

definition of, 556 

evaluating, 558 

person-centered therapy, 556 
Hunches (gut reactions), 258, 331-332f 
Hunger. See also Eating 

biological factors in regulation 

of, 318-319 

hypothalamus role in, 319f 
Hyperthymestic syndrome, 208 
Hypnosis 

consciousness during, 155-156 

definition of, 154 

dissociation from, 155 

as pain strategy, 123 

process of, 154 
Hypochondriasis, 518 
Hypothalamus, 76f, 319f 
Hypotheses. See also Theories 

confirmation bias of, 260 

description of, 35 

dopamine, 527 

facial-affect program, 336 

facial-feedback, 336 

linguistic-relativity, 269-270 

operational definition of, 35 

significant outcome to confirm, 45 


l 
Id, 440 
Identical twins, 383 
Identity 
definition of, 416 
Erikson’s theory of psychosocial 
development on, 416f-417 
Identity-versus-role-confusion 
stage, 416 
Immersion vs. bilingual 
programs, 271-272 
Implicit Association Test (IAT), 601 
Implicit memory, 224-225 
Impression formation, 583-584 
Imprinting, 399 
In vitro fertilization (IVF), 392 
Income gender gap, 345f 
Independent variables, 42 
Individual differences vs. universal 
principles issue, 23f, 24-25 
Individualist orientation, 588 
Industrial-organizational (I/O) 
psychology, 595 
Industry-versus-inferiority stage, 404 
Ineffability, 158 
Inferential statistics 
definition of, A-14-A-15 
significant outcome of, A-15 
Inferiority complex, 446 
Infertility treatment, 391-392 
Information. See also Schemas 
confirmation bias of, 260 
decay or loss of, 233 


1-20 Subject Index 


forgetting, 233f-237 
how culture frames, 230 
language used to communi- 
cate, 265 
thinking as manipulating mental 
representations of, 243-249 
Information processing 
as approach to intelligence, 
281, 283 
description of, 408 
Information retrieval 
constructive process in memory 
for, 226-230 
explicit and implicit memory 
and, 224-225 
flashbulb memories and, 225-226f 
levels of processing for, 223-224 
priming phenomenon of, 
224-225 
retrieval clues used in, 222-223f 
tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon 
of, 222 
Informed consent 
definition of, 49 
of research participants, 49-50 
Informed consumers. See also 
Psychology 
adjusting to death, 432 
using behavior analyst to modify 
behavior, 194-195 
using biofeedback, 92-93 
choosing the right therapist, 573 
dealing effectively with anger, 613 
deciding when you need help, 539 
dieting and losing weight success- 
fully, 323-324f 
effective coping strategies, 483 
employer personality testing, 
467—468 
lowering risks of date rape, 375 
pain management strategies, 123 
scoring better on standardized 
tests, 292-293f 
sleeping better, 152 
strategies for improving 
memory, 237 
thinking critically and 
creatively, 262-263 
understanding drug use, 169 
Ingroups, 599-600 
Inhibited ejaculation, 374 
Inhibited sexual desire, 374 
Inhibitory message, 66 
Initiative-versus-guilt stage, 404 
Instincts, 309-310 
Intellectual ability 
intellectually gifted, 297-298 
mental retardation (or intellectual 
disabilities), 278, 295-297, 390 
Intellectually gifted, 297-298 
Intelligence. See also Thinking 
adaptive and standardized testing 
of, 292-293f 
artificial, 286 
assessing, 287-292 
biological basis of, 283-284 
considering the range and varieties 
of, 278, 279-280 
crystallized, 281, 287f, 429 
definition of, 280 
fluid, 280-281, 287f, 429 
Flynn effect and flexibility of, 
302-303 
g or g-factor of, 280 
genetic and environmental determi- 
nants of, 300-303 
practical and emotional, 284-286, 
287f 
separate brain areas involved in 
different kinds of, 284f 
theories of, 280-287f 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Trukese navigation technique 
example of, 279 
Intelligence quotient (IQ) 
bell-shaped distribution of, 289f 
definition of, 286 
deviation IQ scores, 289 
gauging intelligence using contem- 
porary, 289-291 
reliability and validity of, 291-292 
testing for, 287-289, 300-301 
Intelligence quotient (IQ) tests 
age differences and, 383-384 
bias against minority groups, 
300-301 
correlation coefficient of mothers/ 
daughters scores, A-17 
culture-fair, 301 
debate over racial differences 
in, 462 
Flynn effect and, 302-303 
group, 291 
older adults and, 428 
Stanford-Binet Intelligence 
Scale, 289 
Wechsler Adult Intelligence 
Scale-IV (WAIS-IV), 290f 
Wechsler Intelligence Scale for 
Children-IV (WISC-IV), 290 
Intelligence theories 
fluid and crystallized intelli- 
gence, 280-281, 287f 
Gardner’s multiple intelli- 
gences, 281-282f, 287f 
information-processing approach 
to intelligence, 281, 283, 287f 
Interactionist approaches to language 
development, 269 
Interference 
definition of, 234 
proactive and retroactive, 
234-235f 
Intermittent (or partial) reinforcement 
schedule, 189 
Interneurons, 72 
Interpersonal attraction, 604-605 
Interpersonal intelligence, 282 
Interpersonal therapy (IPT), 558 
Intersex, 370 
Intimacy-versus-isolation, 417 
Intrapersonal intelligence, 282f 
Intrinsic motivation, 313 
Introspection, 15 
Is This Aggression? quiz, 609f 


J 
“Jabberwocky” (Carroll), 265 
James-Lange theory of emotions, 
331-332f 
Jet lag, 151 
Judgment 
evaluating solutions, 257 
first impressions, 451 
impression formation, 583-584 


K 

Kidneys, 76f 

Kinsey sexual behavior surveys 
(1930s), 364, 368f 

Kitty Genovese bystander case, 32, 
34-35, 41—46f, 611 

Kohlberg’s theory of moral develop- 
ment, 414-415 

Korsakoff’s syndrome, 236 

Kiibler-Ross stages of grief, 432 


L 


Language. See also Communication 
animals and use of, 270-271 
association areas of the brain 

and, 87-88 


brain hemisphere and processing 
of, 90-91 
definition of, 265 
development of, 266-269 
grammar system of, 265-266 
hearing and perception of speech 
and, 117 
influence on thinking by, 269-270 
sign, 266, 270-271 
US. diversity of, 272f 
Language development 
babbling, 266 
interactionist approaches to, 269 
learning theory approaches, 
267-268 
nativist approaches to, 268 
production of language and, 
266-267 
Language-acquisition device, 268 
Latency period, 442 
Latent content 
of dreams, 147 
psychoanalysis to uncover, 546 
Latent learning, 198-200 
Lateralization (brain hemisphere), 90 
Learned helplessness, 481, 523 
Learning 
analytical versus relational 
approaches to, 203f 
biological constraints on, 193 
biologically prepared for, 181 
classical conditioning, 175-178 
cultural influences on, 202-203 
definition of, 175 
extinction phenomenon of, 
179f-180 
latent, 198-200 
observational, 200-203, 452, 551, 
599, 610 
scaffolding to facilitate, 410 
Learning styles 
comparing analytical vs. rela- 
tional, 203f 
cultural differences in, 202-203 
Learning-theory approach to lan- 
guage, 267-268 
Leptin, 321 
Levels-of-processing theory, 223-224 
Licensed professional counselors, 545f 
The Life Orientation Test-Revised 
quiz, 463f 
Life review, 432 
Light 
eye response to, 105f-106 
visible spectrum of, 104f 
Light therapy, 123 
Limbic system 
description of, the, 83-84 
functions of the, 84f 
Linear perspective, 129 
Linguistic intelligence, 282f 
Linguistic-relativity hypothesis, 
269-270 
“Little Albert” conditioning case 
study, 178 
Liver, 76f 
Logical-mathematical intelligence, 282f 
Long-term memory 
constructive process in recall- 
ing, 226-230 
definition of, 210 
explicit and implicit, 224-225 
flashbulb memories, 225-226f 
levels of processing, 223-224 
modules of, 215-216f 
rehearsal transfer from short-term 
to, 213 
retrieval clues, 222-223f 
semantic networks and, 216-217f 
three-stage model of memory and, 
210f, 214-217 


Long-term potentiation, 219-220 
Longitudinal research, 384 
Love 
companionate, 605-606 
marriage partner and, 607f 
passionate (or romantic), 605 
three components of, 606-607f 
LSD (lysergic acid diethylamide), 163f, 
168-169 


Major depression, 521 
Male circumcision, 360 
Mania, 522f-523 
Manifest content 
of dreams, 147, 148f 
psychoanalysis to move beyond 
the, 546 
MAO inhibitors, 565f 
Marijuana 
as hallucinogen, 163f, 168 
level of teenage use, 168f 
Marital sex, 367f 
Marriage. See also Divorce 
adulthood development and, 425 
rank ordering of desired character- 
istics in a mate, 607f 
same-sex, 363 
Maslow’s hierarchy of needs, 313-315 
Mass media 
stereotypes reinforced by, 599 
violence influenced by, 201-202 
“Master gland” (pituitary gland), 
76f-77 
Masturbation, 362, 364-365f 
MDMA (Ecstasy), 163f, 168-169 
Mean 
comparing median, mode, 
and, A-8 
definition of, A-6 
finding the average, A-6 
finding the middle, A-7 
standard deviation from, 
A-11f-A-12f 
Means-ends analysis, 255-256 
“Media copycat” killings, 201 
Medial temporal lobes, 217 
Median 
comparing mode, mean, and, A-8 
definition of, A-7 
Medical advice 
communication role in compliance 
with, 
493—494f 
following, 492—495 
increasing compliance with, 
494—495 
Medical perspective of abnormal- 
ity, 505f-506 
Medical student’s disease, 539 
Medication. See Drugs 
Meditation 
definition of, 156 
regulating state of consciousness 
through, 156-158 
Medulla, 76f, 83f 
Memory 
autobiographical, 229f 
definition of, 209 
explicit and implicit, 224-225 
eyewitness, 227-229 
flashbulb, 225-226f 
hyperthymestic syndrome of, 208 
increased span with age, 409f 
long-term, 210f, 214-217, 222-230 
neuroscience of, 217-221 
older adults and changes in, 430 
recalling, 222-230 
repressed and false, 228-229 
role sleep in, 144 
sensory, 210f-211 


short-term, 210f, 211-213 

source amnesia of, 225 

strategies for improving, 237 
studies on erasing traumatic, 220 


three basic processes of, 209f-210f 
three-system approach to, 209-210f 


tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon 
of, 222 
working memory, 213-214f 
Memory dysfunctions 
Alzheimer’s disease as, 235-236f 
amnesia as, 236 
Korsakoff’s syndrome as, 236 
strategies for improving, 237 
Memory failure 
proactive and retroactive interfer- 
ence and, 234-235 
reasons for, 233f—234 
research on causes of, 232f-233 
strategies for improving, 237 
Memory traces, 233 
Men. See also Gender differences 
brain lateralization by, 90-91 
range of ages for major sexual 
changes, 413f 
sense of smell by women ver- 
sus, 118-119 
sexual biology of, 356-357f 
sexual response cycle for, 359f 
Menopause, 423-424 
Mental age, 288 
Mental images 
definition of, 244f-245 
thinking as manipulation of, 
243-249 
Mental problems. See also 
Psychological disorders 
psychophysiological disorders 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Kohlberg’s theory of, 414-415 
in women, 415-416 


Morality (caring), 415-416 
Mothers 


correlating IQ scores of daughters 
and, A-17f 

prenatal development and 
physical/emotional state 
of, 390-391 


Motion perception, 131 
Motivation 


Alfred Adler on quest for self- 
improvement, 446 

applying different approaches 
to, 315f-316 

Aron Ralston’s story on, 309, 
315-316 

arousal approaches to, 311 

cognitive approaches to, 313 

definition of, 309 

drive-reduction approaches 
to, 310-311f 

hunger and eating needs 
and, 317-319f 

incentive approaches to, 311-313 

instincts and role in, 309-310 

intrinsic and extrinsic, 313 

measuring achievement, 325 


Motivational needs, 313-315 
Motor area of cortex, 86 
Motor (efferent) neurons, 72 
Miiller-Lyer illusion, 133f 
Multitasking, 101 

Musical intelligence, 282f 
Myelin sheath, 62f 


and, 476-477 
reported by college 
students, 535f 
Mental retardation (or intellectual 
disabilities). See also People 
with disabilities 
definition of, 295-296 
down syndrome, 278, 296, 390 
familial retardation, 296 
identifying the roots of, 296 
integrating individuals with, 
296-297 
Mental set, 259 
Metabolism, 319 
Metacognition, 408-409 
Methadone, 167 
Methamphetamine (“Meth”), 163 
Midbrain, 81, 83f 
“Midlife crisis,” 424 
Midlife transition, 424-425 
Milgram’s obedience experiment 
(1960s), 595-596f 
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality 
Inventory-2 (MMPI-2), 
463-464 f, 465 
Mirror neurons, 64, 200-201f 
Mnemonics, 213 
Mode 
comparing median, mean, 
and, A-8 
definition of, A-7 
Money and happiness, 497-498 
Mood disorders 
causes of, 523-524 
definition of, 520 
major depression, 521 
mania and bipolar disorder, 
522f-523 
A Test for Depression quiz, 521f 
Mood stabilizers, 566-567 
Moral development 
brain research on moral deci- 
sion, 613 


N 
N 


N 
N 


N 
N 
N 
N 
N 
N 
N 
N 


N 


N 


N 





Narcissistic personality disorder, 530 
arcolepsy, 150 
arcotics 
overview of, 163f-164f 
pain management using, 167-168 
arcotics Anonymous, 559 
ASA’s Columbia shuttle disaster 
(2003), 591-592 
ational Council on Alcoholism, 169 
ational Institute on Alcohol Abuse 
and Alcoholism, 169 
ational Institute on Drug Abuse, 169 
ative Americans, 419 
ativist approach to language devel- 
opment, 268 
aturalist intelligence, 282f 
aturalistic observation 
compared to other methods, 45f 
description of, 37-38 
ature (heredity) vs. nurture (environ- 
ment) issue, 23f-24 
ature-nurture issue 
description of, 381-382 
determining relative influence 
of, 383 
azi “only following orders” 
defense, 596 
eed for achievement, 325 


Need for affiliation, 325-326 
The Need for Cognition quiz, 580f 
Need for power, 326 


Needs 


achievement and success, 325 
affiliation and friendship, 325-326 
hunger and eating, 317-324f 
Maslow’s hierarchy of, 313-315 
power and influence, 326 
self-actualization, 314f 


Negative correlation, 39-40f 


N 


egative reinforcers, 186, 187f 


Negative relationship 


definition of, A-15, 39-40f 
scatterplots showing, A-16 


Nembutal, 162f, 167 


Neo-Freudian psychoanalysts, 445—446 
Neonates (newborns) 


definition of, 393 

development of the senses, 
394-395f 

movement development of, 394f 

reflexes of, 393-394 


Nervous systems 


biofeedback used to recover from 
damage to, 92-93 

CNS (central nervous system), 
70-71f 

divisions of autonomic, 72-73f 

drug use impact on, 160f 

evolutionary foundations of 
the, 73-75 

neurons linking the, 70-72 

PNS (peripheral nervous sys- 
tem), 70, 71f-72 

spinal cord and, 70-71f 


Neurogenesis, 88 
Neurons 


definition of, 61 

feature detection by visual 
stimuli, 109 

how they fire electrical charge, 
62-64f 

interneurons, 72 

memory at the level of, 219-221 

mirror, 64, 200-201f 

motor (efferent), 72 

nervous systems linked by, 70-72 

neurogenesis of, 88 

research on personality traits and 
related brain, 457f 

sensory (afferent), 72 

structure of, 61-62f 

synapse between two, 65f 


Subject Index 1-21 


Nicotine. See also Smoking 
fetal development and, 391 
as stimulate drug, 162 
Night terrors, 150 
Nightmares, 147 
Nine-dot problem solution, 265f 
Noise stimulus, 100-101 
Noncompliance 
effective communication to pre- 
vent, 493-494f 
forms of, 492-493 
Nontasters, 119 
Nonverbal communication, 349 
Norm of reciprocity, 594 
Normal distribution, A-8f 
Norms 
debate over race and ethnicity 
used to establish, 462-463 
group formation of, 590 
premarital sex and changing, 
365-367 
psychological tests based on, 462 
reciprocity, 594 
test measure, 291-292 
Not-so-free sample, 594 
Nutrition, 390 
Nutritionist, 320 


Oo 
Obedience 
definition of, 595 
Milgram’s shock experiment 
on, 595-596f 
Nazi “only following orders” 
defense of, 596 
Obesity 
BMI (body mass index) to 








Neuroplasticity, 88-89f 
Neuroscience 


measure, 317, 318f 
definition of, 317-318 
nontasters and, 119 


perspective of behavioral, 6, 7f, 18, 
19, 23f, 217-221, 333-334 
social, 26, 600 


Neuroscience in your life. See also 


Behavioral neuroscience; Brain 
bilingualism and the brain, 273 
brain areas involved in different 
kinds of intelligence, 284f 
brain research on prejudice, 600 
categorizing our world using con- 
cepts, 246f 

cognitive processing in anorexic 
patients, 323f 

experience, memory, and the 
brain, 219f 

how behavioral therapy changes 
your brain, 552f 

lack of sleep impact on emo- 
tions, 146f 

learning through observation, 201f 

moral decisions and the brain, 613 

research on neurons and relation- 
ship to personality, 457f 

seeing vision in the brain, 110f 

stress and social support, 482 

understanding OCD (obsessive- 

compulsive disorder), 517 
why it’s so difficult to quit, 488f 
young infants recognition of emo- 

tions, 396f-397 


Neurotic anxiety, 443 
Neuroticism, 449f 
Neurotransmitters 


behavior and role of, 66-68 

description of, 65f 

excitatory and inhibitory messages 
of, 66 

glutamate, 565 

names of specific, 67f 

reuptake of, 66 


Neutral stimulus, 176, 177f, 178 


nutritionist role in treating, 320 
roots of, 321-322 
understanding and preventing, 21 
Object permanence, 405 
Observable behavior vs. internal men- 
tal processes, 23f, 24 
Observational learning 
aggression through, 610 
behavior therapists use of, 551 
description of, 200-201, 452 
learning styles and, 202-203 
media violence and, 201-202 
mirror neurons and role in, 201f 
prejudice and discrimination, 599 
Obsession, 515 
Obsessive-compulsive disorder 
(OCD), 515-517 
Occipital lobes, 85f 
Oedipal conflict, 442 
Older adults 
adjusting to death, 432 
life review, 432 
memory changes in, 430 
social world of, 431-432 
video games improving cognitive 
functioning, 429 
Olfactory bulb, 83f 
Olfactory cells, 118, 119 
On the Origin of Species (Darwin), 8 
“Only following orders” defense, 596 
Operant conditioning 
the basics of, 184-193 
biological constraints on, 193 
comparing classical and, 
193-194f 
definition of, 183 
discrimination and generalization 
in, 191 
positive /negative reinforces and 
punishment, 185-186 
reinforcement of, 184-185f 


1-22 Subject Index 


reinforcement versus punish- 
ment, 186-189 
schedules of reinforcement 
for, 189-191 
shaping through, 192 
Thorndike’s law of effect, 183-184f 
Operant conditioning techniques, 
550-551 
Operational definition, 35 
Opponent-process theory of color, 112 
Opportunity NYC, 188 
Optic chiasm, 108 
Optic nerve, 107f-108, 109f 
Optogenetics, 81 
Oral histories, 229-230 
Oral stage, 441-442 
Organic mental disorders. See also 
Alzheimer’s disease, 531 
Orgasm, 359 
Outgroups, 599 
Ovaries 
endocrine system and, 76f 
puberty and development of, 
412-413f 
Overeating coping strategy, 480 
Overgeneralization of language 
rules, 267 
Ovulation, 356 
Oxycodone, 163f, 168 


P 


Pain 
gate-control theory of, 122 
sense of, 120-123 
strategies to fight chronic, 123, 156 
Pancreas, 76f 
Panic disorder, 514-515 
Paraphilias, 531 
Parasympathetic division, 72-73f 
Parathyroids, 76f 
Parenting styles 
four types of, 401-402f 
social development and, 402-403 
Parents-adolescent relationship, 419 
Parietal lobes, 85f 
Parkinson’s disease, 67 
Parthenon, 131-132f 
Partial (or intermittent) reinforcement 
schedule, 189 
Participant expectations, 53 
Participants 
experimental research, 42 
importance of using representa- 
tive, 51f 
informed consent of research, 49-50 
pros and cons of college students 
as research, 50f 
random assignment to condi- 
tion, 42-43 
Passionate (or romantic) love, 605 
Patients 
communication between health- 
care providers and, 493-494f 
compliance by, 493-495, 593-596f 
deinstitutionalization and mental 
health, 570f 
Pavlov’s classical conditioning, 16f, 
176-177f, 180 
Peers 
childhood relationships with, 400 
“Fearless Peer” model, 551 
People with disabilities. See also 
Mental retardation (or intellec- 
tual disabilities) 
Chris Burke’s contributions as, 278 
thought-based interface technol- 
ogy helping, 82 
Perception 
constancy, 130-131 
definition of, 99 
depth, 129-130, 133-134f 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


illusions or deceptions of, 131-135 
motion, 131 
top-down and bottom-up process- 
ing, 127-128 
Perceptual constancy, 130-131 
Perceptual disorders, 525 
Perceptual illusions 
devil's tuning fork, 133f 
ESP (extrasensory perception), 135 
Miiller-Lyer illusion, 133f 
Parthenon example of, 131-132f 
subliminal, 134 
visual, 132-134f 
Peripheral nervous system (PNS) 
automatic division of, 71f, 72 
definition of, 72 
overview of the, 70, 71f-72 
somatic division of the, 71f, 72 
Peripheral route processing, 580 
Peripheral vision, 106 
Permissive parents, 401—-402f 
Permissiveness with affection, 366 
Person-centered therapy, 556-557 
Personal fables, 418 
Personal stressors, 474, 476f 
Personality 
biological and evolutionary 
approaches to, 454—457f 
comparing approaches to, 459f 
defense mechanisms of the, 442-444 
definition of, 438 
first impressions judgment of 
others,’ 451 
Freud’s psychoanalytic theory on 
the unconscious, 439-444 
gender differences related to, 
348-349f 
humanistic approaches, 457-459 
id, ego, and superego model of, 
440-441 
inherited roots of, 455f-456 
learning approaches to, 451-454 
psychodynamic approaches to, 439 
psychosexual stages of developing, 
441-442 
research on neurons and relation- 
ship to, 457f 
temperament of, 455-456 
trait theory on, 448-451 
Personality assessment 
behavioral assessment, 466-468 
employer personality testing, 
467-468 
Implicit Association Test (IAT), 601 
The Life Orientation Test-Revised 
quiz for, 463f 
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality 
Inventory-2 (MMPI-2) for, 
463-464 f, 465 
projective personality tests, 465 
psychological tests for, 461-462 
Rorschach test (inblots), 465/466 
self-report measures of, 463-465 
Thematic Apperception Test 
(TAT), 466 
Personality disorders 
antisocial (sociopathic), 529-530 
borderline personality dis- 
order, 530 
definition of, 529 
narcissistic personality 
disorder, 530 
Personality learning approaches 
B. F. Skinner’s behaviorist 
approach, 452 
evaluating, 454 
self-efficacy, 452 
self-esteem, 453f 
situationism and CAPS theory, 
453 
social cognitive approaches, 452 


Personality psychology 
assessing personality, 461-468 
biological and evolutionary 
approaches, 454—457f 
comparing approaches, 459f 
definition of, 6, 7f 
humanistic approaches, 457-459 
learning approaches, 451-454 
psychodynamic approaches, 
439-446 
trait approaches, 448-451 
Personality types 
hardy, 481-482 
research on cancer and, 487 
Type A, B, and D, 485-486 
Persuasion 
central route processing for, 580 
changing attitudes through, 
579-580 
The Need for Cognition quiz, 580f 
peripheral route processing 
for, 580 
routes to, 580-581 
PET (positron emission tomography), 
79, 80f 
Phallic stage, 442 
Phenobarbital, 162f, 167 
Phenylketonuria (PKU), 390 
Pheromones, 119 
Phobias, 178-179 
Phobic disorder, 513-514f 
Phonemes, 265 
Phonology, 265-266 
Physical activity calorie expendi- 
tures, 324f 
Physical development 
adolescence, 412-420 
adulthood, 423-424 
average heights and weights dur- 
ing childhood, 398f 
childhood, 397 
size of head relative to body 
during, 398f 
Physicians 
effective communication between 
patients and, 493-494f 
following medical advice of, 
492-495 
Piaget’s theory of cognitive develop- 
ment, 405/408 
Pineal, 76f 
Pituitary gland 
endocrine system and the, 76f-77 
location in the brain, 83f 
PKM-zeta, 220 
Place theory of hearing, 116 
Placebo, 53 
Plateau phase, 359 
Pleasure principle, 440 
Pons, 81, 83f 
Population 
definition of, A-14 
sample of, A-14 
Positive correlation, 39-40f 
Positive reinforcers, 185-186, 187f 
Positive relationship 
definition of, A-15, 39-40f 
scatterplots showing, A-16 
Postal (video game), 202 
Posterior pituitary gland, 76f 
Posttraumatic stress disorder. See 
PTSD (posttraumatic stress 
disorder) 
Power 
need for, 326 
sexual harassment as motivated 
by, 347 
Practical intelligence, 284—285f, 287f 
Preconscious, 439-440 
Predisposition model of schizophre- 
nia, 529 


Prefrontal lobotomy, 568 
Prejudice 
brain research on effects of, 600 
definition of, 598 
foundations of, 599-600 
measuring, 601 
reducing consequences of, 601-602 
Premarital sex, 365-367 
Premature ejaculation, 374 
Premenstrual dysphoric disorder, 
536-537 
Prenatal development. See also 
Developmental psychology 
basics of genetics related to, 
386-388 
early fetal, 388-389 
environmental influences, 390-391f 
genetic influences on the 
fetus, 389-390 
Preoperational stage, 405f—406 
Pressure sense, 120-123 
Primacy effect, 215 
Primary drives, 310 
Primary reinforcers, 185 
Priming, 224-225 
Principle of conservation, 406, 407f 
Principles of Psychology (James), 16f 
Proactive interference, 234, 235f 
Problem solving 
algorithms used for, 248 
creativity and, 261 
evaluating or judging solu- 
tions, 257 
generating solutions, 255-257 
heuristic strategy for, 248-249 
hunches used for, 258 
impediments to, 258—260f 
preparation for, 252-255f 
Tower of Hanoi puzzle, 
251f, 252 
Problem-focused coping, 480 
Problems 
different kinds of, 252-254f 
evaluating solutions to, 257 
generating solutions to, 255-257 
impediments to solving, 258-260 
nine-dot, 265f 
representing and organizing, 
254f-255f 
subgoals dividing parts of, 256 
Problems of inducing structure, 252, 
253f, 254f 
Procedural memory, 215, 216f 
Progesterone, 356 
Projective personality tests, 465 
Prosocial behavior 
altruism, 612 
basic steps of helping, 611f-612 
definition of, 611 
Prototypes, 245, 247f 
Proximity gestalt law, 127f 
Prozac, 565f, 566 
Psychiatric social workers, 545f 
Psychiatrists, 545f 
Psychoactive drugs, 159 
Psychoactive substance use dis- 
order, 531 
Psychoanalysis 
Freud’s theory of, 439-444, 
546-547 
as treatment approach, 444 
Psychoanalysts, 545f 
Psychoanalytic theory 
description of, 439-440 
perspective of abnormality, 
505f, 506 
Psychodynamic perspective 
description of, 18, 19 
key psychology issues and posi- 
tion of, 23f 
on personality, 439 


Psychodynamic therapy 


contemporary approaches to, 547 
definition of, 546 
evaluating, 547-548 
Freud’s psychoanalysis ther- 
apy, 546-547 


Psychological Bulletin (journal), 135 
Psychological dependence, 159 
Psychological disorders. See also 


Mental problems 

abnormality, 503-508 

anxiety, 513-518 

childhood, 530-531 

controversies underlining our 
understanding of, 535-537 

different perspectives on, 505f-508 

dissociative, 518-520 

DSM-IV-TR diagnostic criteria 
on, 508-510 

molecular genetics and, 74-75 

mood, 520-524 

other, 531 

personality, 529-530 

preventing, 572 

problems reported by college 
students, 535f 

schizophrenia, 524-529 

social and cultural context of, 
535-537 

somatoform, 518 

study on prevalence of, 534 


Psychological tests 


debate over race and ethnicity 
norms used in, 462—463 

description of, 461 

norms as basis of, 462 


Psychologists 


breakdown of degrees among 
U.S., 10f 
career positions available to, 11-12f 
education of, 11 
a portrait of, 10-11 
work settings of, 9-10 


Psychology. See also Applying psychol- 


ogy; Developmental psychol- 
ogy; Informed consumers 

cognitive, 242-263 

community, 570f-571 

definition of, 5 

developmental, 6, 7t, 380, 381, 
393-410, 412-420 

evolutionary, 7f, 8, 73-75, 523 

experimental, 6, 7f, 41-46 

founding mothers/women in, 
17-18 

future trends and applications 
of, 26-27 

gestalt, 16f-17, 126f-127f, 128f 

health, 7f, 8, 472-498f 

industrial-organizational (I/O), 595 

key issues and positions taken 
in, 23f 

major milestones in development 
of, 16f 

personality, 6, 7f, 439-468 

roots of, 15-18 

subfields of, 6, 7f-9 

test your knowledge of, 5f-6 


Psychology perspectives 


behavioral, 18, 19-20, 23f, 505f, 506 

behavioral neuroscience, 6, 7f, 18, 
19, 23f, 217-221, 333-334 

cognitive, 18, 20, 23f, 123, 323f, 
505f, 506-507 

humanistic, 18, 20, 23f, 505f, 507 

psychodynamic, 18, 19, 23f, 439, 
546-548 


Psychoneuroimmunology (PNI) 


definition of, 472 
stress and, 479-480 


Psychophysics, 99 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Psychophysiological disorders, 
476-477 
Psychosexual stages 
definition of, 441 
latency, 442 
oral, 441-442 
phallic, 442 
Psychosocial development 
definition of, 403 
Erikson’s theory of, 403—404 
Psychosurgery, 568 
Psychotherapy 
behavioral approaches to, 548-552f 
cognitive approaches to, 552-554 
definition of, 544 
evaluating effectiveness of differ- 
ent approaches, 559-562 
getting help from the right 
person, 545f 
group, family, and self-help 
groups, 558-559 
HRT (habit-reversal training), 544 
humanistic, 556-558 
interpersonal, 558 
psychodynamic approaches 
to, 546-548 
racial and ethnic factors in, 562 
Psychoticism, 449f 
PsychWork 
advertising agency creator, 581 
case manager and substance abuse 
counselor, 557 
child protection caseworker, 403 
director of special education, 297 
human resources manager, 467 
licensed social worker, 10 
nutritionist, 320 
rehabilitation counselor, 86 
seeing eye guide dog trainer, 192 
sleep technologist, 150 
PTSD (posttraumatic stress disorder) 
anxiety disorder classification 
of, 517 
classical conditioning leading 
to, 178-179 
definition of, 474 
symptoms of, 474-475 
World Trade Center attack (2001) 
and, 474f 
Puberty 
definition of, 413 
myth versus reality of stormy 
adolescence during, 417-418 
physical and sexual changes dur- 
ing, 412-414 
Punishment 
definition of, 86 
reinforcement versus, 186-189 
types of, 187f 


Quitting smoking, 488/489 

Quizzes. See also Tests/testing 
BMI (body mass index), 318f 
Do You Seek Out Sensation?, 312f 
How Sensitive Are You? quiz, 100f 
How Stressful Is Your Life?, 477f 
Is this Aggression?, 609f 
The Life Orientation Test- 

Revised, 463f 

The Need for Cognition, 580f 
Sleep, 141f 
A Test for Depression, 521f 


R 
Race. See also Ethnicity 
as biological concept, 26 
divisions on acceptable names 
for, 26 
how behavior is influenced 


by, 25f-26 


Racial/ethnicity differences. See also 
African Americans; Asian 


Americans; Exploring diversity 


adolescent suicide and, 418-419 
alcohol consumption and, 165 
date rape and, 370-371 
IQ test basis and, 300-301 
learning styles and, 203 
masturbation and, 364 
psychological tests using norms 
of, 462-463 


psychotherapy consideration of, 562 


Random assignment to condition, 
42-43 
Range, A-10 
Rape 
date, 370-371 
definition of, 370 
Rational-emotive behavior ther- 
apy, 553 
Reactance, 492-493 
Reactive schizophrenia, 526 
Reality principle, 440 
Reasoning. See also Cognitive abilities 
algorithms and heuristics used 
for, 248-249 
definition of, 247 
Kohlberg’s theory on moral, 415f 
syllogistic, 247-248 
Recall memory task, 222 
Recency effect, 215 
Reciprocity-of-liking effect, 605 
Recognition memory task, 222-223f 
Reflexes 
definition of, 70 
neonates (newborns), 393-394 
Refractory period, 359 
Rehabilitation counselor, 86 
Rehearsal, 213 
Reinforcement 
definition of, 184-185 
positive, negative, and punish- 
ment, 186-189 
schedules of, 189-191 
Reinforcers 
negative, 186 
positive, 185-186 
primary and secondary, 185 
punishment versus, 186-189 
types of, 187f 
Relational learning style, 203f 
Relationship harmony, 453 
Relative size, 130 
Relaxation, 549f 
Relaxation techniques, 123 
Reliability, 291 
REM (rapid eye movement) sleep, 
143f-144, 149 
Replicated research, 46 
Representative participants 
college students as not being, 50f 
importance of using, 51f 
Repressed memories, 228-229 
Repression, 443 
Research. See also Scientific method 
archival, 37, 45f 
case study, 38-39, 45f 
cross-sectional, 383-384 
definition of, 37 
descriptive and correlation, 
39-4 0f, 45f 
experimental, 41—46f, 45f 
longitudinal, 384 


naturalistic observation, 37-38, 45f 


replicated, 46 
sequential, 384 
summary of different methods 
of, 45f 
survey, 38 
Research issues 
animal use in research, 52f-53 


Subject Index 1-23 


avoiding experimental bias, 53-55 
ethics, 49-51f 
Research subjects 
experimental research, 42 
importance of using representa- 
tive, 51f 
informed consent of, 49-50 
pros and cons of college students 
as, 50f 
random assignment to condi- 
tion, 42—43 
Resilience, 482 
Resolution stage, 359 
Resting state, 62 
Retina, 106 
Retrieval clues 
definition of, 222 
recalling long-term memories 
and, 222-223f 
Retroactive interference, 234-235f 
Retrograde amnesia, 236 
Reuptake, 66 
Road rage, 610 
Robot artificial intelligence, 286 
Rods (retina), 106 
Rohypnol (“date rape drug”), 162f, 167 
Rorschach test (inkblots), 465f-466 
RSDS (reflex sympathetic dystrophy 
syndrome), 120 
Rubella (German measles), 391f 


S 
Safer sex practices, 373 
St. John’s wort, 566 
Same-sex marriages, 363 
Sample (population), A-14 
Scaffolding, 410 
Schachter-Singer theory of emo- 
tions, 332f-333 
Schedules of reinforcement 
continuous, 189 
definition of, 189 
fixed-interval, 190f-191 
fixed-ratio, 189-190f 
partial (or intermittent), 189 
variable-interval, 190 
variable-ratio, 190f 
Schemas. See also Information 
gender, 354 
social cognition for creating, 583 
stored in memory, 226-227 
Schizophrenia 
biological causes of, 526-527 
brain changes with, 528f 
characteristics of, 524-526 
definition of, 524 
dopamine relationship to, 68 
DSM-IV-TR two classifications 
of, 526 
environmental perspectives 
on, 528 
multiple causes of, 529 
predisposition model of, 529 
risk based on genetic related- 
ness, 527f 
Rosenhan’s study on pretending 
symptoms of, 510 
Type I and Type II, 526 
types of, 525f 
unusual art created by individual 
with, 526f 
Schools. See also Education 
integrating individuals with men- 
tal retardation, 296-297 
sexual harassment reports from 
students, 347f 
special education, 296-297 
stereotype vulnerability of minor- 
ity students in, 602 
Science and Human Behavior 
(Skinner), 17f 


1-24 Subject Index 


Scientific method. See also Research 
definition of, 33 
hypotheses used as part of the, 35 
illustrated diagram of, 33f 
Latané and Darley’s bystander 
study as illustrating, 41-46f 
theories used as part of the, 34-35 
Seasonal affective disorder, 151 
Seconal, 162f, 167 
“Second shift,” 426 
Secondary drives, 310 
Secondary reinforcers, 185 
Secondary traits, 449 
Seeing eye guide dog trainer, 192 
Selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors 
(SSRIs), 565, 566f 
Self-actualization, 314f, 458f 
Self-affirmations testing, 47f 
Self-defeating personality disor- 
der, 535-536 
Self-determination theory, 315 
Self-efficacy 
definition of, 452 
as personality approach, 452 
Self-esteem 
cycles of, 453f 
definition of, 453 
gender differences related to, 
348-349f 
group membership to maximize 
sense of, 599 
happiness related to, 495-496 
Self-fulfilling prophecy, 598 
Self-help therapy, 559 
Self-service bias, 587 
Semantic memory, 215, 216f 
Semantic networks of memory, 
216-217f 
Semantics, 266 
Semicircular canals, 115f, 117-118 
Sensations 
definition of, 99 
Do You Seek Out Sensation? 
quiz, 312f 
incentive approaches to motiva- 
tion and, 311-313, 315f 
Sense of balance, 117-118 
Senses 
hearing, 114-118 
How Sensitive Are You? test 
on, 100f 
neonates (newborns) development 
of the, 394-395f 
smell, 118-119 
synesthesia condition of interact- 
ing, 123-124f 
taste, 119-120f 
touch, pressure, temperature, and 
pain, 120-123 
vision, 104—112f 
sensitive period, 389 
Sensorimotor stage, 405f 
Sensory adaptation, 102 
Sensory (afferent) neurons, 72 
Sensory area of cortex, 86-87 
Sensory memory 
definition of, 210 
three-stage model of memory 
and, 210f-211 
September 11th, 2001, 225, 474f 
Sequential research, 384 
Serial position effect, 215 
Serotonin, 67f, 68 
Sex differences. See Gender differences 
Sexism 
benevolent, 347 
definition of, 343 
on the job, 345f-347f 
sexual harassment as, 346-347f 
Sexual arousal disorders, 531 
Sexual behavior. See also Behavior 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


basic biology of, 356-357f 
diversity of, 362-375 
marijuana use affecting, 168 
phases of sexual response, 358-359f 
pheromones signaling interest, 119 
physiological aspects of sexual 
excitement, 358 
safer sex practices, 373 
Sexual behavior diversity 
approaches to sexual normal- 
ity, 363-364 
heterosexuality, 365-367 
historic changes in, 362 
homosexuality and bisexual- 
ity, 367-369 
masturbation, 362, 364-365f 
transsexualism, 369-370 
Sexual Behavior in the Human Female 
(Kinsey et al.), 364 
Sexual Behavior in the Human Male 
(Kinsey et al.), 364 
Sexual changes 
adolescence development and 
puberty, 412-414 
range of ages for major, 413f 
Sexual desire disorders, 531 
Sexual difficulties 
childhood sexual abuse, 371-372f 
rape and date rape, 370-371, 375 
sexual problems, 374 
STIs (sexually transmitted infec- 
tions), 372-373f 
Sexual disorders, 531 
Sexual fantasies, 358 
Sexual harassment 
description of, 346-347 
students reporting on, 347f 
Sexual infidelity 
current surveys on amount of, 367 
gender differences in perception 
of, 351 
Sexual orientation 
heterosexuality, 365-367f 
homosexuality and bisexual- 
ity, 367-369 
Kinsey sexual behavior surveys 
(1930s) on, 364, 368f 
transsexualism, 369-370 
Sexual response phases, 358-359f 
Sexually transmitted infections (STIs) 
definition of, 372 
estimates of new cases, 373f 
major types of, 372-373 
Shaping, 192 
Short-term memory 
definition of, 210 
rehearsal transfer to long-term 
from, 213 
three-stage model of memory and, 
210f, 211-213 
Sickle-cell anemia, 390 
Sign language 
babbling in, 266 
primates taught to use, 270-271 
Significant outcome, 45, A-15 
Similarity gestalt law, 127f 
Simplicity gestalt law, 127f 
Situational causes, 585-586 
Situationism, 453 
Skewed distribution, A-8f-A-9f 
Skin senses, 120-123 
Skin sensitivity, 121f 
Skinner box experiment, 185f 
Sleep. See also Dreams 
disturbances and problems, 
149-150 
how much is needed, 145f-146 
memory, thinking, and role of, 144 
REM (rapid eye movement), 
143f-144, 149 
stages of, 142-143f 


understanding, 141f 
Sleep disturbances 
description and types of, 149-150 
suggestions for coping with, 152 
Sleep Quiz, 141f 
Sleep technologist, 150 
Sleeptalkers, 150 
Sleepwalkers, 150 
Small intestine, 76f 
Smell sense (olfaction), 118-119 
Smoking. See also Nicotine 
adolescents and, 487, 489f 
international marketing promot- 
ing, 490 
neuroscience on why it’s so diffi- 
cult to quit, 488f 
reasons for, 487-488 
Smoking cessation 
difficulty of, 488-489 
hypnosis used for, 156 
“Sniff tests,” 118-119 
Social behavior. See also Violence 
aggressive, 608-610 
helping others, 611f-613 
interpersonal attraction, 604-605 
liking and love, 605-608 
Social cognition. See also Cognitive 
abilities 
as approach to personality, 452 
attribution biases: to err is 
human, 586-587 
attribution processes: understand- 
ing causes of behavior, 584-586 
definition of, 583 
impression formation, 583-584 
understanding what others are 
like, 583 
Social development 
adulthood, 424—425 
attachment and, 398-399 
childhood, 397-404 
Erikson’s theory of psychosocial 
development, 403-404, 
416f-417 
older adults and continued, 
431-432 
Social identity theory, 599 
Social influences 
culture, ethnicity, and race as, 
25f-26 
language and verbal skills and, 
90-91 
Social learning. See Observational 
learning 
Social neuroscience 
definition of, 600 
evolving field of, 26 
Social phobia, 514 
Social pressure, 593-595 
Social relationships. See also 
Friendship 
childhood development and 
peer, 400 
Facebook online, 606 
interpersonal attraction and, 
604-605 
liking and love and, 605-608 
marriage, 363, 425, 607f 
reciprocity-of-liking effect, 605 
relationship harmony, 453 
Social roles 
conformity to, 592-593 
gender-specific, 343-344, 426 
Social support 
definition of, 482 
research on brain activation 
and, 482f 
Social supporter, 591 
Socialization. See also Environmental 
factors 
definition of, 353 


gender differences in cognitive 
abilities and, 350f 
gender differences and impact 
of, 353-354 
gender schema produced 
through, 354 
moral development and gender- 
specific, 415-416 
Sociocultural perspective of abnormal- 
ity, 505f, 507-508 
Socioeconomic status-IQ test score 
relationship, 301 
Sociopathic (antisocial) personal- 
ity, 529-530 
Solution impediments 
examining the realities of, 258-259 
functional fixedness and mental 
set, 259f-260f 
inaccurate evaluation of solu- 
tions, 260 
Solutions 
forming subgoals, 256 
inaccurate evaluation of, 260 
judgment for evaluating, 257 
means-ends analysis for, 255-256 
nine-dot problem, 265f 
sudden awareness leading to, 
256-257 
Somatic division, 72 
Somatoform disorders, 518 
The Sopranos (TV show), 201 
Sound 
definition of, 114 
ear structure and sensing, 114—115f 
physical aspects of, 115-116 
range by age group, 117f 
theories of, 116-117 
Sound waves, 116f 
Source amnesia, 225 
Source traits, 449 
Space sickness, 118 
Spatial intelligence, 282f 
Special education 
educators involved in, 297 
integrating students with mental 
retardation, 296-297 
Spermarche, 413 
Spinal cord 
brain and the, 83f 
definition of the, 70 
nervous system and the, 71f 
Split-brain patients, 91-92f 
Spontaneous recovery, 179f-180 
Spontaneous remission, 560 
Spreading activation, 217 
SSRIs (selective serotonin reuptake 
inhibitors), 66 
Stage 1 sleep, 142f 
Stage 2 sleep, 142f 
Stage 3 sleep, 142f 
Stage 4 sleep, 142f 
Standard deviation 
calculating, A-11—A-12f 
definition of, A-11 
Standardized test scoring, 292-293f 
Stanford-Binet Intelligence Scale, 289 
Statistics 
definition of, A-5 
descriptive, A-5-A-9f 
inferential and correlation, 
A-14-A-17 
measures of variability, 
A-10-A-12 
Stereotype threat, 349 
Stereotype vulnerability, 602 
Stereotypes 
definition of, 598 
gender, 344f, 354 
mass media reinforcement of, 599 
prejudice and discrimination 
based on, 598-602 


Stereotyping 
definition of, 343 
gender, 344f 
Steroids, 77f 
Stimulants 
amphetamines, 162-163 
caffeine, 161f-162 
cocaine, 164 
definition of, 161 
nicotine, 162 
Stimulus 
absolute thresholds, 100-101 
adaptation to, 102 
CS (conditioned stimulus), 176, 
177f, 178 
definition of, 99 
difference thresholds, 101-102 
discriminative, 191 
feature detection by visual, 109 
habituation, 395 
neutral, 176, 177f, 178 
noise, 100-101 
UCS (unconditioned stimu- 
lus), 176, 177f, 178 
Weber’s law on, 102 
Stimulus control training, 191 
Stimulus generalization, 191 
Stomach, 76f 
Storytellers /oral history, 229-230 
Stress 
college students suffering 
from, 537 
coping with, 478f-479, 480-483 
definition of, 473 
GAS (general adaptation syn- 
drome) on course of, 478f-479 
high cost of, 475-480 
How Stressful Is Your Life? 
quiz, 477f 
PNI (psychoneuroimmunology) 
and, 472, 479-480 
resilience and, 482 
three major types of consequences 
of, 479f 
Stressors 
categorizing, 474-475 
nature of, 473-474 
Structuralism, 15 
Subjective well-being, 495 
Subliminal perception, 134 
Substance abuse counselors. See also 
Drug use/abuse, 557 
Substance P, 121 
Sudden awareness process, 256-257 
Sudden infant death syndrome 
(SIDS), 150 
Suicide of adolescents, 418-420 
Super-recognizers, 98 
Superego, 441 
Supertasters, 119 
Suprachiasmatic nucleus (SCN), 151 
Surgery 
pain management through, 123 
psychosurgery, 568 
Survey research 
compared to other methods, 45f 
description of, 38 
Sybil (film), 518 
Syllobistic reasoning, 247-248 
Sympathetic division, 72, 73f 
Synapse, 65f 
Synesthesia, 123-124f 
Syntax, 265 
Syphilis, 373, 391 
Systematic desensitization, 
549-550 


T 


Take a Taste Test, 120f 
Taste buds, 119 
Taste (gustation) 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


sense of, 119 
Take a Taste Test, 120f 
Tay-Schs Disease, 390 
Teenagers. See Adolescence 
Telegraphic speech, 267 
Telephone “hot lines,” 571 
Television violence, 201-202 
Temperament, 455-456 
Temperature sense, 120-123 
Temporal lobes, 85f 
Terminal buttons, 61 
Terrorism causality, 21 
Terry-cloth “monkey” attachment 
experiment, 399f 
A Test for Depression quiz, 521f 
Test standardization, 464 
Testes 
endocrine system and, 76f 
puberty and development of, 
412-413f 
Testosterone 
abuse by athletes of steroids 
and, 77 
gender behavior and fetal expo- 
sure to, 352 
marijuana use affecting, 168 
Tests /testing. See also Quizzes 
adaptive, 292 
employer personality testing, 
467-468 
IAT (Implicit Association Test), 601 
IQ (intelligence quotient), 287-292, 
383-384, 462 
Minnesota Multiphasic Personality 
Inventory-2 (MMPI-2), 463- 
464f, 465 
projective personality test, 465 
psychological, 461—462 
Rorschach test (inkblots), 465466 
standardized, 292-293f 
test standardization technique 
for, 464 
Thematic Apperception Test 
(TAT), 466 
Texting while driving, 21, 101 
Texture gradient, 130 
Thalamus, 83f 
That’s-not-all technique, 594 
Thematic Apperception Test (TAT), 
325, 466 
Theories. See also Hypotheses 
activation-synthesis, 147f, 149 
activity theory of aging, 431 
attribution, 585 
behavioral theories of depres- 
sion, 523 
biological and evolutionary 
approaches to personality, 
454—-457f 
Cannon-Bard theory of emo- 
tions, 332f 
CAPS (cognitive-affective process- 
ing system), 453 
cognitive learning theory, 198-203 
cognitive theory of depression, 523 
cognitive theory of schizophre- 
nia, 528 
description of, 34-35 
diffusion of responsibility, 35, 
41-46f, 611 
disengagement theory of 
aging, 431 
dreams-for-survival, 147f, 148-149 
Erikson’s theory of psychosocial 
development, 403-404, 
416f-417, 446 
fluid and crystallized intelligence, 
280-281, 287f 
frequency theory of hearing, 
116-117 
Freud’s psychoanalytic, 439-444 


frustration-aggression, 609-610 
Gardner’s multiple intelligences, 
281-282f, 287f 
GAS (general adaptation syn- 
drome), 478f-479 
gate-control theory of pain, 122 
genetic preprogramming theories 
of aging, 427, 428 
humanistic approaches to person- 
ality, 457-459 
information-processing approach 
to intelligence, 281, 283, 287f 
James-Lange theory of emotions, 
331-332f 
Jung’s collective unconscious, 
445-446 
Kohlberg’s theory of moral 
development, 414-415 
learning-theory approach to lan- 
guage development, 267-268 
levels-of-processing, 223-224 
opponent-process theory of 
color, 112 
Piaget’s theory of cognitive 
development, 405f—408 
place theory of hearing, 116 
Schachter-Singer theory of emo- 
tions, 332f-333 
self-determination, 315 
sexual orientation, 368-369 
Skinner’s behaviorist, 452 
social cognitive approaches to per- 
sonality, 452 
social identity, 599 
three dream, 147f-149 
trait, 448-451 
trichromatic theory of color 
vision, 111 
unconscious wish fulfill- 
ment, 147f-148 
wear-and-tear theories of 
aging, 427-428 
A Theory of Cognitive Dissonance 
(Festinger), 17f 
Theory of multiple intelligences, 
281-282f, 287f 
Thinking. See also Cognitive abili- 
ties; Intelligence 
artificial intelligence, 249 
cognitive dissonance of thoughts 
or, 582f-583 
concepts used when, 245-247f 
definition of, 243 
divergent and convergent, 261-262 
hunches and, 258 
influence of language on, 269-270 
mental images used when, 
244f-245 
motivation and role of, 313 
reasoning and, 247-249 
role sleep in, 144 
Thorndike’s law of effect, 183-184f 
The Three Faces of Eve (film), 518 
Three-system approach to memory, 
209-210f 
Thresholds 
absolute, 100-101 
difference, 101-102 
Thyroid, 76f 
Tip-of-the-tongue phenomenon, 222 
TMS (transcranial magnetic stimula- 
tion imaging), 79, 80f-81 
Top-down processing, 127-128 
Touch sense, 120-123 
Tourette’s syndrome, 544 
Tower of Hanoi puzzle, 251f, 252 
Trait theory 
definition of, 448-449 
evaluating approaches to personal- 
ity, 450-451 
Gordon Allport’s, 449 


Subject Index 1-25 


Traits 
Big Five personality, 449-450f 
cardinal, central, secondary, and 
source, 449 
central, 584 
definition of, 448-449 
halo effect of positive, 586 
hardiness, 481-482 
resilience, 482 
source, 449 
Transcranial magnetic stimulation 
(TMS), 567 
Transference, 547 
Transformation problems, 252, 
253f, 254f 
Transgenderism, 370 
Transsexuals, 369-370 
Treatment 
experimental research, 41-42 
placebo, 53 
Trephining, 15, 16f 
Trichomoniasis, 372-373 
Trichromatic theory of color vision, 111 
Tricyclic drug, 565f 
Trukese navigation technique, 279 
Trust-versus mistrust stage, 404 
12-step programs, 559 
Twin studies, 455 
Twins 
identical, 383 
nature-nurture issue and, 381 
Type A behavior pattern, 485 
Type B behavior pattern, 486 
Type D behavior pattern, 486 
Type I schizophrenia, 526 
Type II schizophrenia, 526 


U 
Ulysses (Joyce), 503 
Umami (taste category), 119 
Unconditional positive regard, 
458, 557 
Unconditioned response (UCR), 
176, 177f 
Unconditioned stimulus (UCS), 
176, 177f 
Unconscious 
collective, 445-446 
definition of, 439 
Freud’s psychoanalytic theory on 
the, 439-444 
Unconscious wish fulfillment dream 
theory, 147f-148 
Uninvolved parents, 402 
United States 
changes in marriage and divorce 
in the, 425 
diversity of language in, 272f 
female circumcision outlawed in 
the, 360 
individuals rating themselves as 
happy in the, 498f 
population suffering from major 
depression in the, 521 
World Economic Forum gender 
equality ranking of, 344f 
Universal grammar, 268 
Uplifts events, 475, 476f 
U.S. National Institute of Child Health 
and Development, 400-401f 


v 
Validity, 291 
Valium, 565f, 567 
Variability 
definition of, A-10 
range, A-10 
standard deviation, A-11f-A-12f 


1-26 Subject Index 


Variable-interval reinforcement sched- 
ule, 190f, 191 
Variable-ratio reinforcement sched- 
ule, 190f 
Variables 
correlation coefficient measure of, 
A-16-A-17 
definition of, 39 
independent and dependent, 42 
positive and negative correlations 
of, 39-40f, A-15 
Vestibular system, 117-118 
Video games 
improving cognitive functioning 
in older adults, 429 
violence of, 202 
Violence. See also Social behavior 
aggressive behavior and, 608-610 
gender differences in aggression 
and, 348 
media influence on, 201-202 
Visible spectrum, 104f 


www.urdukutabkhanapk.blogspot.com 


Vision. See also Eyes 
blind spot, 107-108f 
color blindness and color, 110-112 
comparing camera to, 105f 
peripheral, 106 
visible spectrum of, 104f 
Visual cortex, 334f 
Visual illusions, 132-134 
Vomeronasal organ, 119 
Vygotsky’s zone of Proximal develop- 
ment (ZPD), 409-410 


WwW 

Wear-and-tear theories of aging, 
427-428 

Weber’s law, 102 

Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale-IV 
(WAIS-IV), 290f 

Wechsler Intelligence Scale for 
Children-IV (WISC-IV), 290 

Weight set point, 319, 321-322 

Weightlessness, 118 


Wellness. See also Disease 
characteristics of happy peo- 
ple, 495-497 
contagion of happiness and, 496 
subjective well-being and, 495 
Wernicke’s aphasia, 87 
What Every Boy Should Know (1900s 
manual), 362 
WHO (World Health Organization), 
534, 536f 
WHO World Mental Health Survey 
Consortium, 534, 536f 
Withdrawal, 526 
Women. See also Gender differences 
biological factors of depression 
in, 524 
brain lateralization by, 90-91 
moral development in, 415-416 
premenstrual dysphoric disorder 
in, 536-537 
range of ages for major sexual 
changes, 413f 


“second shift” of, 426 
sense of smell by men versus, 
118-119 
sexual biology of, 356-357f 
sexual response cycle for, 359f 
stereotype threat experienced 
by, 349 
Working memory, 213-214f 
Working women, 426 
World Trade Center attack (2001), 
225, 474f 


X 


Xanax, 565f, 567 


Z 

Zone of Proximal development (ZPD), 
409f-410 

Zygote, 388 

Zygote intrafallopian transfer 
(ZIFT), 392